Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 20 September 2019
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 11 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00263

Playing Exergames Facilitates Central Drive to the Ankle Dorsiflexors During Gait in Older Adults; a Quasi-Experimental Investigation

  • 1Institute of Human Movement Sciences and Sport, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 2Division of Physiotherapy, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 3SRH Hochschule für Gesundheit, Gera, Germany

Purpose: Gait training might be of particular importance to reduce fall risk in older adults. In the present study we explore the hypothesis that video game-based training will increase tibialis anterior (TA) muscle EMG-EMG coherence and relates to functional measures of lower limb control.

Methods: We focus on video game-based training performed in standing position, where the subjects have to lift their toes to place their feet on different target zones in order to successfully play the game. This type of training is hypothesized leading to progressive changes in the central motor drive to TA motor neurons and, consequently, improved control of ankle dorsiflexion during gait.

Results: Twenty older adults, 79 ± 8 years old, 13 females/7 males, participated. Results showed a significant difference against 0 in the experimental ΔPOST condition in dual-task walking and beta Frequency Of Interest (p = 0.002). Walking under dual task condition showed significant change over time in minimal Toe Clearance for both the left [χ2(2) = 7.46, p = 0.024, n = 20] and right [χ2(2) = 8.87, p = 0.012, n = 20] leg. No change in lower extremity function was detectable.

Conclusion: Overall we conclude that the initiation of an exergame-based training in upright standing position improves neural drive to the lower extremities in older adults, effects on minimal Toe Clearance and seems an acceptable form of physical exercise for this group.

Introduction

Increasing the number of years of good health while maintaining independence and quality of life as long as possible is a primary public health goal. Avoidance of disease and disability, maintaining high physical and cognitive function, and sustained engagement in social and productive activities are components of healthy aging that together define successful aging (Rowe and Kahn, 1997).

A large component of successful aging interventions aims to maximize physical performance levels. Being able to fully participate in daily life activities may be affected when the capability to easily perform common physical functions decreases (Rowe and Kahn, 1997). Consequently, in older adults health status can be regarded an important indicator of quality of life (Johnson and Wolinsky, 1993; Spirduso and Cronin, 2001). The way how middle-aged and older adults perceive their health seems especially related to components of health-related fitness and functional performance, or to chronic conditions and diseases that influence these fitness components (Johnson and Wolinsky, 1993; Malmberg et al., 2002; Malmberg et al., 2005).

Older adults that are physically active or who regularly exercise help in preventing the development and progression of chronic degenerative diseases (Physical Activity Fundamental To Preventing Disease, 2002; Chodzko-Zajko et al., 2009; Stuart et al., 2009). Being physically active or adherent to regular exercise are means to consistently improve age-related muscle weakness, physical function, cognitive performance, and mood in older adults (Landi et al., 2010). Where a sedentary lifestyle in older adults will increase the risk of unintentional falls, it is observable that falls risk is reduced by being physically active (Thibaud et al., 2012). Approximately 30% of older people experience falls on a yearly basis (Berg et al., 1997; Hausdorff et al., 2001; Gill et al., 2005) and falling can be seen as a common problem in the growing elderly population (de Bruin et al., 2012).

Individuals with gait impairments have an increased risk for repeated falls (Tinetti et al., 1995) and, accordingly, walking ability and the risk of falling are linked with each other. The three most frequent motor control related direct causes of falling are tripping, slipping and loss of balance (Lord et al., 1993; Sherrington et al., 2004). Furthermore, forward walking activity is associated with a high proportion of falls (Robinovitch et al., 2013).

Greater variability in Minimum Foot Clearance (MFC) is a contributing factor to a trips risk increase, and concomitant associated heightened falls occurrence, in those cases where older are compared with younger adults and older fallers to older non-fallers. MFC is “the minimum vertical distance between the lowest point of the foot of the swing leg and the walking surface during the swing phase of the gait cycle (Barrett et al., 2010).” Toe clearance control with the central nervous system in critical situations is impaired in older adults at high risk of tripping (Hamacher and Schega, 2014), an observation in line with research indicating that corticospinal transmission to skeletal muscle may be impaired with advancing age (Manini et al., 2013).

Motor-cognitive training with exergames may help in effectively supporting physical, psychological, and cognitive rehabilitative outcomes in older adult populations (Zeng et al., 2017; van Santen et al., 2018). Patients with mobility problems, for example, have shown transfer of training effects obtained in a virtual environment to real-life (Erren-Wolters et al., 2007). Two systematic reviews have shown that especially cognitive-motor stepping interventions with video-games positively effect on gait of older adults (Schoene et al., 2014; Okubo et al., 2017) and this approach, furthermore, is considered task-specific for neuroplasticity improvements (Netz, 2019). Previous studies from our group pointed to the potential of video game-based training and highlighted that specifically designed game-based training improves walking (Pichierri et al., 2012a, b; Fraser et al., 2014) and effects on the brain (Eggenberger et al., 2016; Schattin et al., 2016; Stanmore et al., 2017). The game play also requires ankle dorsiflexion to play the game, a MFC-associated factor (Sato, 2015). Here we explore the hypothesis that video game-based training effects on corticospinal transmission to the tibialis anterior (TA) muscle assessed by means of EMG-EMG coherence and lower limb control. We focus on video game-based training performed in standing position, where the subjects have to lift their toes to place their feet on different target zones in order to successfully play the game. We hypothesize that this type of training leads to changes in the central drive to TA and, thus, to improved ankle dorsiflexion control during gait.

Materials and Methods

In this “pretest – posttest” quasi experimental single group design the older adults acted as their own controls to control for inter-subject variability (Willerslev-Olsen et al., 2015). Comparing against a not training group of older adults would complicate the comparison of data given the expected diversity of functional abilities in older adults. Furthermore, this design allowed determining whether intervention effects can be explained by simple test-retest variability and whether significant changes would be observed in the measured variables during a 6-week control period comparable to that of the 6-week intervention period.

The study was designed for autonomous and independent living older adults. For study inclusion, the older adults had to be 65 years or above, had to be in good physical health by self-report (assessed by means of a Health Questionnaire), had to be able to walk at least 500 m independently (with or without walking aids), and they had to be not experienced in exercising with virtual reality-based games before this study. Moreover, they were considered eligible if they had not been diagnosed for cognitive impairments and they had a Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score of 26 or more points. Participants were not eligible when exhibiting acute or unstable chronic diseases, rapidly progressing or terminal illnesses or were suffering from severe health problems (e.g., recent cardiac infarction, uncontrolled diabetes or hypertension). The ethics committee of the ETH Zurich, Switzerland (EK 2017-N-22) approved the study protocol. Before any measurements were performed, an informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki was administered and signed by each eligible participant.

