Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 26 October 2016
Sec. Parkinson’s Disease and Aging-related Movement Disorders
Volume 8 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00247

Dysfunction of the Default Mode Network in Drug-Naïve Parkinson’s Disease with Mild Cognitive Impairments: A Resting-State fMRI Study

Yanbing Hou1† Jing Yang1† Chunyan Luo1 Wei Song1 Ruwei Ou1 Wanglin Liu1 Qiyong Gong2* Huifang Shang1*
  • 1Department of Neurology, West China Hospital, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China
  • 2Huaxi MR Research Center, Department of Radiology, West China Hospital, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China

Objective: Cognitive impairments are common in Parkinson’s disease (PD) and can even occur in the early stages. The default mode network (DMN) is highly relevant for cognitive processes; however, it remains largely unknown if changes in the DMN connectivity are related to the cognitive decline in drug-naïve early stage PD patients with a mild cognitive impairment (MCI). This study used resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) to explore the brain connectivity of the DMN in early stage drug-naïve PD patients with MCI.

Method: We recruited 32 early stage drug-naïve PD patients and 22 matched healthy controls (HC). Among the PD patients, 14 were classified as having MCI (PD-MCI) and 18 were classified as having unimpaired cognition (PD-CU). The functional integration of the DMN was evaluated by a seed-based correlation approach.

Results: The brain connectivity analysis revealed reduced functional connectivity (FC) in both PD subgroups compared with HC. The PD-MCI group showed a significant reduction in FC between the DMN and a set of regions, including the precentral gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, insula, anterior inferior parietal lobule and middle frontal gyrus. Compared to the PD-CU group, the PD-MCI group demonstrated a significantly decreased FC in the middle frontal and middle temporal gyri. Additionally, compared to HC, the PD-MCI group had a significantly decreased FC within the DMN, mainly in the FC between the hippocampal formation and inferior frontal gyrus, between the posterior cingulate cortex and posterior inferior parietal lobule, and between the anterior temporal lobe and inferior frontal gyrus. Compared to the PD-CU group, the only significantly decreased FC within the DMN in the PD-MCI group was between the anterior temporal lobe and inferior frontal gyrus. In all PD patients, the decreased FC between anterior temporal lobe and middle temporal gyrus was positively correlated with attention/working performance, and the reduced FC between the hippocampal formation and inferior frontal gyrus was also positively correlated with memory function.

Conclusion: Our findings suggest that an altered DMN connectivity characterizes PD-MCI patients. These findings may be helpful for facilitating the further understanding of the potential mechanisms underlying MCI in PD. However, our results are preliminary, and further investigation is needed.

Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD), as a common neurodegenerative disease, is characterized by cardinal motor symptoms, including tremors, rigidity, bradykinesia and postural instability, and numerous non-motor symptoms (NMS) (Jankovic, 2008; Luo et al., 2014). Cognition impairments are prevalent NMS in PD (Williams-Gray et al., 2007) and could gradually lead to PD-associated dementia (Emre et al., 2007). Some non-demented patients exhibit significant deficits in one or more cognitive domains and are referred to as having a mild cognitive impairment (MCI), which is common in PD, even in the early stage of the disease (Aarsland et al., 2009; Poletti et al., 2011), with estimates of prevalence as high as 55% in newly diagnosed PD patients (Janvin et al., 2003). In PD patients with MCI, cognitive dysfunction does not interfere considerably with daily life, but they are at a high risk of subsequently developing dementia (Aarsland et al., 2009; Litvan et al., 2012). A better understanding of MCI in PD would facilitate the early administration of pharmacological and non-pharmacological interventions.

Convergent evidence from functional brain imaging has suggested an association between changes in DMN connectivity and neuropsychological performance (van Eimeren et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2011; Tessitore et al., 2012; Wolf et al., 2012; Amboni et al., 2015; Baggio et al., 2015). The existence of a DMN in the human brain was first confirmed via positron emission tomography (PET) and then primarily via functional MRI (fMRI). A number of studies attempted to understand the components and function of the DMN, and its interaction with other resting state networks (Wu et al., 2011). Then, the core brain regions constituting the DMN were consistently identified, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), inferior parietal cortex and hippocampal formation (HF), which were characterized by increased basal activity during rest and decreased (“deactivated”) basal activity during cognitive tasks. The most consistent observation is that the DMN is highly relevant for cognitive processes (Raichle et al., 2001). This result was further supported by recent evidence in different neurodegenerative disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD), frontotemporal dementia and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, where DMN alterations have been associated with patients’ clinical status and cognitive impairment (Zhou et al., 2010; Agosta et al., 2012; Tedeschi et al., 2012). Although the pathophysiology underlying cognitive deficits in PD remains not entirely clear, efforts should be made to explore the role of the DMN in cognitive dysfunction in PD (Buckner et al., 2008). Previous studies have found that major nodes of the DMN, such as the mPFC and PCC, demonstrated significant dysfunction in PD patients in the resting state (Gorges et al., 2015) and while performing executive tasks (van Eimeren et al., 2009). However, little is known about the changes in DMN connectivity related to cognitive decline in drug-naïve early stage PD patients with MCI. Examining the DMN as an integrative network can provide new insights into the large-scale neural communication in the brain of PD patients with MCI.

