Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 23 June 2015
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 7 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00118

The disengagement of visual attention in Alzheimer's disease: a longitudinal eye-tracking study

Trevor J. Crawford* Alex Devereaux Steve Higham Claire Kelly
  • Dementia Research and Eye Tracking Lab, Department of Psychology, Centre of Aging Research, Centre for Learning and Human Development, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK

Introduction: Eye tracking provides a convenient and promising biological marker of cognitive impairment in patients with neurodegenerative disease. Here we report a longitudinal study of saccadic eye movements in a sample of patients with Alzheimer's disease and elderly control participants who were assessed at the start of the study and followed up 12-months later.

Methods: Eye movements were measured in the standard gap and overlap paradigms, to examine the longitudinal trends in the ability to disengage attention from a visual target.

Results: Overall patients with Alzheimer's disease had slower reaction times than the control group. However, after 12-months, both groups showed faster and comparable reductions in reaction times to the gap, compared to the overlap stimulus. Interestingly, there was a general improvement for both groups with more accurately directed saccades and speeding of reaction times after 12-months.

Conclusions: These findings point to the value of longer-term studies and follow-up assessment to ascertain the effects of dementia on oculomotor control.

Introduction

Alzheimer's Disease (AD) is a major cognitive disorder of older adults that blights the lives of millions of people and their families across the globe (Stokes, 2013). Many sufferers are undiagnosed due to the lengthy clinical and psychometric procedures that are often used by local and national health services. Eye tracking provides a convenient and promising biological marker of cognitive impairment in patients with neurodegenerative disease (Crawford et al., 2013), and is likely to enhance the current procedures for early diagnosis and long-term monitoring of disease progression. Comprehensive studies on the profile of eye movement control in dementia are essential in order to fully exploit its full potential.

Clinicians and researchers have tended to focus on the problems in memory retrieval, which may occur relatively late in the evolution of AD. However, there is increasing evidence that there are subtle impairments in visual attention and other cognitive domains that occur early in the course of the disease. Several studies have reported a dysfunction in the disengagement of attention in AD (Della Sala et al., 1992; Parasuraman et al., 1992; Parasuraman and Haxby, 1993; Scinto et al., 1994; Perry and Hodges, 1999; Baddeley et al., 2001; Solfrizzi et al., 2002; Tales et al., 2002) which appears to coincide with the progressive decline in working memory and executive function (Awh and Jonides, 1998; Parasuraman and Greenwood, 1998). Parasuraman et al. (1992) reported that AD patients, of mild severity, displayed an attention-shifting or disengagement deficit in a letter-discrimination task, in a similar way to patients with hemi-neglect that was caused by a lesion in the parietal lobe (Posner et al., 1984). In AD, the speeding-up of response times to a “valid” cue (i.e., a cue that signaled the correct location of the target), did not differ from healthy controls. In contrast, the slowing-down of response times following an “invalid” cue (i.e., a cue that signaled an incorrect location of the target), was abnormally high, compared to healthy controls. This implied that attention to spatial locations was preserved in early AD, whereas the ability to disengage (or “unplug”) attention was impaired. Using PET Parasuraman et al. (1992) also reported that the degrees of disengagement deficit was correlated with the level of hypo-metabolism in the superior parietal lobe.

The process of disengagement has also been widely investigated using the gap and overlap saccadic paradigms (Saslow, 1967; Fischer and Boch, 1983; Kalesnykas and Hallett, 1987; Braun and Breitmeyer, 1988; Fischer and Weber, 1993). These paradigms have also been explored in newborns (Farroni et al., 1999) and non-human species (Kano et al., 2011). In a traditional “Gap” paradigm (Figure 1) the onset of the peripheral target is preceded by a short period (usually 200 ms), when the current fixation point is switched-off, leaving a brief “gap” between the offset of the fixation point and the onset of the target for the saccade. In the “overlap” paradigm, the fixation point remains on for a period of time, when the peripheral target is presented. The gap paradigm facilitates the disengagement of attention from the fixation point and therefore yields faster saccadic response times, since there is no other visual stimulus to compete with the target. The overlap slows the disengagement of attention since the persistence of the fixation point continues to capture attention whilst the target is presented (see Figure 2, based on Crawford et al., 2013). The ability to “unplug” visual attention can be readily estimated by measuring the time it takes for the eye to begin a saccadic eye movement towards the gap stimulus, in relation to the overlap paradigm. According to one widely supported scheme (Findlay and Walker, 1999), the gap manipulation helps to resolve the competition that occurs between two mutually exclusive activities: the eye fixation and saccade initiation systems. One important physiological arena for this competition is the superior colliculus (Munoz and Wurtz, 1993a,b; Dorris and Munoz, 1995; Dorris et al., 1997). Top-down saccadic eye movement signals are also controlled by various cortical areas including the parietal and frontal cortex (Dias and Bruce, 1994; Hanes and Schall, 1996; Müri et al., 1998; Munoz and Everling, 2004).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. An illustration of the “gap” and overlap paradigms. In the gap task the fixation point is withdrawn prior to the presentation of the peripheral target. In the Overlap paradigm the fixation remains on at the onset of the target.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Histograms showing that there is a shift in the distribution of saccade latencies in the overlap stimuli in comparison to the gap stimuli in various patient groups, Alzheimer's Disease, Parkinson's disease, Older and young controls.

