Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Genet., 24 November 2022
Sec. Behavioral and Psychiatric Genetics
This article is part of the Research Topic Epigenetics and Neurodevelopment in Psychiatry View all 5 articles

Do the diverse phenotypes of Prader-Willi syndrome reflect extremes of covariation in typical populations?

  • Crespi Laboratory, Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Vancouver, BC, Canada

The phenotypes of human imprinted neurogenetic disorders can be hypothesized as extreme alterations of typical human phenotypes. The imprinted neurogenetic disorder Prader-Willi syndrome (PWS) features covarying phenotypes that centrally involve altered social behaviors, attachment, mood, circadian rhythms, and eating habits, that can be traced to altered functioning of the hypothalamus. Here, we conducted analyses to investigate the extent to which the behavioral variation shown in typical human populations for a set of PWAS-associated traits including autism spectrum cognition, schizotypal cognition, mood, eating, and sleeping phenotypes shows covariability that recapitulates the covariation observed in individuals with PWS. To this end, we collected data from 296 typical individuals for this set of phenotypes, and showed, using principal components analysis, evidence of a major axis reflecting key covarying PWS traits. We also reviewed the literature regarding neurogenetic syndromes that overlap in their affected traits with PWS, to determine their prevalence and properties. These findings demonstrate that a notable suite of syndromes shows phenotypic overlap with PWS, implicating a large set of imprinted and non-imprinted genes, some of which interact, in the phenotypes of this disorder. Considered together, these findings link variation in and among neurogenetic disorders with variation in typical populations, especially with regard to pleiotropic effects mediated by the hypothalamus. This work also implicates effects of imprinted gene variation on cognition and behavior in typical human populations.

Introduction

The hypothalamus is a highly conserved region in vertebrate brains, that has been found to regulate physiological homeostasis via several neural mechanisms and specific signaling cascades, including biorhythmicity, sleep-wake control and the regulation of satiety and hunger states [reviewed in Xie and Dorsky (2017) and Arrigoni et al. (2019)]. The primary functions of the hypothalamus have been highlighted by syndromes involving hypothalamic dysfunction, which demonstrate phenotypes involving disrupted metabolic control and altered regulation of sleep (Ivanova and Kelsey, 2011; Angulo et al., 2015). Furthermore, mice model studies that involve knockouts of genes active in the hypothalamus tend to show altered phenotypes of food intake, sleep or both (Kozlov et al., 2007; Ivanova and Kelsey, 2011; Mercer et al., 2013; Qi et al., 2015; Pace et al., 2020). Recent gene knockout studies also appear to show that hypothalamic neuron populations primarily expressing a specific neural transmitter may simultaneously exert pleiotropic effects on several behaviorally distinct aspects of energy homeostasis. For example, model mice with a specific lack of expression for the melatonin-concentrating hormone (MCH), typically expressed exclusively in the hypothalamus, show a pleiotropic phenotype with increased wakefulness and an increase in activity during fasting periods, which may indicate that MCH neurons typically function to promote sleep and suppress food-seeking behaviors for conservation of energy balance (Willie et al., 2008). In contrast, mouse knockout studies with orexin-expressing neurons have shown that orexin may simultaneously promote feeding, energy expenditure and wakefulness (Yamanaka et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2005; Arrigoni et al., 2019). While pleiotropy in homeostatic neural circuits may merely represent an ancestral state conserved across vertebrate lineages, it may also be theorized that hypothalamic neural circuits have been selected to promote a multitude of co-adapted behavioral and physiological responses to facilitate comprehensive behavioral strategies. The pleiotropy of hypothalamic neural circuits appears to extend beyond homeostatic mechanisms, as animal models have also shown that maternal care, which is intricately linked to both feeding and sleep-wake cycles in infancy, is in part mediated by hypothalamic neural circuits in both the mother and the offspring (Chiavegatto et al., 2012; Bridges, 2015). Finally, such pleiotropic effects in hypothalamic control may sometimes be subject to maladaptive effects in humans, as eating disorders, mood and anxiety and schizophrenia may all be associated with dysfunctions of hypothalamic neural circuits (Romano et al., 2016; Bernstein et al., 2019).

Syndromic conditions of hypothalamic dysfunction, particularly neurogenetic conditions involving altered gene dosage, may be hypothesized as extreme manifestations of genetic regulatory mechanisms involved in hypothalamic function. Prader-Willi syndrome, a neurogenetic disorder involving hypothalamic dysfunction (Angulo et al., 2015) highlights alterations in genomic imprinting, an epigenetic regulatory mechanism in which a specific gene is expressed mainly or entirely from one parental allele. Imprinted genes are typically found in evolutionarily conserved clusters where several epigenetic mechanisms including DNA methylation, non-coding RNAs, histone modifications and sequence-specific regulatory proteins function in tandem to regulate imprinted gene expression (Nicholls and Knepper, 2001; Landers et al., 2004; Ivanova and Kelsey, 2011; Marty and Cavaillé, 2019). In PWS, affected individuals carry a genomic alteration, typically due to a meiotic error, which leads to a complete lack of expression for a co-regulated set of ∼20 imprinted genes which are typically expressed solely from paternally inherited alleles in healthy individuals (Cassidy and Driscoll, 2009). The neurobehavioral phenotype of PWS shows evidence for disruption of homeostatic mechanisms regulated in the hypothalamus: PWS involves failure to thrive and comparably increased somnolence particularly in infancy, followed by gradual development of hyperphagia, with lack of satiety and obsessive food-seeking behaviors later in childhood (Haig and Wharton, 2003; Kotler and Haig, 2018). The behavioral phenotypes of PWS also extend beyond altered homeostatic mechanisms, as individuals with PWS typically show mood disturbances, anxiety and frustration towards changes in routine (Schwartz et al., 2021).

Individuals with PWS also show a high prevalence of psychotic symptoms, particularly within the genotype of maternal uniparental disomy (matUPD) which involves both a lack of expression for the affected paternally expressed imprinted genes and increased dosage for the maternally expressed imprinted gene UBE3A. Several independent cohort studies have shown that ∼60% of PWS individuals with the matUPD genotype develop psychotic symptoms in adulthood, and thus, it has been theorized that an imbalance in genomic imprinting via increased dosage of UBE3A may predispose PWS matUPD individuals for development of psychosis (Vogels et al., 2004; Soni et al., 2007; Sinnema et al., 2011; Crespi et al., 2018). The hypothesis may thus imply that the matUPD genotype of PWS represents an extreme imbalance towards psychological phenotypes mediated by maternally expressed imprinted genes (Crespi and Badcock, 2008; Kotler and Haig, 2018). Interestingly, it has also been shown that dysfunctions of the hypothalamus are also associated with psychotic disorders (Bernstein et al., 2019; Millard et al., 2022), which might further imply that the extreme psychological phenotype shown with the matUPD genotype of PWS is causal due to hypothalamic dysfunction.