Resting-state fMRI is a cognitively unbiased approach to explore functional brain connectivity (Raichle et al., 2001). This method demands minimal patient compliance and is relatively easy to implement in clinical studies. This technique has been successfully used to detect abnormal functional integration in PD. However, previous neuroimaging experiments were conducted on patients who had been chronically exposed to anti-Parkinson medications (Amboni et al., 2015), and even performed in the ON state (Tessitore et al., 2012). Chronic exposure to anti-Parkinson medications may result in the reorganization of functional integration, which might not reflect the primary pathophysiological changes induced by PD (Buhmann et al., 2003). In addition, dopaminergic therapy has been reported to have a dose-dependent effect on the DMN integrity in PD patients, such as generating an enhanced functional connectivity (FC) of the DMN in the PCC (Krajcovicova et al., 2012). Therefore, compared with studies on patients chronically exposed to anti-Parkinson medications, studies on drug-naïve PD patients may be critical to elucidate the role of the DMN in PD with MCI.

Our goal in this study was to use resting-state fMRI to characterize connectivity changes in the DMN in early stage drug-naïve PD patients with and without MCI, compared with a cohort of normal controls, using a priori seed-based analysis. We also aimed to assess the relationship between these changes in the patterns of network connectivity and the performance of cognitive functions frequently affected in PD, that is, attention/working memory, executive, memory, language and visuospatial (VS).

Materials and Methods

Participants

The local research ethics committee approved this study, and written informed consent was obtained from all of the participants. Patients who were recruited consecutively in our cohort were first diagnosed at the Movement Disorders Outpatient Clinic of West China Hospital of Sichuan University. These patients did not take anti-Parkinson medicines (i.e., drug-naïve) since they were not diagnosed correctly before visiting us. The recruited patients fulfilled the PD Society Brain Bank diagnostic criteria (Hughes et al., 1992). From this cohort, patients were excluded if they had (1) head motion being visible to the naked eyes; (2) cerebrovascular disorders, including previous stroke, history of head injury, history of seizure, hydrocephalus, intracranial mass, previous neurological surgery and other neurologic diseases; (3) anti-Parkinson, antidepressant or neuroleptic treatment before enrollment; and (4) history of dementia. After enrollment, the patients were followed-up for at least 1 year to confirm the diagnosis of PD because patients with PD at enrollment were in the early stage. Additionally, 22 normal subjects [healthy controls (HC) group] with no history of neurologic or psychiatric diseases were recruited and matched for age, gender and education with the PD subjects.

Movement disorder specialists collected data pertaining to handedness, age, sex, disease duration, and education years, and evaluated clinical symptoms using rating scales prior to MRI examination. The severity of PD was assessed using the Hoehn & Yahr staging scale (H&Y) and the Unified PD Rating Scale (UPDRS). The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) and the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HARS) were used to quantify depression and anxiety, respectively. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was used to assess global cognitive function. Moreover, all participants underwent a complete neuropsychological battery. Attention/working memory [adaptive digit ordering test (DOT-A) and Golden Stroop test], executive [verbal fluency test (VFT) and clock drawing test (CDT)], memory [Hopkins verbal learning test-revised (HVLT-R) and brief visuospatial memory test revised (BVMT-R)], language [Wechsler intelligence scale for adults-Chinese revised (WAIS-RC) and Boston naming test (BNT)] and VS [Benton line orientation (BLO) and clock copying test (CCT)] functions were tested in all subjects. The cognitive performance scores were converted to z-scores and adjusted for age, sex, and education. Patients were classified as PD-MCI according to the new criteria of the Movement Disorder Society Task Force for mild cognitive impairment in PD. The criterion was met when patients performed approximately 1–2 standard deviation below the normative mean score on at least two neuropsychological tests, represented by either two impaired tests in one cognitive domain or one impaired test in two different cognitive domains (Litvan et al., 2012). In the current study, the PD-CU patients were defined as having a cognitive performance with normal scores or with abnormal scores with less than 1 standard deviation below the normative mean score. A total of 14 patients diagnosed as having mild cognitive impairment (PD-MCI group) with multiple domains and 18 patients diagnosed as cognition unimpaired (PD-CU group) were enrolled. PD-MCI patients were matched with PD-CU patients for PD disease severity. We arranged the enrolled patients to undergo the clinical evaluation and MRI scanning as soon as possible (usually on the second day after making a diagnosis of PD). The clinical assessment and MRI scanning were conducted on the same day prior to the initiation of any treatment. Then the patients started the anti-Parkinson treatment (usually on the same day after the clinical evaluation and MRI scanning).

