Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 03 September 2018
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 10 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00264

Factors Explaining Language Performance After Training in Elders With and Without Subjective Cognitive Decline

  • 1Department of Experimental Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
  • 2Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience, Center of Biomedical Technology, Polytechnical University – Complutense University, Madrid, Spain
  • 3Centre for the Prevention of Cognitive Impairment, Madrid, Spain
  • 4Biological and Health Psychology Department, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain

The present study explores if cognitive reserve, executive functions, and working memory capacity are predictive of performance in the language domain (specifically in sentence comprehension and naming) after a cognitive training intervention. Sixty-six Spanish older adults voluntarily participated in the study, classified either as older adults with subjective cognitive decline according to Jessen et al.’s (2014) criteria (n = 35; 70.94 ± 4.16 years old) or cognitively intact (n = 31; 71.34 ± 4.96 years old). Written sentence comprehension and visual confrontation naming were assessed both immediately after recruitment (at the baseline), and then 6 months later, once each participant had completed his/her cognitive training (a well-known program in Spain, called UMAM; English translation: Madrid City Council Memory Unit Program). Cognitive reserve, executive functions (cognitive flexibility and controlled interference efficiency), and working memory capacity were measured for all participants at the baseline. Results pointed out that the subjective cognitive decline group presented greater benefits in the language domain than cognitively intact participants. We also observed that lower executive functioning and working memory capacity at the baseline predicted larger benefits in language performance after training, but only in the group of cognitively intact older adults. However, selected predictors hardly explained subjective cognitive decline participants’ results in language performance after training.

Introduction

Nowadays there is an increasing interest in non-pharmacological intervention effects on older adult cognition. Some studies have shown that cognitive training (CT) is beneficial for older adults’ memory (Gross et al., 2012; Rosi et al., 2017) but also for other domains in this population, as for attention (Rahe et al., 2015), working memory, reasoning and language comprehension (Carretti et al., 2013; Karbach and Verhaeghen, 2014), processing speed and reasoning (Willis and Caskie, 2013), or executive functions (Jackson et al., 2012; Mowszowski et al., 2016); CT is also beneficial for older adults with subjective cognitive decline (SCD) or people with mild cognitive impairment (Martin et al., 2011; Rebok et al., 2014; Smart et al., 2017).

Another issue that requires attention is the one concerning the factors that are strongly associated with greater benefits after CT application. For example, lifestyle factors, occupation attainment or education (cognitive reserve proxies) influence individuals’ cognition along life (Stern, 2012). In this vein, Mondini et al. (2016) have shown that cognitive reserve (CR) can modulate the general cognitive status (measured by Mini-Mental State Examination; MMSE onward) after a CT program and should be considered as a predictor of CT efficacy in older adults with mild to moderate dementia. In Spain a recently published study (Lojo-Seoane et al., 2018) have shown the influence of CR on episodic memory, WM, and general cognitive performance in adults with subjective cognitive complaints evaluated at baseline and at a follow-up after an interval of about 18 months. The results exhibited the positive effect of CR on cognitive performance at baseline and at follow-up, confirming the mediating role of WM on episodic memory and general cognitive performance. Another study (Clark et al., 2016) has indicated that educational attainment modulates the effectivity of training in cognitively intact (CI) older adults, being participants with low educational level the ones with the best performance after training. These and other examples reveal that there are important factors at the baseline which might modulate CT gains, both general or/and in specific cognitive domains (Kraemer et al., 2002; Ranganath et al., 2011). In a study conducted by Bamidis et al. (2015) with a community-dwelling sample of CI and cognitively impaired older adults, the authors reported a robust modulation (inverse) effect of executive functions’ baseline performance on training benefits in global cognition. Additionally, López-Higes et al. (2018) have shown a compensation effect regarding general cognitive status in SCD participants with lower interference efficiency at the baseline. Following the administration of a task-switching intervention in three different populations (children, young, and older adults), Karbach et al. (2017) demonstrated that this training program led to a reduction of age differences. They also observed that baseline abilities predicted both training and transfer gains. Specifically their results showed a compensation effect, since those participants with lower abilities were the ones who improved the most. However, there are other studies that have found opposite results. For example, Guye et al. (2017) have demonstrated very limited evidence for individual differences as predictors of training outcomes, although they found that cognitive performance at baseline was related to training improvement. In particular, those with higher abilities seemed to benefit more, indicating a magnification effect, which was especially apparent in their young adult population.

An additional related issue that has generated a considerable debate is the question of whether and how CT may produce improvements in untrained tasks (see for example, Karbach and Verhaeghen, 2014; Au et al., 2015, 2016; Greenwood and Parasuraman, 2016, for evidence in favor to transfer effects; see also, Melby-Lervåg and Hulme, 2013 or Simons et al., 2016, for results against the existence of such effects). Carretti et al. (2013) have investigated the efficacy of a verbal WM training program in CI older adults, considering the specific training-related benefits in a verbal WM task (criterion) and the transfer effects on measures of WM updating, reasoning, and on language comprehension. They found that the trained older adults performed better than controls in the criterion task and retained this benefit 6 months later. Transfer effects were seen in reasoning and language comprehension performance and were substantially maintained at the 6-month follow-up. In the same line, a study conducted by Payne and Stine-Morrow (2017) examined the effects of a novel home-based computerized CT program focused in verbal WM in healthy older adults in comparison with an active component-control group. Participants in the WM training group showed non-linear improvements in performance on trained verbal WM tasks. In contrast with the control group, WM training participants also showed improvements on untrained verbal WM tasks and across untrained dimensions of language, including sentence memory, verbal fluency, and comprehension of syntactically ambiguous sentences.