Yang et al. (2013) found that the “gap” effect was increased in patients with mild cognitive impairment and mild to moderate Alzheimer's disease, suggesting a potential biological marker for AD. This implies that they have difficulty in unplugging, or transferring attention away from events. However, no study has yet examined the longitudinal effects of the disease on eye movements in these paradigms. Therefore, we examined the hypothesis that the magnitude of the “gap” effect will increase over time, due to a deterioration in the ability to disengage attention. An alternative outcome is that the magnitude of the “gap” effect will not increase, but that there will be a proportional change in saccade latency for both the gap and overlap paradigms. This would result in a “gap” effect that remains relatively stable across the disease, showing that this effect changes with age, rather than the disease (Crawford et al., 2013).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Elderly control participants were volunteers from the local community (N = 25; age range = 62–80 years; mean = 70.6; SD = 4.9; male n = 8; female n = 17; see Table 1). The AD group comprised mild-to-moderate patients with Alzheimer's disease recruited from the Dementia Research Project at Lytham Hospital Memory Clinic, United Kingdom. All patients fulfilled the criteria for probable Alzheimer's disease according to the American Psychiatric Association's DSM IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke (NINCDS) (N = 11; age range = 71–88; mean = 78; SD = 4.94; male n = 6; female n = 5, see Table 2). Three patients from the original sample with a diagnosis of vascular or mixed dementia were not included in these analyses. All patients underwent a detailed clinical history, physical/neurological examination and routine investigations: hemoglobin, full blood count, erythrocyte sedimentation rate, urea and electrolytes, liver function tests, blood glucose, thyroid function tests, serum vitamin B12, and folate, serology for syphilis and urinalysis. Cognitive impairment was assessed with the Standardized Mini Mental State Examination (SMMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975; Molloy et al., 1991) and the cognitive sub-scale of the Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale (European version; EADAS-cog); (Rosen et al., 1984; Dahalke et al., 1992). Dementia severity was conducted at baseline using the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR) (Hughes et al., 1982). All participants underwent a detailed neuropsychological assessment (see Crawford et al., 2013), [National Adult Reading Test (NART) (Nelson, 1982), Verbal Fluency (Storandt et al., 1984), Trail Making Form A and B (Reitan, 1958), Digit Span from Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III (Wechsler, 1997a), Spatial Span from Wechsler Memory Scale III (Wechsler, 1997b) and the Gibson Spiral Maze (Pattie and Gilleard, 1987)]. All participants were right-handed, with normal or corrected Snellen chart visual acuity. No participant demonstrated visual neglect on the line bisection task (Schenkenberg et al., 1980). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after a detailed description of the study, which was approved by the Blackpool, Wyre, and Fylde Local Research Ethics Committee.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and cognitive assessment at baseline and at the 12-month (12) follow-up in the control group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographics and cognitive assessment at baseline and at the 12-month (12) follow-up in the Alzheimer group.

Assessment of Saccadic Eye Movements

Saccadic eye movements were recorded using an infra-red scleral reflection eye-tracker “ExpressEye” (Optom, Freiburg, Germany). The eyes were recorded monocularly with a temporal resolution of 1 ms and spatial resolution of 0.1°. The system is linear over a 15° visual field. The stimulus display presented a central fixation point within an unfilled 0.75 × 0.75° empty square marker; the target was a red 0.4° spot, which was projected left and right horizontally. The device projected these stimuli from a head-mounted laser onto a white tangential screen at 57 cm. The laser output was 0.2 mW, with a wavelength of 635 nm with a luminance of 66.37 cd/m2. The three head-mounted lasers provided a simple way to compensate for lateral head motion.

Gap prosaccade paradigm

Each trial commenced with the central fixation point (see Figure 1) that was presented within a central square marker for 1000 ms. The central square remained visible throughout all of the trials and provided a useful reference point for the stabilization of the head. The fixation point was then removed for a period of 200 ms (i.e., “gap”) before the saccade target was presented at ±4° (randomized). The target was switched off for an interval of 1200 ms when only the central square was visible. The central fixation point was then presented at the start of the next trial.

