Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 03 August 2018
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 10 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00235

Gray Matter Volume and Cognitive Performance During Normal Aging. A Voxel-Based Morphometry Study

Stephen Ramanoël1* Elena Hoyau2 Louise Kauffmann2,3 Félix Renard4 Cédric Pichat2 Naïla Boudiaf2 Alexandre Krainik4,5 Assia Jaillard4 Monica Baciu2
  • 1INSERM/CNRS, Institut Vision, Sorbonne University, Pierre and Marie Curie Universities (UPMC) Paris 06, Paris, France
  • 2CNRS LPNC UMR 5105, University of Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble, France
  • 3CNRS, Grenoble INP, GIPSA-lab, University of Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble, France
  • 4UMS IRMaGe Grenoble Hospital, University of Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble, France
  • 5Grenoble Institute of Neuroscience, University of Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble, France

Normal aging is characterized by decline in cognitive functioning in conjunction with extensive gray matter (GM) atrophy. A first aim of this study was to determine GM volume differences related to aging by comparing two groups of participants, middle-aged group (MAG, mean age 41 years, N = 16) and older adults (OG, mean age 71 years, N = 14) who underwent an magnetic resonance images (MRI) voxel-based morphometry (VBM) evaluation. The VBM analyses included two optimized pipelines, for the cortex and for the cerebellum. Participants were also evaluated on a wide range of cognitive tests assessing both domain-general and language-specific processes, in order to examine how GM volume differences between OG and MAG relate to cognitive performance. Our results show smaller bilateral GM volume in the OG relative to the MAG, in several cerebral and right cerebellar regions involved in language and executive functions. Importantly, our results also revealed smaller GM volume in the right cerebellum in OG relative to MAG, supporting the idea of a complex cognitive role for this structure. This study provides a broad picture of cerebral, but also cerebellar and cognitive changes associated with normal aging.

Introduction

Normal aging is associated with various behavioral, cognitive and cerebral changes that affect multiple functions such as attention (Geerligs et al., 2014), working memory (Nyberg et al., 2012), processing speed (Seidler et al., 2010) and cognitive control (Coppin et al., 2006; Salthouse, 2010; Marchand et al., 2011). The degree of cognitive decline is not identical for all cognitive processes, i.e., some functions undergo more severe change than others. For example, while executive functions are typically the first to show impairment during normal aging, language abilities remain relatively intact (Meyer and Federmeier, 2010), or even improve in terms of vocabulary, semantics and speech processing (Kavé et al., 2009; Verhaegen and Poncelet, 2013; cited in Baciu et al., 2016). Cognitive abilities can be generally classified as domain-general (e.g., executive functioning) and domain-specific functions (e.g., language abilities; Fedorenko, 2014; Karmiloff-Smith, 2015; Karmiloff-Smith et al., 2016). A large body of evidence indicates that aging modifies the cerebral representation of domain-general and domain-specific cognitive processes in terms of functional reorganization. For instance, several studies have reported increased involvement of anterior frontal areas, relative to posterior occipital and medial temporal areas during tasks involving both domain-general and domain-specific cognitive processes associated with aging (Gutchess et al., 2005; Davis et al., 2008; see Wingfield and Grossman, 2006; Morcom and Johnson, 2015; Helder et al., 2016), suggesting supplemental recruitment of executive areas to perform a given task. The modulation of cerebral activity by performance is another important aging effect. Aging, however, is not a uniform process, old adults vary in the degree of their cognitive abilities’ decline. For example, Wierenga et al. (2008) showed that, in addition to the predominant left-hemispheric activation classically elicited during word retrieval, older adults tend to exhibit supplementary right hemispheric activation that is modulated by their performance: the higher the performance, the higher the activation in right inferior frontal cortex. Other studies also point to a dissociation between high- and low-performing older adults, with the latter exhibiting less hemispheric asymmetry reduction than the former (Cabeza et al., 2002).

In terms of brain anatomy, normal aging is associated with structural changes, on account of extensive gray matter (GM) atrophy (Raz et al., 2005; Driscoll et al., 2009). It is worth noting that, as is the case with cognitive functions, the spatial localization and degree of atrophy are not homogeneous across the brain in older adults (for a review see Fjell et al., 2014). The frontal and temporal lobes exhibit the highest degree of GM atrophy. Substantial changes have also been observed in the parietal lobe, whereas the occipital lobe appears to remain relatively intact. Recent findings have also reported GM volume reduction in the cerebellum (Good et al., 2001; Alexander et al., 2006; Abe et al., 2008) including a number of cerebellar regions that are mainly involved in cognitive rather than motor functions (Buckner, 2013).

In order to noninvasively assess the structural cerebral changes associated with normal aging in vivo, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) has now become a routine method in the neuroimaging community. The VBM method enables an automated, quantitative and objective evaluation of the tissue volume (GM volume) across the brain (Kurth et al., 2015). Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have demonstrated clear relationships between cognitive decline and the atrophy of specific brain regions. For example, reduced performance in episodic memory (EM) in normal older adults is correlated with reduced volume of the entorhinal cortex of the medial temporal lobe (Jessen et al., 2006). Similarly, executive deficits in normal aging are associated with greater atrophy of prefrontal regions (Raz and Rodrigue, 2006). However, only a limited number of longitudinal studies have evaluated the link between cognition in general and GM (Nyberg et al., 2010; Lovden et al., 2013; Pudas et al., 2013; Gorbach et al., 2017; Leong et al., 2017) and only a few were language-specific (Shafto et al., 2010).

