Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 04 August 2014
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Not only cardiovascular, but also coordinative exercise increases hippocampal volume in older adults

\r\nClaudia NiemannClaudia Niemann1Ben Godde,Ben Godde1,2Claudia Voelcker-Rehage,*\r\nClaudia Voelcker-Rehage1,2*
  • 1Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and Institutional Development, Jacobs University Bremen, Bremen, Germany
  • 2AgeAct Research Center, Jacobs University Bremen, Bremen, Germany

Cardiovascular activity has been shown to be positively associated with gray and white matter volume of, amongst others, frontal and temporal brain regions in older adults. This is particularly true for the hippocampus, a brain structure that plays an important role in learning and memory, and whose decline has been related to the development of Alzheimer’s disease. In the current study, we were interested in whether not only cardiovascular activity but also other types of physical activity, i.e., coordination training, were also positively associated with the volume of the hippocampus in older adults. For this purpose we first collected cross-sectional data on “metabolic fitness” (cardiovascular fitness and muscular strength) and “motor fitness” (e.g., balance, movement speed, fine coordination). Second, we performed a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Results revealed that motor fitness but not metabolic fitness was associated with hippocampal volume. After the 12-month intervention period, both, cardiovascular and coordination training led to increases in hippocampal volume. Our findings suggest that a high motor fitness level as well as different types of physical activity were beneficial to diminish age-related hippocampal volume shrinkage or even increase hippocampal volume.

Introduction

It has repeatedly been shown that physical activity is positively related to brain structure and function (see Kramer et al., 2006; Hillman et al., 2008; Park and Reuter-Lorenz, 2009) as well as cognitive performance (for a review see Etnier et al., 2006) and that it might reduce the risk of developing dementia (Sumic et al., 2007; Erickson et al., 2012). Particularly the hippocampal formation has been in the focus of these studies investigating the positive effect of physical activity on brain volume because this brain structure is thought to be significantly involved in diseases related to memory impairment like, e.g., Alzheimer’s disease (Driscoll et al., 2003; Barnes et al., 2009). Whereas positive correlations between cardiovascular activity and hippocampal volume have been shown, it is unknown whether this applies to other types of physical activity like, e.g., coordination training, as well. With this paper we aim to extend the knowledge about the effects of different physical activity interventions in older adults and its potential to diminish the decline in hippocampal volume during the aging process.

The Hippocampus and its Shrinkage Across the Adult Lifespan

The hippocampal formation as part of the limbic system is located in the medial temporal lobe. It is highly involved in processes of episodic memory formation (Tulving and Markowitsch, 1998; Van Petten, 2004) and spatial navigation (O’Keefe, 1990; Maguire et al., 2000). Both cognitive dimensions are especially vulnerable to performance loss in late adulthood (cf. Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004). Recently, the hippocampus has also been associated with motor sequence consolidation (Albouy et al., 2008). Over the adult lifespan, on average, hippocampal volume shrinks about 0.86% per year, but this development is highly non-linear (Raz et al., 2004b). Whereas for adults below the age of 50 annual hippocampal volume reductions of only 0.51% were observed, adults above the age of 50 revealed a much steeper annual volume decline of 1.18% (Raz et al., 2004b). These volume reductions are mainly attributed to reductions in the neuropil part of the brain structure and – to a smaller extend – to cell body shrinkage and changes in vascularization (Thomas et al., 2012).

Effects of Cardiovascular Fitness/Training on the Hippocampus

The hippocampus seems to be especially sensitive to cardiovascular activity. This has been demonstrated in research with rodents (e.g., van Praag et al., 1999, 2005; Kronenberg et al., 2006; Pereira et al., 2007; Fabel and Kempermann, 2008; Kempermann et al., 2010) and humans (e.g., Pereira et al., 2007; Erickson et al., 2009, 2010, 2011). Rodent studies revealed two main mechanisms to be responsible for structural benefits after cardiovascular training: the enhancement of capillary density (angiogenesis; Black et al., 1990) and the generation of new neurons (neurogenesis; van Praag et al., 1999), whereby the latter is specifically restricted to the dentate gyrus. Typically, old rodents and humans reveal a substantial decline in hippocampal neurogenesis (approx. 10-fold for rodents, cf. Ben Abdallah et al., 2010; and approx. fourfold for humans, cf. Spalding et al., 2013). In old rats the rate of decline in neurogenesis could be halved by 1 month of cardiovascular training and this reduced rate of decline was associated with better spatial learning abilities (van Praag et al., 2005; Kronenberg et al., 2006). These findings indicate a beneficial influence of cardiovascular activity on hippocampal volume and function even in older ages.

In a human sample aged 59–81 years, larger hippocampal volume mediated the relationship between better cardiovascular fitness and higher spatial memory performance (Erickson et al., 2009); and 12 months of cardiovascular training were associated with an increase in hippocampal volume by about 2% in older adults (Erickson et al., 2011). This increase in hippocampal volume was associated with improvement of cardiovascular fitness (VO2 max) and spatial memory performance, respectively (Erickson et al., 2011).

Effects of Motor Fitness and Coordination Training on the Brain

Physical activity, however, does not only comprise exercises that require metabolic and energetic processes, like cardiovascular training, but also motor-demanding exercises that require perceptual and higher-level cognitive information processing. This is, for example, true for coordinative exercises, like balance, eye–hand coordination or leg–arm coordination tasks (cf. Voelcker-Rehage and Niemann, 2013). Research on animals suggests that effects of motor-demanding activity on brain metabolism differ from those of cardiovascular activity (Black et al., 1990; Anderson et al., 1994). A motor-demanding training based on coordinative skills (e.g., crossing obstacles, balancing on ropes, etc.) facilitated synaptic growth and re-construction (synaptogenesis; Black et al., 1990; Seo et al., 2010). It also led to a higher degree of survival of newly generated cells than cardiovascular exercise (cf. above Curlik et al., 2013). Results from studies with older adults point in a similar direction. They revealed that motor fitness (e.g., action speed, reaction speed, balance) and coordination training facilitate brain function (particularly in those areas related to visual–spatial processing) and cognitive performance in older adults (Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2010, 2011). The associations of motor fitness with and the influences of coordination training on brain volume and specifically on hippocampal volume in older adults have not been investigated so far.

Research Questions and Hypotheses

In a first step, analyzing a cross-sectional sample of older adults, we analyzed the relationship between metabolic/cardiovascular fitness and the volume of the hippocampus (Erickson et al., 2009). Moreover, for the first time, we tested the hypothesis that motor fitness is also associated with hippocampal volume in older adults. Based on our own findings revealing a positive association of motor fitness and brain function, we assumed to find a positive relationship between motor fitness and hippocampal volume as well. This seems particularly reasonable as the hippocampus is involved in spatial navigation and motor sequence consolidation (O’Keefe, 1990; Albouy et al., 2008), two processes probably associated with motor fitness.