Overlap prosaccade paradigm

An identical stimulus was used in the overlap display, where the procedure was similar to the gap display apart from the timing of the removal of the central fixation point. Here, the fixation point “overlapped” for 200 ms with the presentation of the pro-saccade target, whereas the fixation point was removed before the target was presented in the gap task described previously.

GO/NO–GO paradigm

At the start of each trial a central fixation light was illuminated for 1000 ms. This central light was then switched off, followed by a 200 ms “GAP.” At the termination of the “GAP” period a target was presented at ±4° for 700 ms, while the central fixation point remained off. The next trial commenced after an interval of 1200 ms during which only the central square was presented. Three versions of this paradigm examined the ability to maintain central attention and to ignore a target that was presented in the left, right or both visual fields. (A) NO-GO: Participants were instructed to ignore the target light and to maintain fixation at the center of the screen for the duration of the trial. (B) GO-RIGHT/NO-GO-LEFT: Participants were instructed to “look” at the targets that were presented in the right visual field, but to suppress eye movements to all targets in the left field. (C) GO-LEFT/NO-GO-RIGHT: Participants were required to “look” at the targets that were presented in the left field but to suppress eye movements to all targets in the right field. Inhibitory control was assessed at the baseline assessment in a subset of patients and controls.

Measurement of Saccadic Parameters

The start and end of a saccade was initially detected at the point at which the eye velocity crossed 30°/s threshold. These measurements include: the amplitudes and reaction times of the primary saccade that was generated toward or away from the target, and proportion of correctly directed saccades toward the target. Tests for homogeneity of variance were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 21.

Results

Saccade Reaction Times

The present work investigated whether or not the magnitude of the reflexive “gap” effect changes longitudinally in patients with AD. AD were slower to generate a saccadic eye movement towards the target in comparison to the control group (mean = 226 ms, SE = 4.97); AD group {mean = 249 ms, SE = 7.4; [F(1, 35) = 6.43, p = 0.016]}. There was also a significant effect of test session [F(1, 35) = 4.77, p = 0.036] with a general reduction of mean saccadic reaction times (RTs) at the 12-month session (see Figure 3), but with no interaction with group [F(1, 38) = 0.739, ns]. The size of the “gap” effect was measured as the difference between the overlap and gap RTs. The “gap” effect was highly significant [F(1, 35) = 86.45, p = 0.001]; overall both groups revealed slower reaction times in the overlap, compared to the gap task. Both groups benefited from the gap effect: At the baseline assessment the control group “gap” effect = 46 ms; whilst the “gap” effect for the AD patients = 48 ms; At 12-months the control group revealed a “gap” effect = 54 ms and the dementia patients showed a “gap” effect of 65 ms. There was no overall group difference in the magnitude of the “gap” effect [F(1, 35) = 0.37, ns] and no change in the effect at 12-months [F(1, 35) = 2.7, ns]. Clearly, the “gap” effect is relatively well preserved in patients with AD. Indeed, Yang et al. (2013) reported a large “gap” effect for AD patients (115 ms) and for the control group (88 ms). However, it is not clear whether the “gap” effect increases with age, disease progression or both (see Crawford et al., 2013).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A,B) Figure shows the mean saccadic reaction times (in ms, with standard error bars) to the gap and overlap stimuli at baseline and 12-months for AD patients (A) and elderly controls (B).

Saccadic Amplitudes (Degrees)

There was a significant effect of the stimulus gap on saccadic amplitudes for both groups [F(1, 34) = 8.3, p = 0.007]. Saccades were generally of larger amplitude and more accurate to the overlap stimulus (see Figure 4). There was no significant effect of group on the mean amplitude of the prosaccades [F(1, 37) = 1.008, ns]; no effect of test session [F(1, 37) = 2.48, ns], and no interaction between session and group status [F(1, 34) = 0.049, ns]. In comparison to other eye movement features (see Crawford et al., 2005, 2013) the ability to modulate the amplitude of a saccade is clearly preserved well into the course of the disease.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A,B) Figure shows the mean saccadic amplitudes (in degrees, with standard error bars) to the gap and overlap stimuli at baseline and 12-months for AD patients (A) and elderly controls (B).