Protocol and Training Intervention

Twenty older adults (average age 79 years old, 13 females, and 7 males) from the Alterszentrum Kehl (Baden, Switzerland) were recruited and participated voluntarily during a 12 weeks period. Gait analysis, EMG recordings of TA activity, physical functioning, and cognition were assessed during three test sessions separated by approximately 6 weeks intervals. All test sessions took place in the Alterszentrum Kehl (Baden, Switzerland1). The first test session occurred approximately 6 weeks before training commenced, and the second testing was organized 1 week before training initiation. The final test session took place following the last training day. All three testing sessions included the same measurements performed in the same order on every occasion and at the same time of the day. Test sessions began with concurrent gait analysis and EMG recordings during over ground walking, followed by lower extremity function and cognitive testing.

The participants performed a total of 18 training sessions lasting 20 min (with a break of 10 min in between). Each training session was performed on a Senso exergame system (dividat, Schindellegi, Switzerland; Figure 1). The Exergame had to be played using body movements to trigger sensors positioned on a base plate that was connected to a TV screen. Through these body movements participants apply forces and steps of which the dynamics are recorded. Real-time visual and auditory feedback on Exergame performance was provided through electronic sensors in the dance pad detecting position and timing information.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Participant flow chart with an exemplified use of the exergame platform used for this study. Each participant was involved in the study for a period of 12 weeks. After the 6 weeks control period (between the 1 and 2 test session), each participant trained 18 times within a period of 6–7 weeks.

The equipment was set up in a quiet room in the Alterszentrum Kehl (Baden, Switzerland). The participant stood in front of a computer screen positioned at eye-level during training. A handrail was placed in front of the participant to provide safety support, if needed. During the first session exercise tasks were explained by a study coordinator to the participant. During subsequent sessions, participants conducted training using screen-based feedback only. To guarantee safety the study coordinator remained with the participant during training.

The used exergames are designed to train different cognitive domains; e.g., divided attention, working memory, inhibition, attention shifting, spatial orientation and postural control (Table 1). The training intervention program was specifically designed for healthy seniors and the participants were gradually accustomed to the games. During the individual training period, each participant was constantly monitored by the instructors and received one-on-one supervision. The training protocol contained a variety of exercises in which the difficulty progressively adapted to an individual skill level. Attendance to the training sessions was monitored by the instructors. Participants were provided with individual diaries at each training session, to record the date of training, which specific exergames were used, and both intensity and duration of each exergame performed in that session.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of the video games.

EMG Data Acquisition and Pre-processing

Surface EMG signals were recorded at a sampling frequency of 1500 Hz (Noraxon DTS TeleMyo, Scottsdale, AZ, United States). For this two pairs of bipolar Ag-AgCl electrodes (Ambu Blue Sensor N, Ambu A/S, Ballerup, Denmark) over the left and right TA muscles (Figure 2) were used. Each bipolar configuration of the pair was placed either proximally or distally with respect to the muscle belly according to previously described anatomical landmarks (van Asseldonk et al., 2014). The inter-electrodes distance (electrodes’ center-to-center) was set to 2 cm, whereas the two bipolar configurations, within each pair, were separated by ∼10 cm (range: ∼8.50 cm to ∼13.50 cm) in order to reduce the risk of cross-talk as well as the recording of muscle activity from overlapping motor unit areas (Hansen et al., 2005). To ensure the replication of the two bipolar placements across all the assessments, placements (for each TA muscle) were recorded by keeping trace of distance metrics from anatomical landmarks at each subject level. The skin was prepared (i.e., cleaned and, when necessary, shaved) before placing the EMG electrodes using a specific paste (H+H Medizinprodukte GbR, Münster, Germany). Moreover, in order to detect the onset of the heel strikes, two footswitches were placed approximately on the midpoint of the calcaneus in both feet. Continuous EMG data was first demeaned and de-trended by removing the zero- and first-order polynomial, respectively, and then high-pass filtered at 10 Hz (zero-phase 4th-order Butterworth filter). Subsequently, powerline noise and its harmonics were filtered out using a notch filter based on Discrete Fourier Transformation (DFT) followed by a low-pass filter at 500 Hz (zero-phase 4th-order Butterworth filter) and rectification of the Hilbert transform of the filtered data. The latter non-linear transformation produces an output similar to performing solely rectification without previous transforming by Hilbert (Myers et al., 2003; Boonstra and Breakspear, 2012). This approach represents a widely used strategy in the preprocessing steps before computing either cortico-muscular coherence (i.e., by means of EEG and EMG) or intramuscular coherence (Schoffelen et al., 2011; Boonstra et al., 2015). Considering the debate on rectification as a proper preprocessing step before coherence analysis (Halliday and Farmer, 2010; Neto and Christou, 2010), the adopted filtering strategy with included rectification has shown to produce more reliable EMG-EMG coherence results during walking in comparison to different preprocessing strategies (van Asseldonk et al., 2014). The left and right heel-strike events were merged and used to epoch the EMG data relative to the gait cycle, similar as in previous analyses (Petersen et al., 2012; van Asseldonk et al., 2014; Willerslev-Olsen et al., 2015; Kitatani et al., 2016). The duration of the resulting epochs started one sample before the onset of the heel-strike (to avoid excessive contamination with artifacts of the EMG data caused by the collision of the foot with the ground) to the preceding 400 ms. The preprocessed EMG data segments were subsequently down-sampled to 500 Hz.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Placement over both tibialis anterior muscles of a pair of bipolar EMG electrodes using an inter-electrodes distance of ∼2 cm in each bipolar set and a distance of ∼10 cm between the two bipolar set of EMG electrodes to reduce the risk of cross-talk. Footswitches placed over the midpoint of each heel were placed in order to record heel-strike events during the overground gait trials.

Spectral Analysis of EMG Data

A multi taper frequency transform was used to achieve cross- and power spectra from the preprocessed data segments, by tapering each of these with a variable set of discrete prolate spheroidal (Slepian) sequences. The used epochs of data of 400 ms duration, yielded to a frequency resolution of 2.5 Hz. A broad power- and cross-spectra was calculated (0–100 Hz). Within these, three frequency bands were of interest (FOI) for further analysis: beta (15–30 Hz), low-gamma (32.5–47.5 Hz), and high-gamma (50–65 Hz). The focus on the first two FOI (beta and low-gamma) is based on mounting evidence of a high association of the coherence in these frequency bands (gathered range: ∼15–50 Hz) with the quality of the neural drive to the muscle during walking and, therefore, with the integrity of the pyramidal system. Furthermore, they also show a high potential of being modifiable by several types of training intervention; e.g., gait training (Norton and Gorassini, 2006; Barthelemy et al., 2010; Petersen et al., 2012; Willerslev-Olsen et al., 2015; Kitatani et al., 2016). The focus on the third FOI (high-gamma) is based on its high association with corticospinal interaction effectiveness, as shown in experimental paradigms involving both motor and cognitive resources; e.g., reaction time readiness (Schoffelen et al., 2005), where the role of coherence in high-gamma during locomotion (e.g., gait) has been investigated in first promising research work (Clark et al., 2013).