In the second step, we analyzed interventional data from a subsample of the same dataset to investigate the effects of a 12-month cardiovascular or coordination training on volume of the hippocampus. We expected not only cardiovascular training to positively influence hippocampal volume (as shown by Erickson et al., 2011), but also coordination training. This assumption is based on findings in rodents suggesting that motor activity benefits synaptogenesis in activated brain regions (e.g., Black et al., 1990).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Ninety-two older adults between 62 and 79 years of age were recruited to participate in the Old Age on the Move project. Only participants were included who did not report any history of cardiovascular disease, neurological disorder, or motor or cognitive restriction (e.g., metal implants, a score of less than 27 in the Mini Mental Status Examination, MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975). One participant had to be excluded due to cognitive impairment (MMSE <27, cf. O’Bryant et al., 2008 for cut-off level, remaining participants N = 91). All subjects participated voluntarily in the study and provided written informed consent to the procedures of the study, which was approved by the ethics committee of the German Psychological Society. The study conformed to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

Participants were tested for normal vision (Freiburg Visual Acuity Test; Bach, 2007) and hearing (simultaneous auditory thresholds at multiple frequencies for both ears; Presentation® software, Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, Canada; Yund, 2003) and were given a demographics and health questionnaire to determine characteristics of the sample (cf. Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic information [mean (M) and Standard deviation (SD)] for participants of the cross-sectional sample (N = 75) and of the two experimental groups (cardiovascular training, coordination training) and the control group (stretching and relaxation; N = 49) at measurement time point t1.

Cross-sectional analysis

For the cross-sectional analysis, we excluded 11 participants from data analysis due to incomplete MRI data or data of quality too low for structural data analysis, and five participants due to incomplete motor data. The remaining 75 participants had a mean age of 68.65 years (SD = 3.54; range 62–79 years; 55 females).

Analysis of intervention effects

The 91 participants were randomly assigned to two experimental groups and an active control group with respect to their place of residence and participated in a 12-month intervention study. In addition to the participants excluded from the cross-sectional analysis, we excluded 10 participants from the intervention sample due to incomplete MRI data at measurement time point t2 and/or t3 or structural MRI data with a quality too low for analysis. From the remaining sample (N = 65) we also excluded participants from data analysis (N = 16) who were absent for more than 33% of the training sessions. The remaining group consisted of 49 participants between 62 and 79 years (32 women and 17 men, M = 68.92, SD = 3.75; cardiovascular training: N = 17, 12 women, 5 men; coordination training: N = 19, 13 women, 6 men; control group: N = 13, 7 women, 6 men; cf. Table 1 for sample characteristics at t1 within each training group). For analysis of selectivity effects see Voelcker-Rehage et al. (2011).

Motor Tests

Fitness assessments

Motor fitness at baseline as well as after 6 and 12 months of intervention was assessed by using a heterogeneous battery of eight motor tests representing different domains of motor fitness: flexibility (shoulder flexibility by Rikli and Jones, 1999), action speed (agility by Adrian, 1981; hand tapping by Oja and Tuxworth, 1995; feet tapping by Voelcker-Rehage and Wiertz, 2003), balance (one-leg-stand with eyes open and closed by Ekdahl et al., 1989; backwards beam walk by Kiphard and Schilling, 1974) as well as fine coordination (Purdue Pegboard test by Tiffin and Asher, 1948). An overall index of motor fitness was calculated from these individual performances. For a more detailed description see Voelcker-Rehage et al. (2010). To prove effects of motor coordination training across the 12-month study interval (after 6 and 12 months), we used three tests that characterize motor coordination performance, namely feet tapping (as indicator for action speed), a stick-fall-test (as indicator for reaction speed, Ehrler and Huth, 2000), and one-leg-stand with eyes open (as indicator for balance performance).

Metabolic fitness was assessed by calculating an index of grip force (as indicator of muscular strength by Igbokwe, 1992) and peak oxygen consumption (VO2 peak, cf. also Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2010). VO2 peak as an indicator of cardiovascular fitness was assessed by a spiroergometry (ZAN300, a measurement system of oxygen consumption and for indirect calorimetric assessment). During spiroergometry participants completed a submaximal graded exercise test (a modified Porszasz protocol, Porszasz et al., 2003) on a Lode Valiant motor-driven treadmill with electrocardiography activity monitored by a ten-lead fully digital stress system (Kiss, GE Healthcare, Munich, Germany; cf. Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2010). For training prescriptions (cardiovascular group) we calculated the aerobic (AerTGE) and anaerobic (AnTGE) gas exchange thresholds. The AerTGE of healthy, untrained older people occurred at ~60% VO2 peak (Prioux et al., 2000). For data analysis the mean VO2 value of the highest complete performance level achieved by the participants, expressed as VO2 peak ml/kg, was used. Analyses were also done for cardiovascular fitness only.

Interventions

Interventions took place three times per week for 45–60 min. Training interventions were led by an experienced exercise leader and took place in groups of 10–15 participants each.

Coordination training focused on the improvement of complex movements for the whole body such as balance, eye–hand coordination, leg–arm coordination as well as spatial orientation and reaction to moving objects/persons. Different fitness equipment such as exercise balls, twist boards, fitness balls, jump ropes, exercise bands, and stability boards were used to offer an individually challenging and diversified program.

Cardiovascular training was designed to improve cardiorespiratory fitness via a Nordic Walking program. Training intensity prescriptions were based on heart rate (HR) responses to spiroergometry exercise testing. Participants of the cardiovascular training group trained with their individual HR in an intensity zone at and above AerTGE (but below AnTGE, extensive training, Meyer et al., 2005) as described elsewhere (Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2011).

Stretching and relaxation training served as an active control condition to evaluate the potential effects of being involved in a guided group activity for 12 months as well as controlling for retest and social participation effects. A program of relaxation techniques (e.g., imaginary journey, progressive muscle relaxation, and massage), stretching and limbering for the whole body especially designed for older adults was offered to this group. Using different equipment such as fitness balls and rubber tubes provided program variation.

