Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 08 October 2014
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 6 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00270

KIBRA gene polymorphism has no association with verbal or visual episodic memory performance

Katherine H. Franks Mathew J. Summers James C. Vickers*
  • Wicking Dementia Research and Education Centre, Faculty of Health, University of Tasmania, Hobart, TAS, Australia

Inter-individual variability in memory performance has been suggested to result, in part, from genetic differences in the coding of proteins involved in long-term potentiation (LTP). The present study examined the effect of a single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the KIBRA gene (rs17070145) on episodic memory performance, using multiple measures of verbal and visual episodic memory. A total of 256 female and 130 male healthy, older adults (mean age = 60.86 years) were recruited from the Tasmanian Healthy Brain Project (THBP), undergoing both neuropsychological and genetic testing. The current study showed no significant effect of the KIBRA polymorphism on performance on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task, Logical Memory test, Paired Associates Learning test or Rey Complex Figure Task. The results suggest there is little to no functional significance of KIBRA genotype on episodic memory performance, regardless of modality.

Introduction

The term “memory” encapsulates different systems that can be differentiated by the type of information processed. Declarative memory, one of two major distinctions of memory, involves the conscious recollection about facts (semantic memory) and events (episodic memory; Squire, 2004). Decline in memory performance, particularly episodic memory, is a major characteristic of dementia, particularly in early-middle stages. However, it is also well established that there is normal aging-associated decline of episodic memory in the absence of dementia (Mormino et al., 2009). Therefore, with aging populations, it is not only important to investigate strategies to delay dementia onset, but also to increase resistance to aging-related cognitive decline. As the heritability estimate of memory ability is approximately 50% (McClearn et al., 1997), genetic components have been investigated to determine whether they have either a protective effect on, or predict a faster decline of, normal aging-associated cognitive decline. The association of genes such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and apolipoprotein-E (ApoE) with cognitive decline and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (AD) has been investigated (e.g., Corder et al., 1993; Slooter et al., 1998; Hariri et al., 2003; Cosentino et al., 2008). Such insight assists in informing the development of strategies to increase resistance to memory decline in older adults.

It has been proposed that the KIBRA gene (also known as WWC1) may also play a role in human memory (Papassotiropoulos et al., 2006), potentially accounting for some of the individual variability in memory decline. The KIBRA single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) (rs17070145) was originally identified in a genome-wide association study (GWAS), as it met criteria of being associated with memory performance (Papassotiropoulos et al., 2006). Follow-up studies demonstrated that the C → T substitution within the ninth intron of the KIBRA gene was significantly associated with greater episodic memory performance (Papassotiropoulos et al., 2006). Further, there was no significant relationship between genotype and performance on attention, concentration and working memory tasks, suggesting that KIBRA has a specific association with episodic memory. Additionally, this study also demonstrated that T-allele carriers had significantly better visual episodic memory performance than non-carriers, suggesting the effect of KIBRA genotype is independent of stimulus modality.

The KIBRA gene has been shown to encode KIBRA protein, which is expressed in brain regions related to memory, including the hippocampus and temporal lobe in humans (Papassotiropoulos et al., 2006). Animal studies provide preliminary evidence that the gene and associated protein are implicated in cellular processes underlying memory and, thus, are related to memory performance. This is illustrated by adult KIBRA knockout mice demonstrating significant deficits in learning and memory, specifically in long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression in the hippocampus (Makuch et al., 2011). Long-term potentiation has been proposed to underlie memory formation, involving the activation of proteins, gene expression, protein synthesis and the development of synaptic connections (Lynch, 2004). There is evidence to suggest that KIBRA may be involved in LTP, as KIBRA protein interacts with proteins that play a role in altering synaptic plasticity for memory formation, such as protein kinase M zeta, dendrin and synaptopodin (Kremerskothen et al., 2003; Schneider et al., 2010).