Two sets of tapers were adopted in two runs, to obtain an optimal spectral concentration and sensitivity relative to the three FOI. For the FOI up to 30 Hz (i.e., beta) three tapers were used with a resulting spectral smoothing of ±5 Hz around each frequency bin, whereas for the FOI higher than 30 Hz (i.e., low- and high-gamma) nine tapers were used with a resulting spectral smoothing of ±12.5 Hz around each frequency bin. Usually, the beta frequency bandwidth is represented by ∼10 Hz and the gamma frequency bandwidth by ∼25 Hz (Schoffelen et al., 2011), thus supporting the utilization of this spectral smoothing strategy. The following equation was used to calculate power- and cross-spectra:

S x y ( f ) = F x ( f ) × F y ( f ) * (1)

where Fx(f) [or Fx(f)] denotes the Fourier transform of the signal x (or y) relative to the frequency f and denotes the complex conjugate. In this analysis signal x and signal y represent proximal and distal EMG data segments, respectively. When xy, Sxy(f) denotes the cross-spectra between signal x and signal y, relative to the frequency f. When x=y, Sxy(f) is reduced to Sxx(f) [or Syy(f)], which consists of the (auto) power spectra of the signal x (or y), relative to the frequency f. Single segments of power- and cross-spectra yielded after averaging across tapers were used in order to calculate the coherence estimate between proximal and distal EMG data, with the following equation:

C o h x y = | S x y | S x x × S y y (2)

where ⟨⋅⟩ denotes the obtained power- or cross-spectra after averaging across data segments. Coherence is a spectral measure representing the linear correlation between signal x and signal y, where the estimate ranges between 0 and 1, with 0 representing no linear association and 1 perfect relation at a specific frequency f.

In previous studies, 70–100 heel-strikes (i.e., data segments) were used to calculate the coherence estimates (Norton and Gorassini, 2006; van Asseldonk et al., 2014). However, it has been shown that a rather small number of trials (i.e., 25 or 50) may result in a larger variability of gait-related coherence estimates, which tends to decrease as the number of segments increase (van Asseldonk et al., 2014). Notably, a large number of trials (i.e., 200) has been shown to relate with a still rather large variability (i.e., 50%) to gait-related coherence estimates (van Asseldonk et al., 2014). This large variability may be problematic when comparing coherence estimates obtained from an unequal number of segments within several testing sessions (Maris et al., 2007). In this study, all the data segments from the obtained heel-strike events in all the measurement time points were used for further analysis (mean ± SD across the three measurement time points in normal and dual-task walking: 224 ± 96, 213 ± 102, 246 ± 89, 244 ± 106, 212 ± 102, 252 ± 80, respectively).

Gait Analysis

Toe clearance (cm), was measured with the Physilog (Gait up Sàrl, Lausanne, Switzerland) wearable movement sensors (50 × 37 × 9.2 mm, 19 g). Data transfer to a computer for further analysis was allowed through a micro-USB port. Elastic straps fixed the sensors at the right and left forefoot of the participants to allow flat over ground gait analysis. Physilog provides a valid quantitative assessment of gait kinematic parameters (Aminian et al., 1999; Dubost et al., 2006; de Bruin et al., 2007).

A figure-8 walking path was settled up, by placing two structures at a distance of approximately 7 m, and a gait testing protocol with at least 50 gait cycles (five repetitions of the path; Figure 3) had to be accomplished by the participants. Such a number of gait cycles has been shown to be sufficient to reliably estimate gait kinematics parameters (König et al., 2014). Participants performed five repetitions of the figure-8 gait path during both single- and dual-task condition as well as both under preferred and fast walking speed. In the dual-task walking condition, participants counted backward in steps of seven after receiving a random given number between 200 and 250. The participants were told to count loud; otherwise, the trial was recorded as failure. The dual task-condition quantifies the automaticity of movement and multi-tasking capabilities (Abernethy, 1988; Wright and Kemp, 1992; Wulf et al., 2001) and assessed the control of MTC under dual task conditions as a marker of motor control (Hamacher et al., 2016). For each participant, we calculated the variability of toe clearance (Mills et al., 2008). A measure of variability (e.g., standard deviation) of MTC height in preferred walking is indicative for diminished gait control in older adults (Begg et al., 2007; Mills et al., 2008).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Pooled data for both normal and dual-task walking (in grayish and reddish color palette, respectively), across the three measurement time points (1st pre-intervention, 2nd pre-intervention and post-intervention, denoted by the numeric subscripts: 1, 2 and 3, respectively) at different preprocessing stages: Proximal and distal emg sensors over the tibialis anterior muscle (TA) were first filtered (A1–A3) and then rectified (B1–B3). The filtered and rectified emg data were then epoched from the heel strike (excluded) to the preceding 400 ms before performing spectral analysis of frequency (C1–C3) and intramuscular coherence (D1–D3) between proximal and distal TA (TA-TA). Then, Z-transformed coherence of the 1st pre-intervention measurement (D1) was subtracted from the Z-transformed coherence of the 2nd pre-intervention measurement (D2) as well as (D2) from the post-intervention measurement (D3) (ΔPRE and ΔPOST, respectively). In the Dual-Task condition (red color) of ΔPOST, statistically significant difference of Δz-coherence resulted in the averaged beta frequency band of the spectrum (i.e., 15–30 Hz).

Lower Extremity Functioning

The Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB) was used to assess lower extremity functioning. The test battery contains (i) a balance test, (ii) a 4-meter gait test, and (iii) a 5-chair-rise test. Timed results from each test are categorized into variables ranging from 0 (unable to perform) to 4 (best performers) according to well-established cut-off values (Guralnik et al., 2000). The sum of the results [tests (i)–(iii); theoretically ranging from 0 to 12] is used for the analyses where 12 indicates the highest degree of functioning.

At the pre-clinical stage this test is a predictor of subsequent disability (Vasunilashorn et al., 2009) and is applicable in routine clinical settings for monitoring of functioning (Perera et al., 2005). Test administration criteria have been published at “www.grc.nia.nih.gov/branches/ledb/sppb/index.htm.” The participants were tested within a single session lasting around 10 min.

Montreal Cognitive Assessment

The MoCA “paper-and-pencil-test” screens cognitive domains such as memory, language, executive functions, visuospatial skills, attention, concentration, and orientation (Nasreddine et al., 2005; Julayanont et al., 2013). The maximal score possible is 30 points and reflects a quantitative estimate of the overall cognitive abilities (Koski et al., 2011). A cut-off score below 26 points is often taken as indicator of possible mild cognitive impairment and dementia. The instrument is sensitive to change (Tiffin-Richards et al., 2014) and available in German.