In Spain, a well-known multifactorial CT program, called UMAM by its name in Spanish [(programa de la Unidad de Memoria del Ayuntamiento de Madrid); English translation: Madrid City Council Memory Unit Program; Montejo et al., 2013] has proven its efficacy on subjective and objective memory measures (and also in mood) in older adults without cognitive impairment, both in the post-training assessment and at 6-month follow-up (Montejo, 2015). The benefits of the UMAM CT program on other cognitive domains, such as language, have been explored in a previous study comparing APOE ε4 carriers and non-carriers (López-Higes et al., 2017). That study revealed that there were consistent treatment benefits in complex sentence comprehension, but only in the APOE ε4 non-carrier group.

The present study explores the role of CR and executive functions at the baseline as factors which might modulate the efficacy of the UMAM program on complex sentence comprehension and naming in CI and in SCD older adults. The three main executive functions that have been used as predictors in this study: (a) alternation (cognitive flexibility), (b) updating and monitoring of WM representations, and (c) inhibition (as they appear in Miyake et al., 2000 model). In the Miyake and Friedman (2012) model, the part equivalent to the inhibition of the previous model would be a common part of all the executive functions, while the other two, updating and monitoring as well as alternation, would maintain a corresponding specific part within the model. Comprehension and naming are abilities related to successful daily living, since they are needed in various everyday activities and are relevant to older adults’ health and well-being. If older adults with SCD have higher risk of developing dementia (Jessen et al., 2014), it is of great interest to investigate benefits of CT in this population in comparison with CI participants, as well as to find which factors might be associated with greater gains subsequently. Considering a recent work where results evidenced that the UMAM CT program was most beneficial for SCD participants in explaining that their efficiency in inhibition support their general cognitive performance, while the Cis’ WM capacity was the most important performance predictor (López-Higes et al., 2018), our hypothesis are as follows: (1) We expected that CR will not have a significant role as predictor of CT outcomes in language in any group; (2) We hypothesized that sentence comprehension and naming will be most improved in the SCD’s participants after the CT intervention than in those pertaining to the CI’s older group; (3) Provided that the UMAM program is multifactorial, we anticipate a positive effect on participants’ executive functioning which, in turn, will explain linguistic performance; Finally, we assume that program effects will be different depending on the group considered. In short, within the group of older adults with SCD a greater weight of inhibition is expected as a predictor/mediator of linguistic performance, while in the group of CI participants the variable with the greater weight will be a measure related to updating and monitoring representations in WM.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixty-six older Spanish-speaking adults voluntarily participated in the study. They were recruited from the Center for the Prevention of Cognitive Impairment (a public health institute in Madrid) and, were also enrolled in the UMAM program.

General inclusion criteria to participate in the study were: (1) Normal performance in the Logical Memory delayed recall subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale – Third Edition (Wechsler, 1997; more than 10 units for people with 16 years of formal education or more; and more than six units for people with 8–15 years of formal education); (2) Yesavage Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15; Sheikh and Yesavage, 1986) lower or equal to 9; (3) MMSE (Folstein et al., 1975; Spanish adaptation by Lobo et al., 1999) higher than 24 points at the baseline. All participants had normal or corrected hearing and vision.

Thirty-five participants were identified as older adults with SCD (10 males and 25 females) according to Jessen et al.’s (2014) criteria, that is: (a) they had requested medical advice services about their memory complaints; (b) they presented self-perception of cognitive decline, mainly associated with memory loss; (c) they felt that their subjective decline affected daily life activities; (d) they set the onset of their subjective decline within the last 5 years, and (e) concerns associated with their subjective decline were confirmed by a reliable informant. The remaining 31 participants (12 males and 19 females) did not meet criteria for SCD, and they formed a group of CI older adults. The selection process described along these last lines is summarized in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Flowchart showing the inclusion/selection criteria of the 66 participants. All of them were given two neuropsychological assessments: at baseline (0 months) and at the endpoint (6 months). The UMAM cognitive training program was applied between the baseline and the endpoint period. (1) Wechsler Memory Scale III. (2) Mini-Mental State Examination. (3) Geriatric Depression Scale-Short Version. (4) Subjective Cognitive Decline. (5) Cognitively Intact.

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for socio-demographic variables (age, years of formal education) as well as scores of global cognitive status (MMSE), depressive symptomatology (GDS-15), CR, executive functions and WM capacity in both groups at the baseline. SCD older adults had significant lower scores in interference efficacy as a group than CI participants [F(1, 64) = 5.948; p = 0.018; ηp2 = 0.016; observed power = 0.669]. No significant differences between groups arose for any other variable.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics for socio-demographic variables (age, years of formal education) as well as scores of, global cognitive status (MMSE), depressive symptomatology (GDS-15), cognitive reserve, executive functions and working memory capacity in both groups at baseline.

All participants were informed about the objectives of the study and were invited to participate after signing a written informed consent form. The present study complied with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethical Committee of the San Carlos Clinical Hospital in Madrid, which is one of the main medical institutions that participates in the current research project.