Design and MR Image Acquisition

At baseline (t1), as well as after six (t2) and after 12 months (t3), each participant completed the fitness assessment in one laboratory session of approximately 2 h. All tasks were administered in a fixed order. On another day but within 1 week, participants underwent the MRI scanning at a 3-Tesla Allegra Scanner. Using a MPRAGE sequence with 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm isotropic resolution, a TR of 2300 ms, and 160 slices, T1-weighted anatomical scans were collected. Assessment of the structural MR images was part of a larger MRI testing protocol, the results of which are reported elsewhere (cf. Godde and Voelcker-Rehage, 2010; Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2010, 2011).

MRI Data Processing and Analysis

The T1-weighted MR images were realigned to correct for head tilt, pitch, and rotation passing through the anterior and posterior commissure line (AC-PC alignment) by using Brain Voyager (Brain Innovation B.V., Netherlands). In the next step, hippocampal volumes were measured on coronal sections by manual tracing using Analyze (Analyze Version 10.0, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, USA). Hippocampal volume of the right and left hemisphere as well as intracranial volume (ICV) were defined and traced according to the rules described below (original rules cf. Raz et al., 2003 for the ICV; Raz et al., 2004b for the hippocampus). Then, volumes were computed by multiplying the sum of the areas within each slice by slice thickness. Finally, individual (i) raw volumes were adjusted using the following formula: Volume_adji = volume_rawi - b (ICVi - ICVsamplemean; Raz et al., 2003) to correct for inter-individual ICV differences, where b is the regression slope of volume_rawi on ICV. Reliability of volume determination was secured by calculating the intraclass correlation coefficient ICC (2; for more information on the tracing reliability process see Raz et al., 2004a). All ICC(2) values were above the critical level of.90.

Intracranial volume was estimated by tracing its volume on every 10th image slice (approx. 16 slices) between the first slice following the orbits and the last slice on which brain tissue was visible. This method has shown to save a lot of time (in comparison with tracing every slice) by only having negligible reductions in reliability (Eritaia et al., 2000). Tracing started at the right side of the head proceeding clockwise while following the inner table of the skull until the homologous mark on the left side of the head was reached. At this point, tracing was stopped and automatically a straight line back to the origin was drawn by the program (cf. Raz et al., 2003).

Hippocampal volume was estimated on every slice from the first slice where the mammillary bodies appear most bulbous. The posterior boundary was determined by looking for the slice that cuts through the fornices as they rise after leaving the fimbria of the hippocampus and then using the next slice as the last one (cf. Raz et al., 2004b). The volume of the hippocampus was estimated from 22 to 32 coronal sections. Further demarcation lines were identical as described by Raz et al. (2004a; cf. Figure 1 for example images).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Example images and representative analyses of hippocampal tracing rules from anterior to posterior brain sections. Marked structure on the right-hand side of the pictures represents hippocampal formation of left hemisphere, marked structure on the left-hand side on the pictures represents hippocampal formation of right hemisphere.

Statistical Analyses

Cross-sectional analysis (t1)

We conducted two linear regression analyses to investigate the relationship between motor fitness, metabolic fitness, and the interaction between motor and metabolic fitness (independent variables) as well as motor fitness, cardiovascular fitness and their interaction term (independent variables) on hippocampal volume (dependent variable), respectively. Hippocampal volume did not correlate with age, sex, years of formal education, subjective health, and estrogen replacement therapy. However, as motor fitness correlated with age and sex, we included those variables in the regression analyses.

Analysis of intervention effects

To investigate the effects of the intervention programs on cardiovascular fitness, a 3 × 3 mixed factor ANOVA with repeated measures was computed with SESSION (t1, t2, t3) as the within-subject variable and GROUP (cardiovascular training, coordination training, control group) as the between-subject variable. Intervention effects on motor fitness were analyzed by a 3 (SESSION: t1, t2, t3) × 3 (motor fitness TASK: action speed, reaction speed, one-leg-stand with eyes open) × 3 (GROUP: cardiovascular training, coordination training, control group) repeated-measures ANOVA. Furthermore, a 3 × 3 mixed factors ANOVA with repeated measures was computed with SESSION (t1, t2, t3) as the within-subject variable and GROUP (cardiovascular training, coordination training, control group) as the between-subject variable to investigate the effects of the intervention programs on total hippocampal volume (sum of right and left HC volume). We further analyzed hippocampal volume of the left and right hemisphere separately to test for differences of intervention effects on both hemispheres. Greenhouse Geisser adjustment is reported in case the sphericity assumption was violated. To determine effects within the exercise groups, analyses were computed separately for each exercise group using estimated marginal means collapsed over the between and within-subject variables. Post hoc pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni adjustment were conducted to determine pre-to-post-test changes within and between the experimental and control groups. Effect sizes were calculated for significant results by partial eta-square (η2), expressing the amount of variance explained in the dependent variables by the respective effect. Experimental and control groups were statistically similar to one another on measures of age, sex, years of formal education, BMI, hypertension, and estrogene replacement therapy (for women) as well as motor and cardiovascular fitness (cf. Table 1), and did not reveal a noticeable selectivity effect (for IQ, health, activity index and positive affect cf. Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2011). Thus, we abstained from including covariates.

Bringing together changes in fitness domains and hippocampal volume after the intervention, we calculated three linear regression analyses with hippocampal volume at t3 (sum of both hemispheres, left HC, right HC) as dependent variable and changes in motor tests from t1 to t3 (action speed, balance, and cardiovascular fitness) as independent variables. Reaction speed did not show significant changes and was therefore not included in the regression analysis. Hippocampal volume and change scores (t3 minus t1) did not correlate with possible covariates, thus, we abstained from including other covariates in the analyses. For all analyses, the significance level was set to α = 0.05.

Results

Cross-Sectional Data: Association of Fitness with Volume of the Hippocampus

Linear regression analysis revealed that fitness dimensions and their interaction as well as age and sex together explained 10.9% of variance in hippocampal volume (see Table 2). Motor fitness uniquely explained 7% of variance in hippocampal volume, whereas the other variables did not further explain variance in hippocampal volume significantly (shared variance 1.5%; cf. additionally Figure 2 for a scatterplot of hippocampal volume against motor fitness (Figure 2A) and metabolic fitness (Figure 2B). The second regression analysis with cardiovascular fitness instead of metabolic fitness revealed comparable results explaining 13.1% of total variance in hippocampal volume (see Table 3 and Figure 2C for a scatterplot of hippocampal volume against cardiovascular fitness). Again, motor fitness uniquely explained 9% of variance in hippocampal volume and none of the other variables did significantly explain additional variance.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Scatterplots showing sum of hippocampal volume of both hemispheres (in cm3) in relationship with (A) motor fitness (significant), (B) metabolic fitness, and (C) cardiovascular fitness at t1.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Regression statistics for hippocampal (HC) volume (sum HC volume: sum of left and right volume, left HC volume, right HC volume) as dependent variable and age, sex, motor fitness (MF), metabolic fitness (MBF), and their interaction term as independent variables.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Regression statistics for hippocampal (HC) volume (sum HC volume: sum of left and right volume, left HC volume, right HC volume) as dependent variable and age, sex, motor fitness (MF), cardiovascular fitness (CF), and their interaction term as independent variables.