Statistical Analysis; EMG-EMG Coherence

Statistical analysis of the EMG-EMG coherence estimates was performed as follows. The Z-coherence spectra z(f) was computed at subject level to account for the variability before performing statistical testing according previously reported procedures (Maris et al., 2007; Schoffelen et al., 2011) and calculating the estimated change between measurement time points, using:

Z ( f ) = ( T a n h - 1 ( C o h 1 ) - 1 / ( 2 t a p 1 - 2 ) ) - ( T a n h - 1 ( C o h 2 ) - 1 / ( 2 t a p 2 - 2 ) ) ( 1 / ( 2 t a p 1 - 2 ) + 1 / ( 2 t a p 2 - 2 ) ) (3)

where Tanh−1 is the inverse hyperbolic tangent, Coh1 denotes the coherence estimate in measurement time point n and tapn denotes the total number of tapers used for the spectral estimation of the frequency in measurement time point n. The total number of tapers (tapn) was obtained by multiplying the tapers used (in this analysis either 3 or 9, as described above) by the number of data segments for each subject and measurement time point. The individual ΔZ-coherence spectra obtained using equation (3) were performed by subtracting, for each frequency band, the 1st measurement time point from the 2nd (ΔPRE) and the 2nd measurement time points from the 3rd (ΔPOST). For the study design here presented, the ΔPRE was considered the “control” condition while ΔPOST represented the “experimental” condition. This is assuming that no significant difference is expected from pre-measurement time points one to two, and a significant difference is expected between the 2nd and the 3rd measurement time points.

Statistical inference was based on pooled individual ΔPRE and ΔPOST separately for each using a non-parametric permutation test approach as described elsewhere (Maris et al., 2007; Schoffelen et al., 2011). Briefly, pooled ΔPRE and ΔPOST for both normal and dual-task walking condition and for each frequency band (where the average across frequencies, within frequency band, was used) were tested for significant differences from 0 by approximating the p-values by 100000 Monte Carlo permutations. The level of significance was set to α = 0.05 and a two-tailed test was adopted and corrected by multiplying the p-values with a factor of two prior to α thresholding.

Statistical Analysis; Secondary Outcomes

Normality was tested for the remaining outcomes data before analysis with the Shapiro–Wilk test. Then the Friedman test was used to test for differences between time points because of non-normal data distributions. Post hoc analysis with Wilcoxon signed-rank tests and Dunn-Bonferroni correction was conducted when the Friedman test revealed significant values. All values are reported as means ± 95% confidence intervals (CI) and all statistical analyses were performed with SPSS.

Pearson’s correlation was used for Effect Size determination with r = 0.1 meaning “small”, r = 0.3 “medium,” and r = 0.5 meaning a “large” effect (Cohen, 1988).

Results

Demographics and clinical characteristics of included participants (N = 20) is summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographics and clinical characteristics of participants (N = 20).

EMG-EMG Coherence

All the control ΔPRE conditions revealed non-significant differences in both normal (p = 0.815, p = 0.428, and p = 0.963, for beta, low and high-gamma FOI, respectively) and dual-task walking (p = 0.503, p = 0.056, and p = 0.066, for beta, low and high-gamma FOI, respectively).

Regarding the coherence estimates during normal and dual-task over ground walking, the non-parametric permutation test showed a significant difference against 0 in the experimental ΔPOST condition in dual-task walking and beta FOI (p = 0.002), while no significant difference was observed in the low and high gamma FOI (p = 0.307 and p = 0.372, respectively). No significant difference was observed in the normal walking condition in both beta, low and high gamma FOIs (p = 0.168, p = 0.257, and p = 0.715, respectively).

Gait Analysis and MTC

No significant change over time for MTC was observed for walking under single task condition.

Walking under dual task condition showed significant change over time in MTC for both the left [χ2(2) = 7.46, p = 0.024, n = 20] and right [χ2(2) = 8.87, p = 0.012, n = 20] leg.

Post hoc testing (Dunn-Bonferroni) revealed the difference (left leg) to be between time points 1 and 3: p13 = 0.027; p12 ≥ 0.9; and p23 = 0.173. Effect sizes were large and medium-large (time point 1 – 3 r13 = 0.58 and time point 2 – 3 r23 = 0.42). A small-medium Effect resulted for time point 1 – 2 (r12 = 0.16).

Post hoc testing (Dunn-Bonferroni) revealed the difference (right leg) to be between time points 1 and 2: p12 = 0.022; p13 ≥ 0.9, p23 = 0.066). Effect sizes were large (time point 1 – 2 r12 = 0.6 and time point 2 – 3 r23 = 0.51) and small (time point 1 – 3 r13 = 0.08).

Secondary Outcomes

The inferential statistical testing for the secondary outcomes, revealed no significant changes in lower extremity functioning: χ2(2) = 3.68, p = 0.159, n = 20, whereas a significant change in MoCA scores over time was observed with χ2(2) = 11.76, p = 0.003, n = 20). Pairwise comparisons revealed this difference to be between time points 1 and 3 (p13 = 0.008; p12 = 0.081; and p23 ≥ 0.9) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Resulting MTC, SPPB, and MoCA values at the different measurement time points.

The amount of points achieved in the video games in the final training week was significantly higher compared to the 1st week of training for all video games played (“Simple”: z = −3.659, p < 0.001; “Targets”: z = −3.920, p < 0.001; “Divided”: z = −3.920, p < 0.001; “Simon”: z = −3.884, p < 0.001; “Flexi A+B”: z = −3.921, p < 0.001; “Snake”: z = −3.921, p < 0.001; “Tetris”: z = −3.809, p < 0.001) (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Points achieved in the video games in the 1st week vs. the last week.

Discussion

This study aimed to explore whether video game-based training effects on the central drive to the ankle dorsiflexors during over ground walking and on MTC as measure of lower limb control. We hypothesized that this type of training would lead to changes in the corticospinal transmission to TA muscles and, thus, to improved motor control of ankle dorsiflexion while walking. The main finding was an observed change in the experimental ΔPOST condition in dual-task walking and the beta FOI which, thus, indicates the intervention effected on neural drive through enhanced quality of the neural drive (Norton and Gorassini, 2006; Barthelemy et al., 2010). This finding closely resembles investigations where intramuscular coherence in the beta frequency has shown to be related to neural drive to the muscle and, thus, the control of gait (Petersen et al., 2012; van Asseldonk et al., 2014; Kitatani et al., 2016). Furthermore, this finding gets supported by the observed change of the mean value of MTC that is accompanied by decreasing standard deviation values. Evidence of a possible linkage between lower extremity activation through training and changes in the neural drive also stem from a study from Dalgas et al. (2013). These authors showed that lower extremity resistance training in multiple sclerosis improves the neural drive to lower limb muscles. Similar results for resistance training for non-impaired individuals were reported (Aagaard et al., 2002). However, changes in the neural drive are also seen with other forms of initiating chronic physical activity (Enoka, 1997). This study is the first to show enhanced neural drive due to an exergame intervention. This finding is relevant for our society that is currently challenged to find an answer for supporting public health policies aimed at helping senior citizens achieving the goals of primary and secondary (i.e., reducing readmission rates) prevention to remain independence in functioning (McCaskey et al., 2018). Physical activity and exercise for older adults may help in this context to foster physical and cognitive functioning at the highest possible levels (DiPietro, 2001; American College of Sports Medicine, 2004; Elsawy and Higgins, 2010; de Souto Barreto et al., 2016; Karssemeijer et al., 2017) and considering that motivation to continue and adhere to conventional exercise is often difficult (Phillips et al., 2004). More research that aims at establishing the best ways to encourage older adults to be more physically active in the long term is needed (Bennett and Winters-Stone, 2011). The incorporation of progressively intense but short exercise as part of a tailored and combinatory program; e.g., with exergames, may be beneficial (Hwang et al., 2016). This type of training is also feasible for co-morbid geriatric patients Guadalupe-Grau et al. (2017).