In the present study, we evaluated the effect of age (older group, OG relative to middle-aged group, MAG) on GM volume, using a whole brain voxel-related GM analysis, in conjunction with a cognitive-score evaluation. To this end, we focused on how GM volume differs between MAG and OG in domain-specific regions involved in language and semantic memory but also in the domain-general regions involved in transversal cognitive processes and executive functions (i.e., the high-level cognitive abilities necessary for successful adaptive behavior to cope with complex situations). Moreover, given the fact that cerebellar atrophy occurs with age (Good et al., 2001; Alexander et al., 2006; Abe et al., 2008) and as more and more studies have emphasized the important role of this structure for cognition (see for example Sokolov et al., 2017) we also included a specific pipeline for cerebellum analyses. In comparison with standard pipelines for whole brain analyses, the present one allowed us to improve the accuracy of inter-subject alignment during normalization step and removal of supra-tentorial GM which could bias final results. We carried out a whole-brain analysis and specific cerebellar analysis to evaluate GM differences between middle-aged and older adults relative to middle-aged adults, we expected old adults to exhibit smaller GM volume in cerebral and cerebellar regions involved in both domain-general and specific processes, such as frontal areas involved in language processes and executive functioning. Furthermore, we expected this reduced GM volume to be associated with differences in performance in cognitive tests evaluating the respective processes.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Among the 31 healthy participants initially included, we retained 30 who were divided into two age groups: a MAG (N = 16; 11 males; mean age ± standard deviation (SD): 40.8 ± 8.6 years; range 30–57 years, and an older group (OG, N = 14; 10 males; mean age ± SD: 70.5 ± 6.6 years; range 59–84). The excluded participant was 84 years old and showed aberrant (superior to 2*SD OG) GM (543 cm3) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) volume (706 cm3) values relative to the mean CSF and GM volumes of the OG (CSF; mean = 389 cm3; SD: 107 cm3; GM: mean = 664 cm3; SD: 57 cm3). All participants were native French speakers; had a high level of education (mean ± SD MAG: 4 ± 0; OG: 3.85 ± 0.36; low-performing OG (lOG): 3.85 ± 0.36; high-performing OG (hOG): 3.85 ± 0.36) as measured by the Poitrenaud questionnaire (Kalafat et al., 2003). There was no difference in education level between groups (MAG vs. OG and lOG vs. hOG). Participants were right-handed (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; Oldfield, 1971); had normal or corrected-to-normal vision; and reported no history of neurological disorder or sensorimotor dysfunctions. The other inclusion criteria were the absence of general cognitive (Mini Mental State Examination, MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) or EM (“5 words” test; Dubois et al., 2002) deficits, as well as the absence of anxiety and depression (Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale, HAD; Zigmond and Snaith, 1983). Participants gave their written informed consent to participate in the study which was approved by the local ethics committee (CPP N°: 2014-A00569-38; Comité de protection des personnes Sud Est (South East People’s Protection Committee)). The demographic and inclusion criteria are reported in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic information and inclusion criteria for all participants.

Cognitive Assessment

Several cognitive tests were administered to evaluate the cognitive level of each participant in terms of domain-general and domain-specific processes. The domain-general evaluations included short-term memory (Digit Span Memory test; Weschler, 2008); processing speed and mental flexibility (Trail Making Test part B, TMT-B; Tombaugh, 2004); visual scanning and motor processes (Trail Making Test part A; TMT-A; Tombaugh, 2004); and a global evaluation of executive functions and frontal efficiency (Frontal Assessment Battery, FAB; Dartinet and Martinaud, 2005). Domain-specific evaluations concerned mainly language and semantic memory and included assessments of vocabulary and verbal intelligence (Mill-Hill vocabulary scale; Deltour, 1993); lexical retrieval and generation (picture naming, PN; DO-80, Metz-Lutz et al., 1991); semantic processing (Pyramid and Palm Tree test, PPT; Howard and Patterson, 1992) and the Verbal Automatisms test (Beauregard, 1971), which was used to determine levels of overlearned semantic information. As this test is sensitive to aging, older adults generally perform better than younger participants. Finally, we tested word fluency using a categorical fluency test (Cardebat et al., 1990), which assesses the following: integrity of lexico-semantic processes; strategic processes for searching and retrieval; and the integrity of phonetic and articulatory processes. Cognitive scores and significant differences between groups are reported in Table 2. Based on these scores, the OG was then subdivided into hOG and lOG subgroups (see “Results” section, Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean (Mean_MAG; Mean_OG) and standard deviation (SD_MAG; SD_OG) of scores obtained on cognitive tests in each group and statistical values (F, p) for the inter-group comparisons.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cerebral regions exhibiting an effect of age on the gray matter (GM) by comparing (a) middle-aged (MAG) > older (OG) adults; and (b) older (OG) > middle-aged (MAG) adults using two-sample t-tests.