Results separately for both hemispheres are similar as for total hippocampal volume (cf. Tables 2 and 3; for the left hippocampus, all variables accounted for 11.5% of variance in volume; for the right hemisphere the explained variance was 7.7%. Motor fitness uniquely explained 5.7% of the variance in left hippocampal volume and 6% in right hemisphere. Neither metabolic fitness, cardiovascular fitness or the respective interaction terms with motor fitness nor the control variables age and sex did explain additional variance.

Effects of Cardiovascular and Coordination Training on Fitness

To compare efficiency of the specific training programs we analyzed training effects on cardiovascular and motor fitness, respectively, for all three study groups (cf. Figure 3A and Table 4). Overall, for cardiovascular fitness we found a significant SESSION × GROUP interaction [F(3.55,81.54) = 3.68, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.14], but no SESSION [F(1.77,81.54) = 0.93, p = 0.40, η2 = 0.02] or GROUP effect [F(2,46) = 1.15, p = 0.33, η2 = 0.05]. Separate analyses for the three groups revealed that cardiovascular fitness in the cardiovascular training group significantly increased by 13.65% across the 12-month intervention [F(2,45) = 5.58, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.20] with post hoc pairwise comparisons showing significant improvements only from t2 to t3 (p < 0.01), but not from t1 to t2. No significant changes could be observed in the coordination training group [1.98% increase; F(2,45) = 0.16, p = 0.88, η2 = 0.01] and the control group [5.65% decrease; F(2,45) = 0.98, p = 0.38, η2 = 0.04].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Change of (A) cardiovascular fitness (VO2 peak in ml/kg), (B) balance performance (one-leg-stand in seconds, max 20 sec.), (C) reaction speed performance (stick-fall-task in cm), and (D) action speed performance (feet tapping in 20 sec.) within training groups (cardiovascular training, coordination training, control group) over the intervention period (t1: baseline, t2: after 6 months, t3: after 12 months). *Significant change (p < 0.05).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Mean (M) and Standard deviation (SD) for motor fitness (z-value of overall motor fitness index) and cardiovascular (cardio.) fitness for all three measurement time points (t1, t2, t3) and for the two experimental groups (cardiovascular training, coordination training) and the control group (stretching and relaxation).

For the overall change in motor tasks, we found a main effect of SESSION [F(1.70,78.25) = 31.38, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.41] and TASK [F(1.37,63.16) = 1538.42, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.97] and a significant SESSION × TASK interaction [F(2.40,110.48) = 10.47, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.19]. There was no GROUP effect [F(2,46) = 0.07, p = 0.93, η2 = 0.00] and no GROUP × SESSION interaction [F(3.40,78.25) = 1.43, p = 0.24, η2 = 0.06], no GROUP × TASK interaction [F(2.75,63.16) = 1.78, p = 0.16, η2 = 0.07], and no GROUP × SESSION × TASK interaction [F(4.80,110.48) = 1.31, p = 0.24, η2 = 0.05]. Calculating separate analyses for the study groups showed that after the 12-month intervention all study groups significantly improved their performance in the three selected motor fitness tasks [cardiovascular group: F(2,45) = 11. 22, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.33; coordination group: F[2,45] = 18.44, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.45; control group: F(2,45) = 4.03, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.15]. Analyses separately for the three tasks action speed, reaction speed and balance revealed a different development for motor fitness indicators within the three groups (cf. Figures 3B–D). The cardiovascular training group improved their action speed performance by 42% [F(2,45) = 12.62, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.36; with significant improvements from t1 to t2 (p < 0.001), but not t2 to t3]. Similarly, the coordination training group revealed an increase in action speed performance of 21.1% [F(2,45) = 8.35, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.27; with significant improvements from t1 to t2 (p < 0.001), but not from t2 to t3] and in addition an 21.6 % improvement of performance in one-leg-stand with eyes open [F(2,45) = 5.25, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.19; with significant improvements from t1 to t2 (p < 0.01), but not from t2 to t3]. Changes in reaction speed did not reach significance level (cardiovascular group: 3.6% improvement [F(2,45) = 1.31, p = 0.28, η2 = 0.06]; coordination group: 8.4% improvement [F(2,45) = 0.88, p = 0.42, η2 = 0.59)]. The control group did not show any significant changes across the 12-month intervention period in these three tasks.

Effects of Cardiovascular and Coordination Training on Volume of the Hippocampus

Hippocampal volume

Regarding the intervention effects on the hippocampal volume (sum score of volume of right and left hemisphere), the repeated measures ANOVA with three measurement time points (t1, t2, t3) revealed a main effect for SESSION, but no GROUP effect and no GROUP × SESSION interaction (cf. Table 5). Analyses separated by exercise groups revealed that in the cardiovascular group hippocampal volume significantly increased by 3.60% [F(2,45) = 4.76, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.17] across the 12-month exercise intervention (cf. also Table 6). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed only a significant overall increase, but no significant changes in hippocampal volume from t1 to t2 or from t2 to t3 (cf. Figure 4A and Table 6). Further, there was a significant increase of hippocampal volume (2.84%) after the coordination training [F(2,45) = 4.73, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.17; with significant improvements from t2 to t3 (p < 0.05), but not from t1 to t2; cf. Figure 4A and Table 6]. Volume increase across the 12 months in the control group (1.67%) was not significant [F(2,45) = 1.63, p = 0.21, η2 = 0.07; cf. Figure 4A and Table 6].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Change in volume of (A) hippocampal volume of both hemispheres, (B) left hippocampus, and (C) right hippocampus (in cm3) within training groups (cardiovascular training, coordination training, control group) over the intervention period (t1: baseline, t2: after 6 months, t3: after 12 months). *Significant change (p < 0.05).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Statistics for repeated measure ANOVA with main effects SESSION (t1, t2, t3) and GROUP (cardiovascular training, coordination training, control group) and interaction SESSION × GROUP for hippocampal (HC) volume (sum HC: sum of left and right volume, left HC volume, right HC volume).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Mean (M) and Standard deviation (SD) for hippocampal volume in cm3 collapsed over both hemispheres (sum HC: sum of left and right volume) and separately for left (left HC volume) and right hemisphere (right HC volume) for all three measurement time points (t1, t2, t3).