Multiple investigations have subsequently investigated the association between KIBRA T-allele and superior verbal episodic memory performance. While many studies provide further support for this association (Almeida et al., 2008; Schaper et al., 2008; Bates et al., 2009; Preuschhof et al., 2010; Kauppi et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2013), some report no association between KIBRA genotype and verbal episodic memory performance (Need et al., 2008; Burgess et al., 2011; Wersching et al., 2011; Laukka et al., 2013; see Schneider et al., 2010 for a review). In addition to this, the KIBRA T-allele has also been reported to have a deleterious effect, whereby T-allele carriers have worse performance on verbal episodic memory tests (Nacmias et al., 2008). Despite this, the majority of results from studies investigating the effect of KIBRA genotype are consistent with the original findings. Further, studies utilizing relatively small samples (e.g., Schaper et al., 2008) still demonstrate an effect of KIBRA on episodic memory performance (Rey Auditory Learning Verbal Learning Test, RAVLT), suggesting that the effect of KIBRA on episodic memory may be of a sufficient magnitude to be detected within smaller samples.

The aim of the present study was to clarify the effect of KIBRA polymorphism on episodic memory performance of healthy aging individuals, using multiple measures of both verbal and visual episodic memory, and by examining immediate and delayed recall.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The sample in the present study consisted of 386 participants who were drawn from the Tasmanian Healthy Brain Project (THBP), a world-first, large-scale longitudinal study investigating the protective effect of later-life education on age-related cognitive decline in people aged between 50 and 79 years (Summers et al., 2013). The sample consisted of 256 females and 130 males, with a combined mean age of 60.86 (SD = 6.79). Participants were excluded from the THBP if they had a history of medical, psychological or psychiatric conditions independently associated with impairments to cognitive function (see Summers et al., 2013). All participants provided informed consent prior to commencing the study and ethics approval was obtained from the Tasmanian Social Sciences Human Research Ethic Committee (H11070).

Genotyping

Oragene DNA Self-Collection Kits (DNA Genotex, Ottawa, Canada) were used to collect saliva samples for DNA extraction, which was carried out using standard methods. KIBRA polymorphism was determined using restriction polymerase chain reaction-fragment length polymorphism (PCR-RFLP) analysis. Similarly to Wersching et al. (2011), two primers were used to select a 198 basepair (bp) fragment containing the polymorphic site rs17070145 (forward: atcctcttgaggcttcactgg, backward: actttcaacacaatgaacaagg). The total volume of amplification reactions was 12 µl, consisting of 1 µl of DNA, 0.6 µl of each primer, 6 µl of MyTaq DNA polymerase and 3.8 µl of DEPC treated water. Following the PCR program, the products were then digested in 5 U of MseI restriction enzyme for 16 h at 37°C. As there is no MseI site in the C-allele, but this is present in the T-allele, the PCR product was cut into two fragments of 128 and 70 bp in length only when the T-allele was present. Therefore, a single band at 198 bp indicated a CC genotype, whereas a band at both 128 bp and 70 bp indicated a TT genotype. Bands at all three bp locations indicated a CT genotype. Polymerase chain reaction products were then viewed using 3.5% agarose gels stained with Sybr-safe to allow visualization of DNA. All samples were genotyped twice to ensure accuracy, with 87.3% of samples in concordance. In the instance of conflicting results, the triplicate result was taken.

Neuropsychological Testing

As part of the THBP, participants underwent annual cognitive assessment requiring participants to complete a standardized battery of neuropsychological measures selected to assess core cognitive function. A qualified examiner administrated the tests in an individual assessment session as part of standard protocol of the THBP (described by Summers et al., 2013). The cognitive measures analyzed in the current study formed a part of the larger cognitive battery. Verbal episodic memory performance was assessed using the RAVLT and the Logical Memory Test (LM; a subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale—Third Edition; WMS-III). Visual episodic memory performance was assessed using the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) and the Paired Associates Learning Task (PAL; a test from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Assessment Battery, CANTAB).