The UMAM Cognitive Training Program

The UMAM program was initially designed and implemented in 1995 by the Memory Training Unit of the City Council of Madrid (Unidad de Memoria del Ayuntamiento de Madrid: UMAM). More than 20,000 older adults have benefited from this CT program. This multifactorial intervention consists of 30 sessions that has a duration of 90 min each. Twenty-eight sessions are carried out along 3 months followed by two additional maintaining-booster sessions. Training is organized in groups of 12–18 participants that come to the center in three mornings per week. A typical session begins with 10 min of relaxation, 15 min for homework and other activation activities, followed by 60 min of CT of specific activities, and concluded with suggested homework for the next day (5 min).

The UMAM program is organized in modules:

1. Module of cognitive stimulation and learning of specific strategies. This module stimulates basic cognitive processes such as attention, concentration, or perception. The most important memory strategies are taught and practiced: visualization, association, categorization, elaboration, etc. Basic cognitive processes are stimulated by pursuing specific objectives and working on with specific exercises involving attention (focused attention, attentive listening) or perception (a basic process in the registration phase of memorization). Exercises include visual and auditory material. Language acts as mediator in the processes related to codification and retrieving information from memory. Through language exercises the program aims to increase verbal fluency as well as encourage evocation.

The main strategies that are worked on in the program are visualization, association, categorization, and repetition.

2. Module of memory concepts. In this module professionals present the most important aspects of memory functioning, highlighting those whose knowledge may have an impact on daily life: types and phases of memory, memory in the elderly and factors that affect memory performance.

3. Application module to daily life. It is about practicing and adapting memory strategies to the most frequent daily forgetfulness. In addition, specific techniques are taught and practiced for some memory problems: forgetting where something has been put, forgetting names, understanding and remembering texts, etc. The UMAM program includes techniques that are of interest for the daily forgetfulness in this population. The method of the 3 “R” is emphasized, since it is a procedure to improve the comprehension and memory of texts, as well as the use of an specific technique to recall names. Another aspect that we also deal with is the use of external aids, resources such as diaries, alarms or warnings, telephone directory, putting notes, lists, labels, etc.

From the first session, it is highlighted the importance of reflecting on one’s own daily memory failures (causes and related variables; metamemory) and on the methods used to avoid or overcome them. The UMAM program has been analyzed during its years of implementation, always obtaining good results. The most recent evaluations of this program, such as that observed by of Montejo (2003) where 1,083 users over 65 years of age show that the UMAM program produced a 40% improvement in objective memory in 77% of the cases, and on a subjective level they improved until the 75%, a percentage that was even maintained along the 6 months follow up. In another sample of 2,553 users (Montejo et al., 2013) the normal pre-treatment memory level was 24.8%, reaching a post level of 56%. As mentioned before, the program has proven its efficacy in older adults without cognitive impairment, both in the post-training assessment and at 6-month follow-up (Montejo, 2015).

Materials

Predictor Variables (CR and Executive Functions)

Cognitive reserve was estimated using the CR Questionnaire (CRQ; Cuestionario de Reserva Cognitiva; Rami et al., 2011), a brief questionnaire well suited for clinical context. Inhibition efficiency and alternation (cognitive flexibility) were assessed by the Stroop test (Golden, 1978) and the Trail Making Test parts A and B (TMT-A and TMT-B; Reitan, 1994), respectively. The assessment protocol also included a digit reordering task (MacDonald et al., 2001), which involves maintaining and manipulating information in WM (Hill et al., 2010).

Dependent Variables (Naming and Sentence Comprehension)

The Boston Naming Test (Kaplan et al., 1983) was selected to explore word retrieval by visual confrontation naming. Sentence comprehension was assessed by means of a sentence–picture simple verification task included in the ECCO_Senior test (Exploración Cognitiva de la Comprensión de Oraciones para mayores; English translation: Cognitive Assessment of Sentence Comprehension for seniors; López-Higes et al., 2012). This test includes 36 semantically reversible sentences that are either congruent or incongruent with the picture that accompanies them. Spanish is a Subject–Verb–Object (SVO) language (e.g., El hombre persigue al perro; English translation: The man chases the dog). Non-canonical word order (as for example in passive sentences: The man is kicked by the woman) or number of propositions in a sentence (one in The girl kissed her boyfriend vs. two in The woman kissed by her grandmother was sitting in a chair) are factors that makes sentences’ processing and interpretation more difficult (Thompson and Shapiro, 2007).

Procedure

An extensive neuropsychological assessment of each participant, including different cognitive domains (memory, executive functions, language), was conducted at two different times, one immediately after recruitment (baseline) and then 6 months later (post-training measure or endpoint). Neuropsychological assessments were conducted by an experienced psychologist or psychiatrist at the Center for the Cognitive Impairment Prevention. In the first session, the participants completed the screening tests (MMSE, GDS-15) and CRQ. In addition they were informed about the main goals of the study and signed an informed consent document. All the remaining neuropsychological and cognitive tests were applied in two additional sessions of 50 min each (approximately). Although there was a fixed block of tests arranged for each session, the order in which the tests were presented was randomized in each session for each participant. Tests were applied and scored following the instructions provided in the users’ manuals.