Left and right hippocampal hemisphere separately

Results for the left and right hemisphere confirmed results for the total hippocampal volume with a significant main effect for SESSION, but neither a GROUP effect nor a GROUP × SESSION interaction (cf. Table 5). Analyses separated by exercise groups revealed that only the cardiovascular group significantly increased hippocampal volume of the left hemisphere by 4.22% [F(2,45) = 5.10, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.19] across the 12 months exercise intervention (only significant overall change, but no changes from t1 to t2 or from t2 to t3; cf. Figure 4B). Analyses for the coordination and control groups revealed only non-significant increases of 1.78% and 1.65%, respectively. Interestingly, the coordination group significantly increased hippocampal volume of the right hemisphere by 3.91% [F(2,45) = 6.03, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.21] across the 12-month exercise intervention [with a significant change from t2 to t3 (p < 0.01), but not from t1 to t2, cf. Figure 4C]. Analyses for the cardiovascular and control groups revealed only non-significant increases of 2.98% and 1.68% volume of the right hippocampus, respectively.

Relationship between Changes in Fitness and Hippocampal Volume

Using a linear regression analysis with total hippocampal volume at t3 as dependent variable, only change in cardiovascular fitness (VO2 peak ml/kg) significantly explained variance (9.3%, p = 0.04) in hippocampal volume (cf. Table 7). When analyzing both hemispheres separately, again, only VO2 peak significantly explained 12.1% (p = 0.02) of variance in volume of the left hippocampus (cf. Table 7) – the side of the hippocampus that revealed a volume increase in the cardiovascular training group. No such effect was found for the right hemisphere.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 7. Regression statistics for hippocampal volume at t3 (sum HC volume: sum of left and right volume, left HC volume, right HC volume) as dependent variable and changes in motor fitness domains that showed significant changes (Δ Balance, Action speed) and changes in cardiovascular fitness (Δ VO2 peak) from t1 to t3 as independent variables.

Discussion

We investigated whether metabolic/cardiovascular and motor fitness are associated with hippocampal volume in healthy older adults and whether a 12-month cardiovascular or coordination training induces changes in hippocampal volume in these participants. Cross-sectional data indicate that only motor fitness (not cardiovascular fitness or metabolic fitness) is associated with hippocampal volume. However, after 12 months of training, both types of exercise, cardiovascular and coordination training, positively but differently, influence volume of the hippocampus.

Cross-Sectional Analyses

In line with our hypothesis for the cross-sectional analyses, motor fitness was significantly associated with hippocampal volume accounting for 7% of explained variance. To our knowledge, this is the first study that described a relationship between hippocampal volume and motor fitness, i.e., the component of fitness that requires perceptual and higher-level cognitive processes. Herewith we are adding significantly to previous functional brain data revealing a positive association between motor fitness and functional brain activation in frontal and parietal areas accompanied by better performance in perceptual speed and executive functions (Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2010).

We could not demonstrate a relationship between hippocampal volume and metabolic fitness (comprising cardiovascular fitness and grip strength) or cardiovascular fitness alone. This is in line with cross-sectional studies by Honea et al. (2009), Flöel et al. (2010), and Smith et al. (2011) who also did not find correlations between volume of the medial temporal lobe area, or more specifically the hippocampus, and cardiovascular fitness in different samples of healthy older adults (50+ years of age). However, a relationship between those factors was assumed by Erickson et al. (2009) as well as by Szabo et al. (2011) who both demonstrated a positive association between higher cardiovascular fitness and larger hippocampal volume in samples of healthy American older adults. Whereas our sample did not differ from theirs in age and presumably the level of cardiovascular fitness, differences in data analysis might account for diverging results: it has been shown that using (semi-)automated computer-programs (e.g., VBM, FIRST, cf. Erickson et al., 2009; Szabo et al., 2011) might reveal different results regarding brain volume than tracing predetermined brain regions manually (cf. Kennedy et al., 2009 for a comparison of different analyzing methods). Interestingly, Honea et al. (2009) confirmed a correlation between medial temporal lobe and cardiovascular fitness in patients suffering from early stages of Alzheimer‘s disease (Honea et al., 2009). Overall, although associations between hippocampal volume and cardiovascular fitness might be plausible, results are not such consistent and might depend on reasons mentioned above or on moderating variables still not identified.

Intervention Study

Data of the 12-month intervention study revealed a positive effect on total hippocampal volume (sum scores of right and left hemisphere) for both types of exercise, cardiovascular and coordination training. Interestingly, our findings for cardiovascular training mirror the results of Erickson et al. (2011) who also found an increase in hippocampal volume after 12 months of cardiovascular training, although cross-sectional results were different (see above). Possibly, training effects in a controlled trial are more reliable and stronger than cross-sectional associations of hippocampal volume and cardiovascular fitness that are influenced to a higher degree by sample characteristics and further uncontrolled factors.

Whereas Erickson et al. (2011) revealed an increase in left and right hippocampal volume by 2.12% and 1.97%, respectively, we found an even higher (twice as high) volume increase in the left hippocampus of 4.22% and an increase of 2.98% for the right hippocampus. The increase in the right hippocampus was, however, not statistically significant in our cardiovascular training group, probably due to the high variability within our sample. The higher level of volume increase, particularly for the left hippocampus, in our study might be explained by the magnitude of change in VO2 peak performance. In our sample, participants of the cardiovascular training group improved by 13.65%, whereas Erickson et al. (2011) found increases of cardiovascular fitness only by 7.78%. As Erickson et al. (2011) and our sample support the assumption that there might be an association between change in VO2 peak and hippocampal volume at post-test, one might suggest that there is a linear relationship between both variables.

Effects of Coordination Training

In addition, for the first time, we showed that not only changes in cardiovascular fitness seem to be associated with hippocampal volume in older adults, but also a 12-month coordination training led to changes in total hippocampal volume. In contrast to cardiovascular training, these changes were more pronounced in the right hippocampus (right: 3.91%; left: 1.78% non-significant change). Particularly the right hippocampus is known to be involved in spatial memory processes (Smith and Milner, 1981; Nunn et al., 1999; Burgess et al., 2002). As coordinative exercises to a high degree rely on and practice spatial orientation, this might be one explanation for our finding.