The RAVLT (Strauss et al., 2006) is a serial word list-learning task in which 15 words are presented across five trials (list A), with recall assessed after each trial. Recall for an interference list of 15 words is then assessed (list B), followed by delayed recall of the initial list of 15 words (list A recall). The LM test (Strauss et al., 2006) assesses a participant’s capacity to recall two prose passages immediately after presentation and again following a 30-min delay.

The RCFT (Strauss et al., 2006) assesses participants’ capacity to recall a complex geometric design. The participant was shown a standardized design, which they were asked to reproduce as best as possible while it remained visible to them. Following this, both the stimulus figure and the participant’s copy were removed from view. After a delay of 20–30 min, the participant redrew the design from memory, without prior warning that they would be required to do so. The design was scored based on the accurate placement and reproduction of 18 specific design elements, with higher scores indicating greater performance.

The PAL test (Cambridge Cognition Ltd., 2004) was conducted on a touch-screen computer that displayed six boxes opening in a random order. One or more boxes contained a pattern, which at the end of all the boxes opening, were displayed individually in the middle of the screen. Participants then had to touch the box where the pattern was originally located. If an error was made, the patterns were re-presented to remind the participant of the location. Throughout the test, the difficulty increased by increasing the number of patterns presented.

These memory tests have established high levels of reliability and validity and have been used extensively to assess episodic memory in age-related cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment and dementia (Strauss et al., 2006).

Analysis

Differences in episodic memory performance were compared across the three KIBRA genotype groups: homozygous CC, homozygous TT and heterozygous CT. The three groups were dummy coded prior to analyses (0 = CC, 1 = CT, 2 = TT). The CT and TT genotypes were also combined for further analysis with respect to the CC group, to examine the effect of inheritance of any T variant.

Raw scores from the four tests of episodic memory were used for the analyses, as the correlations between age and raw scores were small (r = <0.3), suggesting that z-score transformations were not required. One-way ANOVAs were used to compare the mean scores on the RAVLT, LM test, PAL test and RCFT across the three groups. Assumptions of homogeneity of variance were assessed using Levene’s test and were met unless explicitly stated. Further, two 3 (Genotype: CC, CT, TT) × 5 (Trial: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5) mixed ANOVAs were conducted to assess the effect that genotype has on learning in order to determine whether an earlier stage of memory may be susceptible to genotype effects. Violations of the assumption of sphericity were adjusted using Greenhouse-Geisser corrections.

In addition, linear hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to determine whether the demographic variables of age, intelligence, previous education and gender mask the effect of genotype on episodic memory performance. This allowed the variance of such variables to be partialled out, demonstrating the underlying relationship between genotype and episodic memory performance.

Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS version 21.0.

Results

Genetic analysis demonstrated that 176 participants were genotyped as CC carriers, 180 as CT carriers and 30 as TT carriers of the KIBRA gene. The observed genotype distribution was not significantly different from the frequency distribution previously observed in an Australian sample (Almeida et al., 2008; χ2 (2, N = 386) = 2.04, p = 0.361), suggesting that the sample used in the present study is in Hardy-Weinburg equilibrium. There were no significant differences between the three genotype groups in regards to age, sex, years of previous education, premorbid intellectual capacity (Wechsler Test of Adult Reading) or current intellectual capacity (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III-SF1; see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Difference between KIBRA genotype groups on demographic variables.

Verbal Episodic Memory Performance

Performance on the RAVLT and the LM test was assessed using three measures from each test. Rey Auditory Learning Verbal Learning Test performance was assessed by the total number of words recalled from all trials, mean number of words recalled from list A and mean number of words recognized from list A. Performance on the LM test was assessed by scores of immediate recall from the first story, delayed recall from the second story and mean percent retention from the second story. No significant differences between KIBRA genotype groups on any measure of verbal episodic memory performance were present (p > 0.05; see Table 2). There was no statistical difference between T and non-T carriers in performance.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Verbal episodic memory performance across KIBRA genotype.