Statistical Analysis

First, we computed descriptive statistics by group across dependent variables in the baseline and in post-training phases and across five categories of outcomes (percentages). We conducted a repeated measures ANOVA to explore intragroup differences between the baseline and the endpoint across dependent variables. Differences between groups at the baseline across dependent measures or across other relevant factors (age, CR, etc.), as well as those taking into account a measure of training effects (differences between post- and pre-measure in each dependent variable), were computed by means of General Linear Model ANOVAs. Effect sizes were estimated by means of partial eta-square (ηp2); observed power was also given when is needed. In all the analyses we used IBM SPSS statistical program v. 22.0. Regarding predictor variables, we obtained a total score for each participant reflecting an estimation of her/his CR. With respect to digit reordering, we considered the number of series correctly ordered by each participant as a measure of his/her WM capacity (reflecting updating and monitoring of WM representations). With respect to TMT, we used the B/A ratio score (proposed by Lamberty et al., 1994) since it tries to eliminate the effect of the visual search and the perceptomotor speed also present in part B, to measure only the effect of the executive function, finding significant correlations with other tasks that measure alternation or task-switching ability (Arbuthnott and Frank, 2000). Other authors (Maruta et al., 2011) have considered that part B is a common and sensitive measurement of cognitive flexibility (see also Lezak, 1995). That is why we have assumed that the ratio B/A is a more fine-grained score of that function. Finally, with regards to the Stroop test, we used the Interference index proposed by Chafetz and Matthews (2004). In order to test the role of different predictors regarding CT outcomes in the language domain we have followed a procedure described in Mondini et al. (2016). The measures employed in the analyses resulted from subtracting the baseline scores to the endpoint scores in each of the selected dependent variables. All resulting models were compared using four indexes providing goodness-of-fit measures: Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayes Factor (BF), and R2. Higher BF and R2 values and lower BIC and AIC indexes, denote better model fit to data. BIC, AIC and R2 were computed using IBM SPSS statistical software v. 20.0, but the Bayes Factor index was computed with version 0.9.8 of the BayesFactor package developed by Perception and Cognition Lab, Department of Psychological Sciences at the University of Missouri1.

Results

Baseline Comparisons Between Groups

Table 2 shows means and standard deviations (SD) by group across language performance measures (pre and post).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Mean and standard deviation (in brackets) in dependent measures at the baseline (pre-) and at the endpoint (post-training) for both groups.

At the baseline the CI group presented a significant better performance in naming [F(1, 64) = 8.174; p = 0.006; ηp2 = 0.106; observed power = 0.805] than the SCD group. Differences in sentences not fitted to canonical word order in Spanish almost reached statistical significance [F(1, 64) = 3.724; p = 0.058; ηp2 = 0.051; observed power = 0.477].

Training Outcomes Differences

Intragroup analysis in the CI group showed that the difference between pre- and post-training measures in sentences including two propositions reached statistical significance [F(1, 30) = 5.025; p = 0.033; ηp2 = 0.145; observed power = 0.583]. However, in the SCD group the analysis revealed that performance in naming improved after cognitive training [F(1, 34) = 12.612; p = 0.001; ηp2 = 0.240; observed power = 0.934]. Effect sizes in both cases were large, but especially in SCD older adults.

No significant differences between groups were observed in any of the scores representing the effect of training (post – pre) in the three dependent measures.

Training Outcomes Categorization

Once differences in measures between post and pre-training were computed for all participants, it was possible to recode values in five categories of outcomes: (a) a positive value greater or equal to one SD was considered as moderate improvement, (b) positive values between 0 and 1 SD were mild improvement, (c) a value equal to zero meant null improvement, (d) negative values between 0 and 1 SD formed a category of mild worsening, and (e) negative values greater or equal to 1 SD were considered moderate worsening. Table 3 summarizes percentages in these five categories by group. Pearson Chi-square did not reach statistical significance in any case [χ2(4) = 0.664, p = 0.324; χ2(4) = 1.500, p = 0.827; χ2(4) = 4.857, p = 0.302].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Percentages of cases across outcome categories in CI and in SCD groups.

Prediction Models and Benefits in Linguistic Performance After CT

Regarding the main objective of the study, Table 4 shows that for CI participant’s performance on sentences not fitted to the canonical word order in Spanish the model with best goodness of fit was the 2nd (BIC = 124.01; AIC = 119.81; BF = 2.20; R2 = 0.15; p < 0.026), with TMT ratio as the only significant predictor (t = 2.26, p = 0.032). In SCD older adults the best model was the 7th, which includes CR and digit reordering (BIC = 127.87; AIC = 122.68; BF = 1.92; R2 = 0.11; p < 0.030). However, nor CR (t = -1.79, p = 0.086) neither digit reordering (t = 0.55, p = 0.583) reached statistical significance.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Linear regression models for sentences not fitted to canonical word order in Spanish included in ECCO_Senior.

Considering sentences with two propositions, the model with the best goodness-of-fit for the CI group was the 9th (Table 5) with TMT ratio and digit reordering as predictors (BIC = 130.15; AIC = 124.55; BF = 12.51; R2 = 0.34; p < 0.003). Both variables made a significant contribution to the model, but with different signs, thus in different directions (TMT ratio: t = 2.16, p = 0.039; digit reordering: t = -2.58, p = 0.016). Model 12th also had a good fit, and included as predictors CR, TMT ratio and digit reordering (BIC = 131.29; AIC = 124.28; BF = 10.62; R2 = 0.39; p < 0.003). CR did not contribute in a significant manner to the model (t = -1.43, p = 0.165), but TMT ratio and digit reordering did it in the same direction than in the previous model (TMT ratio: t = 2.28, p = 0.031; digit reordering: t = -2.32, p = 0.028; see Figure 2). In the SCD group the 1st model, with CR as the only predictor (see again Table 6), only approached significance (BIC = 126.31; AIC = 122.42; BF = 1.13; R2 = 0.08; p < 0.052). CR did not have a significant role (t = -1.58,p = 0.126).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Linear regression models for sentences with two propositions included in ECCO_Senior.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Cognitive reserve, Trail Making Test ratio (TMT ratio) and digit reordering effects on the difference in sentences with two propositions between the endpoint (Tl) and the baseline (TO) for the control group, according to model 12 (Table 5). Dotted lines delimit 95% confidence interval.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Linear regression models for the Boston Naming test.