MRI Acquisition

An MRI was performed on a 3.0 Tesla MRI System (Siemens 3.0 T Trio Tim, Germany) by using an eight-channel phased-array head coil. High-resolution T1-weighted images were acquired via a volumetric three-dimensional spoiled gradient recall sequence. The acquisition parameters were as follows: repetition time [TR] = 1900 ms, echo time [TE] = 2.26 ms, flip angle [FA] = 90°, field of view [FOV] = 256 mm × 256 mm, slice thickness = 1 mm, no slice gap, voxel size = 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm, number of slices = 176. The MR images sensitized to changes in the BOLD signal levels (TR = 2000 ms, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°) were obtained via a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging sequence (EPI). The slice thickness was 5 mm (no slice gap) with a matrix size of 64 × 64 and a field of view of 240 mm × 240 mm, resulting in a voxel size of 3.75 mm × 3.75 mm × 5 mm. Each brain volume comprised 30 axial slices, and each functional run contained 240 image volumes. We provided ear plugs to each subject to reduce the noise interference during scanning and used foam to fix the participants’ head position and avoid head motion. The fMRI scanning was performed in darkness, and the participants were explicitly instructed to relax, close their eyes and not fall asleep (confirmed by subjects immediately after the experiment) during the resting-state MR acquisition.

Preprocessing of fMRI Data

The functional image preprocessing and statistical analysis were conducted by using SPM1. The first 10 volumes of functional images were discarded for the signal equilibrium and participant adaptation to the scanning noise. The remaining EPI images were preprocessed via the following steps: slice timing, motion correction, spatial normalization to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) EPI template in SPM8, and resample to 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm, followed by spatial smoothing with a 6 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. In our study, the scrubbing was not performed. However, the headers are modified for each of the input images, which can also reflect the relative orientations of the data. Then the details of the transformation are displayed in the results window as plots of translation and rotation, which can be modeled as confounds within the general linear model. In addition, according to the record of head motions within each fMRI run, all participants had less than 0.5 mm maximum displacement in the x, y, or z plane, and less than 0.5° of angular rotation about each axis.

Functional Connectivity Analysis

Functional connectivity was examined by a seed-based correlation approach. Thus, we selected the following areas as seeds in the DMN: anterior, dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), superior frontal gyrus (SFG), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), posterior inferior parietal lobule (pIPL), precuneus, PCC, temporal parietal junction, anterior temporal lobe (ATL), superior temporal sulcus and HF. We defined the 18 nodes using the MNI coordinates (Spreng et al., 2013) (see Supplementary Table S1) and 10 mm radius circular masks. Using REST2, after bandpass filtering (0.01–0.08 Hz) and linear trend removal, the reference time series for each seed region was extracted by averaging the fMRI time series of all voxels within each region of interest. Correlation functional analyses were performed by computing the temporal correlation between each seed reference and the rest of the brain in a voxel-wise manner. Eight nuisance covariates were regressed to remove the possible variances from the time course of each voxel, including the white-matter signal, the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) signal and six head motion parameters. The global signal regression was not included, because it is a controversial issue (Murphy et al., 2009). However, the results with global signal regression were included in the Supplementary Table S2 and Supplementary Figure S1, and they were largely overlapped with the results without global signal regression. The correlation coefficients in each voxel were then transformed to z-value images by using the Fisher r-to-z transformation to improve normality. Hence, an entire brain z-value map was created for each subject.

In addition, to characterize the abnormal FC within the DMN, we chose these 18 nodes as being representative of the DMN and performed an additional region-wise manner. The mean-time series were extracted by the same way as above and were correlated between these 18 nodes of each subject. A Fisher’s r-to-z transformation was applied to normalize the correlation coefficient. Finally, the z-value was extracted for each participant, and we then compared z-values between the groups.