MRI Acquisition

Magnetic resonance images (MRI) were acquired using a whole-body 3T Achieva Philips scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Netherlands) with a 32-channel head coil at the MRI facility IRMaGe in France. We acquired a T1-weighted high-resolution three-dimensional anatomical volume, by using a 3D Modified Driven Equilibrium Fourier Transform (MDEFT) sequence (number of slices = 160, echo time (TE) = 3.98 ms, repetition time (TR) = 25 ms, flip angle = 15°, field of view (FOV) = 256 × 240 × 160 mm, acquisition resolution 0.94 × 0.96 × 1.00 mm, acquisition matrix 272 × 249, reconstruction matrix 288 × 288, resolution reconstruction 0.89 × 0.83 × 1). None of the participants exhibited abnormalities in brain structures.

Data Processing

Cognitive Scores

The effect of age on cognitive scores was determined by performing a MANOVA analysis on the performance obtained for each test in MAG and in OG controlled for gender and socio-educational effect (see “Results” section, Table 2). Cognitive scores were normalized based on the mean and SD considering all participants. In order to evaluate how cognitive performance varies among older adults and classify OG participants in the hOG and lOG, we first evaluated the normal distribution (Kolmogorov-Smirnov) of the scores for each test. We controlled for outliers by examining that scores did not exceed three times the interquartile interval.

MRI Data

Whole Brain Analyses

Data processing was performed using SPM12 release 6685 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK1) implemented in MATLAB 7 (Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). We processed the data via Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration, using the Exponential Lie algebra algorithm (DARTEL, Ashburner and Friston, 2005; Ashburner, 2007) for the segmentation and normalization steps. The DARTEL segmentation procedure makes use of a number of tissue probability maps including GM, white matter (WM), CSF, soft tissue, skull and non-brain regions of the image. After segmentation, we performed a visual inspection and a quality check of the data, by applying the modules “display one slice for all images” and “check sample homogeneity using covariance” implemented in the VBM12 toolbox2. Next, the GM, WM and CSF tissue classes obtained during the segmentation step, were used to create a custom template based on our sample. For each participant, flow-fields were computed during template creation to provide the transformation matrix from each native image to the template. Finally, images obtained in the previous step were normalized to the MNI space (voxel size of 1 mm isotropic), modulated and smoothed using an 8-mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. Importantly for subsequent statistical analyses, the total intracranial volume (TICV) was computed from the GM, WM and CSF modulated images. Morphological analyses were performed with the general linear model (Friston et al., 1995) with SPM12 implemented in MATLAB 7. We performed a whole brain analysis using the GM images in a two-sample t-test to compare GM volume between MAG and OG. We used an absolute implicit mask with a recommended threshold value fixed at p > 0.2 for GM voxel analyses (Callaert et al., 2014). In addition, in order to prevent potential bias related to brain size and gender differences between groups, the TICV and gender were included in the statistical model, as covariates of no interest. Differences in GM volume were considered significant if they exceeded a voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE) corrected, with a minimum cluster extent of 20 voxels. Finally, we converted the MNI coordinates of voxels with maximal statistical significances into Talairach (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) coordinates, by using the MNi2TAL function Matthew Brett3 to facilitate comparisons with other studies.

Cerebellum Analyses

Based on results reported by previous studies showing the role of the cerebellum in several cognitive domains (Buckner, 2013) and to deal with the poor alignment performance for cerebellar structures during spatial processing steps of the whole-brain analysis, we performed a specific cerebellum GM voxel based analysis using the SPM toolbox SUIT (Diedrichsen, 2006; Diedrichsen et al., 2009; Spatially Unbiased Atlas Template4). This cerebellar-specific pipeline also allowed us to better remove supra-tentorial GM which could bias final results. First, for each participant, the anatomical scans were manually reoriented in order to set them to the origin (0, 0, 0; anterior commissure). Next, the Isolate function of SUIT was used to obtain segmentation maps of the cerebellum. Then, GM isolated maps were corrected to exclude images with the GM located outside the cerebellum. Finally, the images were normalized to the SUIT space using the new DARTEL flow fields obtained from the SUIT toolbox, modulated and smoothed, using a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.

Results

Cognitive Scores

Aging Effect

As shown in Table 2, and relative to the OG, the MAG were faster for TMT-A (visual scanning and processing speed; F(1,30) = −9.5 p = 0.005) and PPT (semantic processing; F(1,30) = −51.834, p < 0.05). The OG also produced more words than the MAG for categorical fluency (F(1,30) = 6.624, p = 0.016). For TMT-B (mental flexibility) the OG had lower scores than the MAG (TMT-B, F(1,30) = 23.737, p < 0.05). We did not observed difference between MAG and OG on the FAB (global frontal functioning; F(1,30) = 2.494, p = 0.126) and on the PN (F(1,30) = 0.814, p = 0.375). As expected, the OG had higher scores for verbal automatisms (F(1,30) = 10.975, p = 0.003) than the MAG. The cognitive-test results suggest that aging is mainly associated with lower performances for domain-general processes—and, to a lesser extent, for domain-specific processes.

MRI Morphological Results

Whole Brain

The whole brain voxel-based GM analyses and comparisons between groups revealed significantly smaller GM volume for OG compared to MAG in frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital regions, as well as of the cerebellum (Table 3 and Figure 1). More specifically, we observed bilateral GM decreased of the middle (BA 10) and inferior (BA 45, 47) frontal gyri, insula, supramarginal (BA 40), superior occipital (BA 18), cerebellum, as well as of the right angular (BA 39), superior temporal (BA 41), left postero-superior temporal (BA 22) and post-central (BA 3, 2, 1) gyri.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cerebral regions (whole brain analysis) showing the significant differences between groups (older adults vs. middle-aged adults) on the gray matter (GM) volume (in mm3) of (A) the frontal cortex; (B) the temporal and parietal cortices; and (C) the occipital cortex and cerebellum. The significance threshold for clusters and individual voxel level was defined as p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE), corrected for multiple comparisons (T > 5.77), with extent threshold defined as 20 voxels (voxel size = 1 mm3).