Saccadic Direction (%correct)

Saccadic direction to the gap and overlap targets revealed a marginal trend of a group effect [F(1, 35) = 3.7, p = 0.062]. Figure 5 shows a high proportion of correctly directed saccades for the AD and control groups (overall mean = 86%, SE = 2.1; control mean = 91%, SE = 1.44). Patients did not have great difficulty aiming their eyes in the correct direction and were correct on most trials. There was a strong effect of test session [F(1, 35) = 18.5, p = 0.001] with a general improvement in accuracy in both groups after 12-months. There was a strong effect of the stimulus gap [F(1, 35) = 20.72, p = 0.001] for both patients and controls revealing more correctly directed eye movements towards the overlap target (Figure 5), but with no interaction with participant group or test session.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A,B) Figure shows the mean frequency of the correctly directed saccades (with standard error bars) to the gap and overlap stimuli at baseline and 12-months for AD patients (A) and elderly controls (B).

Z-score plots

From the perspective of clinical diagnosis it is important to supplement eye-tracking analyses that are based on group data with evidence from individual cases. Therefore, we examined the data for AD patients who were tested across the full range of oculomotor paradigms. Each saccadic parameter is expressed as a z-score (z=(x¯x)SD) with reference to the equivalent mean score for the control group. Figure 6 shows the chart of z-scores across the cognitive assessments, together with the eye-tracking z-scores. Unsurprisingly, AD patients revealed high z-scores for the EADAS Cog and MMSE cognitive assessments, which helped to inform the clinical diagnosis. Z-scores across the battery of neuropsychological measures were typically within ±1 standard deviation of the control group (0-score reference line). For the eye-tracking assessments the patients varied in their profile of z-scores across the saccadic features. ADs differed from the controls by 2 standard deviations or more for at least one of the oculomotor parameters. This implies that oculomotor assessment in AD will benefit from a comprehensive range of oculomotor tests with reference to a standardized z-score profile, as an alternative to the more common approach that employs limited measures from a specific oculomotor paradigm.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Figure shows the chart of z-scores across cognitive assessments, together with eye-tracking z-scores. The charts represent standardized (standard deviation) scores with reference mean of the control group (0-line). (A,C,E,G,I,K) Show the z-scores (with reference to the control group) from the various measures in the eye-tracking tests for six mild-moderate AD patients. (B,D,F,H,J,L) Show the equivalent z-scores for these AD patients using our test battery of traditional cognitive assessments. The traditional cognitive tests are generally close to the 0-line, and are relatively flat in comparison to the more sensitive measures of eye-tracking. VF “F,” Verbal fluency–letter F; VF “P,” Verbal fluency–letter P; EADAS Rec, Recall subtest; EADAS Recog, Recognition subtest; Nart predicted FSIQ, NART full scale IQ; Trails A and B, Trail making mean (A and B); DS, Digit Span; SS, Spatial span; NG, No-Go task; GLNR, Go Left, No Go Right in the Go-No-Go task; GRNL, Go Right, No Go Left in the Go-No-Go task; R, right; L, Left; NG-Err, frequency of direction errors in direction of the target; Go-L corr, frequency of correct saccades toward the target in Go-No task; Amp, saccade amplitude; Lat, saccade latency; Err, saccade direction errors.

Cognitive Assessment (MMSE and EADAS); Years of Education and Age

A substantive level of cognitive impairment was revealed at the baseline assessment by the lower scores on the MMSE [F(1, 35) = 50.54, p < 0.01] and the higher EADAS scores [F(1, 35) = 46.94, p < 0.01] in the AD group. This cognitive impairment was also evident at the 12-month follow-up assessment [MMSE F(1,35) = 29.38, p < 0.01; EADAS F(1, 35) = 47.83, p < 0.01]. A strong correlation was revealed between the cognitive scores at baseline and at follow-up for the AD group (MMSE, r = 0.698, p = 0.017; EADAS Cog, r = 0.942, p = 0.001). At the 12-month follow-up the AD group revealed a decline in spatial memory [t(10) = 2.472, p = 0.03] from the baseline assessment. There was no reliable change in the AD scores for MMSE [t(10) = 0.986, ns], EADAS [t(10) = −0.666, ns]. Similarly, NART errors, digit span, Trails A and B, verbal fluency, Gibson maze–all ns) all showed no change at the 12-month follow-up. Importantly, the groups were matched on years of education [F(1, 35) = 2.005, ns]. The mean age of the control group (70.6 years), was lower than the AD group (78 years). However, there was no correlation of saccadic reaction times with age at baseline (r = 0.119, p = 0.55 ns) nor at the 12-month follow-up (r = 0.134, p = 0.397 ns). Thus, age did not predict saccadic reaction times within this cohort. To further confirm that neither age nor years of education were a predictor of performance, a further analysis of variance was conducted on a subgroup of AD patients and controls matched on mean age and years of education. The results confirmed clear effects of test session and saccade task (faster reaction times for the gap task for both groups).