Statistical Analysis

Differences between groups in terms of demographic and clinical variables were performed by the Pearson χ2 test, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), or Student’s t-test, as appropriate. A voxel-based comparison of z-value maps among the PD patients with MCI, PD patients with CU, and HC was performed by using a design model of one-way ANOVA with age, sex and education as covariates, followed by post hoc two-sample t-tests. The significance threshold was set at p < 0.001. Family-wise error (FWE) correction for multiple comparisons was also conducted at the cluster level. In addition, a comparison of the correlation values (z-values) among groups was performed by one-way ANOVA, followed by post hoc two-sample t-tests (p < 0.05, two-tailed). To explore the possible relationship between changes in the patterns of network FC and performance in cognitive functions, we conducted Pearson correlations between FC values of significant inter-group differences and cognitive scores, and the significance was set at p < 0.05 (two-tailed). FC values of significant inter-group differences were required from two ways of FC analyses. One is the voxel-wise analysis, and the statistical analysis model was one-way ANOVA among the three groups, followed by post hoc two-sample t-tests. The significance threshold was set at p < 0.001, and family-wise error (FWE) correction for multiple comparisons was performed at the cluster level. The other one is the region-wise analysis. We only chose the FC values showing significant inter-group differences within the DMN in the Pearson correlation analysis. Then, the Pearson correlation analyses were performed between these FC values of significant inter-group differences and cognitive scores in all PD patients.

Voxel-Based Morphometry Analysis

In our study, diffeomorphic anatomic registration through an exponentiated lie algebra algorithm (DARTEL) (Ashburner, 2007) was used to improve registration of the MR images. Before segmentation, scanner artifacts and gross anatomic abnormalities for each subject were checked, and the image origin was set to the anterior commissure. Images were then segmented into gray matter (GM), white matter and CSF using the unified segmentation model in SPM8. In the next step, a GM template was generated through an iteratively non-linear registration. The GM template was normalized to MNI space, and the resulting deformations were applied to the GM images of each participant. Finally, spatially normalized images were modulated to ensure that the overall amount of each tissue class was not altered by the spatial normalization procedure and smoothed with a 10 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Voxel-based comparisons of GM volume were performed between groups using two-sample t-tests with age, sex and education as covariates. The significance thresholds were at voxel-wise p < 0.001 and the cluster level of p < 0.05 corrected by FWE correction.

Results

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

The demographic and clinical features of all subjects are listed in Table 1. Handedness, age and sex were not significantly different among the HC, PD-MCI and PD-CU groups. Education was not significantly different between total PD patients and controls, whereas, the PD-CU group had significantly more education years than the PD-MCI group. No significant differences in the disease duration, H&Y stage, UPDRS score and HDRS/HARS score between PD-CU and PD-MCI groups were found. By definition, the MoCA scores of PD-CU patients were significantly higher than those of PD-MCI patients (p < 0.000). The scores of all neuropsychological tests of all subjects are provided (Supplementary Table S3).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of all subjects.

FC Analysis in the DMN

To characterize the abnormal FC in the DMN, we chose 18 seed regions as representatives of the DMN. Relative to HC, the PD-MCI group showed significantly reduced connectivity of the DMN with a set of regions in the brain cortex. Specifically, the key hubs, mPFC, SFG and PCC, showed a decreased connectivity pattern with the right precentral gyrus and bilateral insula. Also, the mPFC and SFG showed a reduced coupling with the anterior inferior parietal lobule (aIPL); the mPFC and PCC showed a reduced coupling with the bilateral middle frontal gyri (MFG). FC analysis of the bilateral ATLs also showed a decreased connectivity pattern with the bilateral middle temporal gyri (MTG). The PD-MCI group presented significantly reduced connectivity of the DMN with the MFG and MTG compared to the PD-CU group. There was no significant difference between the PD-CU group and HC (see Table 2, Figure 1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Differences in functional connectivity among PD subgroups and normal subjects.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Differences in the connectivity patterns are shown between PD-MCI and HC (Left side), PD-MCI and PD-CU (Right side) (p < 0.001, FWE-corrected) in hot color. The regions with red circles mean the same areas between the left side and the right side. PD, Parkinson’s disease; PD-MCI, mild cognitive impaired PD patients; PD-CU, cognitive unimpaired PD patients; HC, healthy control; amPFC, anterior medial prefrontal cortex; dmPFC, dorsal medial prefrontal cortex; vmPFC, ventral medial prefrontal cortex. SFG, superior frontal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; ATL, anterior temporal lobe; L, left; R, right.