Finally, when visual confrontation naming was considered (Table 6), the best model for the CI group was the 3rd (BIC = 143.11; AIC = 138.90; BF = 2.75; R2 = 0.17; p < 0.033), with interference as the only significant predictor (t = -2.40, p = 0.023). None of the regression models computed for this dependent measure in the SCD group reached statistical significance.

How Single Predictors Explain Language performance After CT

When sentences not fitted to canonical word order in Spanish are taken into account (see Table 4 again) there was a clear difference between groups, since in CI participants TMT ratio explains a 15% of criterion’s total variance, whereas CR was the variable which explains more than the other predictors (10%) in the SCD group. Regarding sentences with two propositions, digit reordering was the predictor with the highest explanation power in the CI group (it explains by itself 21% of the total variance), whereas CR (once again) explains only 8% (Table 5) in SCD participants. As Table 6 shows, Stroop interference was the predictor that best explained naming performance after CT in CI participants (17%), and consistently with previous results, CR explained 7% of criterion in the SCD group.

Discussion

The present study aims to draw some conclusions regarding UMAM benefits in the language area. Based on our results, we might say in first place that the UMAM program produces effects in abilities related to older people’s daily life, that is, in sentence reading comprehension and naming. Considering the percentages of moderate improvement (of greater relevance from a clinical point of view), as well as the resulting differences between groups in CT outcomes and intra-group differences between pre- and post-training measures, we can conclude that SCD is the group in which benefits are greater, confirming one of our previous hypothesis. This group considerably improves their results in naming (in which we have observed the largest effect size) and, in to a lesser extent, in sentences not fitted to canonical word order in Spanish at the post-treatment phase. It should be noted that these results are especially favorable for UMAM program, since the older adults with SCD had at baseline greater susceptibility to distraction or lesser efficiency in inhibiting irrelevant information (assessed with the Stroop test) than the CI group, as well as worse results in the two aforementioned indicators. In the post-training phase, the two groups showed an equivalent performance. Some studies have reported similar benefits in language comprehension, but after a verbal WM training program which was applied to CI older adults aged 65–75 years (Carretti et al., 2013). Those results are also in line with that obtained in the study conducted by Payne and Stine-Morrow (2017) using a novel home-based computerized CT program focused on verbal WM, and comparing CI older adults with a control group. The UMAM program includes vocabulary exercises, and other requiring reading texts or following instructions, which are all related to written language comprehension. These features can lead to positive training outcomes associated with sentence comprehension skills and naming, and could be considered as near transfer effects.

With respect to the main objective of the study, that is, to explore the role of CR and executive functions as factors which could modulate or predict the efficacy of the UMAM program in language (complex sentence comprehension and naming), there is a clear difference between the two groups. With respect to non-canonical sentence comprehension the results for the CI group emphasize that high TMT ratios (meaning low cognitive flexibility) are significantly related to high CT benefits (since t statistical test value was positive), and that this variable explains 15% of the total criterion variance. However, in the SCD group although we found a model including CR and digit reordering which explained 11% of the total variance, none of these variables reached statistical significance. Thus, the results show that CT outcomes regarding sentences not fitted to canonical word order in Spanish were higher for those CI older adults who had lower cognitive flexibility scores at baseline.

Furthermore, CI older adults who had higher TMT ratio scores (low cognitive flexibility) and lower digit reordering scores at baseline, also showed a significant improvement in their performance after CT in sentences with two propositions. These two factors explain 34% of the total variance (reaching 39% if CR is added), so their weight in sentence comprehension is moderately high. This result is consistent with that obtained in a good number of studies that show the important role of working memory in understanding complex sentences, especially linked to the number of propositions in the sentence (Waters and Caplan, 2004). In the SCD group, CR almost reached statistical significance for this type of sentences, presenting a slight tendency implying better training outcomes for participants with lower CR at baseline.

Regarding participants’ performance in naming, the results pointed out that CI older adults with lower inhibition efficiency at baseline had better results in BNT after CT, and that this factor explains 17% of the total variance associated with criterion. However, in the SCD group none of the models computed for this dependent measure reached statistical significance.

These results enable drawing another conclusion: executive functions at baseline (Ranganath et al., 2011) play a role as predictors of language performance after training, but only in CI older adults. The association between executive functions at the baseline and CT benefits is especially important in complex sentence comprehension (exemplars with two verbs or propositions) and, to a lesser extent, in naming, but restricted to older adults in the CI group. Selected predictors hardly explain SCD older adults’ post-training performance, so a substantial part of its variance remained unexplained, suggesting that other variables not considered in the study may account for it. This pattern of results contradicts, at least in part, our hypothesis regarding the role of executive functions in both groups as factors explaining language performance.