Analysis of learning over trials on the RAVLT (Figure 1) demonstrated there was no significant interaction between trial number and KIBRA genotype on mean number of words recalled (F (6.60,1263.45) = 0.62, p = 0.732, ηp2 = 0.003), following a Greenhouse-Geisser correction (ɛ = 0.83), see Figure 1. The ANOVA did show a significant main effect of trial (F (3.30,1263.45) = 680.24, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.640), following a Greenhouse-Geisser correction, but the main effect of genotype was not significant (F (2,383) = 1.01, p = 0.360, ηp2 = 0.005).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean number of words recalled on each trial of the RAVLT across KIBRA genotype, showing no significant interaction. Error bars display 95% confidence intervals.

Visual Episodic Memory Performance

Performance on the PAL test and the RCFT was assessed using three and two measures of memory performance, respectively. Paired Associates Learning Task test performance was assessed using mean adjusted total errors, mean trials to success and mean total errors when six shapes were presented. Performance on the RCFT was assessed using mean recall and percent retention (the ratio of what the participant recalled relative to what they copied). No significant differences between KIBRA genotype groups existed on any measure of visual episodic memory performance (p > 0.05; see Table 3). In addition, there was no statistical difference between T and non-T carriers in performance.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Visual episodic memory performance across KIBRA genotype.

Analysis of learning over trials on the PAL test (Figure 2) demonstrated that there was no significant interaction between trial number and KIBRA genotype on mean number of errors made (F (2.88,551.44) = 0.35, p = 0.784, ηp2 = 0.002), following a Greenhouse-Geisser correction (ɛ = 0.36), see Figure 2. The ANOVA did show a significant main effect of trial (F (1.44,551.44) = 128.24, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.251), following a Greenhouse-Geisser correction, but the main effect of genotype was not significant (F (2,383) = 0.01, p = 0.988, ηp2 < 0.001).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean number of total errors made across trials on the PAL test, showing no significant interaction between KIBRA genotype. Error bars display 95% confidence intervals.

Hierarchical Linear Regression

Hierarchical linear regression analyses were used to determine the underlying relationship between KIBRA genotype and episodic memory performance after the variance attributed by known demographic variables of age, gender, prior education, WTAR estimated IQ, and WAIS-III-SF1 FSIQ had been partialled out. The analyses demonstrated that KIBRA genotype, entered in the second step, resulted in a non-significant change in the amount of variance explained above that of the demographic variables for all measures of both verbal and visual episodic memory performance (p > 0.05).

Discussion

This study sought to examine the effect of KIBRA polymorphism, particularly specific genotypes, on verbal and visual episodic memory performance of healthy aging individuals. The CC genotype of the KIBRA was not associated with significantly lower performance on the RAVLT, the LM test, the PAL test and the RCFT compared to those with CT or TT genotypes. The results provide no evidence of a gene-dose relationship present for the T-allele, as there was no advantage to heterozygous T-allele carriers (CT genotype) over non T-allele carriers (CC genotype). These findings suggest that KIBRA polymorphism has little to no influence on either verbal or visual episodic memory performance. The present study incorporated multiple measures of both verbal and visual episodic memory to ensure reliable assessment and to examine different components of memory, such as immediate and delayed recall. As there was no effect of KIBRA polymorphism on either delayed or immediate recall across all tests of episodic memory, this suggests that the individual polymorphism effect does not selectively influence only one component of memory. Further, analyses of the PAL test and the RCFT learning curves suggest that KIBRA polymorphism does not have an effect on the rate of acquisition of information (learning), indicating that KIBRA polymorphism does not affect an earlier stage of memory. In addition, the linear hierarchical regression analyses demonstrated no significant contribution of KIBRA polymorphism to the variance explained in verbal or visual episodic memory performance, despite suggestions that the effect of KIBRA polymorphism might be masked by other factors (Burgess et al., 2011).

The finding that KIBRA polymorphism does not affect verbal episodic memory performance clearly contradicts the findings of the Papassotiropoulos et al. (2006) study, which demonstrated differential performance between T-allele and non T-allele carriers. Differences between the genotype groups on delayed, but not immediate recall, was also not supported by the present study. This may be a result of delayed free recall being dependent on a large-scale memory network, rather than one specific area related to memory (Dickerson and Eichenbaum, 2010). Therefore, the delayed free recall test may be relatively non-specific and involve other brain areas where the KIBRA protein is not as highly expressed and other proteins may exert some influence on episodic memory performance.