Both within and beyond the scope of PWS, mice models of genomic imprinting further show that paternally expressed genes in particular may take part in regulation of hypothalamic function and simultaneously modify both homeostatic and social behaviors (Ivanova and Kelsey, 2011, also see Supplementary Table S2) In a genome-wide characterization of imprinting in the mouse brain, a significantly larger number of paternally expressed imprinted genes were expressed in the adult hypothalamus, as compared to the number of maternally expressed imprinted genes, while the embryonic mouse brain showed a comparably larger number of active maternally expressed genes (Gregg et al., 2010). A developmental transition in genomic imprinting and hypothalamic control of feeding and somnolence may in part explain the early failure to thrive and the gradual development of hyperphagia in the PWS phenotype (Kotler et al., 2016; Salminen et al., 2019).

Mouse models of PWS also show evidence of pleiotropic phenotypes mediated by altered hypothalamic function, from several lines of evidence. Firstly, SNORD116-deficient model mice show phenotypes of altered food intake (Qi et al., 2015; Polex-Wolf et al., 2018) and sleep-wake cycles (Lassi et al., 2016) and also show a reduced number of orexin-expressing neurons in their lateral hypothalamus as compared to controls (Pace et al., 2020). As orexin-expressing neurons promote wakefulness and food consumption (Arrigoni et al., 2019), these results show an interesting parallel to the PWS phenotype. Lack of expression for MAGEL2 has also been shown to produce a reduction in orexin-expressing neurons, and thus SNORD116 and MAGEL2 may act in a dose-dependent manner in PWS [(Kozlov et al., 2007; Pace et al., 2020)].

Secondly, the MAGEL2 and NDN genes have also been shown to interact with circadian clock proteins in the suprachiasmatic nucleus, affecting phenotypes of circadian rhythmicity (Kozlov et al., 2007; Lu et al., 2020). Circadian rhythmicity affects energy homeostasis and the regulation of sleep-wake cycles via diurnally regulated epigenetic cascades affecting gene expression patterns of several thousands of genes downstream (Takahashi, 2017). It has also been shown that lack of expression for SNORD116 alters the diurnal rhythm of gene methylation in model mice, potentially affecting energy homeostasis and epigenetic regulation of several thousands of differentially methylated sites (Coulson et al., 2018).

Thirdly, the social behavioral changes found in PWS may also be altered by a hypothalamic imbalance of oxytocin and oxytocin receptors (Einfeld et al., 2014). Individuals with PWS show reduced numbers of oxytocin-expressing neurons (Swaab et al., 1995; Swaab, 1997) and reduced gene expression for the oxytocin receptor gene, OXTR (Bittel et al., 2007), as compared to controls. However, individuals with PWS also appear to show elevated levels of oxytocin in both blood and cerebrospinal fluids, as compared to controls (Martin et al., 1998; Johnson et al., 2016), which may further indicate a disruption of feedback in oxytocin signaling. Mouse models of PWS, with deletions of MAGEL2 further show that lack of expression for MAGEL2 leads to deficits in social behaviors, but the phenotype may be rescued with post-natal oxytocin treatment (Schaller et al., 2010; Meziane et al., 2015). Thus, evidence from both PWS and other neurogenetic disorders involving hypothalamic dysfunction and animal models of genomic imprinting show that hypothalamic mechanisms may simultaneously affect phenotypes of sleeping, eating, social behaviors and affection, perhaps via multiple co-adapted and overlapping neural pathways.

The prevalence for the traits analyzed here within the diagnostic criteria for PWS range from ∼90% for hyperphagic behaviors to 30%–50% for minor criteria such as sleep disturbances (Gunay-Aygun et al., 2001). While individual variation in behavior is known within PWS, the shared genetic origin and further evidence from animal models of hypothalamic function implies that intellectual disability alone cannot account for the observed suite of behaviors in PWS (Whittington and Holland, 2010), thus further implying that phenotypes of social behaviors and affection may also be affected by hypothalamic mechanisms.

Amongst typical human populations, co-adapted pleiotropic phenotypes of hypothalamic function might be manifested as overlapping effects across multiple co-associated phenotypes. As we might suppose that small-effect genetic and epigenetic variation for both imprinted and non-imprinted genes involved in regulation of hypothalamic function may also be circulating among typical human populations, might we also expect to detect non-clinical degrees of co-occurring behavioral variation across a corresponding set of behavioral phenotypes among typically developing individuals? To evaluate this hypothesis, we phenotyped a population of typical individuals for psychological and behavioral traits central to PWS, including autism spectrum cognition, schizotypal cognition, mood, eating, and sleeping, and we estimated the pattern and degree of covariation shown for these traits via principal component analysis. While this analysis is limited to behavioral variation and cannot directly implicate specific underlying genetic variation, the co-occurrence of the behavioral variation shown across the relevant phenotypes resembles the set of phenotypes also altered in PWS, thus linking a neurodevelopmental axis highlighted by phenotypical variation in neurodevelopmental disorders to behavioral variation among populations of typical individuals.

Materials and methods

Questionnaire data was collected between 2019 and 2020 from 589 psychology students (416 females, 170 males, 3 other) at Simon Fraser University and the University of Alberta. To ensure quality of the collected data, the analysis was limited to individuals who had answered every question in the set of questionnaires. After these procedures, a population of 296 individuals (106 males, 190 females) remained. Ethical approval was obtained from the Simon Fraser University and University of Alberta Departments of Research Ethics. The study was advertised in a university questionnaire study portal for psychology students and course credits were offered for participation. The students all provided written informed consent.

A set of demographic variables including biological sex, age, and self-identified ethnicity were collected from all participants. The Autism spectrum quotient (AQ) (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) was used to characterize the extent of variation for autism spectrum cognitive traits and social behaviors, in ranges relevant to typical human populations while typical variation in schizotypy was assessed with the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief Revised (SPQ-BR) (Cohen et al., 2010) questionnaire. Variation in styles of attachment across relationships with the individual’s mother or mother figure, father of father figure, close friend, and romantic partner were characterized with the Experiences in Close Relationships—Relationship Structures Questionnaire (ECR-RS) (Fraley et al., 2011). The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire (Hills and Argyle, 2002) was used in assessing the mood and psychological well-being of the subjects and answers for depression items of the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI) (Costa and McCrae, 1992) were also collected to assess the phenotypes of mood. A modified version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (Buysse et al., 1989) without the questions filled out by the partner or roommate was collected to assess the variation in the sleeping habits of the students. The Reduced Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (RMEQ) (Adan and Almirall, 1991) was used to assess the ranges of variation in biological rhythmicity. The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) (van Strien et al., 1986) was used to characterize variation in eating habits and preferences. The questionnaire data was typically collected in early to late afternoon although the timing of the collection was not specifically regulated.

The principal components analysis was conducted in R (stats 3.6.3, 2020) with the princomp function, which calculates eigenvalues and eigenvectors from either a covariance or a correlation matrix of multiple variables to express the covariability within a given data set in principal components. As the set of questionnaires varied highly across their scales, we chose to restrict our analysis to a correlation matrix, which standardizes the effects for each of the variables.