Discussion

There have been relatively few longitudinal investigations of eye movements in dementia. To our knowledge there have been only two such studies (Hutton et al., 1984; Bylsma et al., 1995). Here we report a longitudinal study with assessments at baseline and a 12-month follow-up. Yang et al. (2013) reported a larger “gap” effect in patients with AD in comparison to the controls. These longitudinal data revealed that this robust “gap” effect in AD, was maintained at a 12-month follow-up. Crawford et al. (2013) reported that the “gap” effect was larger in older groups compared to younger participants, which suggests that the “gap” effect may provide a marker of aging, rather than neurodegenerative disease. Surprisingly, the AD reaction times were selectively decreased in the gap task over the course of the assessments. It is remarkable that both groups revealed a similar change in reaction times and saccade direction at the 12-month follow-up session. Why could this be? A number of non-specific factors are likely to have contributed to the overall improvement. Saccadic eye movements toward a target (i.e., prosaccades) are a low, level visuomotor behavior that we produce hundreds of times every day. It should not be surprising that this everyday activity can improve in a laboratory setting. Initial test anxiety, is likely to be reduced with increased task familiarity and less distractibility in the research environment. What is intriguing is that these factors may also be relevant to people with mild dementia. Although speculative, it may be possible that eye-tracking paradigms can tap into a reserved capacity to respond to implicit cues that has been previously reported in patients with dementia (Gabrieli et al., 1993; Verfaellie et al., 2000; Ballesteros and Reales, 2004; Debra and Fleischman, 2007). Future work will explore this further to determine whether implicit learning is corroborated with converging evidence from additional sources.

These findings revealed that the prosaccade tasks were able to discriminate AD patients from healthy controls at baseline assessment. Z-score eye-tracking charts revealed large standardized deviations at the level of individuals with reference to the data from the control group. However, the ability to detect AD is distinct from the challenge of monitoring the progressive changes in the disease. The current evidence suggests that prosaccades, as a tool to examine attentional disengagement, may help to meet the former, but not the latter challenge. To fully explore eye movements as a tool for monitoring the progressive brain changes in AD, will require additional paradigms, that can incorporate cognitive operations, such as spatial memory (see Crawford et al., 2013). Given that spatial memory was the principal measure of decline at follow-up, it is not surprising that there was little decline in prosaccades. In contrast to other saccade tasks (e.g., antisaccades) prosaccades do not place a high cognitive load on spatial memory.

The “Gap” Effect and the Brain

Saccadic eye movements are generated by the reciprocal activation of saccade-related neurons and the inhibition of fixation neurons in the superior colliculus (Munoz and Wurtz, 1993a,b; Dorris and Munoz, 1995; Dorris et al., 1997). According to the Findlay and Walker (1999) model, this process is enhanced in the “gap” task. Switching off the fixation point reduces the activation of the fixation cells (i.e., the equivalent of releasing the brakes in a car), and releases the movement cells from inhibition. By reducing the activity of the fixation cells, the offset of the fixation point, therefore shortens the reaction time of the saccadic eye movement (Dorris and Munoz, 1995). Conversely, when the fixation point remains on (as in the overlap condition), disengagement will be delayed, due to the activation of the fixation cells and the inhibition of the movement cells.

Event related potential (ERP) studies have revealed that a number of neural correlates contribute to the gap effect, including preparatory neural activity in prefrontal cortex, increased visual cortical activity on gap trials and increased parietal activity in the overlap condition (Csibra et al., 1997). Kawakubu et al. (2002) discovered that target-locked ERPs in the gap task facilitated attentional disengagement, some 60 ms prior to the onset of the target stimulus. Saccade-locked ERPs revealed that pre-saccadic activity was greater in the overlap condition, compared to the gap condition. FEF appears to be critically involved in the generation of volitional saccades and in the disengagement of fixation (Rivaud et al., 1994; Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2004). In the saccade overlap task, saccadic latencies were increased in patients with a lesion that incorporated FEF (Rivaud et al., 1994). However, in the gap paradigm saccadic latency was unaffected in these patients (Pierrot-Deseilligny, 1991; Rivaud et al., 1994). Patients with lesions of the PPC showed a bilateral increase of the latencies of saccades in the gap task (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1987, 1991), while latency is even more prolonged in the overlap task (Walker and Findlay, 1996). The selective effects in the overlap task indicate that the FEF and PPC play an important role in the disengagement of fixation.