Although CT benefited language performance in older adults with SCD more than in CI participants, the chosen factors (executive functions) did not explain these benefits. On the contrary, the same factors predicted the benefits of CT for CI individuals. This is an paradoxical pattern of results that demand an explanation. It has been suggested that CT may cause a decrease in depressive symptomatology, or an improvement in general mood, well-being or motivation in SCD participants’ performance (Bherer, 2015). Therefore, the aforementioned changes, could be responsible for the observed improvement in the area of language in the group of older adults with SCD. Other authors have suggested that a change in lifestyle (variety of cognitive, physical, and social activities) associated to voluntary participation in training program has positive effects in global cognition and memory (Küster et al., 2016). Some studies have also found effects of multitasking training on processes such as perception and attention (Anguera et al., 2013), which are basic for others of higher level, so they could also be considered as factors to be taken into account. A complementary explanation arise when the pattern of differences across predictors (in isolation) are observed: (a) CR has a marginal weight as a factor explaining language performance in the SCD group (also in the CI group); (b) However, in the group of CI older adults two specific executive functions, alternation (cognitive flexibility) and updating and monitoring (evaluated by digit reordering), explain a significant portion of the variance associated to sentence comprehension. Inhibition efficiency, a common part for all the executive functions (following Miyake and Friedman model), explains a substantial portion of naming performance in this later group. Thus, it seems that UMAM program enhances CI older adults’ executive functioning, which benefits, in turn, CI participant’s performance in the linguistic tasks considered here (many studies have pointed out close relationships between executive functions and language, especially in the process of reanalysis of complex sentences; see for example: Caplan and Waters, 2013; Yoon et al., 2015). This would be an adequate explanation given that, as some authors maintain (for example: Lustig et al., 2009), intervention programs that incorporate multiple strategies and a great variety of tasks produce greater (near) transfer effects. It should bear in mind that the UMAM program includes vocabulary exercises, and other requiring reading texts or following instructions, which are all related to written language comprehension. In summary, four main conclusions can be drawn of this study. Firstly, the ECCO_Senior test has been revealed as a screening instrument with enough sensitivity to detect differences in written language comprehension among adults with and without SCD. Secondly, older adults with SCD obtain greater benefits in the language domain after training in comparison to CI older adults. However, near transfer effects to language in the elderly group SCD cannot be explained adequately with the predictors selected in this study. Thirdly, the UMAM program seems to enhance CI older adults’ executive functioning, which benefits, in turn, CI participants’ performance in sentence comprehension and naming. Finally, as we expected, CR has a marginal role as predictor of participants’ linguistic performance.

Since we have obtained results that contrast with other studies which have also considered cognitive functioning at baseline (Guye et al., 2017), future research should address if these differences might emerge as a function of the population considered, the type of training, or other factors such as motivation, etc. A possible line of future research would involve exploring the role of other factors/predictors such as those that have been suggested to explain the pattern of results obtained in the group of older adults with SCD; it would involve to study the changes that occur in mood, general well-being or daily activity (physical, social, intellectual) in this population for their participation in the UMAM program, as well as the relevance of these variables in the prediction of SCD participants’ performance in different cognitive domains, including language. Likewise, it would be interesting to explore the relationships between executive functions and language in cognitively intact older adults, checking if the training program produces a significant improvement in the executive functions (especially those which are more related to language), and if this is also related directly with an improvement in language skills.

The UMAM program has demonstrated its efficacy improving memory in older adults with and without SCD and in mild cognitive impairment. From a practical point of view the present results are useful since they allow us to know which factors explain to a large extent the benefits that the UMAM program has in the language area in cognitively intact older adults. On the other hand, the results found in the group of participants with SCD, which reveal important differences with CI older adults in relation to the mechanisms that underlie the benefits produced by the UMAM program in a specific area such as language, serve as an incentive to deepen the study of other predictors that provide an adequate explanation. Taken as a whole, the results of this study could be indicating the need to adapt the UMAM program in a flexible way to the characteristics of the users as a consequence of its location in the continuum that goes from normality to mild cognitive deterioration, and to the specific functions that are intended to maintain or improve.

Potential limitations of this study would be that the percentage of females were greater than the corresponding of males (66% vs. 34%). Similarly, the sample size could also be extended in future studies to strengthen conclusions.

Author Contributions

Study design and concept, as well as data analyses and interpretation were done by RL-H, JP, and SR-V. Critical revision of results and discussion were done by IR-R, JdF-L, MM, and PM. Evaluation and data collection were conducted by ML and DP.

Funding

This work was supported by the Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness and FEDER funds (PSI2015-68793-C3-3-R), by a predoctoral fellowship from the Spanish Ministry of Education, Culture and Sports (FPU13/02064) to IR-R, and by a predoctoral fellowship to JdF-L from La Caixa Foundation.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnote

  1. ^http://pcl.missouri.edu/bf-reg

References

Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., and Faraji, F. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501, 97–101. doi: 10.1038/nature12486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbuthnott, K., and Frank, J. (2000). Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 518–528. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;FT518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Au, J., Buschkuehl, M., Duncan, G. J., and Jaeggi, S. M. (2016). There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is NOT effective: a reply to Melby-Lervåg and Hulme (2015). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 331–337. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0967-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., and Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 366–377. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamidis, P. D., Fissler, P., Papageorgiou, S. G., Zilidou, V., Konstantinidis, E. I., Billis, A. S., et al. (2015). Gains in cognition through combined cognitive and physical training: the role of training dosage and severity of neurocognitive disorder. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:152. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bherer, L. (2015). Cognitive plasticity in older adults: effects of cognitive training and physical exercise. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1337, 1–6. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caplan, D., and Waters, G. (2013). Memory mechanisms supporting syntactic comprehension. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 243–268. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0369-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carretti, B., Borella, E., Zavagnin, M., and de Beni, R. (2013). Gains in language comprehension relating to working memory training in healthy older adults. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28, 539–546. doi: 10.1002/gps.3859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chafetz, M. D., and Matthews, L. H. (2004). A new interference score for the Stroop test. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 19, 555–567. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. O., Xu, H., Unverzagt, F. W., and Hendrie, H. (2016). Does targeted cognitive training reduce educational disparities in cognitive function among cognitively normal older adults? Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 31, 809–817. doi: 10.1002/gps.4395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test: Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Chicago, IL: Stoelting.