Cerebellum

The cerebellum voxel-wise GM analysis using the SUIT toolbox (Diedrichsen, 2006; Diedrichsen et al., 2009) revealed a significant cluster (340 mm3) in the right cerebellar hemisphere (p < 0.05 FWE corrected) reflecting decreased GM volume in the OG. The location of this region corresponds to the lobule crus I of the right cerebellar hemisphere (Figure 2). This result was displayed on a two-dimensional cerebellum template implemented in the SUIT toolbox (Diedrichsen and Zotow, 2015). Figure 3 shows a comparison of the two spatial processing pipelines used.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Cerebellum region (cerebellar analysis) showing significant differences between groups (older adults vs. middle-aged adults) on the GM (in mm3), and represented on a two-dimensional template with lobules indicated by roman numerals from I to X with a prepended H and V for the hemispheric and vernal compartment, respectively. On this image, the largest lobule HVII is divided into Crus I, Crus II (both corresponding to HVIIa) and HVIIb. Statistical significance threshold for the cluster level was set at p < 0.05 FWE corrected for multiple comparisons. We reported only one significant cluster (T = 4.99) belonging to the Crus I of the right cerebellum, with extent threshold defined as 20 voxels (voxel size = 1 mm3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Visual comparison of co-registration during spatial processing using (A) the SPM-DARTEL pipeline; and (B) the SUIT-DARTEL pipeline.

Discussion

The present study aimed to assess: (1) how aging impacts GM volume; in relation to; (2) how aging impacts performance in tasks involving both domain-general (i.e., executive functioning) and domain-specific functions (i.e., language and semantic memory).

The older adults included in this study exhibited: (a) decreased performance on tests reflecting executive functioning, frontal efficiency and processing speed; and (b) difficulty in retrieving and generating words and performing semantic processing. Older adults also showed a greater frequency of verbal automatisms. These findings are consistent with previous studies (Bherer et al., 2004; Zelazo et al., 2004; Huizinga et al., 2006; Collette and Salmon, 2014; Baciu et al., 2016; Boudiaf et al., 2016; Hoyau et al., 2017) showing a generally lower cognitive performance in the course of normal aging.

At the cerebral level, VBM analyses comparing the OG to the MAG revealed that aging was also associated with significantly smaller bilateral GM volume in frontal, temporal, parietal and occipital regions. Smaller GM volume in the frontal lobe concerned first the middle frontal gyrus, with a greater extent in the right hemisphere. This region is known to be involved in cognitive control and to play a key role in reorienting attention according to the task demands (Rossi et al., 2009; Japee et al., 2015). This pattern is consistent with a number of studies reporting changes in the capacity to switch between tasks or to disregard distractors in older participants (Rajah et al., 2008, 2010). GM volume differences also concerned the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, as well as the left superior temporal gyrus. These regions are involved in motor planning and articulation, and in executive processing related to accessing phonological representations (Hickok, 2009). Finally, significantly smaller GM volume of the bilateral insula, involved in word retrieval and generation (Abel et al., 2009) was observed in OG as compared to MAG, with an extensive cluster in the left hemisphere. Interestingly, the OG in the present study exhibited lower cognitive performance for a number of language tests reflecting lexical retrieval and generation and semantic processing, and recruiting executive functions such as switching, inhibition and strategic search abilities, as well as working memory processes (Troyer et al., 1997; Bryan and Luszcz, 2000). Together with behavioral results, the smaller GM volume of frontal areas in aging observed here therefore appears to be consistent with previous studies suggesting a relationship between executive decline and GM volume of the frontal lobe (Good et al., 2001; Tisserand et al., 2004; Lemaitre et al., 2012; Manard et al., 2016).