The analysis was further limited to variables considered relevant for hypothalamic function, in respect of the behavioral phenotype of PWS, including total score autism spectrum cognition (AQ), the dimension of cognitive perception in schizotypy (SPQ-BR), attachment-related anxiety in relationship with the individual’s mother (ECR-RS), total score for the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, and the scale of Emotional eating (DEBQ). Two different measures for phenotypes of depression and foul mood were also included: the total score for the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire was reversed by subtracting the individual’s score from the highest score in the data set, creating a scale from 0 to 138 to represent one’s unhappiness in themselves and their circumstances. The scored values for the depression-endorsing items (8 in total) of the NEO personality inventory were also summed up to create an alternative measure of low mood.

A literature review (Supplementary Table S1) was also conducted on known syndromes and case studies of disorders displaying “Prader-Willi–like” phenotypes. Articles were searched using the Web of Science search engine using “Prader-Willi” and the known, hypothalamic phenotypes as search terms. The review was further supplemented by manual searches using references of previously published reviews (Crespi, 2008; Juriaans et al., 2022).

Results

The analyzed data consisted of a set of 296 individuals for whom complete data was available (190 females and 106 males) with a mean age of 19.6 years (standard deviation of 3.4). The majority of the participants reported their ethnicity as either Caucasian or Asian descent, 46% and 27% respectively. Within the combined population, the reversed score for the OHQ, representing unhappiness, correlated strongly and positively with all of the other traits hypothesized to be relevant for the PWS phenotype of hypothalamic dysfunction while the other traits were also strongly and positively correlated with one another (Table 1). The alternative measure for low mood (NEO-PI depression items) also correlated strongly and positively with traits relevant to the PWS phenotype, indicating a high degree of robustness for the interaction between low mood and PWS-related phenotypes. The correlations, calculated via Pearson’s product-moment correlation, remained significant after correction for false discoveries with the Benjamini-Hochberg method. Finally, we find that the phenotypes of Restrained eating and External eating, which are not characteristic of PWS and can thus serve as forms of “control” variables, did not show significant correlations with the reversed OHQ score or the total PSQ score but did correlate strongly with Emotional eating (results not shown).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Pearson moment-product correlations across a of set psychological and behavioral traits deemed to reflect typical degrees of variation in phenotypes relevant to the phenotype of Prader-Willi syndrome. Correlations remained significant after Benjamini-Hochberg correction for false discoveries, as shown in the p adjusted-column.

The first principal component (PC1) accounted for ∼35% of the total variance within the population and included positive loadings for all of the PWS-related traits (see Tables 2, 3). PC2 accounted for ∼15% of the variance and included a negative loading for the reversed OHQ scores, and the total PSQI score, while showing positive loadings for the other traits (See Table 4). The three largest principal components accounted for 65% of the total variance within the combined population. Alternative analyses using the NEO-PI depression items instead of the reversed OHQ score performed in a similar manner, but PC2 also included a negative loading for Emotional eating (DEBQ) (See Tables 3, 4). Analyses divided by sex showed minor differences to the multivariate structures shown for the combined population. The proportion of variance covered by the largest principal component is somewhat larger (∼42% males, 39% for females %) compared to combined population. The largest principal component was similarly characterized by positive loadings for all six variables with both sexes. However, females uniquely show a negative loading for anxiety in mother relationships for PC2, while the trait loaded positively on PC2 with males (Tables 5, 6).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Principal components analysis of the variation for the reversed OHQ score and phenotypes in ranges of typical variation for schizotypy, autism spectrum cognition, attachment-related anxiety, sleep and emotional eating, as also shown in Table 1. The principal components were calculated from eigenvalues on the correlation matrix, only the three largest principal components are shown for each analysis.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Alternate principal components analysis replacing the reversed OHQ score with a sum of the depression-endorsing items of the NEO Personality inventory.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Loadings for each variable across the three largest principal components in the analyses for the combined population (N = 296).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Loadings for each variable across the three largest principal components in the subdivided analyses limited to females (N = 190).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Loadings for each variable across the three largest principal components in the subdivided analyses limited to males (N = 106).

The covariability shown for the diverse set psychological and physiological traits analyzed may indicate that typical variation for phenotypes of schizotypy, attachment, mood, sleep and eating habits are partly mediated by a hypothalamic axis of neural regulation. The variation for each of the traits correlated positively with one another, showing for example that high degrees of schizotypy were positively correlated with less secure attachment, comparably higher tendencies for emotional eating and comparably more severe sleep problems. In addition, the reversed OHQ, which corresponds to one’s unhappiness and lack for sense of fulfilment, showed a strongly positive and significant correlation with the other traits as shown in Table 1, while the NEO-PI depression items performed similarly, indicating robustness for the effect observed.

To further explore if hypothalamic dysfunctions may simultaneously alter covarying sets of behavioral phenotypes, we reviewed a large body of literature, focusing on conditions where individuals have been reported to show behavioral phenotypes partly overlapping with the set of behavioral phenotypes also altered in PWS. As also shown in Supplementary Table S1, a set of clinical conditions and differential diagnoses or “Prader-Willi–like” syndromes can be recognized, whereby individuals present with a set of phenotypes resembling some of those of PWS yet lack the methylation alteration, maternal uniparental disomy, or large deletion diagnostic of this syndrome.

The main findings from this review were threefold: Firstly, a number of imprinted and non-imprinted genes that are implicated in “Prader-Willi–like” syndromes, may also interact directly with the imprinted loci involved in PWS. The IPW non-coding RNA gene, located within the 15q11-q13 genomic region has been found to also interact with transcription of the imprinted genes within DLK1-DIO3 imprinted locus, which in turn is affected in the Kagami-Ogata and Temple syndromes. It has been proposed that such crosstalk between imprinted loci may reflect common evolutionary origins and co-adaptation between the two or perhaps several imprinted loci (Stelzer et al., 2014). Secondly, while phenotypical presentations showed variation on the individual level due to varying genomic alterations across cases, rather than resembling PWS with one specific phenotype, PWS-like conditions tend to show pleiotropic sets of behavioral phenotypes, partly resembling the full set of phenotypes similarly also altered in PWS. Thirdly, mouse and cell model studies focusing on potential neural mechanisms of the candidate genes involved in PWS-like conditions further highlight both hypothalamic mechanisms and mechanisms related to the excitation-inhibition balance of the brain in particular. This review thus provides evidence that a diverse set of covarying phenotypes are altered in both PWS and the “Prader-Willi–like” syndromes and dysfunctions of hypothalamic mechanisms may be further implicated in these conditions via alterations of both imprinted and non-imprinted genes, further implying a hypothalamic axis of neural and genetic effects on human behavior that are not wholly separate from one another, such that alterations on a singular regulatory mechanism may have several pleiotropic phenotypic effects downstream.

