Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 29 November 2016
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias
This article is part of the Research Topic Human and animal models for translational research on neurodegeneration: challenges and opportunities from South America View all 23 articles

Cognitive Intervention As an Early Non-pharmacological Strategy in Alzheimer's Disease: A Translational Perspective

  • 1Department of Biochemistry, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil
  • 2Department NVS, Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Translational Alzheimer Neurobiology, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 3Department of Physiology, Federal University of Sergipe, São Cristóvão, Brazil
  • 4Brain Institute of Rio Grande do Sul (BraIns), Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil

Brain amyloid-β (Aβ) accumulation is currently considered the main causative pathophysiological event in Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Hardy and Higgins, 1992; Karran et al., 2011). Importantly, this process is thought to precede the onset of AD clinical symptoms by more than two decades, indicating that early therapeutic strategies prior to symptomatology offer the best chance of success. In line with this, there is growing attention being paid to the concept of cognitive reserve (CR) (Stern et al., 1994; Stern, 2002). CR concept is based on extensive epidemiological data indicating that those with higher lifetime levels of social, physical, and cognitive engagement have a lower risk of developing dementia despite the presence of brain pathology (Fratiglioni et al., 2004; Nithianantharajah and Hannan, 2009). Recently, cognitive intervention (CI)—such as cognitive training (Bahar-Fuchs et al., 2013), cognitive stimulation (Woods et al., 2012), and cognitive rehabilitation (Clare et al., 2003)—has emerged as a potential non-pharmacological strategy for the treatment and prevention of AD (Gates and Sachdev, 2014). Although based upon distinct theoretical constructs, these CI strategies are frequently not distinguished in clinical trials.

Clinical data are supported by compelling evidence from experimental studies, which have demonstrated that early-life exposure to environmental enrichment (EE), an experimental method of CR in animals, is able to prevent memory decline in AD-like animal models (Valero et al., 2011; Verret et al., 2013; Polito et al., 2014). However, the precise mechanisms behind this phenomenon remain elusive. Environmental enrichment for animals involves stimulating not only their senses (e.g., smell, sight, touch), but also stimulating their ability to learn and adapt when exposed to novelty and challenge, including via stimulation of innate behaviors, such as foraging and partner seeking. The same line of thinking can be applied to humans, on its own scale. This would translate into exercise, social interaction, learning new things, exploring new environments, keeping the brain active with cognitive training, i.e., challenging the brain to self-adapt to novelty.

In a recent study, published in Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, Bezzina et al. (2015) reared mice harboring a human pathological double mutation in the amyloid precursor protein gene (APP, model tg2576) in standard or enriched housing conditions for 10 weeks, starting at 3 months of age (pre-Aβ plaque phase). Two weeks after EE exposure, transgenic animals housed in standard or EE cages presented similar seizure susceptibility to pentylenetetrazole (PTZ), a well-established GABA receptor antagonist. Moreover, the frequency of interictal spikes—indexed by electroencephalography (EEG)—after EE was similar between groups. In short, the authors show that EE is not capable of halting the Aβ-induced aberrant neuronal activity in the Tg2576 AD-like model. These findings indicate that preventive effects of EE on cognitive decline are not necessarily related to changes in neuronal activity. Although negative, these findings are very relevant since they help rule out abnormal neuronal activity as a mechanism by which EE exerts its beneficial impact on memory performance.