The exergame used in this study is a motor-cognitive exercise from which can be hypothesized that it improves the synapse communication in brain networks responsible for movement coordination and execution (Eggenberger et al., 2016; Schattin et al., 2016; McCaskey et al., 2018). This, in turn, could positively influence the communication from the motor area of the brain to the muscles. Previous studies pointed to the potential of video game-based training and highlighted that video games that are specifically designed for training purposes improve walking (Pichierri et al., 2012a, b; Fraser et al., 2014) and effect on the brain (Eggenberger et al., 2016; Schattin et al., 2016). Emerging evidence indicates that part of the age-associated loss in muscle strength is due to an impaired communication between the brain and the effector organs (Clark and Manini, 2008; Manini et al., 2013); e.g., the muscles of the lower extremities. Furthermore, such deficits in the neural drive can lead to much of the muscle weakness observed in older adult populations (Clark and Taylor, 2011). Improved neural drive to the lower extremity muscles might explain why strength gains in lower extremity muscles are observable following exergame interventions (Jorgensen et al., 2013; Sato et al., 2015).

Older adults exhibit large muscle activation deficits (Clark and Fielding, 2012) which may explain the divergence between the rates in loss of strength and muscle mass, and may account for up to one third of loss in force production. Furthermore, traditional rehabilitation programs that use conventional strength training in patients with impaired muscle activation may not be optimal to reverse the loss in muscle strength (Stevens et al., 2003). Part of the muscle strength loss seen in older adults may be due to qualitative and quantitative changes in the motor cortex that negatively impacts on voluntary activation of the muscles (Clark and Taylor, 2011). Voluntary activation can been defined as the “level of voluntary drive during an effort” (Gandevia, 2001; Taylor, 2009). The descending drive from the motor cortex is considered the major determinant of the timing and strength of voluntary contractions (Clark and Taylor, 2011). Motor unit firing rates for the ankle dorsiflexor, for example, show slower rates for older than younger men (Roos et al., 1997; Todd et al., 2003). It can be hypothesized that training programs that focus on aspects of voluntary muscle activation in addition to more conventional types of resistance training, such as in the exergame we used, may result in greater strength gains in individuals that show larger voluntary activation deficits. Future longitudinal studies that use such combinations are warranted.

Our result seems at variance with the lack of an effect on the lower extremity function measured by SPPB. However, when we observe the values for all measurement time points for this parameter, it becomes clear that we may have possibly suffered a ceiling effect for this measurement in our sample. The values for this measure indicate we had a high level of lower extremity functioning in our selected senior sample.

Although we had a significant change in the MoCA outcome measure, this finding should also be interpreted with prudence in relation to its clinical relevance. First, we had high values for this measure indicating high levels of cognitive functioning in our sample and, second, we know from previous research that we can only be confident that an observed change would not be due to measurement error when we see individuals changing their score with 4 or more points (Feeney et al., 2016) and this should be two points or above for group assessments (Krishnan et al., 2017). Based on these values it becomes clear that there was not enough room for meaningful improvement for this outcome in our sample.

Limitations of the Study

A limitation of our study relates to the testing threat known to possibly occur in pre-post research design. It might have been that testing our subjects with for example the MoCA at pretest made some of the participants more aware of cognitive skills and, hence, “primed” these individuals for the test so that when they repeated the measurement they were ready for it in a way that they wouldn’t have been without the pretest. Mortality Threat, used metaphorically here, might have been another limitation. This means that people are dropping out of the study. It is difficult to estimate whether the observed loss of three individuals (±10%) between pretest and posttest was non-trivial notwithstanding that the reasons reported for dropping out were not related to the intervention. However, compared to rates that might be expected for community dwelling older trainees after 12 months (Nyman and Victor, 2012) the dropout rate seems acceptable and in line with what could be expected.

The used research design, although having strengths, also has some limitations attached to it. To deal with the single group threats to internal validity this study should be replicated using a more stringent research design in which a control group is considered; e.g., a randomized control design that also considers blinding of participants and assessors where possible. In this scenario, we would have two groups: one receiving the exergames and the other one doesn’t with the aim of ruling out the single-group threats to internal validity.

Overall we can conclude that the initiation of an exergame-based training in upright standing position indicates to improve neural drive to the lower extremities in older adults and seems an acceptable form of physical exercise for this group. Further studies with more stringent designs are needed to refute or confirm this finding.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The ethics committee of the ETH Zurich, Switzerland (EK 2017-N-22) approved the study protocol. Before any measurements were performed, an informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki was administered and signed by each eligible participant.

Author Contributions

EB and FG developed the research question. The concept and design were established by EB while FG acted as methodological council. NP and LR conducted the data acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of the results (secondary outcomes) with editing and improvement by EB and FG. FG performed EMG data analysis and interpretation of the EMG results which was edited and improved by EB. EB produced a first version of the manuscript. FG substantially revised the manuscript to bring it to its current version. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

EB was a co-founder of dividat, the spin-off company that developed the video step platform used for the training of the seniors and is associated to the company as an external advisor. No revenue was paid (or promised to be paid) directly to EB or his institution over the 36 months prior to submission of the work.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.daskehl.ch/

References

Aagaard, P., Simonsen, E. B., Andersen, J. L., Magnusson, P., and Dyhre-Poulsen, P. (2002). Increased rate of force development and neural drive of human skeletal muscle following resistance training. J. Appl. Physiol. 93, 1318–1326. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00283.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abernethy, B. (1988). Dual-task methodology and motor skills research: some applications and methodological constraints. J. Hum. Mov. Stud. 14, 101–132.