The age-related smaller GM volume observed in our study also pertained to regions that are part of a larger fronto-parietal network involved in attentional processes, including the right supramarginal and angular gyri. These regions are involved in attention processes related to executive functions (Seghier, 2012) and the control of attentional shifts in space (Chen et al., 2012) and exert top-down attention-switching control signals to visual areas (Rossi et al., 2009). This network, including notably the left frontal cortex as a hub, in relation to the dorsal attentional network (DAN) and the default-mode-network (DMN), have been also reported to play a key role to maintain the memory performance in normal and pathological aging (Franzmeier et al., 2017a,b). Finally, diminished GM volume of the bilateral superior occipital gyri and transverse temporal gyrus (primary auditory cortex) was also observed in the OG. This result is consistent with a previous finding that reported global thinning of the cerebral cortex, including the cerebral regions dedicated to perceptual processing with age (Salat et al., 2004). To sum up, our findings of age-related smaller GM volume in frontal, temporal and occipital cortical areas consistent with previous results, which suggest that decreased GM volume of these areas is associated with diminished executive efficiency (Tisserand et al., 2004). Although previous structural and functional studies on cognitive and GM changes in normal aging mostly focused on the cerebral cortex, the present study also addressed GM changes in the cerebellum, by using a specific analysis pipeline optimized for this structure. VBM analyses in the cerebellum revealed significantly smaller GM volume in the OG in the right lobule HVIIa, including the crus I and crus II regions (x = −36; y = 12; z = 4; t-value = 7.13). Over the past decades, evidence has indicated that cerebellar functioning extends beyond the scope of classical sensorimotor control and is related to domains such as attention, language, executive function and social cognition. In a recent opinion article, Sokolov et al. (2017) assert that cerebellar computations—based on of their extensive reciprocal connections with the frontal, parietal and temporal associative cortices—are universal across sensorimotor and associative processes, along with two key phenomena, prediction and error-based learning. Recent results show that the most human cerebellum maps are related to cerebral association networks. Furthermore, Kelly and Strick (2003) found that large regions near crus I and crus II exhibit connections with prefrontal cortex area 46 (which is involved in executive processes) in non-human primates. Importantly, this cortical circuit between the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex overlaps with regions dedicated to motor control (Buckner, 2013). Another study revealed that lobule HVIIa, including crus I, is connected to a large association network involved in executive control (Habas et al., 2009). The cerebellum is also significantly involved in language, social cognition and cognition in general (Buckner, 2013). Right crus I atrophy in older adults may be related to their lower semantic cognitive scores, given that this region is connected to the left cerebral regions that are thought to be involved in the semantic demands of task processing (Petersen et al., 1989; Stoodley, 2012). Overall, the smaller GM volume in the cerebellum observed in the OG extends previous findings on GM atrophy in normal aging related to cognitive abilities (Lemaître et al., 2005; Raz et al., 2005; Driscoll et al., 2009) and particularly to executive functions.

The present VBM study has a number of limitations. In particular, as this analysis is exclusively cross-sectional it provides no information concerning causality and temporal changes. This issue remains particularly relevant for gaining a greater understanding of the concurrent changes in cognitive scores and GM volume as they relate to cognitive performance. Indeed, one critical question remains regarding the extent to which smaller GM volume in older relative to middle-aged participant also varies according to cognitive performance. It is for example possible that older adults with higher cognitive performance exhibit less GM volume reduction than those with lower cognitive performance. For example, a recent study adopted a cross-sectional approach to structural MRI to investigate differences in GM volume in relation to cognitive performance (Nissim et al., 2017). Using a longitudinal approach, another study (Tisserand et al., 2004) carried out a 3-year comparison of GM density between a “stable” and a “decliner” group. These studies suggested a link between GM volume of the frontal lobe and cognitive performance, suggesting that GM volume in this region might be a predictor of cognitive functioning in older adults. When it comes to normal or pathological aging, cerebral reserve (Katzman, 1993; Satz, 1993) and cognitive reserve (Stern, 2002, 2009) are key concepts that need to be taken into consideration. Cerebral reserve designates the amount of cerebral deterioration that can be tolerated before a critical threshold is reached whose clinical or functional consequences are inevitable, whereas cognitive reserve refers to the ability to use the available cerebral reserve to perform a task flexibly and efficiently. With respect to this conceptual framework, several researches on animal and humans posit the benefic role for successful aging of a rich environment and a greater education to maintain cognitive performance relatively well in face of age-related brain modifications and pathology (Freret et al., 2015; Franzmeier et al., 2017a,b; Gelfo et al., 2018). However, due to the relatively small sample of older participant, further analyses comparing GM volume between low- and high performers could not be performed in the present study. Moreover, our sample size remains too week to discern subtle GM differences or a distinct pattern of results depending on the sex of the participants (Weiner et al., 2017). Further studies including a larger sample would thus allow to precisely test these hypotheses. Further research, including the collection of genetic, structural and functional brain connectivity information, is also needed on the individual trajectories of the brain structure in normal aging, and in relation to cognitive scores. Such research would help to make the distinction between transitional phases and normal cognitive and brain aging patterns in pathological settings.

Conclusion

The present VBM study aimed to assess the effect of aging on GM and to interpret results in relation to the cognitive performance. Overall, this study provided a broad picture on domain-general cognitive functioning, but also language-specific processes in normal aging, as well as the associated anatomical differences in the whole brain, including the cerebellum. A decrease of GM volume was observed in several regions in older compared to middle-aged adults, interpreted in relation to lower cognitive scores in tests assessing either domain general or specific processes. These results replicate and further support previous findings suggesting a general decline in cognitive processes associated with smaller GM volume in the course of normal aging. Moreover, these results emphasize the importance of taking into account the cerebellum structure when studying changes associated with normal aging, which clearly extend beyond the cerebral cortex. These findings open up new perspectives for the development and application of innovative training methods and programs that aim to promote successful normal aging.

Author Contributions

MB and NB: conceived the study. NB, CP, AK and AJ: data acquisition. SR, EH and FR: data processing. SR, EH, MB and LK: manuscript writing.