Google Scholar

Greenwood, P. M., and Parasuraman, R. (2016). The mechanisms of far transfer from cognitive training: review and hypothesis. Neuropsychology 30, 742–755. doi: 10.1037/neu0000235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, A. L., Parisi, J. M., Spira, A. P., Kueider, A. M., Ko, J. Y., Saczynski, J. S., Rebok, G. W. (2012). Memory training interventions for older adults: a meta-analysis. Aging Ment. Health 16, 722–734. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2012.667783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guye, S., De Simoni, C., and von Bastian, C. C. (2017). Do individual differences predict change in cognitive training performance? A latent growth curve modeling approach. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 1, 374–393.

Google Scholar

Hill, B., Elliott, E., Shelton, J., Pella, R., O’Jile, J., and Gouvier, W. (2010). Can we improve the clinical assessment of working memory? An evaluation of the Wechsler adult intelligence scale-third edition using a working memory criterion construct. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 32, 315–323. doi: 10.1080/13803390903032529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, J. J., Hill, P. L., Payne, B. R., Roberts, B. W., and Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2012). Can an old dog learn (and want to experience) new tricks? Cognitive training increases openness to experience in older adults. Psychol. Aging 27, 286–292. doi: 10.1037/a0025918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Bretelerc, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chételat, G., et al. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Karbach, J., Könen, T., and Spengler, M. (2017). Who benefits the most? Individual differences in the transfer of executive control training across the lifespan. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 1, 394–405. doi: 10.1007/s41465-017-0054-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karbach, J., and Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychol. Sci. 25, 2027–2037. doi: 10.1177/0956797614548725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraemer, H. C., Wilson, G. T., Fairburn, C. G., and Agras, W. S. (2002). Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 877–883. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.10.877

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Küster, O. C., Fissler, P., Laptinskaya, D., Thurm, F., Scharpf, A., Woll, A., et al. (2016). Cognitive change is more positively associated with an active lifestyle than with training interventions in older adults at risk of dementia: a controlled interventional clinical trial. BMC Psychiatry 16:315. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1018-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamberty, G. J., Putnam, S. H., Chatel, D. M., and Bieliauskas, L. A. (1994). Derived trail making test indices: a preliminary report. Neuropsychiatry. Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 7, 230–234.

Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lobo, A., Saz, P., Marcos, G., Día, J. L., De la Cámara, C., Ventura, T., et al. (1999). Revalidación y normalización del Mini-examen cognoscitivo (primera versión en castellano del Mini-mental status examination) en la población general geriátrica [Re-validation of the mini-examen cognoscitivo (first Spanish version of the mini-mental status examination) in elderly people]. Med. Clín. 112, 767–774.

Google Scholar

Lojo-Seoane, C., Facal, D., Guàrdia-Olmos, J., Pereiro, A. X., and Juncos-Rabadán, O. (2018). Effects of cognitive reserve on cognitive performance in a follow-up study in older adults with subjective cognitive complaints. The role of working memory. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:189. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Higes, R., Martín-Aragoneses, M. T., Rubio-Valdehita, S., Delgado-Losada, M. L., Montejo, P., Montenegro, M., et al. (2018). Efficacy of cognitive training in older adults with and without subjective cognitive decline is associated with inhibition efficiency and working memory span, not with cognitive reserve. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:23. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Higes, R., Rodríguez-Rojo, I. C., Prados, J. M., Montejo, P., Del Río, D., Delgado-Losada, M. L., et al. (2017). APOE ε4 modulation of training outcomes in several cognitive domains in a sample of cognitively intact older adults. J. Alzheimers Dis. 58, 1201–1215. doi: 10.3233/JAD-161014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Higes, R., Rubio, S., Martín-Aragoneses, M. T., Del Río, D., and Mejuto, G. (2012). Evaluación de la comprensión gramatical en el envejecimiento normal y patológico: un resumen de los resultados obtenidos con las baterías ECCO y ECCO-Senior [assessment of grammar comprehension in normal and pathological aging: a summary of the results obtained with ECCO and ECCO_senior tests]. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 5, 96–108. doi: 10.21500/20112084.766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lustig, C., Shah, P., Seidler, R., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2009). Aging, training, and the brain: a review and future directions. Neuropsychol. Rev. 19, 504–522. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9119-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, M. C., Almor, A., Henderson, V. W., Kempler, D., and Andersen, E. S. (2001). Assessing working memory and language comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Lang. 78, 17–42. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, M., Clare, L., Altgassen, A. M., Cameron, M. H., Zehnder, F. (2011). Cognition-based interventions for healthy older people and people with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 1, 1–48. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD006220.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maruta, C., Guerreiro, M., de Mendonca, A., Hort, J., and Scheltens, P. (2011). The use of neuropsychological tests across Europe: the need for a consensus in the use of assessment tools for dementia. Eur. J. Neurol. 18, 279–285. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2010.03134.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melby-Lervåg, M., and Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 49,270–291. doi: 10.1037/a0028228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mondini, S., Madella, I., Zangrossi, A., Bigolin, A., Tomasi, C., Michieletto, M., et al. (2016). Cognitive reserve in dementia: implications for cognitive training. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:84. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montejo, P. (2003). Multifactorial memory training programme in elderly with memory impairment: results and predictors. Rev. Esp. Geriatr. Gerontol. 38, 316–326. doi: 10.1016/S0211-139X(03)74908-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montejo, P. (2015). Estudio de los Resultados de un Programa de Entrenamiento de Memoria y Estimulación Cognitiva para Mayores de 65 años Sin Deterioro Cognitivo. Ph.D. thesis, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid.