Studies in young adults have shown that short-term motor learning (e.g., performing a balance task, learning to write with the left non-dominant hand, etc.) seems to induce very rapid volume changes (increases and decreases within two to six training sessions) in training-related brain regions (Taubert et al., 2010; Gryga et al., 2012; Hamzei et al., 2012). We could not reveal changes in hippocampal volume after 6 months of training, but found a significant increase in volume after the second half of the intervention. These results might indicate that rapid volume changes might fluctuate in the beginning of training (Taubert et al., 2010; Gryga et al., 2012) and, since patterns of plastic changes might differ in regard to training duration (cf. Hamzei et al., 2012), are more stabilized after the continued training of complex motor tasks. In contrast to the cardiovascular training, we could not identify changes in specific motor tasks to cause changes in hippocampal volume. One reason might be that the changes in the motor system due to coordination training were not adequately measured by the selected motor tests.

In addition, in animal research, so far, few studies have compared effects of cardiovascular training and motor-demanding activities (corresponding to coordination training in humans, cf. Voelcker-Rehage and Niemann, 2013 for a further discussion). Greenough and colleagues (Black et al., 1990; Isaacs et al., 1992; Anderson et al., 1994) provided evidence that cardiovascular training increases capillary density in rodents, while acrobatic training in rodents induces the generation of new synapses and glia cells as well as dendritic hypertrophy (cf. Kleim et al., 1996, 1997, 1998; Seo et al., 2010). The motor-based training initiated blood vessel growth only to maintain the diffusion distance (Isaacs et al., 1992). Diverging underlying mechanisms might also explain the different time courses of volume change that we observed after the two types of exercise training. While the cardiovascular training group showed almost linear increase in hippocampal volume, hippocampal volume of the coordination training group increased from month 6 to 12 of the intervention only (cf. Figure 4). A recent review (Thomas et al., 2012) provided time courses for the different components of brain plasticity, i.e., angiogenesis and capillary density, neurogenesis, astrocyte volume, and neuropil volume. Following them, angiogenesis as well as neurogenesis show a rapid time course. For example, in mice capillary density is already enhanced after three days of increased aerobic activity, but also returns to baseline within 24 h of sedentary behavior (Van der Borght et al., 2009). Similarly, in rats astrocyte changes occur rapidly but return to baseline volume as soon as activity is stopped (Kleim et al., 2007). In contrast, again in rats changes in the neuropil are more stable and might last for several weeks (Kleim et al., 1997, 4 weeks after removed from enriched environment; see also a review by Markham and Greenough, 2004). Although to date, it is not terminally resolved whether the time course in brain plasticity is transferable from studies in rodents to humans, these findings from the animal literature let us suggest that also different underlying mechanisms might be responsible for the volume changes accompanied by different time courses in our sample of healthy older adults.

In addition, animal studies focusing on neurogenesis in the hippocampus support our diverging findings for cardiovascular and coordination training on hippocampal volume. They indicate that different types of activity stimulate the generation of new neurons in different ways (van Praag et al., 1999). In rodents, both types go in line with an enhancement of cell numbers: cardiovascular activity induces the proliferation of newborn cells in the hippocampus to a large extent. In contrast, an “enriched environment” condition consisting of a complex combination of physical, motor and social stimulation also promoted the proliferation of new born cells (approx. 50% of cardiovascular training effects) leading, in the long run, to a higher degree of integration and survival of the neurons (van Praag et al., 1999; Curlik et al., 2013). This finding could also add to our results in humans after 6 months of training, where we found changes in the cardiovascular group but not the coordination group and might explain why we found comparable volume increases after 12 months of intervention for both types of training. However, as we do not have follow up data after cessation of the exercise training, we, unfortunately, do not have any information about how long exercise changes last.

Conclusion

We were able to show that strengthening motor fitness and engaging in regular exercise (cardiovascular or coordination training) is beneficial for hippocampal volume in older adults. We herewith add to the exercise and cognition literature that mainly focuses on cardiovascular fitness another promising fitness component that is worth to be further investigated, namely motor fitness and coordination training. Although first studies are able to show, that cardiovascular activity-related increases in hippocampal volume in older adults go in line with benefits in spatial memory performance (cf. Erickson et al., 2009, 2011), further research is needed to clarify whether slowing down the shrinkage of the hippocampus volume might also diminish decline in hippocampal-dependent cognitive functioning in healthy older adults as well as people suffering from mild cognitive impairment or early stages of Alzheimer’s disease.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by a grant from the Robert Bosch Foundation (12.5.1366.0005.0) and from the German health insurance company DAK as well as funds of the Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and Institutional Development.

We would like to thank our cooperation partner Ursula M. Staudinger. We further thank Astrid Beck, Mala Kamenidu, Alexander Kowanz, Kathrin Linke, Fabian May, Pit Pletz, Tanja Truhart, and Julia Wolf for conducting the training interventions; Laura Fettkenhauer, Alexander Knop, Fabian May, and Pit Pletz for their help with data collection, Melanie Löbe and Peter Erhardt for their help with conducting the MRI scans, Katja Glinka for exchange on manual tracing processes and all participants for participating in the study.

There is nothing in the professional or financial affiliations of any of the authors that may have biased the presentation of material in the paper.

References

Adrian, M. J. (1981). “Flexibility in the aging adult,” in Exercises and Aging: The Scientific Basic, eds E. L. Smith and R. C. Serfass (Hillside, NJ: Enslow Publishers), 45–58.

Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Balteau, E., Vandewalle, G., Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T.,et al. (2008). Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor sequence memory. Neuron 58, 261–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Anderson, B. J., Li, X., Alcantara, A. A., Isaacs, K. R., Black, J. E., and Greenough, W. T. (1994). Glial hypertrophy is associated with synaptogenesis following motor-skill learning, but not with angiogenesis following exercise. Glia 11, 73–80. doi: 10.1002/glia.440110110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bach, M. (2007). The Freiburg visual acuity test-variability unchanged by post-hoc re-analysis. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 245, 965–971. doi: 10.1007/s00417-006-0474-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barnes, J., Bartlett, J. W., van de Pol, L. A., Loy, C. T., Scahill, R. I., Frost, C.,et al. (2009). A meta-analysis of hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 30, 1711–1723. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.01.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ben Abdallah, N. M., Slomianka, L., Vyssotski, A. L., and Lipp, H. P. (2010). Early age-related changes in adult hippocampal neurogenesis in C57 mice. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 151–161. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Black, J. E., Isaacs, K. R., Anderson, B. J., Alcantara, A. A., and Greenough, W. T. (1990). Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 87, 5568–5572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.14.5568

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., and O’Keefe, J. (2002). The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron 35, 625–641. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9

CrossRef Full Text

Curlik, D. M.2nd, Maeng, L. Y., Agarwal, P. R., and Shors, T. J. (2013). Physical skill training increases the number of surviving new cells in the adult hippocampus. PLoS ONE 8:e55850. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055850

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Driscoll, I., Hamilton, D. A., Petropoulos, H., Yeo, R. A., Brooks, W. M., Baumgartner, R. N.,et al. (2003). The aging hippocampus: cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1344–1351. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg081

CrossRef Full Text

Ehrler, W., and Huth, M. (2000). “Körperliche Leistungsfähigkeit 43- bis 45-jähriger und 62- bis 64-jähriger im Vergleich [A comparison of the motor performance of 43-45 and 62-64 year old],” in Aspekte der Entwicklung im mittleren und höheren Lebensalter, eds P. Martin, K. U. Ettrich, U. Lehr, D. Roether, and A. Fischer-Cyrulies (Darmstadt: Steinkopf), 220–334.