Discussion

In this study, we have collected and analyzed questionnaire data from a population of healthy individuals to evaluate if a set of PWS-associated psychological and physiological traits relevant to hypothalamic function, would also show a corresponding pattern of covariation in the ranges of typical variation found among healthy individuals. The questionnaire data showed that phenotypes of sleeping and emotional eating, governed in part by the hypothalamus, appear to co-vary with phenotypes of schizotypy, mood and anxiety-related aspects of attachment. The overlap of such a diverse set of physiological and behavioral traits in part mediated by the hypothalamus may imply an evolutionary history whereby neural circuits responsible for regulation of hunger, satiety and biorhythmicity were co-opted to jointly regulate affection and social behaviors in the context of maternal care.

The genomic conflict theory of imprinting (Haig and Wharton, 2003; Kotler and Haig, 2018) which states that genomic imprinting has evolved due to a conflict in resource allocation between maternally and paternally inherited alleles, provides a useful explanation for why hypothalamic traits might be mediated by genomic imprinting to regulate phenotypes of sleep, feeding, and attachment, as the phenotypes of these traits among the offspring may be expected to jointly affect the maternal investment of care into their offspring. Thus, alleles of paternally expressed imprinted genes, which may not share their genotype with other members of the offspring due to the statistical probability for mixed parenthood, may have been selected to favor a hypothalamic phenotype comparably more demanding towards the mother and might thus be under selection for effects that involve spending a larger proportion of the subjective day active while also demanding more affection and food from their mother. By contrast, PWS phenotypes, which are due to lack of expression for such paternally expressed imprinted genes and thus involve a bias in gene dosage towards expression of maternally inherited imprinted genes, would be expected to reflect an extreme manifestation of mechanisms that select for a comparably more equal distribution of maternal investment across offspring, and less individually demanding phenotypes with increased somnolence, reduced affection and reduced food intake in infancy, when the child is most dependent on their mother for sustenance (Kotler and Haig, 2018; Salminen et al., 2019). As healthy individuals may also show less extreme, non-clinical variation for gene expression of imprinted, and non-imprinted genes involved in hypothalamic function, co-variation across a corresponding set of behavioral phenotypes may also be shown, due to neural and genetic regulatory mechanisms that underlie typical function of these phenotypes.

In addition to the altered phenotypes of PWS, several lines of evidence also show that hypothalamic mechanisms may also modify phenotypes of social behavior. Firstly, evidence for structural abnormalities and functional deficits in hypothalamic circuits has also been shown in schizophrenia (Bernstein et al., 2019; Millard et al., 2022). Secondly, oxytocin pathways have been consistently associated with bonding between the mother and offspring (Scatliffe et al., 2019) but maladaptive effects of oxytocin pathways in social behavior may be highlighted in studies showing that variation in oxytocin function may be associated with schizotypy, autism spectrum disorders and depression (Feldman et al., 2016). In PWS, variation of sleep phenotypes has also been significantly associated with psychosis-risk symptoms, linking a correlate of hypothalamic dysfunction to psychotic symptomatology in PWS (O’Hora et al., 2022). A small number of subjects both with and without PWS have also been reported to develop manic symptoms or panic attacks due to hypothalamic stimulation with electrodes, which might similarly suggest an association between hypothalamic dysfunction and psychotic symptoms (Franco et al., 2018; Elias et al., 2020).

We have also reviewed a large body of literature (Supplementary Table S1) on disorders displaying phenotypes implying of hypothalamic dysfunction via altered phenotypes of food intake, sleep or both. Our approach was primarily guided by the phenotype of PWS, which involves a lack of expression for multiple paternally expressed imprinted genes and shows a highly pleiotropic phenotype of hypothalamic dysfunction with altered sleep, feeding, affection, mood and social cognition (Angulo et al., 2015; Crespi et al., 2018; Salminen et al., 2019). While the set of disorders resembling PWS is varied and involves dysfunctions of both imprinted and non-imprinted genes (Juriaans et al., 2022), we also find that candidate genes that are associated with hypothalamic phenotypes also tend to be associated with altered behavior among these disorders. Of particular note are phenotypes shown in Kleefstra syndrome, which involves hypotonia, food seeking behaviors and obesity with a sleep phenotype of repeated night awakenings and hyperactivity, while a limited number of cases also involve phenotypes of frustration and temper tantrums in childhood and psychosis, bipolar disorders and apathy among adult cases (Verhoeven et al., 2011; Kleefstra et al., 2012; Adam and Isles, 2017; De Taevernier et al., 2021). Thus, the syndrome features behavioral phenotypes that resemble phenotypes present in PWS and physiological phenotypes mediated by the hypothalamus which both resemble PWS in part while also showing opposite tendencies for altered sleep phenotypes. We also find, as noted in previous work (reviewed in Ivanova and Kelsey, 2011) that paternally expressed imprinted genes appear to be particularly active in the hypothalamus as is also shown by the genome-wide pattern genomic imprinting in the mouse brain (Gregg et al., 2010). The mouse hypothalamus showed a larger number of active paternally expressed imprinted genes as compared to maternally expressed imprinted genes, a pattern opposite to that of the embryonic mouse brain, which may thus highlight the hypothalamus as a hotspot of paternal-origin allelic expression (Gregg et al., 2010; Ivanova and Kelsey, 2011). Paternally expressed imprinted genes expressed in the hypothalamus also show phenotypes relevant to allocation of maternal resources in model mice: In particular, a large of paternally expressed imprinted genes were active in hypothalamic neural circuits and were also associated with altered phenotypes relevant to maternal care including sleep, feeding, parenting behaviors, anxiety or social cognition (Supplementary Table S2 for references), reflecting the affected phenotypes also shown PWS.

Our findings may also be evaluated in the context of previous behavioral studies which have also indicated correlations between pairs of behavioral traits shown in our questionnaire data. Firstly, as PWS has been found to show varying incidence rates for ASDs in prior studies (Dykens et al., 2011), a measure of autism spectrum cognition was also included in our analysis. While our data showed degree of covariation between typical variation of autism spectrum cognition and variation of other phenotypes deemed to be relevant for the PWS phenotype, earlier questionnaire studies have also shown that both clinical and non-clinical variation in behavioral traits that extend towards the spectrums of autism and schizotypy tend to co-occur or correlate positively with one another to some degree (Dinsdale et al., 2013; Kincaid et al., 2017; Sampson et al., 2021). Studies among individuals with clinical forms of psychosis also appear to show elevated rates of ASDs as compared to the typical human populations and autism–like behavioral traits are similarly precent in high percentages, varying between 9%–61% of individuals diagnosed with psychotic symptoms (Kincaid et al., 2017). It has been traditionally theorized that the co-occurrence of schizotypy and autism spectrum traits may be partly due to differential diagnosis and overlapping behavioral phenotypes, particularly between negative psychotic symptoms and social deficits in ASDs [reviewed in (Kincaid et al., 2017)]. However, studies concerning non-clinical variation in schizotypy and autism spectrum traits have shown correlations between both negative and positive schizotypy and autism spectrum traits (Dinsdale et al., 2013; Kincaid et al., 2017).