Google Scholar

Montejo, P., Montenegro, M., Reinoso, A. I., De Andrés, M. E., and Claver, M. D. (2013). El Método Umam de Entrenamiento de Memoria [The Umam Memory Training Method]. Available at http://www.madridsalud.es/centros/monograficos/deterioro/UMAM.pdf

Google Scholar

Mowszowski, L., Lampit, A., Walton, C. C., and Naismith, S. L. (2016). Strategy-based cognitive training for improving executive functions in older adults: a systematic review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 26, 252–270. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9329-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. R., and Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2017). The effects of home-based cognitive training on verbal working memory and language comprehension in older adulthood. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:256. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahe, J., Petrelli, A., Kaesberg, S., Fink, G. R., Kessler, J., and Kalbe, E. (2015). Effects of cognitive training with additional physical activity compared to pure cognitive training in healthy older adults. Clin. Interv. Aging 10, 297–310. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S74071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rami, L., Valls-Pedret, C., Bartrés-Faz, D., Caprile, C., Solé-Padullés, C., Castellvi, M., et al. (2011). Cuestionario de reserva cognitiva. valores obtenidos en población anciana sana y con enfermedad de alzheimer [cognitive reserve questionnaire. scores obtained in a healthy elderly population and in one with Alzheimer’s disease]. Rev. Neurol. 52, 195–201.

Google Scholar

Ranganath, C., Flegal, K. E., and Kelly, L. L. (2011). Can cognitive training improve episodic memory? Neuron 72, 688–691. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rebok, G. W., Ball, K., Guey, L. T., Jones, R. N., Kim, H.-Y., King, J. W., et al. (2014). Ten-year effects of the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly cognitive training trial on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 62, 16–24. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1994). Ward Halstead’s contributions to neuropsychology and the Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery. J. Clin. Psychol. 50, 47–70. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199401)50:1<47::AID-JCLP2270500106>3.0.CO;2-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosi, A., Del Signore, F., Canelli, E., Allegri, N., Bottiroli, S., Vecchi, T., et al. (2017). The effect of strategic memory training in older adults: who benefits most? Int. Psychoger. doi: 10.1017/S1041610217002691. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheikh, J. I., and Yesavage, J. A. (1986). “Geriatric depression scale (GDS). Recent evidence and development of a shorter version,” in Clinical Gerontology: a Guide to Assessment and Intervention, ed. T. L. Brink (New York, NY: The Haworth Press), 165–173.

Google Scholar

Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17, 103–186. doi: 10.1177/1529100616661983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smart, C. M., Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rabin, L. A., Hudon, C., Gates, N., et al. (2017). Non-pharmacologic interventions for older adults with subjective cognitive decline: systematic review, meta-analysis, and preliminary recommendations. Neuropsychol. Rev. 27, 245–257 doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9342-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, C. K., and Shapiro, L. P. (2007). Complexity in treatment of syntactic deficits. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 16, 30–42. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2007/005)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waters, G. S., and Caplan, D. (2004). Verbal working memory and on-line syntactic processing: evidence from self-paced listening. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 57, 129–163. doi: 10.1080/02724980343000170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997). WMS-III. Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Willis, S. L., and Caskie, G. I. L. (2013). Reasoning training in the ACTIVE study: how much is needed and who benefits? J. Aging Health 25. doi: 10.1177/0898264313503987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, J., Campanelli, L., Goral, M., Marton, K., Eichorn, N., and Obler, L. K. (2015). The effect of plausibility on sentence comprehension among older adults and its relation to cognitive functions. Exp. Aging Res. 41, 272–302. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2015.1021646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: subjective cognitive decline, older adults, cognitive training, sentence comprehension, naming, executive functions, working memory, cognitive reserve

Citation: López-Higes R, Prados JM, Rubio-Valdehita S, Rodríguez-Rojo I, de Frutos-Lucas J, Montenegro M, Montejo P, Prada D and Losada MLD (2018) Factors Explaining Language Performance After Training in Elders With and Without Subjective Cognitive Decline. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:264. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00264

Received: 12 April 2018; Accepted: 14 August 2018;
Published: 03 September 2018.

Edited by:

P. Hemachandra Reddy, Texas Tech University Health Sciences Center, United States

Reviewed by:

Patrizia Giannoni, University of Nîmes, France
Bhoomika Kar, Allahabad University, India

Copyright © 2018 López-Higes, Prados, Rubio-Valdehita, Rodríguez-Rojo, de Frutos-Lucas, Montenegro, Montejo, Prada and Losada. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ramón López-Higes, rlopezsa@psi.ucm.es

Download