Conclusions

Previous work (e.g., Currie et al., 1991; Abel et al., 2002; Shafiq-Antonacci et al., 2003; Garbutt et al., 2008; Kaufman et al., 2010, 2012; Yang et al., 2013) has highlighted prosaccades as a useful biological marker for dementia. The z-score charts here, revealed that some features of prosaccades are indeed, impaired in people with AD. However, previous research had not investigated whether prosaccades change over the course of the disease. The current findings revealed that prosaccades do not necessarily deteriorate over a 12-month period in AD. People with AD vary in the specific feature of their eye-tracking impairment. We suggest that future work should not focus on a single abnormality of saccadic eye movement, but should capture a profile/pattern of abnormalities across a range of measures, both within and across the neurodegenerative diseases. We believe that eye-tracking assessments in AD will benefit from the measurement of a wide range of oculomotor parameters.

Eye tracking provides a useful methodology for monitoring changes in cognition (Shafiq-Antonacci et al., 2003; Kaufman et al., 2010, 2012; Garbutt et al., 2008). Previous work by our group and others suggest that this approach has a number of advantages over traditional methods of psychological assessment. Standard and novel experimental paradigms can be developed to evaluate theories of cognitive impairment and to dissociate between various neural networks. A careful selection of the oculomotor paradigms (which may be combined with brain imaging), have opened up opportunities for gaining new insights into the complexities of the cognitive changes in AD. Saccadic eye movements are a quantifiable and non-invasive measure of brain function that is used to identify the affected neural networks in several neuropsychiatric diseases (Broerse et al., 2001). Crawford et al. (2013) examined the saccadic eye movements of patients with Alzheimer's Disease (AD), and found abnormalities of inhibitory control that were clearly distinct from the effects in Parkinson's disease and age-matched controls. Longitudinal studies are essential to evaluate the viability of eye-tracking as a tool for monitoring the cognitive changes in the progression of dementia.

Author Contributions

TC designed the experiments; SH recruited and tested the patients and controls; SH, AD, CK analysed the eye-tracking and psychometric data; TC, AD, CK edited the manuscript. TC wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to T Renvoice, J Patel and the Local volunteers from the Lytham community for their contribution to this work. Funding Sources: SH was funded by The Lytham League of Friends. AD is funded by The Sir John Fisher Foundation. The writing up of this work (TC) was funded by the EPSRC Research council grant: EP/M006255/1.

References

Abel, L. A., Unverzagt, F., and Yee, R. D. (2002). Effects of stimulus predictability and interstimulus gap on saccades in Alzheimer's disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 13, 235–243. doi: 10.1159/000057702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Awh, E., and Jonides, J. (1998). “Spatial Working Memory and Spatial Selective Attention,” in The Attentive Brain, ed R. Parasuraman (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 353–380.

Baddeley, A., Baddeley, H. A., Bucks, R. S., and Wilcock, G. K. (2001). Attentional control in Alzheimer's disease. Brain 124, 1492–1508. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.8.1492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., and Reales, J. M. (2004). Intact haptic priming in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease: evidence for dissociable memory systems. Neuropsychologia 42, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, D., and Breitmeyer, B. G. (1988). Relationship between directed visual attention and saccadic reaction times. Exp. Brain Res. 73, 546–552. doi: 10.1007/BF00406613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broerse, A., Crawford, T. J., and Boer, J. A. (2001). Parsing of cognition in Schizophrenia using saccadic eye movement paradigms: a selective overview. Neuropsychologia 39, 742–756. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00155-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bylsma, F. W., Rasmusson, D. X., Rebok, G. W., Keyl, P. M., Tune, L., and Brandt, J. (1995). Changes in visual fixation and saccadic eye movements in Alzheimer's disease. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 19, 33–40. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(94)00060-R

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, T. J., Higham, S., Mayes, J., Shaunak, S., and Lekwuwa, G. (2013). The role of working memory and attentional engagement in inhibitory control: effects of aging and Alzheimer's disease. Age 35, 1637–1650. doi: 10.1007/s11357-012-9466-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, T. J., Higham, S., Renvoize, T., Patel, J., Dale, M., Suriya, A., et al. (2005). Inhibitory control of saccadic eye movements and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1052–1060. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csibra, G., Johnson, M. H., and Tucker, L. A. (1997). Attention and oculomotor control: a high-density ERP study of the gap effect. Neuropsychologia 35, 855–865. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00016-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Currie, J., Ramsden, B., McArther, C., and Maruff, P. (1991). Validation of a clinical antisaccade eye movement test in the assessment of dementia. Arch. Neurol. 48, 644–648. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1991.00530180102024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahalke, F., DeBerdt, W., Duka, T., Eich, F. X., Fischer, H. J., Hentschel, B., et al. (1992). Manual for the European Version of the Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale (Euro-ADAS). Berlin: Clinical Research Working Group from the Pharmaceutical Industry on Dementia.