The finding that the KIBRA polymorphism does not affect visual episodic memory performance also contrasts with the results from the original study showing a significant difference between T-allele carriers and non-carriers (Papassotiropoulos et al., 2006). However, it does provide support for the results of Vassos et al. (2010) and Yasuda et al. (2010), which both demonstrated that KIBRA polymorphism does not significantly affect visual episodic memory performance. A standardized test of visual episodic memory was not used in the Papassotiropoulos et al. (2006) study and was the only study reporting an effect on visual episodic memory, perhaps as the test may have assessed more than just visual episodic memory ability.

It has been suggested that discrepancies in, or failure to replicate findings in genetic research may be due to false positives originally being reported as true (Kathiresan et al., 2004). For example, although Schaper et al. (2008) supported the original findings of Papassotiropoulos et al. (2006), the small to medium Cohen’s d effect sizes reported (0.48 and 0.74) suggest approximately 57% to 67% overlap between the groups on scores of episodic memory performance (Zakzanis, 2001), which may not be functionally significant. Further, the Papassotiropoulos et al. (2006) study reported the significant difference between groups on delayed memory as only a difference of roughly 1.2 words, indicating a small effect size on performance. This is further supported by a meta-analysis demonstrating that the KIBRA polymorphism potentially explains only 0.5% of the variance in episodic memory performance (Milnik et al., 2012). Hence, very large sample sizes may be required to uncover such minor effect size linkages between the KIBRA polymorphism and memory performance. In this regard, it may be possible that KIBRA gene variations may combine with other brain gene polymorphisms (e.g., CLSTN2, Preuschhof et al., 2010, but not seen in Sédille-Mostafaie et al., 2012; Laukka et al., 2013) to influence memory performance, albeit effect sizes are likely to remain small.

With respect to potential limitations of the current study, as the THBP is a longitudinal study that invites older individuals to attend university to increase their education, this may introduce some bias in the recruitment of participants. For example, only motivated individuals or those who completed secondary school or have previous university experience may be willing to participate, which would explain the above average levels of intelligence and premorbid IQ seen in the current sample. Consequently, this may impact on any generalizability of findings to different populations. However, the THBP sample features did not substantially differ from the previous KIBRA studies that reported prior years of education and intelligence scores (see Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparison of the sample features in the present study to other studies.

The present study has extended the findings of previous research investigating the KIBRA gene, particularly through the analysis of the genotypes rather than the combined alleles, as well as using multiple measures of episodic memory performance. The results showed no influence of KIBRA genotypes on visual and verbal episodic memory performance in healthy older adults.

Conflict of Interest Statement

Dr. M. Summers reports personal fees from Eli Lilly (Australia) Pty Ltd, grants from Novotech Pty Ltd, outside the submitted work.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge Mr Graeme McCormack, Mr David Ward and Miss Kimberley Stuart for technical and laboratory assistance. This project was funded by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Project Grant and also by the JO and JR Wicking Trust (ANZ Trustees).

References

Almeida, O. P., Schwab, S. G., Lautenschlager, N. T., Morar, B., Greenop, K. R., Flicker, L., et al. (2008). KIBRA genetic polymorphism influences episodic memory in later life, but does not increase the risk of mild cognitive impairment. J. Cell. Mol. Med. 12, 1672–1676. doi: 10.1111/j.1582-4934.2008.00229.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, T. C., Price, J. F., Harris, S. E., Marioni, R. E., Fowkes, F. G., Stewart, M. C., et al. (2009). Association of KIBRA and memory. Neurosci. Lett. 458, 140–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.04.050

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, J. D., Pedraza, O., Graff-Radford, N. R., Hirpa, M., Zou, F., Miles, R., et al. (2011). Association of common KIBRA variants with episodic memory and AD risk. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 557.e1–557.e9. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.11.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cambridge Cognition. (2004). Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Assessment Battery. Cambridge: Cambridge Cognition Ltd.