Google Scholar

American College of Sports Medicine (2004). Physical activity programs and behavior counseling in older adult populations. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 36, 1997–2003. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000145451.08166.97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aminian, K., Robert, P., Buchser, E. E., Rutschmann, B., Hayoz, D., and Depairon, M. (1999). Physical activity monitoring based on accelerometry: validation and comparison with video observation. Med. Biol. Eng. Comp. 37, 304–308. doi: 10.1007/bf02513304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, R. S., Mills, P. M., and Begg, R. K. (2010). A systematic review of the effect of ageing and falls history on minimum foot clearance characteristics during level walking. Gait Post. 32, 429–435. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barthelemy, D., Willerslev-Olsen, M., Lundell, H., Conway, B. A., Knudsen, H., Biering-Sorensen, F., et al. (2010). Impaired transmission in the corticospinal tract and gait disability in spinal cord injured persons. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 1167–1176. doi: 10.1152/jn.00382.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Begg, R., Best, R., Dell’Oro, L., and Taylor, S. (2007). Minimum foot clearance during walking: strategies for the minimisation of trip-related falls. Gait Post. 25, 191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennett, J. A., and Winters-Stone, K. (2011). Motivating older adults to exercise: what works? Age Ageing 40, 148–149. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afq182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berg, W. P., Alessio, H. M., Mills, E. M., and Tong, C. (1997). Circumstances and consequences of falls in independent community-dwelling older adults. Age Ageing 26, 261–268. doi: 10.1093/ageing/26.4.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, T. W., and Breakspear, M. (2012). Neural mechanisms of intermuscular coherence: implications for the rectification of surface electromyography. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 796–807. doi: 10.1152/jn.00066.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, T. W., Danna-Dos-Santos, A., Xie, H. B., Roerdink, M., Stins, J. F., and Breakspear, M. (2015). Muscle networks: connectivity analysis of EMG activity during postural control. Sci. Rep. 5:17830. doi: 10.1038/srep17830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chodzko-Zajko, W. J., Proctor, D. N., Fiatarone Singh, M. A., Minson, C. T., Nigg, C. R., Salem, G. J., et al. (2009). American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Exercise and physical activity for older adults. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 41, 1510–1530. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181a0c95c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, B. C., and Manini, T. M. (2008). Sarcopenia =/= dynapenia. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 63, 829–834.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Clark, B. C., and Taylor, J. L. (2011). Age-related changes in motor cortical properties and voluntary activation of skeletal muscle. Curr. Aging Sci. 4, 192–199. doi: 10.2174/1874609811104030192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. J., and Fielding, R. A. (2012). Neuromuscular contributions to age-related weakness. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 67, 41–47. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. J., Kautz, S. A., Bauer, A. R., Chen, Y. T., and Christou, E. A. (2013). Synchronous EMG activity in the piper frequency band reveals the corticospinal demand of walking tasks. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 41, 1778–1786. doi: 10.1007/s10439-013-0832-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). The effect size. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Dalgas, U., Stenager, E., Lund, C., Rasmussen, C., Petersen, T., Sorensen, H., et al. (2013). Neural drive increases following resistance training in patients with multiple sclerosis. J. Neurol. 260, 1822–1832. doi: 10.1007/s00415-013-6884-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Bruin, E. D., Najafi, B., Murer, K., Uebelhart, D., and Aminian, K. (2007). Quantification of everyday motor function in a geriatric population. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 44, 417–428.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

de Bruin, E. D., van Het Reve, E., and Murer, K. (2012). A randomized controlled pilot study assessing the feasibility of combined motor-cognitive training and its effect on gait characteristics in the elderly. Clin. Rehabil. 27, 215–225. doi: 10.1177/0269215512453352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Souto Barreto, P., Morley, J. E., Chodzko-Zajko, W., H Pitkala, K., Weening-Djiksterhuis, E., Rodriguez-Mañas, L., et al. (2016). Recommendations on physical activity and exercise for older adults living in long-term care facilities: a taskforce report. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 17, 381–392. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2016.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiPietro, L. (2001). Physical activity in aging: changes in patterns and their relationship to health and function. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 56, 13–22. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.suppl_2.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubost, V., Kressig, R. W., Gonthier, R., Herrmann, F. R., Aminian, K., Najafi, B., et al. (2006). Relationships between dual-task related changes in stride velocity and stride time variability in healthy older adults. Hum. Mov. Sci. 25, 372–382. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2006.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggenberger, P., Wolf, M., Schumann, M., and de Bruin, E. D. (2016). Exergame and balance training modulate prefrontal brain activity during walking and enhance executive function in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:66. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsawy, B., and Higgins, K. E. (2010). Physical activity guidelines for older adults. Am. Fam. Phys. 81, 55–59.

Google Scholar

Enoka, R. M. (1997). Neural adaptations with chronic physical activity. J. Biomech. 30, 447–455. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9290(96)00170-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erren-Wolters, C. V., van Dijk, H., de Kort, A. C., Ijzerman, M. J., and Jannink, M. J. (2007). Virtual reality for mobility devices: training applications and clinical results: a review. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 30, 91–96. doi: 10.1097/mrr.0b013e32813a2e00

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeney, J., Savva, G. M., O’Regan, C., King-Kallimanis, B., Cronin, H., and Kenny, R. A. (2016). Measurement error, reliability, and minimum detectable change in the mini-mental state examination, montreal cognitive assessment, and color trails test among community living middle-aged and older adults. J. Alzheimers Dis. 53, 1107–1114. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraser, S. A., Elliott, V., de Bruin, E. D., Bherer, L., and Dumoulin, C. (2014). The effects of combining videogame dancing and pelvic floor training to improve dual-task gait and cognition in women with mixed-urinary incontinence. Games Health J. 3, 172–178. doi: 10.1089/g4h.2013.0095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandevia, S. C. (2001). Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol. Rev. 81, 1725–1789. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.4.1725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gill, T., Taylor, A. W., and Pengelly, A. (2005). A population-based survey of factors relating to the prevalence of falls in older people. Gerontology 51, 340–345. doi: 10.1159/000086372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guadalupe-Grau, A., Aznar-Lain, S., Manas, A., Castellanos, J., Alcazar, J., Ara, I., et al. (2017). Short- and long-term effects of concurrent strength and hiit training in octogenarians with COPD. J. Aging Phys. Act. 25, 105–115. doi: 10.1123/japa.2015-0307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guralnik, J. M., Ferrucci, L., Pieper, C. F., Leveille, S. G., Markides, K. S., Ostir, G. V., et al. (2000). Lower extremity function and subsequent disability: consistency across studies, predictive models, and value of gait speed alone compared with the short physical performance battery. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 55, M221–M231.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Halliday, D. M., and Farmer, S. F. (2010). On the need for rectification of surface EMG. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 3547; author reply 3548–3549.