Comparing the mean connectivity values, ordered reductions (HC > PD-CU > PD-MCI) were observed in intra-DMN connectivity, although there was no significant difference between the PD-CU group and HC. Compared with HC, the PD-MCI group showed significant intra-DMN reductions predominantly between HF and IFG, between ATL and IFG, and between PCC and pIPL. The PD-MCI group showed a significantly lower FC between ATL and IFG than the PD-CU group. The means and SDs of z-values are shown in Figure 2 and Supplementary Table S4.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Significantly reduced functional connectivity in the PD-MCI group is indicated by an asterisk (p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01). PD, Parkinson’s disease; PD-MCI, mild cognitive impaired PD patients; PD-CU, cognitive unimpaired PD patients; HC, healthy control; HF, hippocampal formation; SFG/IFG, superior/inferior frontal gyrus; ATL, anterior temporal lobe; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; pIPL, posterior inferior parietal lobule; L, left; R, right.

To determine the relationship between the FC values and neuropsychological scores of PD patients, we performed the analysis in all PD patients. The FC of ATL and MTG was positively correlated with Golden Stroop Test score (a: r = 0.423, p = 0.016), and the functional coupling of HF and IFG was significantly correlated with the HVLT-R total score (b: r = 0.429, p = 0.014) (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Voxel-wise correlation analysis revealed that cognitive scores in PD patients were correlated with differential functional connectivity. (A) The FC of ATL and MTG was positively correlated with Golden Stroop Test score. (B) The FC of HF and IFG was significantly correlated with the HVLT-R total score. PD, Parkinson’s disease; PD-MCI, mild cognitive impaired PD patients; PD-CU, cognitive unimpaired PD patients; ATL, anterior temporal lobe; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; HF, hippocampal formation; IFG, inferior frontal gyri; L, left; R. right; HVLT-R, Hopkins verbal learning test-revised.

There were no significant differences among the three groups in the GM volume, indicating that the altered FC was not caused by anatomic changes.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated the resting-state FC of the DMN in PD patients with and without MCI. Compared with HC, the PD-MCI group had a distributed reduction in connectivity between the DMN and a set of regions, including the precentral gyrus, insula, aIPL, MFG and MTG. Compared to the PD-CU group, the PD-MCI group demonstrated a significantly decreased FC in the MFG and MTG. Additionally, compared to HC, the PD-MCI group had a significantly decreased FC within the DMN, mainly in the FC between the HF and IFG, between ATL and IFG, and between PCC and pIPL. Compared to the PD-CU group, the only revealed significantly decreased FC within the DMN in the PD-MCI group was between the ATL and IFG. The decreased FC between ATL and MTG was positively correlated with attention/working performance. And the reduced FC between HF and IFG was also positively correlated with memory function. By studying a cohort of drug-naïve PD patients in the early stage, we ruled out potential confounding effects of chronic duration and medication.

Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that a set of dynamically interrelated brain intrinsic connectivity networks is considered to play an important role in cognitive processing, including the DMN, dorsal attention network (DAN) and frontoparietal network (FPN) (Seeley et al., 2007; Spreng et al., 2013). The DAN activity was reduced whereas DMN activity was increased during the resting state, and the opposite was observed during cognitive tasks (Kelly et al., 2008). The FPN is interposed between the main DMN and DAN nodes in terms of anatomy and function, and can flexibly connect to one network or the other and mediate the transition between them (Vincent et al., 2008; Spreng et al., 2013). Activity in the DAN and FPN is high when attention is directed externally, associated with the reduction in activity within the DMN. These networks show anti-correlated activity, supporting that the DMN has widespread connections to the DAN and FPN, which has been suggested to be important for efficient cognitive function. In amnestic MCI, degenerated FC in DAN was revealed and the degenerations of the dorsal attention system were along with the progression of the disease and behavioral deficits in attention function (Zhang et al., 2015). In addition, neuroimaging studies have also consistently shown that working memory performance is associated with the FPN, whose task-related activity is increased in PD (Trujillo et al., 2015). Nevertheless, previous studies have mainly focused on changes affecting a single network; thus, little is known about how changes in inter-network connectivity are related to cognitive decline.