Debra, A., and Fleischman, D. A. (2007). Repetition priming in aging and Alzheimer's Disease: an integrative review and future directions. Cortex 43, 889–897. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70688-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Della Sala, S., Laiacona, M., Spinnler, H., and Ubezio, C. A. (1992). A Cancellation test: its reliability in assessing attentional deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Psychol. Med. 22, 885–901. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700038460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias, E. C., and Bruce, C. J. (1994). Physiological correlate of fixation disengagement in the primate's frontal eye field. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 2532–2537.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dorris, M. C., and Munoz, D. P. (1995). A neural correlate for the ‘gap’ effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 2558–2562.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dorris, M. C., Pare, M., and Munoz, D. P. (1997). Neuronal activity in monkey superior colliculus related to the initiation of saccadic eye movements. J. Neurosci. 17, 8566–8579.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Farroni, T., Simion, F., Umilt, C., and Barba, B. D. (1999). The ‘gap’ effect in newborns. Dev. Sci. 2, 174–186. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Findlay, J. M., and Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 661–674. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99002150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, B., and Boch, R. (1983). Saccadic eye movements after extremely short reaction times in the monkey. Brain Res. 260, 21–26. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90760-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, B., and Weber, H. (1993). Express saccades and visual attention. Behav. Brain Sci. 16, 553–567. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00031575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M., Folstein, S., and McHugh, P. (1975). Mini-mental state examination. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabrieli, J. D. E., Corkin, S., Mickel, S. F., and Growdon, J. H. (1993). Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer's disease and in global amnesia. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 899–910. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.6.899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garbutt, S., Matlin, A., Hellmuth, J., Schenk, A. K., Johnson, J. K., Rosen, H., et al. (2008). Oculomotor function in frontotemporal lobar degeneration, related disorders and Alzheimer's disease. Brain 131, 1268–1281 doi: 10.1093/brain/awn047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanes, D. P., and Schall, J. D. (1996). Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science 274, 427–430. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5286.427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, C., Berg, L., Danziger, W., Coben, L., and Martin, R. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 140, 566–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutton, J. T., Nagel, J. A., and Loewenson, R. B. (1984). Eye tracking dysfunction in Alzheimer-type dementia. Neurology 34, 99–102. doi: 10.1212/WNL.34.1.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalesnykas, R. P., and Hallett, P. E. (1987). The differentiation of visually guided and anticipatory saccades in gap and overlap paradigms. Exp. Brain Res. 68, 115–121. doi: 10.1007/bf00255238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., and Tomonaga, M. (2011). The visual strategy specific to humans among hominids: a study using the gap–overlap paradigm. Vision Res. 51, 2348–2355. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, L. D., Pratt, J., Levine, B., and Black, S. E. (2010). Antisaccades: a probe into the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in Alzheimer's disease. A critical review. J. Alzheimers Dis. 19, 781–793. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-1275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, L. D., Pratt, J., Levine, B., and Black, S. E. (2012). Executive deficits detected in mild Alzheimer's disease using the antisaccade task. Brain Behav. 2, 15–21 doi: 10.1002/brb3.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawakubu, Y., Maekawa, H., Itoh, K., and Iwanami, A. (2002). The attentional disengagement processing reflected by ERPs and saccade reaction times during a gap task. Int. Cong. Ser. 1232, 91–95. doi: 10.1016/S0531-5131(01)00716-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molloy, D. W., Alemayehu, E., and Roberts, R. (1991). A standardized mini-mental state examination (SMMSE): its reliability compared to the traditional mini-mental state examination (MMSE). Am. J. Psychiatry 148, 102–105. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.1.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Munoz, D. P., and Everling, S. (2004). Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 218–228. doi: 10.1038/nrn1345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munoz, D. P., and Wurtz, R. H. (1993a). Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of cell discharge. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 559–575.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Munoz, D. P., and Wurtz, R. H. (1993b). Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus.II. Reversible activation and deactivation. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 576–589.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Müri, R. M., Rivaud, S., Gaymard, B., Ploner, C. J., Vermersch, A. I., Hess, C. W., et al. (1998). Role of the prefrontal cortex in the control of express saccades. A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia 37, 199–206. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00094-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, H. E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual, 2nd Edn. Windsor, ON: NFER-Nelson.

Parasuraman, R., and Greenwood, P. M. (1998). “Selective attention in aging and dementia,” in The Attentive Brain, ed R. Parasuraman (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 461–487.