Google Scholar

Hamacher, D., Herold, F., and Schega, L. (2016). Are there differences in the dual-task walking variability of minimum toe clearance in chronic low back pain patients and healthy controls? Gait Post. 49, 97–101. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.06.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamacher, D., and Schega, L. (2014). Towards the importance of minimum toe clearance in level ground walking in a healthy elderly population. Gait Post. 40, 727–729. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, N. L., Conway, B. A., Halliday, D. M., Hansen, S., Pyndt, H. S., Biering-Sorensen, F., et al. (2005). Reduction of common synaptic drive to ankle dorsiflexor motoneurons during walking in patients with spinal cord lesion. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 934–942. doi: 10.1152/jn.00082.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hausdorff, J. M., Rios, D. A., and Edelberg, H. K. (2001). Gait variability and fall risk in community-living older adults: a 1-year prospective study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 82, 1050–1056. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.24893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, C. L., Yoo, J. K., Kim, H. K., Hwang, M. H., Handberg, E. M., Petersen, J. W., et al. (2016). Novel all-extremity high-intensity interval training improves aerobic fitness, cardiac function and insulin resistance in healthy older adults. Exp. Gerontol. 82, 112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2016.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, R. J., and Wolinsky, F. D. (1993). The structure of health-status among older adults - disease, disability, functional limitation, and perceived health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 34, 105–121.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jorgensen, M. G., Laessoe, U., Hendriksen, C., Nielsen, O. B., and Aagaard, P. (2013). Efficacy of Nintendo Wii training on mechanical leg muscle function and postural balance in community-dwelling older adults: a randomized controlled trial. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 68, 845–852. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Julayanont, P., Phillips, N., Chertkow, H., and Nasreddine, Z. S. (2013). “Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA): concept and clinical review,” in Cognitive Screening Instruments, ed. A. Larner (London: Springer), 111–151. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4471-2452-8_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karssemeijer, E. G. A., Aaronson, J. A., Bossers, W. J., Smits, T., Olde, M., Rikkert, G. M., et al. (2017). Positive effects of combined cognitive and physical exercise training on cognitive function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 40, 75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2017.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitatani, R., Ohata, K., Aga, Y., Mashima, Y., Hashiguchi, Y., Wakida, M., et al. (2016). Descending neural drives to ankle muscles during gait and their relationships with clinical functions in patients after stroke. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1512–1520. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.10.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

König, N., Singh, N., Von Beckerath, J., Janke, L., and Taylor, W. (2014). Is gait variability reliable? An assessment of spatio-temporal parameters of gait variability during continuous overground walking. Gait Post. 39, 615–617. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koski, L., Xie, H., and Konsztowicz, S. (2011). Improving precision in the quantification of cognition using the montreal cognitive assessment and the mini-mental state examination. Int. Psychogeriatr. 23, 1107–1115. doi: 10.1017/S1041610210002450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnan, K., Rossetti, H., Hynan, L. S., Carter, K., Falkowski, J., Lacritz, L., et al. (2017). Changes in montreal cognitive assessment scores over time. Assessment 24, 772–777. doi: 10.1177/1073191116654217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landi, F., Abbatecola, A. M., Provinciali, M., Corsonello, A., Bustacchini, S., Manigrasso, L., et al. (2010). Moving against frailty: does physical activity matter? Biogerontology 11, 537–545. doi: 10.1007/s10522-010-9296-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, S. R., Ward, J. A., Williams, P., and Anstey, K. J. (1993). An epidemiological study of falls in older community-dwelling women: the Randwick falls and fractures study. Aust. J. Public Health 17, 240–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-6405.1993.tb00143.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malmberg, J., Miilunpalo, S., Pasanen, M., Vuori, I., and Oja, P. (2005). Characteristics of leisure time physical activity associated with risk of decline in perceived health - a 10-year follow-up of middle-aged and elderly men and women. Prev. Med. 41, 141–150. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malmberg, J. J., Miilunpalo, S. I., Vuori, I. M., Pasanen, M. E., Oja, P., and Haapanen-Niemi, N. A. (2002). A health-related fitness and functional performance test battery for middle-aged and older adults: feasibility and health-related content validity. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 83, 666–677. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2002.32304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manini, T. M., Hong, S. L., and Clark, B. C. (2013). Aging and muscle: a neuron’s perspective. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 16, 21–26. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0b013e32835b5880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maris, E., Schoffelen, J. M., and Fries, P. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of coherence differences. J. Neurosci. Methods 163, 161–175. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCaskey, M. A., Schattin, A., Martin-Niedecken, A. L., and de Bruin, E. D. (2018). Making more of IT: enabling intensive motor cognitive rehabilitation exercises in geriatrics using information technology solutions. Biomed. Res. Int. 2018:4856146. doi: 10.1155/2018/4856146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills, P. M., Barrett, R. S., and Morrison, S. (2008). Toe clearance variability during walking in young and elderly men. Gait Post. 28, 101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, L. J., Lowery, M., O’Malley, M., Vaughan, C. L., Heneghan, C., St Clair Gibson, A., et al. (2003). Rectification and non-linear pre-processing of EMG signals for cortico-muscular analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 124, 157–165. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(03)00004-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neto, O. P., and Christou, E. A. (2010). Rectification of the EMG signal impairs the identification of oscillatory input to the muscle. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 1093–1103. doi: 10.1152/jn.00792.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Netz, Y. (2019). Is there a preferred mode of exercise for cognition enhancement in older age?-a narrative review. Front. Med. 6:57. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2019.00057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, J. A., and Gorassini, M. A. (2006). Changes in cortically related intermuscular coherence accompanying improvements in locomotor skills in incomplete spinal cord injury. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 2580–2589. doi: 10.1152/jn.01289.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyman, S. R., and Victor, C. R. (2012). Older people’s participation in and engagement with falls prevention interventions in community settings: an augment to the Cochrane systematic review. Age Ageing 41, 16–23. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okubo, Y., Schoene, D., and Lord, S. R. (2017). Step training improves reaction time, gait and balance and reduces falls in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 51, 586–593. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-095452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perera, S., Studenski, S., Chandler, J. M., and Guralnik, J. M. (2005). Magnitude and patterns of decline in health and function in 1 year affect subsequent 5-year survival. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 60, 894–900. doi: 10.1093/gerona/60.7.894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, T. H., Willerslev-Olsen, M., Conway, B. A., and Nielsen, J. B. (2012). The motor cortex drives the muscles during walking in human subjects. J. Physiol. 590, 2443–2452. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.227397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, E. M., Schneider, J. C., and Mercer, G. R. (2004). Motivating elders to initiate and maintain exercise. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85(7 Suppl. 3), S52–S57.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Physical Activity Fundamental To Preventing Disease (2002). Physical Activity Fundamental To Preventing Disease. Washington, DC: United States departement of health & human services. Available at: https://aspe.hhs.gov/basic-report/physical-activity-fundamental-preventing-disease (accessed September 10, 2019).