In the current resting state study, PD-MCI subjects displayed decreased FC between the DMN and regions of the precentral gyrus and MTG, which belong to DAN, as well as regions of the insula, aIPL and MFG which belong to the FPN (Spreng et al., 2013). Moreover, the connectivity between ATL and MTG was observed to positively correlate with attention/working performance in the PD subjects, indicating that reduced connectivity was associated with worse performance. In early AD, negative correlations between two intrinsically anti-correlated networks have been observed, suggesting that the disturbance of the anti-correlation might be associated with the attention deficits of AD patients (Wang et al., 2007). Moreover, for normal subjects, a higher magnitude of this anti-correlation was associated with improved behavioral attention responses (Kelly et al., 2008). Taken together, it could be speculated that this decreased FC between the DMN and a set of regions associated with DAN or FPN might be related to an impaired anti-correlation mechanism. However, it was rarely reported and is worth further research.

Aside from the decreased inter-network connectivity, we also observed reduced intra-DMN connectivity, inline with recent resting-state fMRI studies in medicated PD patients (Tessitore et al., 2012; Baggio et al., 2015; Gorges et al., 2015). In the current study, connectivity changes between the HF and IFG were positively correlated with memory performance. In the past decades, studies have focused on the hippocampus and related medial temporal lobe structures for their crucial roles in memory processes. The hippocampus has been reported to be atrophied and to be pathologically involved in both patients with MCI and patients in the early stage of AD (Braak and Braak, 1991; Desikan et al., 2009). Thus, HF was believed to have a close relationship with many cerebral cortexes and constitute the memory network to modulate and facilitate communication, which made it especially meaningful in MCI. Previous studies demonstrated a disrupted connectivity between the hippocampus and a set of regions, including the frontal lobe, temporal lobe and insula in MCI (Wang et al., 2011). Moreover, over half of the patients with PD dementia (PDD) also have significant AD-related pathology with abnormally deposited α-synuclein, amyloid-β and tau proteins in the PDD brain, including in the hippocampus which correlates to the severity of cognitive decline in patients with PDD (Kalaitzakis and Pearce, 2009). Our findings suggest that the change in connectivity between the hippocampus and frontal lobe might be part of the substrate of memory deficits in early stage drug-naïve PD with MCI. Interestingly, we also found decreased FC between ATL and IFG. The ATL, with a high degree of anatomical heterogeneity, makes a critical contribution to semantic cognition (Hurley et al., 2015; Jackson et al., 2016). In addition, semantic cognition depends on a distributed network including the IFG, mPFC, posterior middle temporal gyrus and lateral parietal regions besides ATL (Jefferies, 2013) and is also considered a major component during the resting state (Binder et al., 2009). Our observation may provide the underlying mechanism for the progressive semantic cognitive decline in the early stage PD-MCI group. As to the PCC, a critical node in the DMN, reduced connectivity with pIPL was identified in patients with PD-MCI in our study, which was consistent with results from a previous study in MCI patients (Wang et al., 2012). It is unclear how the PCC changes engaged in cognitive decline in PD-MCI, and further studies are needed to elucidate it.

One limitation of our study is the relatively small sample size of PD patients and HC. However, this limitation is partly due to the stringent quality control of clinical, neuropsychological, and imaging assessment, which in turn can be identified as a major strength of the study. Moreover, it was a possible explanation for the absence of any disruption in DMN associated with language and VS performance. Moreover, another limitation refers to the subjects’ education level where the PD-MCI group was less educated compared to the PD-CU group. The higher level of education is generally associated with a better cognitive reserve and is thought to provide the ability to recruit additional resources to compensate for brain damage (Stern, 2002). Although education was added as a covariate in the analyses, the influence of the difference of education levels cannot be entirely ruled out. In addition, the data are cross-sectional, and the mechanisms by which these network alterations change dynamically remain to be established in longitudinal studies.

Conclusion

This study shows that mild cognitive decline in early stage drug-naïve PD is associated with a loss of inter-network connectivity and a decrease in the connectivity within the DMN. Moreover, our results provide support to the hypothesis that the DMN plays a role in the neural processing of distinct neuropsychological functions. This study represents a first step toward examining the functional integrity of the DMN in PD patients with MCI by using resting fMRI. However, our results are preliminary, and further longitudinal studies are needed to investigate whether early specific connectivity dysfunctions in the DMN could represent an early potential predictor for cognitive decline in PD. In addition, the neural substrate of cognitive decline in PD is not completely understood and is a matter of ongoing research.

Author Contributions

HS and QG planned the study. YH, JY, CL, WS, RO, WL, and HS collected and analyzed clinical data, and made patient follow-ups. YH and JY conducted the imaging studies. YH wrote the article, and HS edited the paper.