Funding

This research was funded by ARC2 “Qualité de Vie & Vieillissement” Région Rhône Alpes, France and by Institut Universitaire de France (IUF). The Grenoble MRI facility IRMaGe was partly funded by the French program “Investissement d’Avenir” run by the “Agence Nationale pour la Recherche”: Grant “Infrastructure d’Avenir en Biologie Santé” (ANR-11-INBS-0006).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants of this study for their valuable contributions. We thank the Grenoble MRI IRMaGE unit for enabling us to perform the MRI acquisitions.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/
  2. ^ http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de
  3. ^ http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging
  4. ^ http://www.diedrichsenlab.org/imaging/suit.htm

References

Abe, O., Yamasue, H., Aoki, S., Suga, M., Yamada, H., Kasai, K., et al. (2008). Aging in the CNS: comparison of grey/white matter volume and diffusion tensor data. Neurobiol. Aging 29, 102–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abel, S., Dressel, K., Kümmerer, D., Saur, D., Mader, I., Weiller, C., et al. (2009). Correct and erroneous picture naming responses in healthy subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 463, 167–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.07.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, G. E., Chen, K., Merkley, T. L., Reiman, E. M., Caselli, R. J., Aschenbrenner, M., et al. (2006). Regional network of magnetic resonance imaging grey matter volume in healthy aging. Neuroreport 17, 951–956. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000220135.16844.b6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J., and Friston, K. J. (2005). Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. Neuroimage 26, 839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018

CrossRef Full Text

Baciu, M., Boudiaf, N., Cousin, E., Perrone-Bertolotti, M., Pichat, C., Fournet, N., et al. (2016). Functional MRI evidence for the decline of word retrieval and generation during normal aging. Age 38:3. doi: 10.1007/s11357-015-9857-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauregard, A. (1971). Le Test des Automatismes Verbaux. Issy-les-Moulineaux: Editions Scientifiques et Psychotechniques.

Bherer, L., Belleville, S., and Hudon, C. (2004). Le déclin des fonctions exécutives au cours du vieillissement normal, dans la maladie d’Alzheimer et dans la démence frontotemporale. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 2, 181–189.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Boudiaf, N., Laboissière, R., Cousin, É., Fournet, N., Krainik, A., and Baciu, M. (2016). Behavioral evidence for a differential modulation of semantic processing and lexical production by aging: a full linear mixed-effects modeling approach. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 25, 1–22. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1257100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryan, J., and Luszcz, M. A. (2000). Measures of fluency as predictors of incidental memory among older adults. Psychol. Aging 15, 483–489. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L. (2013). Perspective the cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80, 807–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage 17, 1394–1402. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callaert, D. V., Ribbens, A., Maes, F., Swinnen, S. P., and Wenderoth, N. (2014). Assessing age-related grey matter decline with voxel-based morphometry depends significantly on segmentation and normalization procedures. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:124. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardebat, D., Doyon, B., Puel, M., Goulet, P., and Joanette, Y. (1990). Formal and semantic lexical evocation in normal subjects. Performance and dynamics of production as a function of sex, age and educational level. Acta Neurol. Belg. 90, 207–217.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chen, Q., Weidner, R., Vossel, S., Weiss, P. H., and Fink, G. R. (2012). Neural mechanisms of attentional reorienting in three-dimensional space. J. Neurosci. 32, 13352–13362. Available online at: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/32/39/13352.abstract

Google Scholar

Collette, F., and Salmon, E. (2014). Fonctionnement exécutif et réseaux cérébraux. Neuropsychologie 6, 256–266. doi: 10.1684/nrp.2014.0321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppin, A. K., Shumway-Cook, A., Saczynski, J. S., Patel, K. V., Ble, A., Ferrucci, L., et al. (2006). Association of executive function and performance of dual-task physical tests among older adults: analyses from the InChianti study. Age Ageing 35, 619–624. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dartinet, V., and Martinaud, O. (2005). La BREF, une batterie rapide d’évaluation frontale. NPG Neurol. Psychiatr. Geriatr. 5, 43–46. doi: 10.1016/s1627-4830(05)82606-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., and Cabeza, R. (2008). Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1201–1209. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deltour, J. J. (1993). Echelle de Vocabulaire de Mill Hill J.C. Raven. Adaptation Française et Normes Europeenes du Mill Hill et du Standard Progressive Matrice de Raven (PM38). Braine-le-Chateau: Editions l’application des techniques modernes.