Google Scholar

Parasuraman, R., Greenwood, P. M., Haxby, J. V., and Grady, C. L. (1992). Visuospatial attention in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain 115, 711–733. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.3.711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parasuraman, R., and Haxby, J. V. (1993). Attention and brain function in Alzheimer's disease: a review. Neuropsychology 7, 242–272. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.7.3.242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pattie, A., and Gilleard, C. (1987). Clifton Assessment Procedures for the Elderly (CAPE). Sevenoaks: Hodder and Stoughton Educational.

Google Scholar

Perry, R. J., and Hodges, J. R. (1999). Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer's Disease; a critical review. Brain 122, 383–404. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.3.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1991). Cortical control of saccades. Neuroophthalmology 11, 63–75. doi: 10.3109/01658109108997298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Milea, D., and Müri, R. (2004). Eye movement control by the cerebral cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 17, 17–25. doi: 10.1097/00019052-200402000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Rivaud, S., Gaymard, B., and Agid, Y. (1991). Cortical control of memory-guided saccades in man. Exp. Brain Res. 83, 607–617. doi: 10.1007/bf00229839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Rivaud, S., Penet, C., and Rigolet, M. H. (1987). Latencies of visually guided saccades in unilateral hemispheric cerebral lesions. Ann. Neurol. 21, 138–148. doi: 10.1002/ana.410210206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. A., and Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury covert orienting of attention. J. Neurosci. 4, 1863–1874.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 8, 271–276. doi: 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivaud, S., Müri, R. M., Gaymard, B., Vermersch, A. I., and Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1994). Eye movement disorders after frontal eye field lesions in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 102, 110–120. doi: 10.1007/BF00232443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, W. G., Mohs, R. C., and Davis, K. L. (1984). A new rating scale for Alzheimers disease. Am. J. Psychiatry 141, 1356–1364. doi: 10.1176/ajp.141.11.1356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 57, 1024–1029. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.57.001024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenkenberg, T., Bradford, D. C., and Ajax, E. T. (1980). Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment. Neurology 30, 509–517. doi: 10.1212/WNL.30.5.509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scinto, L. F., Daffner, K. R., Castro, L., Weintraub, S., Vavrik, M., and Mesulam, M. M. (1994). Impairment of spatially directed attention in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease as measured by eye movements. Arch. Neurol. 51, 682–688. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1994.00540190062016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shafiq-Antonacci, R., Maruff, P., Masters, C., and Currie, J. (2003). Spectrum of saccade system function in Alzheimer's disease. Arch. Neurol. 60, 1272–1278. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.9.1272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solfrizzi, V., Panza, F., Torres, F., Capurso, C., D'Intronio, A., Colaccio, A., et al. (2002). Selective attention skills in differentiating between Alzheimer's disease and normal aging. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 15, 99–109. doi: 10.1177/089198870201500209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stokes, G. (2013). And Still The Music Plays: Stories of People with Dementia. London: Hawker Publications.

Storandt, M., Botwinick, J., Danziger, W. L., Berg, L., and Hughes, C. P. (1984). Psychometric differentiation of mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch. Neurol. 41, 497–499. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1984.04050170043013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tales, A., Muir, J. L., Bayer, A., and Snowden, R. J. (2002). Spatial shifts in visual attention in normal ageing and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia 40, 2000–2012. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00057-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verfaellie, M., Keane, M. M., and Johnson, G. (2000). Preserved priming in auditory perceptual identification in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia 38, 1581–1592. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00073-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, R., and Findlay, J. M. (1996). Saccadic eye movement programming in unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia 34, 493–508. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00156-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997a). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (1997b). Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Yang, Q., Wang, T., Su, N., Xiao, S., and Kapoula, Z. (2013). Specific saccade deficits in patients with Alzheimer's disease at mild to moderate stage and in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Age 35, 1287–1298. doi: 10.1007/s11357-012-9420-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dementia, eye-tracking, Alzheimer's disease, attention, cognition

Citation: Crawford TJ, Devereaux A, Higham S and Kelly C (2015) The disengagement of visual attention in Alzheimer's disease: a longitudinal eye-tracking study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:118. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00118

Received: 16 January 2015; Accepted: 02 June 2015;
Published: 23 June 2015.

Edited by:

Philippe Robert, University of Nice Sophia Antipolis, France

Reviewed by:

Valeria Manera, University of Nice Sophia Antipolis, France
Renaud David, Nice Memory Clinic, France

Copyright © 2015 Crawford, Devereaux, Higham and Kelly. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Trevor J. Crawford, Department of Psychology, Centre of Aging Research, Centre for Learning and Human Development, Lancaster University, Fylde College, Lancaster, Lancashire LA1 4YF, UK, t.crawford@lancaster.ac.uk

Download