Funding

The present study was founded by National Science Fund of China (grant no. 81371394, no. 81511140101, and no. 81401050), and the Science and Technology Bureau Fund of Sichuan Province (2014FZ0072).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the patients for their participation in our study, and thank the technical staff of the Department of Radiology for their collaboration and assistance.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00247

FIGURE S1 | Differences in the connectivity patterns with global signal regression are shown between PD-MCI and HC (Left side), PD-MCI and PD-CU (Right side) (p < 0.001, FWE-corrected) in hot color. The regions with red circles mean the same areas between the Table 2 and the Supplementary Table S2. PD, Parkinson’s disease; PD-MCI, mild cognitive impaired PD patients; PD-CU, cognitive unimpaired PD patients; HC, healthy control; amPFC, anterior medial prefrontal cortex; dmPFC, dorsal medial prefrontal cortex; vmPFC, ventral medial prefrontal cortex. SFG, superior frontal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; ATL, anterior temporal lobe; ST, superior temporal; L, left; R, right.

TABLE S1 | Anatomical regions used as DMN nodes for seed-based connectivity analyses.

TABLE S2 | Differences in functional connectivity among PD subgroups and normal subjects with global signal regression.

TABLE S3 | Neuropsychological performance results for healthy controls and Parkinson’s disease patients.

TABLE S4 | Means and SDs of z values for all participants.

Footnotes

  1. ^http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk
  2. ^http://www.restfmri.net/forum/REST_V1.7