In the data analyzed here, the cognitive-perceptual dimension of schizotypy, as measured via the SPQ-BR questionnaire may be interpreted to represent healthy variation within a spectrum of behaviors that extends towards the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, which include ideas of reference (“I often feel that others have it in for me”), magical thinking (“Have you ever felt that you are communicating with another person telepathically?”), and unusual perceptions (“Are your thoughts sometimes so strong that you can almost hear them?”). Both the cognitive-perceptual scale and SPQ-BR total scores were significantly and positively correlated with AQ total scores in our data. Hence, in the context of our current study and earlier studies, it is not entirely clear whether genetic variation and neural mechanisms prevalent in ASDs may 1) function as risk factors for development of psychotic symptoms later in life, or 2) if the two disorders overlap in phenotype only but are mediated by separate genetic and neural mechanisms. Considering the psychological phenotypes of hypothalamic dysfunction shown in both PWS and the altered phenotypes of attachment and social behavior shown in mouse models with deletions of paternally expressed imprinted genes (Supplementary Table S2), it may be further theorized that genetic variation for imprinted genes and genes related hypothalamic function might simultaneously predispose individuals to covarying effects for altered phenotypes of social behaviors and insecure attachment, which may contribute towards variation involving overlap of social deficits between schizotypy and autism spectrum cognition. Relevant to this hypothesis and in similarity to covariation shown in our results, variation in the continuum of paranoia has been shown to be positively correlated with insecure and anxious attachment styles (Korver-Nieberg et al., 2014; Murphy et al., 2020), which has been further theorized to indicate that dysfunctional Theory of Mind (ToM) cognition may predispose individuals to insecure attachment via overly negative models of how others might perceive oneself (Pietromonaco et al., 2000).

Correlations between sleep problems, typically measured by the PSQI questionnaire have also been observed to correlate positively and significantly with eating disorders and emotional eating in prior studies (Soares et al., 2011; Trace et al., 2012; Saleh-Ghadimi et al., 2019) It has also been previously theorized that such correlations might indicate neural mechanisms in the hypothalamic-pituitary axis jointly regulating sleep and feeding might underlie the correlations observed between sleep problems and disordered eating (Trace et al., 2012).

Our analyses are affected by three main limitations that must be considered in relation to the results shown. Firstly, our analyses are limited to self-evaluated behavioral variation and cannot directly implicate specific underlying genetic variation within the study population. Secondly, the set of behavioral phenotypes studied is intended to represent a set of evolutionarily relevant correlates in ranges of typical function rather than traits directly and fully comparable to PWS phenotypes. As PWS typically involves severe hyperphagia with lack of satiety and food-related obsessive behaviors (Dykens et al., 2007), the observed behaviors may also lack a clear correlate within ranges of typical eating behaviors. For the purposes of our study, we have primarily considered the phenotype of Emotional eating, which involves overeating due to emotional responses and may thus partially reflect hypothalamic function rather than external eating, which involves overeating due to how appetizing the food seems or restrained eating, which may involve a conscious decision to restrict eating (van Strien et al., 1986). Thirdly, individual neural mechanisms of hypothalamic function may only be considered from a theoretical viewpoint, as we lack experimental evidence to assign any specific mechanism of neural function to the behavioral phenotypes shown in the study population.

Apart from the aforementioned limitations, it may also be questioned whether genetic polymorphisms affecting hypothalamic function are circulating among typically developed human populations. We note that as shown within a population of healthy human donors from Miami Brain Bank, published under the Genotype-Tissue Expression Portal project (GTEx Portal genome browser, accessed on 7th October 2022), the long non-coding RNA, PWRN1 shows a high number of expression quantitative trait loci SNPs for gene expression differences in hypothalamic tissues, located both within and in the promoter regions of the gene. PWRN1 may also be expressed in the brain as a part of the SNURF-SNRPN (SNHG14) RNA transcript (Chamberlain, 2013). Furthermore, the SNORD116 snoRNA, which is the only single locus implicated in the main behavioral phenotypes of PWS (reviewed in Hassan and Butler, 2016) is also processed from the introns of the SNURF-SNRPN transcript (Runte et al., 2001), and hence, polymorphisms affecting expression of PWRN1 might also indirectly affect the expression of SNORD116. While differences in gene expression within hypothalamic tissues are not in themselves indicative of behavioral variation, these polymorphisms nevertheless showcase a theoretical mechanism for non-clinical variation in regulation of hypothalamic function.

In conclusion, the co-occurring set of physiological and psychological phenotypes shown in PWS and other “Prader-Willi–like” neurogenetic syndromes implies that of hypothalamic neural circuits and their genetic regulatory mechanisms may have become co-adapted to jointly regulate phenotypes of sleep, food intake and social behaviors in the context of maternal investment. In particular, we highlight the sets of physiological and behavioral phenotypes associated with 1) oxytocin- and 2) orexin-expressing neurons in animal model studies and neurogenetic disorders. Alterations in function of oxytocin neurons have been independently associated with alterations in maternal care (Bridges, 2015), feeding (Romano et al., 2020) and psychological functioning (Bernstein et al., 2019) while alterations in orexin-expressing neurons have similarly been associated with alterations in sleep and feeding (Pace et al., 2020) as well as alterations of social behaviors (Harris et al., 2020), pair-bonding, and parenting behaviors (Donlin et al., 2014; Bridges, 2015). Here, we show that behavioral variation present among healthy individuals shows evidence for patterns of co-variation for a set of behavioral and physiological phenotypes which may represent a non-clinical extension of neural functions in part mediated by hypothalamic neural circuits. Thus, alterations in regulation of hypothalamic circuits might predispose individuals to a diverse set of co-occurring behavioral and psychological alterations, which should be considered in both research of psychological and physiological disorders and counseling among healthy individuals.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Simon Fraser University and University of Alberta Departments of Research Ethics. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

IS took part in the collection of the questionnaire data, conducted a literature review on neurogenetic disorders, analysed questionnaire data and took part in writing the article. SR organized and quality-checked the questionnaire data. BC contributed to the design of the study and took part in writing the article.

Funding

The project was funded by NSERC 2019-04208.

Acknowledgments

The questionnaire data was collected in collaboration with the Simon Fraser University Department of Psychology and Pete Hurd at the University of Alberta, Department of Psychology Science. The Genotype-Tissue Expression (GTEx) Project was supported by the Common Fund of the Office of the Director of the National Institutes of Health, and by NCI, NHGRI, NHLBI, NIDA, NIMH, and NINDS. The data used for the analyses of gene expression variation in hypothalamic tissues cited in this manuscript were obtained from the GTEx Portal on 10/07/22.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fgene.2022.1041943/full#supplementary-material

SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE S1 | Disorders showing pleiotropic behavioral phenotypes resembling the phenotype hypothalamic dysfunction in Prader-Willi syndrome. The affected genes and phenotypes of known cases may vary within the syndromes and the neural mechanisms listed are limited to postulation, based on the genotypes of known cases and relevant mouse model studies.

SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE S2 | Paternally expressed imprinted genes active in hypothalamus showing phenotypes relevant to affection and maternal investment in mouse model studies and neurogenetic disorders.

References

Adam, M. A., and Isles, A. R. (2017). EHMT1/GLP; biochemical function and association with brain disorders. Epigenomes 1, 15. doi:10.3390/EPIGENOMES1030015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adan, A., and Almirall, H. (1991). Horne & östberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire: A reduced scale. Pers. Individ. Dif. 12, 241–253. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angulo, M. A., Butler, M. G., and Cataletto, M. E. (2015). Prader-willi syndrome: A review of clinical, genetic, and endocrine findings. J. Endocrinol. Invest. 38, 1249–1263. doi:10.1007/s40618-015-0312-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arrigoni, E., Chee, M. J. S., and Fuller, P. M. (2019). To eat or to sleep: That is a lateral hypothalamic question. Neuropharmacology 154, 34–49. doi:10.1016/J.NEUROPHARM.2018.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., and Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 5–17. doi:10.1023/A:1005653411471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, H. G., Dobrowolny, H., Bogerts, B., Keilhoff, G., and Steiner, J. (2019). The hypothalamus and neuropsychiatric disorders: Psychiatry meets microscopy. Cell Tissue Res. 375, 243–258. doi:10.1007/S00441-018-2849-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bittel, D. C., Kibiryeva, N., Sell, S. M., Strong, T. V., and Butler, M. G. (2007). Whole genome microarray analysis of gene expression in Prader–Willi syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 143A, 430–442. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.31606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bridges, R. S. (2015). Neuroendocrine regulation of maternal behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 36, 178–196. doi:10.1016/J.YFRNE.2014.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., and Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, S. B., and Driscoll, D. J. (2009). Prader-Willi syndrome. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 17, 3–13. doi:10.1038/ejhg.2008.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlain, S. J. (2013). RNAs of the human chromosome 15q11-q13 imprinted region. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. RNA 4, 155–166. doi:10.1002/WRNA.1150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiavegatto, S., Sauce, B., Ambar, G., Cheverud, J. M., and Peripato, A. C. (2012). Hypothalamic expression of Peg3 gene is associated with maternal care differences between SM/J and LG/J mouse strains. Brain Behav. 2, 365–376. doi:10.1002/BRB3.58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, A. S., Matthews, R. A., Najolia, G. M., and Brown, L. A. (2010). Toward a more psychometrically sound brief measure of schizotypal traits: Introducing the SPQ-brief revised. J. Pers. Disord. 24, 516–537. doi:10.1521/pedi.2010.24.4.516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Coulson, R. L., Yasui, D. H., Dunaway, K. W., Laufer, B. I., Vogel Ciernia, A., Zhu, Y., et al. (2018). Snord116-dependent diurnal rhythm of DNA methylation in mouse cortex. Nat. Commun. 9, 1616–1711. doi:10.1038/s41467-018-03676-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespi, B., and Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 241–261. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespi, B., Read, S., Salminen, I., and Hurd, P. (2018). A genetic locus for paranoia. Biol. Lett. 14, 20170694. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2017.0694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespi, B. (2008). Genomic imprinting in the development and evolution of psychotic spectrum conditions. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 83, 441–493. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2008.00050.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Taevernier, C., Meunier-Cussac, S., and Madigand, J. (2021). First episode of psychosis in Kleefstra syndrome: a case report. Neurocase 27, 227–230. doi:10.1080/13554794.2021.1905851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinsdale, N. L., Hurd, P. L., Wakabayashi, A., Elliot, M., and Crespi, B. J. (2013). How are autism and schizotypy related? Evidence from a non-clinical population. PLoS One 8, e63316. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0063316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donlin, M., Cavanaugh, B. L., Spagnuolo, O. S., Yan, L., and Lonstein, J. S. (2014). Effects of sex and reproductive experience on the number of orexin A-immunoreactive cells in the prairie vole brain. Peptides 57, 122–128. doi:10.1016/J.PEPTIDES.2014.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dykens, E. M., Maxwell, M. A., Pantino, E., Kossler, R., and Roof, E. (2007). Assessment of hyperphagia in prader-willi syndrome. Obesity 15, 1816–1826. doi:10.1038/oby.2007.216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dykens, E. M., Lee, E., and Roof, E. (2011). Prader-Willi syndrome and autism spectrum disorders: An evolving story. J. Neurodev. Disord. 3, 225–237. doi:10.1007/S11689-011-9092-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einfeld, S. L., Smith, E., Mcgregor, I. S., Steinbeck, K., Taffe, J., Rice, L. J., et al. (2014). A double-blind randomized controlled trial of oxytocin nasal spray in Prader Willi syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 164, 2232–2239. doi:10.1002/AJMG.A.36653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elias, G. J. B., Giacobbe, P., Boutet, A., Germann, J., Beyn, M. E., Gramer, R. M., et al. (2020). Probing the circuitry of panic with deep brain stimulation: Connectomic analysis and review of the literature. Brain Stimul. 13, 10–14. doi:10.1016/J.BRS.2019.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Monakhov, M., Pratt, M., and Ebstein, R. P. (2016). Oxytocin pathway genes: Evolutionary ancient system impacting on human affiliation, sociality, and psychopathology. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 174–184. doi:10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2015.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C., Heffernan, M. E., Vicary, A. M., and Brumbaugh, C. C. (2011). The experiences in close relationships-relationship structures questionnaire: A method for assessing attachment orientations across relationships. Psychol. Assess. 23, 615–625. doi:10.1037/a0022898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franco, R. R., Fonoff, E. T., Alvarenga, P. G., Alho, E. J. L., Lopes, A. C., Hoexter, M. Q., et al. (2018). Assessment of safety and outcome of lateral hypothalamic deep brain stimulation for obesity in a small series of patients with prader-willi syndrome. JAMA Netw. open 1, e185275. doi:10.1001/JAMANETWORKOPEN.2018.5275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregg, C., Zhang, J., Weissbourd, B., Luo, S., Schroth, G. P., Haig, D., et al. (2010). High-resolution analysis of parent-of-origin allelic expression in the mouse brain. Science 329, 643–648. doi:10.1126/science.1190830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunay-Aygun, M., Schwartz, S., Heeger, S., O′Riordan, M. A., and Cassidy, S. B. (2001). The changing purpose of Prader-Willi syndrome clinical diagnostic criteria and proposed revised criteria. Pediatrics 108. doi:10.1542/peds.108.5.e92