By contrast, data analyzing the effects of EE on neurogenesis in AD-like animal models have been mostly positive. EE exposure for 6 months was able to restore the impaired hippocampal neurogenesis of a triple transgenic AD-like mouse model (3xTg-AD, a model harboring human mutations in: APP (Swedish, KM670/671NL), Tau (MAPT P301L) and Presenilin 1 (PSEN1 M146V) (Rodríguez et al., 2008, 2011). The density analysis of hippocampal proliferating cells was performed by immunohistochemistry targeting the presence of phosphorylated Histone H3 (HH3), and their potential neuronal and glial phenotype by co-localizing the proliferating cells with the immature neuronal marker doublecortin (DCX), the mature neuronal marker (NeuN) or the specific astroglial marker (GFAP). This very interesting study showed a significant increase in neuronal proliferating cells (HH3/DCX or HH3/NeuN positive) in the dentate gyrus of transgenic animals housed in EE. These results, together with the findings of other experimental studies, suggest the stimulation of adult neurogenesis as a potential process by which EE prevents cognitive impairment in AD-like models (Wolf et al., 2006; Herring et al., 2009; Mirochnic et al., 2009; Lahiani-Cohen et al., 2011). In this context, recent evidence indicates that the benefitial effects of EE are more related to the reorganization of the neuronal network built by these newborn neurons than to the absolute number of newborn neurons per se (Vivar et al., 2016).

However, the impact of EE on Aβ levels in the brain is still largely inconclusive (Mainardi et al., 2014; Polito et al., 2014; Rodríguez et al., 2015). Male APP23, a mouse model expressing the human APP Swedish double mutation, exposed to EE starting at 3 months of age until 7 months, or 18 months, showed a marked cognitive improvement without altering soluble Aβ (7 month-old) but reducing the number of Aβ plaques at 18 month-old. These data indicate that EE effect seems to affect Aβ in a distinct manner.

We are of the opinion that deciphering the underlying mechanisms of EE can provide important information about novel therapeutic targets. With currently available treatments for AD being palliative at best and ineffective at worst—results from drug trials targeting brain Aβ have been either inconclusive or negative—non-pharmacological interventions stimulating cognition in humans hold a measure of promise, not only for delaying symptomatic onset and the loss of cognitive functions, but also in terms of unveiling potential targets.

The take-home message from EE studies is clear, however, and translates to human disease: interventions must occur early on in the disease course, prior to substantial cognitive deficits (Figure 1). The question then becomes: how to translate these findings to clinical research? A promising approach is to study individuals in the so-called “preclinical” or “asymptomatic at-risk” stage of AD [please see revised definition of preclinical AD in Sperling et al. (2011) and Dubois et al. (2016)]. Defined by the absence of cognitive deficits despite in situ AD pathology—established using cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) or positron emission tomography (PET) based biomarkers—cross-sectional and longitudinal studies suggest this preclinical phase to span some 20–30 years (Jansen et al., 2015). Based on this, non-pharmacological interventions directed at CR could be encouraged in so called “amyloid-positive” individuals believed to be on the path to symptomatic AD. In this way, we refer to the concepts of primary and secondary prevention, as defined by Gates et al. (2011). Primary prevention in the form of early intervention, aimed at preclinical individuals, in order to keep them asymptomatic for as long as possible; and secondary prevention in the form of cognitive training and stimulation for those who already show subtle and early cognitive impairment [such as those in the mild cognitive impairment (MCI) phase], to slow the cognitive decline into AD dementia.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothetical model depicting the beneficial effects of cognitive intervention on cognitive function on low and high cognitive reserve individuals as a function of Alzheimer's disease neuropathology. This hypothetical model exemplifies changes in cognitive function changes over Alzheimer's disease (AD) continuum in individuals with high and low cognitive reserve (CR). When AD pathology is absent or possibly below detectable levels using current biomarkers, individuals with high CR have been shown to perform better on neurophysiological tests than individual with low CR. While cognitive intervention probably cannot ultimately arrest the progression of AD pathology, as measured by imaging or fluid based biomarkers, it can potentially delay the onset and progression of cognitive symptoms. In keeping with the observation that individuals with high CR can tolerate greater levels of pathology prior to declines in cognitive performance, the rate of decline is greater in the high CR group.

While follow-up studies with longer assessment periods are essential for more definitive data, early results from several multi-domain non-pharmacologic intervention studies—addressing diet, exercise, cognitive training and vascular risk monitoring—suggest that cognitive functioning can be maintained or even improved in at-risk elderly individuals (Ngandu et al., 2015). The incorporation of amyloid biomarkers into such intervention trials could stand as a valuable adjunct to cognitive and functional outcome measures, possibly allowing for more rapid translational estimations.