Corder, E. H., Saunders, A. M., Strittmatter, W. J., Schmechel, D. E., Gaskell, P. C., Small, G. W., et al. (1993). Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science 261, 921–923. doi: 10.1126/science.8346443

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cosentino, S., Scarmeas, N., Helzner, E., Glymour, M. M., Brandt, J., Albert, M., et al. (2008). APOE epsilon 4 allele predicts faster cognitive decline in mild Alzheimer disease. Neurology 70, 1842–1849. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000304038.37421.cc

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickerson, B. C., and Eichenbaum, H. (2010). The episodic memory system: neurocircuitry and disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 86–104. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.126

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hariri, A. R., Goldberg, T. E., Mattay, V. S., Kolachana, B. S., Callicott, J. H., Egan, M. F., et al. (2003). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism affects human memory-related hippocampal activity and predicts memory performance. J. Neurosci. 23, 6690–6694. doi: 0270-6474/03/236690-05

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kathiresan, S., Newton-Cheh, C., and Gerszten, R. E. (2004). On the interpretation of genetic association studies. Eur. Heart J. 25, 1378–1381. doi: 10.1016/j.ehj.2004.06.035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kauppi, K., Nilsson, L. G., Adolfsson, R., Eriksson, E., and Nyberg, L. (2011). KIBRA polymorphism is related to enhanced memory and elevated hippocampal processing. J. Neurosci. 31, 14218–14222. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3292-11.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kremerskothen, J., Plaas, C., Büther, K., Finger, I., Veltel, S., Matanis, T., et al. (2003). Charatierization of KIBRA, a novel WW domain-containing protein. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 300, 862–867. doi: 10.1016/s0006-291x(02)02945-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laukka, E. J., Lövdén, M., Herlitz, A., Karlsson, S., Ferencz, B., Pantzar, A., et al. (2013). Genetic effetcs on old-age cognitive functioning: a population-based study. Psychol. Aging 28, 262–274. doi: 10.1037/a0030829

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch, M. A. (2004). Long-term potentiation and memory. Physiol. Rev. 84, 87–136. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00014.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makuch, L., Volk, L., Anggono, V., Johnson, R. C., Yu, Y., Duning, K., et al. (2011). Regulation of AMPA receptor function by the human memory-associated gene KIBRA. Neuron 71, 1022–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClearn, G. E., Johansson, B., Berg, S., Pedersen, N. L., Ahern, F., Petrill, S. A., et al. (1997). Substantial genetic influence on cognitive abilities in twins 80 or more years old. Science 276, 1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5318.1560

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milnik, A., Heck, A., Vogler, C., Heinze, H. J., de Quervain, D. J. F., and Papassotiropoulos, A. (2012). Association of KIBRA with episodic and working memory: a meta-analysis. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 159, 958–969. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mormino, E. C., Kluth, J. T., Madison, C. M., Rabinovici, G. D., Baker, S. L., Miller, B. L., et al. (2009). Episodic memory loss is related to hippocampal-mediated beta-amyloid deposition in elderly subjects. Brain 132, 1310–1323. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn320

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nacmias, B., Bessi, V., Bagnoli, S., Tedde, A., Cellini, E., Piccini, C., et al. (2008). KIBRA gene variants are associated with episodic memory performance in subjective memory complaints. Neurosci. Lett. 436, 145–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.03.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Need, A. C., Attix, D. K., McEvoy, J. M., Cirulli, E. T., Linney, K. N., Wagoner, A. P., et al. (2008). Failure to replicate effect of Kibra on human memory in two large cohorts of European origin. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 147, 667–668. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30658

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papassotiropoulos, A., Stephan, D. A., Huentelman, M. J., Hoerndli, F. J., Craig, D. W., Pearson, J. V., et al. (2006). Common Kibra alleles are associated with human memory performance. Science 314, 475–478. doi: 10.1126/science.1129837