Ekdahl, C., Jarnlo, G. B., and Andersson, S. I. (1989). Standing balance in healthy subjects. Evaluation of a quantitative test battery on a force platform. Scand. J. Rehabil. Med. 21, 187–195.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Heo, S., McLaren, M.,et al. (2010). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is associated with age-related decline in hippocampal volume. J. Neurosci. 30, 5368–5375. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6251-09.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Hu, L., Morris, K. S.,et al. (2009). Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus 19, 1030–1039. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20547

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L.,et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Erickson, K. I., Weinstein, A. M., and Lopez, O. L. (2012). Physical activity, brain plasticity, and Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Med. Res. 43, 615–621. doi: 10.1016/j.arcmed.2012.09.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eritaia, J., Wood, S. J., Stuart, G. W., Bridle, N., Dudgeon, P., Maruff, P.,et al. (2000). An optimized method for estimating intracranial volume from magnetic resonance images. Magn. Reson. Med. 44, 973–977. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200012)44:6<973::AID-MRM21>3.0.CO;2-H

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Etnier, J. L., Nowell, P. M., Landers, D. M., and Sibley, B. A. (2006). A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Res. Rev. 52, 119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fabel, K., and Kempermann, G. (2008). Physical activity and the regulation of neurogenesis in the adult and aging brain. Neuromolecular Med. 10, 59–66. doi: 10.1007/s12017-008-8031-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Flöel, A., Ruscheweyh, R., Kruger, K., Willemer, C., Winter, B., Volker, K.,et al. (2010). Physical activity and memory functions: are neurotrophins and cerebral gray matter volume the missing link? Neuroimage 49, 2756–2763. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text

Godde, B., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2010). More automation and less cognitive control of imagined walking movements in high- versus low-fit older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2:139. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00139

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gryga, M., Taubert, M., Dukart, J., Vollmann, H., Conde, V., Sehm, B.,et al. (2012). Bidirectional gray matter changes after complex motor skill learning. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6:37. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamzei, F., Glauche, V., Schwarzwald, R., and May, A. (2012). Dynamic gray matter changes within cortex and striatum after short motor skill training are associated with their increased functional interaction. Neuroimage 59, 3364–3372. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.089

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hedden, T., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Honea, R. A., Thomas, G. P., Harsha, A., Anderson, H. S., Donnelly, J. E., Brooks, W. M.,et al. (2009). Cardiorespiratory fitness and preserved medial temporal lobe volume in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 23, 188–197. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31819cb8a2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Igbokwe, N. U. (1992). Hand grip dynamometer and arm muscle size in teenage boys and girls. J. Phys. Educ. Sport Sci. 4, 15–19.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Isaacs, K. R., Anderson, B. J., Alcantara, A. A., Black, J. E., and Greenough, W. T. (1992). Exercise and the brain: angiogenesis in the adult rat cerebellum after vigorous physical activity and motor skill learning. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 12, 110–119. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1992.14

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kempermann, G., Fabel, K., Ehninger, D., Babu, H., Leal-Galicia, P., Garthe, A.,et al. (2010). Why and how physical activity promotes experience-induced brain plasticity. Front. Neurosci. 4:189. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00189

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kennedy, K. M., Erickson, K. I., Rodrigue, K. M., Voss, M. W., Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F.,et al. (2009). Age-related differences in regional brain volumes: a comparison of optimized voxel-based morphometry to manual volumetry. Neurobiol. Aging 30, 1657–1676. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.12.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kiphard, E. J., and Schilling, F. (1974). Körperkoordinationstest für kinder. Weinheim: Beltz.

Kleim, J. A., Lussnig, E., Schwarz, E. R., Comery, T. A., and Greenough, W. T. (1996). Synaptogenesis and fos expression in the motor cortex of the adult rat after motor skill learning. J. Neurosci. 16, 4529–4535.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kleim, J. A., Markham, J. A., Vij, K., Freese, J. L., Ballard, D. H., and Greenough, W. T. (2007). Motor learning induces astrocytic hypertrophy in the cerebellar cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 178, 244–249. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.12.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kleim, J. A., Swain, R. A., Armstrong, K. A., Napper, R. M., Jones, T. A., and Greenough, W. T. (1998). Selective synaptic plasticity within the cerebellar cortex following complex motor skill learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 69, 274–289. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3827

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kleim, J. A., Swain, R. A., Czerlanis, C. M., Kelly, J. L., Pipitone, M. A., and Greenough, W. T. (1997). Learning-dependent dendritic hypertrophy of cerebellar stellate cells: plasticity of local circuit neurons. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 67, 29–33. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.3742

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., and Colcombe, S. J. (2006). Exercise, cognition, and the aging brain. J. Appl. Physiol. 101, 1237–1242. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00500.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kronenberg, G., Bick-Sander, A., Bunk, E., Wolf, C., Ehninger, D., and Kempermann, G. (2006). Physical exercise prevents age-related decline in precursor cell activity in the mouse dentate gyrus. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1505–1513. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S.,et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 4398–4403. doi: 10.1073/pnas.070039597

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markham, J. A., and Greenough, W. T. (2004). Experience-driven brain plasticity: beyond the synapse. Neuron Glia Biol. 1, 351–363. doi: 10.1017/S1740925X05000219

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meyer, T., Lucia, A., Earnest, C. P., and Kindermann, W. (2005). A conceptual framework for performance diagnosis and training prescription from submaximal gas exchange parameters – theory and application. Int. J. Sports Med. 26(Suppl. 1), S38–S48. doi: 10.1055/s-2004-830514

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nunn, J. A., Graydon, F. J., Polkey, C. E., and Morris, R. G. (1999). Differential spatial memory impairment after right temporal lobectomy demonstrated using temporal titration. Brain 122, 47–59. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.1.47

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O’Bryant, S. E., Humphreys, J. D., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., Graff-Radford, N. R., Petersen, R. C.,et al. (2008). Detecting dementia with the mini-mental state examination in highly educated individuals. Arch. Neurol. 65, 963–967. doi: 10.1001/archneur.65.7.963

CrossRef Full Text

Oja, P., and Tuxworth, B. (1995). Eurofit for Adults – Assessment of Health-Related Fitness. Tampere: Council of Europe Publishing.