Though EE stimulation has been shown to be a promising approach for delaying disease progression in animal models mimicking early onset autosomal dominant AD, which accounts for less than 5% of cases, what about the far more common age-related (sporadic) and multifactorial form of AD? How does EE impact on sporadic models of AD? Here stands a problem in that models replicating sporadic AD are currently lacking. In our opinion, major developments in modeling the sporadic form of AD in rodents will potentially come from the incorporation of genetic variations that increase the risk of developing AD, such as the ε4 allele of apolipoprotein E (APOE) gene, or the R47H allele of the triggering receptor expressed on myeloid cells 2 (TREM2). Animal models expressing such combinations of genetic risk factors may better resemble sporadic AD and ultimately translate into more relevant disease models.

In conclusion, study designs aiming toward deciphering EE specific mechanisms are vital and can potentially drive non-pharmacological strategies. Here we make an argument that the combination of EE with models that better mimic the sporadic form of AD, as well as CSF- and PET-based biomarkers, will potentially provide important insights into AD pathophysiology and highlight novel therapeutic targets, with strong translational value.

Author Contributions

SG and AR were responsible for reviewing the literature and drafting the manuscript. CL, GV and AL were responsible for revising the manuscript. EZ was responsible for the conceptualization and drafting the manuscript. All authors critically revised the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq, Brazil) [460594/2014-1], the INCT for Excitotoxicity and Neuroprotection [573677/2008-5], the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES, Brazil) [PNPD], and the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio Grande do Sul (FAPERGS).

References

Bahar-Fuchs, A., Clare, L., and Woods, B. (2013). Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for persons with mild to moderate dementia of the Alzheimer's or vascular type: a review. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 5, 35. doi: 10.1186/alzrt189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bezzina, C., Verret, L., Halley, H., Dahan, L., and Rampon, C. (2015). Environmental enrichment does not influence hypersynchronous network activity in the Tg2576 mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:178. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clare, L., Woods, R. T., Moniz Cook, E. D., Orrell, M., and Spector, A. (2003). Cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive training for early-stage Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. CD003260. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003260. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, B., Hampel, H., Feldman, H. H., Scheltens, P., Aisen, P., Andrieu, S., et al. (2016). Preclinical Alzheimer's disease: definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimers. Dement. 12, 292–323. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., and Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol. 3, 343–353. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, N. J., and Sachdev, P. (2014). Is cognitive training an effective treatment for preclinical and early Alzheimer's disease? J. Alzheimers Dis. 42(Suppl. 4), S551–S559. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, N. J., Sachdev, P. S., Fiatarone Singh, M. A., and Valenzuela, M. (2011). Cognitive and memory training in adults at risk of dementia: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 11:55. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-11-55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, J. A., and Higgins, G. A. (1992). Alzheimer's disease: the amyloid cascade hypothesis. Science 256, 184–185. doi: 10.1126/science.1566067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herring, A., Ambrée, O., Tomm, M., Habermann, H., Sachser, N., Paulus, W., et al. (2009). Environmental enrichment enhances cellular plasticity in transgenic mice with Alzheimer-like pathology. Exp. Neurol. 216, 184–192. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, W. J., Ossenkoppele, R., Knol, D. L., Tijms, B. M., Scheltens, P., Verhey, F. R., et al. (2015). Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: a meta-analysis. JAMA 313, 1924–1938. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.4668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karran, E., Mercken, M., and De Strooper, B. (2011). The amyloid cascade hypothesis for Alzheimer's disease: an appraisal for the development of therapeutics. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 10, 698–712. doi: 10.1038/nrd3505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahiani-Cohen, I., Lourbopoulos, A., Haber, E., Rozenstein-Tsalkovich, L., Abramsky, O., Grigoriadis, N., et al. (2011). Moderate environmental enrichment mitigates tauopathy in a neurofibrillary tangle mouse model. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 70, 610–621. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e318221bfab