Diedrichsen, J. (2006). A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 33, 127–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichsen, J., Balsters, J. H., Flavell, J., Cussans, E., and Ramnani, N. (2009). A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 46, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichsen, J., and Zotow, E. (2015). Surface-based display of volume-averaged cerebellar imaging data. PLoS One 10:e0133402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Driscoll, I., Davatzikos, C., An, Y., Wu, X., Shen, D., Kraut, M., et al. (2009). Longitudinal pattern of regional brain volume change differentiates normal aging from MCI. Neurology 72, 1906–1913. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a82634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, B., Touchon, J., Portet, F., Ousset, P. J., Vellas, B., and Michel, B. (2002). “The 5 words”: a simple and sensitive test for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Presse Med. 31, 1696–1699.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fedorenko, E. (2014). The role of domain-general cognitive control in language comprehension. Front. Psychol. 5:335. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fjell, A. M., McEvoy, L., Holland, D., Dale, A. M., and Walhovd, K. B. (2014). What is normal in normal aging? Effects of aging, amyloid and Alzheimer’s disease on the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus. Prog. Neurobiol. 117, 20–40. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzmeier, N., Göttler, J., Grimmer, T., Drzezga, A., Áraque-Caballero, M. A., Simon-Vermot, L., et al. (2017a). Resting-state connectivity of the left frontal cortex to the default mode and dorsal attention network supports reserve in mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:264. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzmeier, N., Hartmann, J. C., Taylor, A. N. W., Araque Caballero, M. A., Simon-Vermot, L., Buerger, K., et al. (2017b). Left frontal hub connectivity during memory performance supports reserve in aging and mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 59, 1381–1392. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freret, T., Gaudreau, P., Schumann-Bard, P., Billard, J.-M., and Popa-Wagner, A. (2015). Mechanisms underlying the neuroprotective effect of brain reserve against late life depression. J. Neural Transm. 122, 55–61. doi: 10.1007/s00702-013-1154-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Worsley, K. J., Poline, J. P., Frith, C. D., and Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1995). Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 189–210. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerligs, L., Saliasi, E., Maurits, N. M., Renken, R. J., and Lorist, M. M. (2014). Brain mechanisms underlying the effects of aging on different aspects of selective attention. Neuroimage 91, 52–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelfo, F., Mandolesi, L., Serra, L., Sorrentino, G., and Caltagirone, C. (2018). The neuroprotective effects of experience on cognitive functions: evidence from animal studies on the neurobiological bases of brain reserve. Neuroscience 370, 218–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, C. D., Johnsrude, I. S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N., Friston, K. J., and Frackowiak, R. S. (2001). A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14, 21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorbach, T., Pudas, S., Lundquist, A., Orädd, G., Josefsson, M., Salami, A., et al. (2017). Longitudinal association between hippocampus atrophy and episodic-memory decline. Neurobiol. Aging 51, 167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutchess, A. H., Welsh, R. C., Hedden, T., Bangert, A., Minear, M., Liu, L. L., et al. (2005). Aging and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 84–96. doi: 10.1162/0898929052880048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habas, C., Kamdar, N., Nguyen, D., Prater, K., Beckmann, C. F., Menon, V., et al. (2009). Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. J. Neurosci. 29, 8586–8594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helder, E. J., Zuverza-Chavarria, V., and Whitman, R. D. (2016). Executive functioning and lateralized semantic priming in older adults. Cogent Psychol. 3:1182687. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2016.1182687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G. (2009). The functional neuroanatomy of language. Phys. Life Rev. 6, 121–143. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2009.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, D., and Patterson, K. (1992). The Pyramids and Palm Trees Test: A Test of Semantic Access from Words and Pictures. Bury St. Edmunds, Suffolk: ThamesValley Test Company.

Hoyau, E., Boudiaf, N., Cousin, E., Pichat, C., Fournet, N., Krainik, A., et al. (2017). Aging modulates the hemispheric specialization during word production. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:125. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., and van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2017–2036. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Japee, S., Holiday, K., Satyshur, M. D., Mukai, I., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2015). A role of right middle frontal gyrus in reorienting of attention: a case study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:23. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Feyen, L., Freymann, K., Tepest, R., Maier, W., Heun, R., et al. (2006). Volume reduction of the entorhinal cortex in subjective memory impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1751–1756. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalafat, M., Hugonot-Diener, L., and Poitrenaud, J. (2003). Standardisation et étalonnage français du “Mini-Mental State” (MMS) version GRÉCO. Rev. Neuropsychol. 13, 209–236.

Google Scholar

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2015). An alternative to domain-general or domain-specific frameworks for theorizing about human evolution and ontogenesis. AIMS Neurosci. 2, 91–104. doi: 10.3934/neuroscience.2015.2.91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karmiloff-Smith, A., Al-Janabi, T., D’Souza, H., Groet, J., Massand, E., Mok, K., et al. (2016). The importance of understanding individual differences in Down syndrome. F1000Res. 5:389. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.7506.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzman, R. (1993). Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43, 13–20. doi: 10.1212/WNL.43.1_Part_1.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavé, G., Samuel-Enoch, K., and Adiv, S. (2009). The association between age and the frequency of nouns selected for production. Psychol. Aging 24, 17–27. doi: 10.1037/a0014579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, R. M., and Strick, P. L. (2003). Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J. Neurosci. 23, 8432–8444. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-23-08432.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurth, F., Luders, E., and Gaser, C. (2015). “Voxel-based morphometry,” in Brain Mapping, ed. W. Toga (Waltham, MA: Academic Press), 345–349.

Google Scholar

Lemaître, H., Crivello, F., Grassiot, B., Alpérovitch, A., Tzourio, C., and Mazoyer, B. (2005). Age- and sex-related effects on the neuroanatomy of healthy elderly. Neuroimage 26, 900–911. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaitre, H., Goldman, A. L., Sambataro, F., Verchinski, B. A., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Weinberger, D. R., et al. (2012). Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and grey matter volume? Neurobiol. Aging 33, 617.e1–617.e9. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leong, R. L. F., Lo, J. C., Sim, S. K. Y., Zheng, H., Tandi, J., Zhou, J., et al. (2017). Longitudinal brain structure and cognitive changes over 8 years in an East Asian cohort. Neuroimage 147, 852–860. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovden, M., Schmiedek, F., Kennedy, K. M., Rodrigue, K. M., Lindenberger, U., and Raz, N. (2013). Does variability in cognitive performance correlate with frontal brain volume? Neuroimage 64, 209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manard, M., Bahri, M. A., Salmon, E., and Collette, F. (2016). Relationship between grey matter integrity and executive abilities in aging. Brain Res. 1642, 562–580. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.04.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchand, W. R., Lee, J. N., Suchy, Y., Garn, C., Johnson, S., Wood, N., et al. (2011). Age-related changes of the functional architecture of the cortico-basal ganglia circuitry during motor task execution. Neuroimage 55, 194–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metz-Lutz, M., Kremin, H., Deloche, G., Hannequin, D., Ferrand, L., Perrier, D., et al. (1991). Standardisation d’un test de dénomination orale: contrôle des effets de l’âge, du sexe et du niveau de scolarité chez les sujets adultes normaux. Neuropsychologie 1, 73–95.