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haig, D., and Wharton, R. (2003). Prader-Willi syndrome and the evolution of human childhood. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 15, 320–329. doi:10.1002/ajhb.10150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, T., Bugescu, R., Kelly, J., Makela, A., Sotzen, M., Sisk, C., et al. (2020). Dlk1 expressed in mouse orexin neurons modulates anxio-depressive behavior but not energy balance. Brain Sci. 10, 9755–E1018. doi:10.3390/BRAINSCI10120975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassan, M., and Butler, M. G. (2016). Prader-Willi syndrome and atypical submicroscopic 15q11-q13 deletions with or without imprinting defects. Eur. J. Med. Genet. 59, 584–589. doi:10.1016/J.EJMG.2016.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hills, P., and Argyle, M. (2002). The Oxford happiness questionnaire: A compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Pers. Individ. Dif. 33, 1073–1082. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00213-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanova, E., and Kelsey, G. (2011). Imprinted genes and hypothalamic function. J. Mol. Endocrinol. 47, R67–R74. doi:10.1530/JME-11-0065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, L., Manzardo, A. M., Miller, J. L., Driscoll, D. J., and Butler, M. G. (2016). Elevated plasma oxytocin levels in children with Prader-Willi syndrome compared with healthy unrelated siblings. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 170, 594–601. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.37488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juriaans, A. F., Kerkhof, G. F., and Hokken-Koelega, A. C. S. (2022). The spectrum of the prader-willi-like pheno- and genotype: A review of the literature. Endocr. Rev. 43, 1–18. doi:10.1210/ENDREV/BNAB026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kincaid, D. L., Doris, M., Shannon, C., and Mulholland, C. (2017). What is the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder and ASD traits in psychosis? A systematic review. Psychiatry Res. 250, 99–105. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2017.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleefstra, T., Kramer, J. M., Neveling, K., Willemsen, M. H., Koemans, T. S., Vissers, L. E. L. M., et al. (2012). Disruption of an EHMT1-associated chromatin-modification module causes intellectual disability. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 91, 73–82. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2012.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korver-Nieberg, N., Berry, K., Meijer, C. J., and De Haan, L. (2014). Adult attachment and psychotic phenomenology in clinical and non-clinical samples: A systematic review. Psychol. Psychother. 87, 127–154. doi:10.1111/PAPT.12010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotler, J., and Haig, D. (2018). The tempo of human childhood: a maternal foot on the accelerator, a paternal foot on the brake. Evol. Anthropol. 27, 80–91. doi:10.1002/evan.21579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotler, J., Balko, K., Berall, G., and Haig, D. (2016). Nutritional phases in prader-willi syndrome: Evolutionary and clinical Interpretations. J. Evol. Med. 4, 1–7. doi:10.4303/jem/235968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozlov, S. V., Bogenpohl, J. W., Howell, M. P., Wevrick, R., Panda, S., Hogenesch, J. B., et al. (2007). The imprinted gene Magel2 regulates normal circadian output. Nat. Genet. 39, 1266–1272. doi:10.1038/ng2114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landers, M., Bancescu, D. L., Le Meur, E., Rougeulle, C., Glatt-Deeley, H., Brannan, C., et al. (2004). Regulation of the large (approximately 1000 kb) imprinted murine Ube3a antisense transcript by alternative exons upstream of Snurf/Snrpn. Nucleic Acids Res. 32, 3480–3492. doi:10.1093/nar/gkh670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lassi, G., Priano, L., Maggi, S., Garcia-Garcia, C., Balzani, E., El-Assawy, N., et al. (2016). Deletion of the snord116/SNORD116 alters sleep in mice and patients with prader-willi syndrome. Sleep 39, 637–644. doi:10.5665/sleep.5542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. G., Hassani, O. K., and Jones, B. E. (2005). Discharge of identified orexin/hypocretin neurons across the sleep-waking cycle. J. Neurosci. 25, 6716–6720. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1887-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, R., Dong, Y., and Li, J. D. (2020). Necdin regulates BMAL1 stability and circadian clock through SGT1-HSP90 chaperone machinery. Nucleic Acids Res. 48, 7944–7957. doi:10.1093/nar/gkaa601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A., State, M., Anderson, G. M., Kaye, W. M., Hanchett, J. M., McConaha, C. W., et al. (1998). Cerebrospinal fluid levels of oxytocin in prader-willi syndrome: A preliminary report. Biol. Psychiatry 44, 1349–1352. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00190-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marty, V., and Cavaillé, J. (2019). Imprinted small noncoding RNA genes in brain function and behaviour. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 25, 8–14. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercer, R. E., Michaelson, S. D., Chee, M. J. S., Atallah, T. A., Wevrick, R., and Colmers, W. F. (2013). Magel2 is required for leptin-mediated depolarization of POMC neurons in the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus in mice. PLoS Genet. 9, e1003207. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1003207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meziane, H., Schaller, F., Bauer, S., Villard, C., Matarazzo, V., Riet, F., et al. (2015). An early postnatal oxytocin treatment prevents social and learning deficits in adult mice deficient for Magel2, a gene involved in prader-willi syndrome and autism. Biol. Psychiatry 78, 85–94. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millard, S. J., Bearden, C. E., Karlsgodt, K. H., and Sharpe, M. J. (2022). The prediction-error hypothesis of schizophrenia: New data point to circuit-specific changes in dopamine activity. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 628–640. doi:10.1038/S41386-021-01188-Y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, R., Goodall, K., and Woodrow, A. (2020). The relationship between attachment insecurity and experiences on the paranoia continuum: A meta-analysis. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 59, 290–318. doi:10.1111/BJC.12247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholls, R. D., and Knepper, J. L. (2001). Genome organization, function, and imprinting in Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes. Annu. Rev. Genomics Hum. Genet. 2, 153–175. doi:10.1146/annurev.genom.2.1.153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Hora, K. P., Zhang, Z., Vajdi, A., Kushan-Wells, L., Huang, Z. S., Pacheco-Hansen, L., et al. (2022). Neurobehavioral dimensions of prader willi syndrome: Relationships between sleep and psychosis-risk symptoms. Front. Psychiatry 13, 868536. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2022.868536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pace, M., Falappa, M., Freschi, A., Balzani, E., Berteotti, C., Lo Martire, V., et al. (2020). Loss of Snord116 impacts lateral hypothalamus, sleep, and food-related behaviors. JCI Insight 5, 137495. doi:10.1172/jci.insight.137495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietromonaco, P. R., Feldman, L., Boston College, B., and Feldman Barrett, L. (2000). The internal working models concept: What do we really know about the self in relation to others? Rev. General Psychol. 4, 155–175. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.4.2.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polex-Wolf, J., Lam, B. Y. H., Larder, R., Tadross, J., Rimmington, D., Bosch, F., et al. (2018). Hypothalamic loss of Snord116 recapitulates the hyperphagia of Prader-Willi syndrome. J. Clin. Invest. 128, 960–969. doi:10.1172/JCI97007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qi, Y., Purtell, L., Fu, M., Lee, N. J., Aepler, J., Zhang, L., et al. (2015). Snord116 is critical in the regulation of food intake and body weight. Sci. Rep. 6, 18614. doi:10.1038/srep18614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romano, A., Tempesta, B., Micioni Di Bonaventura, M. V., and Gaetani, S. (2016). From autism to eating disorders and more: The role of oxytocin in neuropsychiatric disorders. Front. Neurosci. 9, 497. doi:10.3389/FNINS.2015.00497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romano, A., Friuli, M., Cifani, C., and Gaetani, S. (2020). Oxytocin in the neural control of eating: At the crossroad between homeostatic and non-homeostatic signals. Neuropharmacology 171, 108082. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2020.108082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runte, M., Hüttenhofer, A., Gross, S., Kiefmann, M., Horsthemke, B., Buiting, K., et al. (2001). The IC-SNURF-SNRPN transcript serves as a host for multiple small nucleolar RNA species and as an antisense RNA for UBE3A. Hum. Mol. Genet. 10, 2687–2700. doi:10.1093/hmg/10.23.2687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saleh-Ghadimi, S., Dehghan, P., Abbasalizad Farhangi, M., Asghari-Jafarabadi, M., and Jafari-Vayghan, H. (2019). Could emotional eating act as a mediator between sleep quality and food intake in female students? Biopsychosoc. Med. 13, 15. doi:10.1186/S13030-019-0154-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salminen, I. I., Crespi, B. J., and Mokkonen, M. (2019). Baby food and bedtime: Evidence for opposite phenotypes from different genetic and epigenetic alterations in Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes. SAGE Open Med. 7, 2050312118823585. doi:10.1177/2050312118823585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampson, K. N., Upthegrove, R., Abu-Akel, A., Haque, S., Wood, S. J., and Reniers, R. (2021). Co-occurrence of autistic and psychotic traits: implications for depression, self-harm and suicidality. Psychol. Med. 51, 1364–1372. doi:10.1017/S0033291720000124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scatliffe, N., Casavant, S., Vittner, D., and Cong, X. (2019). Oxytocin and early parent-infant interactions: A systematic review. Int. J. Nurs. Sci. 6, 445–453. doi:10.1016/J.IJNSS.2019.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaller, F., Watrin, F., Sturny, R., Massacrier, A., Szepetowski, P., and Muscatelli, F. (2010). A single postnatal injection of oxytocin rescues the lethal feeding behaviour in mouse newborns deficient for the imprinted Magel2 gene. Hum. Mol. Genet. 19, 4895–4905. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddq424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, L., Caixàs, A., Dimitropoulos, A., Dykens, E., Duis, J., Einfeld, S., et al. (2021). Behavioral features in prader-willi syndrome (PWS): consensus paper from the international PWS clinical trial consortium. J. Neurodev. Disord. 13, 25–13. doi:10.1186/S11689-021-09373-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinnema, M., Collin, P., Maaskant, M. A., Van Roozendaal, E. P., Schrander-Stumpel, C. T. R. M., Curfs, L. M. G., et al. (2011). Psychiatric illness in a cohort of adults with Prader-Willi syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 1729–1735. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.02.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, M. J., Macedo, A., Bos, S. C., Maia, B., Marques, M., Pereira, A. T., et al. (2011). Sleep disturbances, body mass index and eating behaviour in undergraduate students. J. Sleep. Res. 20, 479–486. doi:10.1111/J.1365-2869.2010.00887.X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soni, S., Whittington, J., Holland, A. J., Webb, T., Maina, E., Boer, H., et al. (2007). The course and outcome of psychiatric illness in people with Prader-Willi syndrome: Implications for management and treatment. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 51, 32–42. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00895.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stelzer, Y., Sagi, I., Yanuka, O., Eiges, R., and Benvenisty, N. (2014). The noncoding RNA IPW regulates the imprinted DLK1-DIO3 locus in an induced pluripotent stem cell model of Prader-Willi syndrome. Nat. Genet. 46, 551–557. doi:10.1038/NG.2968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swaab, D. F., Purba, J. S., and Hofman, M. A. (1995). Alterations in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus and its oxytocin neurons (putative satiety cells) in prader-willi syndrome: a study of five cases. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 80, 573–579. doi:10.1210/JCEM.80.2.7852523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swaab, D. F. (1997). Prader-Willi syndrome and the hypothalamus. Acta Paediatr. Suppl. 86, 50–54. doi:10.1111/J.1651-2227.1997.TB18369.X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, J. S. (2017). Transcriptional architecture of the mammalian circadian clock. Nat. Rev. Genet. 18, 164–179. doi:10.1038/nrg.2016.150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trace, S. E., Thornton, L. M., Runfola, C. D., Lichtenstein, P., Pedersen, N. L., and Bulik, C. M. (2012). Sleep problems are associated with binge eating in women. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 45, 695–703. doi:10.1002/EAT.22003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., and Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 295–315. doi:10.1002/1098-108x(198602)5:2<295:aid-eat2260050209>3.0.co;2-t