Google Scholar

Pichierri, G., Coppe, A., Lorenzetti, S., Murer, K., and de Bruin, E. D. (2012a). The effect of a cognitive-motor intervention on voluntary step execution under single and dual task conditions in older adults: a randomized controlled pilot study. Clin. Interv. Aging 7, 175–184. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S32558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichierri, G., Murer, K., and de Bruin, E. D. (2012b). A cognitive-motor intervention using a dance video game to enhance foot placement accuracy and gait under dual task conditions in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 12:74. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-12-74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinovitch, S. N., Feldman, F., Yang, Y., Schonnop, R., Leung, P. M., Sarraf, T., et al. (2013). Video capture of the circumstances of falls in elderly people residing in long-term care: an observational study. Lancet 381, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61263-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roos, M. R., Rice, C. L., and Vandervoort, A. A. (1997). Age-related changes in motor unit function. Muscle Nerve 20, 679–690. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4598(199706)20:6<679::aid-mus4>3.3.co;2-l

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, J. W., and Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. Gerontologist 37, 433–440.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sato, K. (2015). Factors affecting minimum foot clearance in the elderly walking: a multiple regression analysis. Open J. Ther. Rehabil. 3, 109–115. doi: 10.4236/ojtr.2015.34015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, K., Kuroki, K., Saiki, S., and Nagatomi, R. (2015). Improving walking, muscle strength, and balance in the elderly with an exergame using kinect: a randomized controlled trial. Games Health J. 4, 161–167. doi: 10.1089/g4h.2014.0057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schattin, A., Arner, R., Gennaro, F., de, E., and Bruin, D. (2016). Adaptations of prefrontal brain activity, executive functions, and gait in healthy elderly following exergame and balance training: a randomized-controlled study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:278. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoene, D., Valenzuela, T., Lord, S. R., and de Bruin, E. D. (2014). The effect of interactive cognitive-motor training in reducing fall risk in older people: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 14:107. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-14-107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoffelen, J.-M., Oostenveld, R., and Fries, P. (2005). Neuronal coherence as a mechanism of effective corticospinal interaction. Science 308, 111–113. doi: 10.1126/science.1107027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoffelen, J. M., Poort, J., Oostenveld, R., and Fries, P. (2011). Selective movement preparation is subserved by selective increases in corticomuscular gamma-band coherence. J. Neurosci. 31, 6750–6758. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4882-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherrington, C., Lord, S. R., and Finch, C. F. (2004). Physical activity interventions to prevent falls among older people: update of the evidence. J. Sci. Med. Sport 7(1 Suppl.), 43–51. doi: 10.1016/s1440-2440(04)80277-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spirduso, W. W., and Cronin, D. L. (2001). Exercise dose-response effects on quality of life and independent living in older adults. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 33, S598–S608.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Stanmore, E., Stubbs, B., Vancampfort, D., de Bruin, E. D., and Firth, J. (2017). The effect of active video games on cognitive functioning in clinical and non-clinical populations: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78, 34–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, J. E., Stackhouse, S. K., Binder-Macleod, S. A., and Snyder-Mackler, L. (2003). Are voluntary muscle activation deficits in older adults meaningful? Muscle Nerve 27, 99–101. doi: 10.1002/mus.10279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuart, M., Chard, S., Benvenuti, F., and Steinwachs, S. (2009). Community exercise: a vital component to healthy aging. Healthc. Pap. 10, 23–28; discussion79–83.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. L. (2009). Point: the interpolated twitch does/does not provide a valid measure of the voluntary activation of muscle. J. Appl. Physiol. 107, 354–355. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91220.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibaud, M., Bloch, F., Tournoux-Facon, C., Breque, C., Rigaud, A. S., Dugue, B., et al. (2012). Impact of physical activity and sedentary behaviour on fall risks in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Activ. 9, 5–15. doi: 10.1007/s11556-011-0081-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiffin-Richards, F. E., Costa, A. S., Holschbach, B., Frank, R. D., Vassiliadou, A., Kruger, T., et al. (2014). The montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) - a sensitive screening instrument for detecting cognitive impairment in chronic hemodialysis patients. PLoS One 9:e106700. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinetti, M. E., Doucette, J., Claus, E., and Marottoli, R. (1995). Risk factors for serious injury during falls by older persons in the community. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 43, 1214–1221. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1995.tb07396.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, G., Taylor, J. L., and Gandevia, S. C. (2003). Measurement of voluntary activation of fresh and fatigued human muscles using transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Physiol. 551(Pt 2), 661–671. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.044099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Asseldonk, E. H., Campfens, S. F., Verwer, S. J., van, M., Putten, J., and Stegeman, D. F. (2014). Reliability and agreement of intramuscular coherence in tibialis anterior muscle. PLoS One 9:e88428. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Santen, J., Droes, R. M., Holstege, M., Henkemans, O. B., van Rijn, A., de Vries, R., et al. (2018). Effects of exergaming in people with Dementia: results of a systematic literature review. J. Alzheimers. Dis. 63, 741–760. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasunilashorn, S., Coppin, A. K., Patel, K. V., Lauretani, F., Ferrucci, L., Bandinelli, S., et al. (2009). Use of the short physical performance battery score to predict loss of ability to walk 400 meters: analysis from the InCHIANTI study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 64, 223–229. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gln022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willerslev-Olsen, M., Petersen, T. H., Farmer, S. F., and Nielsen, J. B. (2015). Gait training facilitates central drive to ankle dorsiflexors in children with cerebral palsy. Brain 138(Pt 3), 589–603. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, D. L., and Kemp, T. L. (1992). The dual-task methodology and assessing the attentional demands of ambulation with walking devices. Phys. Ther. 72, 306–312. doi: 10.1093/ptj/72.4.306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G., McNevin, N., and Shea, C. H. (2001). The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 54, 1143–1154. doi: 10.1080/02724980143000118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeng, N., Pope, Z., Lee, J. E., and Gao, Z. (2017). A systematic review of active video games on rehabilitative outcomes among older patients. J. Sport Health Sci. 6, 33–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2016.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: older adults, exergame, central drive, tibialis anterior, foot clearance

Citation: de Bruin ED, Patt N, Ringli L and Gennaro F (2019) Playing Exergames Facilitates Central Drive to the Ankle Dorsiflexors During Gait in Older Adults; a Quasi-Experimental Investigation. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:263. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00263

Received: 29 April 2019; Accepted: 05 September 2019;
Published: 20 September 2019.

Edited by:

Helena Blumen, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Luwen Wang, Case Western Reserve University, United States
Bettina Wollesen, Universität Hamburg, Germany

Copyright © 2019 de Bruin, Patt, Ringli and Gennaro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eling D. de Bruin, eling.debruin@hest.ethz.ch

ORCID: Eling D. de Bruin, orcid.org/0000-0002-6542-7385; Federico Gennaro, orcid.org/0000-0003-2203-2858

Download