References

Aarsland, D., Bronnick, K., Larsen, J. P., Tysnes, O. B., and Alves, G. (2009). Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology 72, 1121–1126. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000338632.00552.cb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agosta, F., Pievani, M., Geroldi, C., Copetti, M., Frisoni, G. B., and Filippi, M. (2012). Resting state fMRI in Alzheimer’s disease: beyond the default mode network. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1564–1578. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amboni, M., Tessitore, A., Esposito, F., Santangelo, G., Picillo, M., Vitale, C., et al. (2015). Resting-state functional connectivity associated with mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. 262, 425–434. doi: 10.1007/s00415-014-7591-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baggio, H. C., Segura, B., Sala-Llonch, R., Marti, M. J., Valldeoriola, F., Compta, Y., et al. (2015). Cognitive impairment and resting-state network connectivity in Parkinson’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 199–212. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., and Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buhmann, C., Glauche, V., Sturenburg, H. J., Oechsner, M., Weiller, C., and Buchel, C. (2003). Pharmacologically modulated fMRI–cortical responsiveness to levodopa in drug-naive hemiparkinsonian patients. Brain 126(Pt 2), 451–461. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desikan, R. S., Cabral, H. J., Hess, C. P., Dillon, W. P., Glastonbury, C. M., Weiner, M. W., et al. (2009). Automated MRI measures identify individuals with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 132(Pt 8), 2048–2057. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emre, M., Aarsland, D., Brown, R., Bum, D. J., Duyckaerts, C., Mizuno, Y., et al. (2007). Clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 22, 1689–1707. doi: 10.1002/mds.21507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorges, M., Muller, H. P., Lule, D., Pinkhardt, E. H., Ludolph, A. C., and Kassubek, J. (2015). To rise and to fall: functional connectivity in cognitively normal and cognitively impaired patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 1727–1735. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, A. J., Daniel, S. E., Kilford, L., and Lees, A. J. (1992). Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 55, 181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurley, R. S., Bonakdarpour, B., Wang, X., and Mesulam, M. M. (2015). Asymmetric connectivity between the anterior temporal lobe and the language network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 464–473. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, R. L., Hoffman, P., Pobric, G., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2016). The semantic network at work and rest: differential connectivity of anterior temporal lobe subregions. J. Neurosci. 36, 1490–1501. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2999-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jankovic, J. (2008). Parkinson’s disease: clinical features and diagnosis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 79, 368–376. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.131045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janvin, C., Aarsland, D., Larsen, J. P., and Hugdahl, K. (2003). Neuropsychological profile of patients with Parkinson’s disease without dementia. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 15, 126–131. doi: 10.1159/000068483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jefferies, E. (2013). The neural basis of semantic cognition: converging evidence from neuropsychology, neuroimaging and TMS. Cortex 49, 611–625. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalaitzakis, M. E., and Pearce, R. K. (2009). The morbid anatomy of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 118, 587–598. doi: 10.1007/s00401-009-0597-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, A. M., Uddin, L. Q., Biswal, B. B., Castellanos, F. X., and Milham, M. P. (2008). Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. Neuroimage 39, 527–537. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krajcovicova, L., Mikl, M., Marecek, R., and Rektorova, I. (2012). The default mode network integrity in patients with Parkinson’s disease is levodopa equivalent dose-dependent. J. Neural. Transm. 119, 443–454. doi: 10.1007/s00702-011-0723-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litvan, I., Goldman, J. G., Troster, A. I., Schmand, B. A., Weintraub, D., Petersen, R. C., et al. (2012). Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: movement disorder society task force guidelines. Mov. Disord. 27, 349–356. doi: 10.1002/mds.24893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, C., Chen, Q., Song, W., Chen, K., Guo, X., Yang, J., et al. (2014). Resting-state fMRI study on drug-naive patients with Parkinson’s disease and with depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 85, 675–683. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-306237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, K., Birn, R. M., Handwerker, D. A., Jones, T. B., and Bandettini, P. A. (2009). The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage 44, 893–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poletti, M., Emre, M., and Bonuccelli, U. (2011). Mild cognitive impairment and cognitive reserve in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism. Relat. Disord. 17, 579–586. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreng, R. N., Sepulcre, J., Turner, G. R., Stevens, W. D., and Schacter, D. L. (2013). Intrinsic architecture underlying the relations among the default, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal control networks of the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 74–86. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tedeschi, G., Trojsi, F., Tessitore, A., Corbo, D., Sagnelli, A., Paccone, A., et al. (2012). Interaction between aging and neurodegeneration in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 886–898. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tessitore, A., Esposito, F., Vitale, C., Santangelo, G., Amboni, M., Russo, A., et al. (2012). Default-mode network connectivity in cognitively unimpaired patients with Parkinson disease. Neurology 79, 2226–2232. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827689d6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trujillo, J. P., Gerrits, N. J., Veltman, D. J., Berendse, H. W., van der Werf, Y. D., and van den Heuvel, O. A. (2015). Reduced neural connectivity but increased task-related activity during working memory in de novo Parkinson patients. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 1554–1566. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Eimeren, T., Monchi, O., Ballanger, B., and Strafella, A. P. (2009). Dysfunction of the default mode network in Parkinson disease: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch. Neurol. 66, 877–883. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., and Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, K., Liang, M., Wang, L., Tian, L., Zhang, X., Li, K., et al. (2007). Altered functional connectivity in early Alzheimer’s disease: a resting-state fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 967–978. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Liang, P., Jia, X., Jin, G., Song, H., Han, Y., et al. (2012). The baseline and longitudinal changes of PCC connectivity in mild cognitive impairment: a combined structure and resting-state fMRI study. PLoS ONE 7:e36838. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Liang, P., Jia, X., Qi, Z., Yu, L., Yang, Y., et al. (2011). Baseline and longitudinal patterns of hippocampal connectivity in mild cognitive impairment: evidence from resting state fMRI. J. Neurol. Sci. 309, 79–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams-Gray, C. H., Foltynie, T., Brayne, C. E., Robbins, T. W., and Barker, R. A. (2007). Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson’s disease cohort. Brain 130(Pt 7), 1787–1798. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, R. C., Sambataro, F., Vasic, N., Wolf, N. D., Thomann, P. A., Saft, C., et al. (2012). Default-mode network changes in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Exp. Neurol. 237, 191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, X., Li, R., Fleisher, A. S., Reiman, E. M., Guan, X., Zhang, Y., et al. (2011). Altered default mode network connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease–a resting functional MRI and Bayesian network study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1868–1881. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., Zheng, H., Liang, K., Wang, H., Kong, S., Hu, J., et al. (2015). Functional degeneration in dorsal and ventral attention systems in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: an fMRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 585, 160–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.11.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Greicius, M. D., Gennatas, E. D., Growdon, M. E., Jang, J. Y., Rabinovici, G. D., et al. (2010). Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 133(Pt 5), 1352–1367. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, default mode network, fMRI, functional connectivity

Citation: Hou Y, Yang J, Luo C, Song W, Ou R, Liu W, Gong Q and Shang H (2016) Dysfunction of the Default Mode Network in Drug-Naïve Parkinson’s Disease with Mild Cognitive Impairments: A Resting-State fMRI Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:247. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00247

Received: 14 July 2016; Accepted: 10 October 2016;
Published: 26 October 2016.

Edited by:

Manuel Menéndez-González, Central University Hospital of Asturias, Spain

Reviewed by:

Eduardo A. Garza-Villarreal, National Institute of Psychiatry Ramón de la Fuente Muñiz, Mexico
Kesong Hu, Cornell University, USA

Copyright © 2016 Hou, Yang, Luo, Song, Ou, Liu, Gong and Shang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Huifang Shang, hfshang2002@126.com Qiyong Gong, huaxigong@126.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Download