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preuschhof, C., Heekeren, H. R., Li, S. C., Sander, T., Lindenberger, U., and Bäckman, L. (2010). KIBRA and CLSTN2 polymorphisms exert interactive effects on human episodic memory. Neuropsychologia 48, 402–408. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaper, K., Kolsch, H., Popp, J., Wagner, M., and Jessen, F. (2008). KIBRA gene variants are associated with episodic memory in healthy elderly. Neurobiol. Aging 29, 1123–1125. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.02.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, A., Huentelman, M. J., Kremerskothen, J., Duning, K., Spoelgen, R., and Nikolich, K. (2010). KIBRA: a new gateway to learning and memory? Front. Aging Neurosci. 2:4. doi: 10.3389/neuro.24.004.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sédille-Mostafaie, N., Sebesta, C., Huber, K. R., Zehetmayer, S., Jungwirth, S., Tragl, K. H., et al. (2012). The role of memory-related gene polymorphisms, KIBRA and CLSTN2, on replicate memory assessment in the elderly. J. Neural Transm. 119, 77–80. doi: 10.1007/s00702-011-0667-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slooter, A. J., Cruts, M., Kalmijn, S., Hofman, A., Breteler, M. M., Van Broeckhoven, C., et al. (1998). Risk estimates of dementia by apolipoprotein E genotypes from a population-based incidence study: the Rotterdam study. Arch. Neurol. 55, 964–968. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.7.964

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 82, 171–177. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Summers, M. J., Saunders, N. L. J., Valenzuela, M. J., Summers, J. J., Ritchie, K., Robinson, A., et al. (2013). The Tasmanian Healthy Brain Project (THBP): a prospective longitudinal examination of the effect of university-level education in older adults in preventing age-related cognitive decline and reducing the risk of dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr. 25, 1145–1155. doi: 10.1017/s1041610213000380

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vassos, E., Bramon, E., Picchioni, M., Walshe, M., Filbey, F. M., Kravariti, E., et al. (2010). Evidence of association of KIBRA genotype with episodic memory in families of psychotic patients and controls. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44, 795–798. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.01.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D., Liu, B., Qin, W., Wang, J., Zhang, Y., Jiang, T., et al. (2013). KIBRA gene variants are associated with synchronization within the default-mode and executive control networks. Neuroimage 69, 213–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wersching, H., Guske, K., Hasenkamp, S., Hagedorn, C., Schiwek, S., Jansen, S., et al. (2011). Impact of common KIBRA allele on human cognitive functions. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 1296–1304. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.16

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yasuda, Y., Hashimoto, R., Ohi, K., Fukumoto, M., Takamura, H., Iike, N., et al. (2010). Association study of KIBRA gene with memory performance in a Japanese population. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 11, 852–857. doi: 10.3109/15622971003797258

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakzanis, K. K. (2001). Statistics to tell the truth, the whole truth and nothing but the truth: formulae, illustrative numerical examples and heuristic interpretation of effect size analyses for neuropsychological researchers. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 16, 653–667. doi: 10.1093/arclin/16.7.653

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: KIBRA, WWC1, episodic memory, aging, single-nucleotide polymorphism

Citation: Franks KH, Summers MJ and Vickers JC (2014) KIBRA gene polymorphism has no association with verbal or visual episodic memory performance. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:270. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00270

Received: 23 February 2014; Accepted: 19 September 2014;
Published online: 08 October 2014.

Edited by:

Rodrigo Orlando Kuljiš, Zdrav Mozak Limitada, Chile

Reviewed by:

Ashok Kumar, University of Florida, USA
Beata Ferencz, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden

Copyright © 2014 Franks, Summers and Vickers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: James C. Vickers, Wicking Dementia Research and Education Centre, Faculty of Health, University of Tasmania, Medical Sciences Precinct, Bag 34, Hobart, TAS 7000, Australia e-mail: James.Vickers@utas.edu.au

Download