Meyer, A. M., and Federmeier, K. D. (2010). Event-related potentials reveal the effects of aging on meaning selection and revision. Psychophysiology 47, 673–686. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00983.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morcom, A. M., and Johnson, W. (2015). Neural reorganization and compensation in aging. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1275–1285. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nissim, N. R., O’Shea, A. M., Bryant, V., Porges, E. C., Cohen, R., and Woods, A. J. (2017). Frontal structural neural correlates of working memory performance in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:328. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., and Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 292–305. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., Salami, A., Andersson, M., Eriksson, J., Kalpouzos, G., Kauppi, K., et al. (2010). Longitudinal evidence for diminished frontal cortex function in aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 22682–22686. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012651108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M., and Raichle, M. E. (1989). Positron emission tomographic studies of the processing of singe words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 153–170. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.2.153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pudas, S., Persson, J., Josefsson, M., de Luna, X., Nilsson, L.-G., and Nyberg, L. (2013). Brain characteristics of individuals resisting age-related cognitive decline over two decades. J. Neurosci. 33, 8668–8677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2900-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajah, M. N., Ames, B., and D’Esposito, M. (2008). Prefrontal contributions to domain-general executive control processes during temporal context retrieval. Neuropsychologia 46, 1088–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajah, M. N., Languay, R., and Valiquette, L. (2010). Age-related changes in prefrontal cortex activity are associated with behavioural deficits in both temporal and spatial context memory retrieval in older adults. Cortex 46, 535–549. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., et al. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, N., and Rodrigue, K. M. (2006). Differential aging of the brain: patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 730–748. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, A. F., Pessoa, L., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2009). The prefrontal cortex and the executive control of attention. Exp. Brain Res. 192, 489–497. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1642-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salat, D. H., Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Greve, D. N., Desikan, R. S. R., Busa, E., et al. (2004). Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb. Cortex 14, 721–730. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 754–760. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satz, P. (1993). Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: a formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology 7, 273–295. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.7.3.273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seghier, M. L. (2012). The angular gyrus. Neurosci. 19, 43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidler, R. D., Bernard, J. A., Burutolu, T. B., Fling, B. W., Gordon, M. T., Gwin, J. T., et al. (2010). Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 721–733. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shafto, M. A., Stamatakis, E. A., Tam, P. P., and Tyler, L. K. (2010). Word retrieval failures in old age: the relationship between structure and function. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1530–1540. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokolov, A. A., Miall, R. C., and Ivry, R. B. (2017). The cerebellum: adaptive prediction for movement and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 313–332. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702813248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoodley, C. J. (2012). The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum 11, 352–365. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talairach, J., and Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional Systm: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. New York, NY: Thieme Medical Publishers Inc.

Google Scholar

Tisserand, D. J., van Boxtel, M. P. J., Pruessner, J. C., Hofman, P., Evans, A. C., and Jolles, J. (2004). A voxel-based morphometric study to determine individual differences in grey matter density associated with age and cognitive change over time. Cereb. Cortex 14, 966–973. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tombaugh, T. N. (2004). Trail making test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 19, 203–214. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6177(03)00039-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., and Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology 11, 138–146. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhaegen, C., and Poncelet, M. (2013). Changes in naming and semantic abilities with aging from 50 to 90 years. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 19, 119–126. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712001178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, M. W., Veitch, D. P., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Cairns, N. J., Green, R. C., et al. (2017). Recent publications from the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative: reviewing progress toward improved AD clinical trials. Alzheimers Dement. 13, e1–e85. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weschler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 4th Edn. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.

Wierenga, C. E., Benjamin, M., Gopinath, K., Perlstein, W. M., Leonard, C. M., Rothi, L. J. G., et al. (2008). Age-related changes in word retrieval: role of bilateral frontal and subcortical networks. Neurobiol. Aging 29, 436–451. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield, A., and Grossman, M. (2006). Language and the aging brain: patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 2830–2839. doi: 10.1152/jn.00628.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F. I. M., and Booth, L. (2004). Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychol. 115, 167–183. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zigmond, A. S., and Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 67, 361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, gray matter, MRI, VBM, brain, cognitive

Citation: Ramanoël S, Hoyau E, Kauffmann L, Renard F, Pichat C, Boudiaf N, Krainik A, Jaillard A and Baciu M (2018) Gray Matter Volume and Cognitive Performance During Normal Aging. A Voxel-Based Morphometry Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:235. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00235

Received: 11 January 2018; Accepted: 18 July 2018;
Published: 03 August 2018.

Edited by:

Aurel Popa-Wagner, Department of Neurology, University Hospital Essen, Germany

Reviewed by:

Vincent Koppelmans, University of Utah, United States
Jean-Francois Demonet, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), France

Copyright © 2018 Ramanoël, Hoyau, Kauffmann, Renard, Pichat, Boudiaf, Krainik, Jaillard and Baciu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephen Ramanoël, stephen.ramanoel@inserm.fr

Download