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhoeven, W. M. A., Egger, J. I. M., Vermeulen, K., van de Warrenburg, B. P. C., and Kleefstra, T. (2011). Kleefstra syndrome in three adult patients: Further delineation of the behavioral and neurological phenotype shows aspects of a neurodegenerative course. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 155, 2409–2415. doi:10.1002/AJMG.A.34186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogels, A., De Hert, M., Descheemaeker, M. J., Govers, V., Devriendt, K., Legius, E., et al. (2004). Psychotic disorders in Prader-Willi syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 127A, 238–243. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.30004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willie, J. T., Sinton, C. M., Maratos-Flier, E., and Yanagisawa, M. (2008). Abnormal response of melanin-concentrating hormone deficient mice to fasting: Hyperactivity and rapid eye movement sleep suppression. Neuroscience 156, 819–829. doi:10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2008.08.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whittington, J., and Holland, A. (2010). Neurobehavioral phenotype in Prader–Willi syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part C Semin. Med. Genet. 154C, 438–447. doi:10.1002/AJMG.C.30283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, Y., and Dorsky, R. I. (2017). Development of the hypothalamus: conservation, modification and innovation. Development 144, 1588–1599. doi:10.1242/DEV.139055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamanaka, A., Beuckmann, C. T., Willie, J. T., Hara, J., Tsujino, N., Mieda, M., et al. (2003). Hypothalamic orexin neurons regulate arousal according to energy balance in mice. Neuron 38, 701–713. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00331-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes, hypothalamus, genomic imprinting, schizophrenia, attachment, sleep, feeding

Citation: Salminen I, Read S and Crespi B (2022) Do the diverse phenotypes of Prader-Willi syndrome reflect extremes of covariation in typical populations?. Front. Genet. 13:1041943. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2022.1041943

Received: 11 September 2022; Accepted: 01 November 2022;
Published: 24 November 2022.

Edited by:

Juliette Salles, Université Toulouse III Paul Sabatier, France

Reviewed by:

Freddy Jeanneteau, INSERM U1191 Institut de Génomique Fonctionnelle (IGF), France
Richard S. Lee, Johns Hopkins University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Salminen, Read and Crespi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Iiro Salminen, isalmine@sfu.ca; Bernard Crespi, bernard_crespi@sfu.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.