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Mainardi, M., Di Garbo, A., Caleo, M., Berardi, N., Sale, A., and Maffei, L. (2014). Environmental enrichment strengthens corticocortical interactions and reduces amyloid-beta oligomers in aged mice. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:1. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirochnic, S., Wolf, S., Staufenbiel, M., and Kempermann, G. (2009). Age effects on the regulation of adult hippocampal neurogenesis by physical activity and environmental enrichment in the APP23 mouse model of Alzheimer disease. Hippocampus 19, 1008–1018. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ngandu, T., Lehtisalo, J., Solomon, A., Levälahti, E., Ahtiluoto, S., Antikainen, R., et al. (2015). A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 385, 2255–2263. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60461-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nithianantharajah, J., and Hannan, A. J. (2009). The neurobiology of brain and cognitive reserve: mental and physical activity as modulators of brain disorders. Prog. Neurobiol. 89, 369–382. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polito, L., Chierchia, A., Tunesi, M., Bouybayoune, I., Kehoe, P. G., Albani, D., et al. (2014). Environmental enrichment lessens cognitive decline in APP23 mice without affecting brain sirtuin expression. J. Alzheimers Dis. 42, 851–864. doi: 10.3233/JAD-131430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, J. J., Jones, V. C., Tabuchi, M., Allan, S. M., Knight, E. M., Laferla, F. M., et al. (2008). Impaired adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of a triple transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. PLoS ONE 3:e2935. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, J. J., Noristani, H. N., Olabarria, M., Fletcher, J., Somerville, T. D., Yeh, C. Y., et al. (2011). Voluntary running and environmental enrichment restores impaired hippocampal neurogenesis in a triple transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Curr. Alzheimers Res. 8, 707–717. doi: 10.2174/156720511797633214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, J. J., Noristani, H. N., and Verkhratsky, A. (2015). Microglial response to Alzheimer's disease is differentially modulated by voluntary wheel running and enriched environments. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 941–953. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0693-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Bennett, D. A., Craft, S., Fagan, A. M., et al. (2011). Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 280–292. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702813248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Gurland, B., Tatemichi, T. K., Tang, M. X., Wilder, D., and Mayeux, R. (1994). Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. JAMA 271, 1004–1010. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03510370056032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valero, J., España, J., Parra-Damas, A., Martin, E., Rodríguez-Álvarez, J., and Saura, C. A. (2011). Short-term environmental enrichment rescues adult neurogenesis and memory deficits in APP(Sw,Ind) transgenic mice. PLoS ONE 6:e16832. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verret, L., Krezymon, A., Halley, H., Trouche, S., Zerwas, M., Lazouret, M., et al. (2013). Transient enriched housing before amyloidosis onset sustains cognitive improvement in Tg2576 mice. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 211–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vivar, C., Peterson, B. D., and Van Praag, H. (2016). Running rewires the neuronal network of adult-born dentate granule cells. Neuroimage 131, 29–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, S. A., Kronenberg, G., Lehmann, K., Blankenship, A., Overall, R., Staufenbiel, M., et al. (2006). Cognitive and physical activity differently modulate disease progression in the amyloid precursor protein (APP)-23 model of Alzheimer's disease. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 1314–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, B., Aguirre, E., Spector, A. E., and Orrell, M. (2012). Cognitive stimulation to improve cognitive functioning in people with dementia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. CD005562. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005562.pub2. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Alzheimer's disease, cognitive intervention, cognitive reserve, dementia, environmental enrichment

Citation: Gehres SW, Rocha A, Leuzy A, Loss CM, Viola GG and Zimmer ER (2016) Cognitive Intervention As an Early Non-pharmacological Strategy in Alzheimer's Disease: A Translational Perspective. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:280. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00280

Received: 09 September 2016; Accepted: 08 November 2016;
Published: 29 November 2016.

Edited by:

Lucas Sedeño, Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO), Argentina

Reviewed by:

James C. Vickers, University of Tasmania, Australia

Copyright © 2016 Gehres, Rocha, Leuzy, Loss, Viola and Zimmer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eduardo R. Zimmer, eduardo.zimmer@ufrgs.br

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.