O’Keefe, J. (1990). A computational theory of the hippocampal cognitive map. Prog. Brain Res. 83, 301–312. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)61258-3

CrossRef Full Text

Park, D. C., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656

CrossRef Full Text

Pereira, A. C., Huddleston, D. E., Brickman, A. M., Sosunov, A. A., Hen, R., McKhann, G. M.,et al. (2007). An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 5638–5643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611721104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Porszasz, J., Casaburi, R., Somfay, A., Woodhouse, L. J., and Whipp, B. J. (2003). A treadmill ramp protocol using simultaneous changes in speed and grade. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 35, 1596–1603. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000084593.56786.DA

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Prioux, J., Ramonatxo, M., Hayot, M., Mucci, P., and Prefaut, C. (2000). Effect of ageing on the ventilatory response and lactate kinetics during incremental exercise in man. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 81, 100–107. doi: 10.1007/PL00013780

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raz, N., Gunning-Dixon, F., Head, D., Rodrigue, K. M., Williamson, A., and Acker, J. D. (2004a). Aging, sexual dimorphism, and hemispheric asymmetry of the cerebral cortex: replicability of regional differences in volume. Neurobiol. Aging 25, 377–396. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00118-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raz, N., Rodrigue, K. M., Head, D., Kennedy, K. M., and Acker, J. D. (2004b). Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe: a study of a five-year change. Neurology 62, 433–438. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000106466.09835.46

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raz, N., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Gunning-Dixon, F., and Acker, J. D. (2003). Differential aging of the human striatum: longitudinal evidence. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 24, 1849–1856.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rikli, R. E., and Jones, C. J. (1999). Development and validation of a functional fitness test for community-residing older adults. J. Aging Phys. Act. 7, 129–161.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Seo, H. G., Kim, D. Y., Park, H. W., Lee, S. U., and Park, S. H. (2010). Early motor balance and coordination training increased synaptophysin in subcortical regions of the ischemic rat brain. J. Korean Med. Sci. 25, 1638–1645. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2010.25.11.1638

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, J. C., Nielson, K. A., Woodard, J. L., Seidenberg, M., Durgerian, S., Antuono, P.,et al. (2011). Interactive effects of physical activity and APOE-epsilon 4 on BOLD semantic memory activation in healthy elders. Neuroimage 54, 635–644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.070

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, M. L., and Milner, B. (1981). The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial location. Neuropsychologia 19, 781–793. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(81)90090-7

CrossRef Full Text

Spalding, K. L., Bergmann, O., Alkass, K., Bernard, S., Salehpour, M., Huttner, H. B.,et al. (2013). Dynamics of hippocampal neurogenesis in adult humans. Cell 153, 1219–1227. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sumic, A., Michael, Y. L., Carlson, N. E., Howieson, D. B., and Kaye, J. A. (2007). Physical activity and the risk of dementia in oldest old. J. Aging Health 19, 242–259. doi: 10.1177/0898264307299299

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Szabo, A. N., McAuley, E., Erickson, K. I., Voss, M., Prakash, R. S., Mailey, E. L.,et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness, hippocampal volume, and frequency of forgetting in older adults. Neuropsychology 25, 545–553. doi: 10.1037/a0022733

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taubert, M., Draganski, B., Anwander, A., Muller, K., Horstmann, A., Villringer, A.,et al. (2010). Dynamic properties of human brain structure: learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections. J. Neurosci. 30, 11670–11677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2567-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thomas, A. G., Dennis, A., Bandettini, P. A., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2012). The effects of aerobic activity on brain structure. Front. Psychol. 3:86. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00086

CrossRef Full Text

Tiffin, J., and Asher, E. J. (1948). The purdue pegboard; norms and studies of reliability and validity. J. Appl. Psychol. 32, 234–247. doi: 10.1037/h0061266

CrossRef Full Text

Tulving, E., and Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8, 198–204. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063 (1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G

CrossRef Full Text

Van der Borght, K., Kobor-Nyakas, D. E., Klauke, K., Eggen, B. J. L., Nyakas, C., Van der Zee, E. A.,et al. (2009). Physical exercise leads to rapid adaptations in hippocampal vasculature: temporal dynamics and relationship to cell proliferation and neurogenesis. Hippocampus 19, 928–936. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20545

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Petten, C. (2004). Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 42, 1394–1413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., and Gage, F. H. (1999). Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 266–270. doi: 10.1038/6368

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Praag, H., Shubert, T., Zhao, C., and Gage, F. H. (2005). Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J. Neurosci. 25, 8680–8685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voelcker-Rehage, C., Godde, B., and Staudinger, U. M. (2010). Physical and motor fitness are both related to cognition in old age. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 167–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07014.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voelcker-Rehage, C., Godde, B., and Staudinger, U. M. (2011). Cardiovascular and coordination training differentially improve cognitive performance and neural processing in older adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:26. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voelcker-Rehage, C., and Niemann, C. (2013). Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37(9 Pt B), 2268–2295. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voelcker-Rehage, C., and Wiertz, O. (2003). Die Lernfähigkeit sportmotorischer Fertigkeiten im Lichte der Entwicklungspsychologie der Lebensspanne. Germany: Universität Bielefeld.

Yund, E. W. (2003). Simultaneous auditory thresholds at multiple frequencies and for both ears. Arch. Neurobehav. Exp. Stimuli 69. Available at: http://www.neurobs.com/ex_files/expt_view?id=69

Keywords: brain aging, cardiovascular fitness, hippocampus, motor fitness, physical activity

Citation: Niemann C, Godde B and Voelcker-Rehage C (2014) Not only cardiovascular, but also coordinative exercise increases hippocampal volume in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:170. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00170

Received: 19 February 2014; Accepted: 30 June 2014;
Published online: 04 August 2014.

Edited by:

Rodrigo Orlando Kuljiš, Zdrav Mozak Limitada, Chile

Reviewed by:

Yvonne Nolan, University College Cork, Ireland
P. Bryant Chase, The Florida State University, USA

Copyright © 2014 Niemann, Godde and Voelcker-Rehage. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Claudia Voelcker-Rehage, Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and Institutional Development, Jacobs University Bremen, Campus Ring 1, 28759 Bremen, Germany e-mail: c.voelcker-rehage@jacobs-university.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.