Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 12 March 2019
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 11 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00046

Moderating Effect of Cortical Thickness on BOLD Signal Variability Age-Related Changes

  • 1School of Biomedical Engineering, Western University, London, ON, Canada
  • 2Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Western University, London, ON, Canada
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Clinical Neurological Sciences, Schulich School of Medicine, Western University, London, ON, Canada

The time course of neuroanatomical structural and functional measures across the lifespan is commonly reported in association with aging. Blood oxygen-level dependent signal variability, estimated using the standard deviation of the signal, or “BOLDSD,” is an emerging metric of variability in neural processing, and has been shown to be positively correlated with cognitive flexibility. Generally, BOLDSD is reported to decrease with aging, and is thought to reflect age-related cognitive decline. Additionally, it is well established that normative aging is associated with structural changes in brain regions, and that these predict functional decline in various cognitive domains. Nevertheless, the interaction between alterations in cortical morphology and BOLDSD changes has not been modeled quantitatively. The objective of the current study was to investigate the influence of cortical morphology metrics [i.e., cortical thickness (CT), gray matter (GM) volume, and cortical area (CA)] on age-related BOLDSD changes by treating these cortical morphology metrics as possible physiological confounds using linear mixed models. We studied these metrics in 28 healthy older subjects scanned twice at approximately 2.5 years interval. Results show that BOLDSD is confounded by cortical morphology metrics. Respectively, changes in CT but not GM volume nor CA, show a significant interaction with BOLDSD alterations. Our study highlights that CT changes should be considered when evaluating BOLDSD alternations in the lifespan.

Introduction

Normal aging is associated with marked functional and structural neuroanatomical alterations in cortical thickness (CT), gyrification, cortical surface area (CA), gray (GM), and white matter volume (WM) (Salat et al., 2009; Thambisetty et al., 2010; McGinnis et al., 2011; Hogstrom et al., 2013). Importantly, magnetic resonance imaging studies (MRI) show that the magnitude and rate of change of these cortical morphometry metrics is not constant across the cortex but rather it varies with age and brain region (Raz, 2005; Jiang et al., 2014; Storsve et al., 2014; Dotson et al., 2016), and is reported to accelerate with increasing age (Driscoll et al., 2009; Jiang et al., 2014). For example, a longitudinal study of alterations in cortical morphometry in older adults found accelerated changes with increasing age in temporal and occipital cortices (Storsve et al., 2014). Furthermore, other studies report that the temporal lobes are most vulnerable to age-related morphometric changes, and that these changes reflect age-related cognitive impairment (Singh et al., 2006; Fjell et al., 2009; Pacheco et al., 2015). There is considerable evidence that neuroanatomical alterations reflect underlying functional alterations, especially in cognition (Fjell et al., 2006; Rossini et al., 2007; Ziegler et al., 2010; Achiron et al., 2013). In fact, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, which rely on the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal as a correlate of neuronal activity, report that changes in cortical morphology across adult lifespan impact the hemodynamic properties of the brain. For example, there are cortical laminar differences in BOLD signal (Tian et al., 2010; Huber et al., 2015), thicker cortical regions were reported to have a lower relative oxygen extraction fraction (Zhao et al., 2016). Therefore, since aging is associated with significant neuroanatomical alterations, these should be considered when assessing function (i.e., cognitive ability) using the BOLD signal.

The Standard Deviation of the BOLD signal can be used to estimate variability (hereafter, “BOLDSD”), and is believed to reflect the brain’s dynamic ability to undergo fast moment-to-moment switching through network reconfigurations (Garrett et al., 2010, 2014; Grady and Garrett, 2014). It is an emerging index of cognitive health in aging, with higher regional BOLDSD being associated with enhanced performance on certain cognitive tasks (i.e., task switching) but not on others (i.e., distractor inhibition) (Armbruster-Genc et al., 2016; Guitart-Masip et al., 2016). Generally, increased BOLDSD is associated with younger age, faster and more consistent performance on cognitive tasks, and cognitive flexibility (Garrett et al., 2013a; Armbruster-Genc et al., 2016). The physiological mechanisms underlying BOLDSD remain largely unknown. For instance, decreased dopaminergic transmission is proposed to be associated with decreased BOLDSD in subcortical areas in older adults compared to younger ones (Guitart-Masip et al., 2016). Another study reported that higher BOLDSD is associated with superior pain coping capabilities (Rogachov et al., 2016). However, there are few studies investigating the interaction between age-related alterations in cortical morphology and BOLDSD. One study reported that increased microstructural integrity of WM pathways is associated with greater BOLDSD (Burzynska et al., 2015). Given that neuroanatomical alterations associated with normative aging are known to influence cognitive performance, and that they influence the BOLD signal, their impact as physiological confounds to BOLDSD should be investigated. This is particularly relevant because there is a considerable degree of inconsistency of methods used and results across studies investigating BOLDSD. In fact, some studies report greater regional BOLDSD in older adults (Garrett et al., 2010, 2011; Nomi et al., 2017), individuals with stroke (Kielar et al., 2016), multiple sclerosis (Petracca et al., 2017), Alzheimer disease (Makedonov et al., 2013, 2016; Scarapicchia et al., 2018) and other neurological disorders (Zöller et al., 2017).

The objective of this study is to investigate the contribution of cortical morphology (i.e., CT, CA, and GM volume) to age-related BOLDSD changes in older adults. A longitudinal framework, consisting of two scan points, should help reduce some of the inter-individual variance in neuroanatomy by accounting for external factors such as lifestyle, and various socio-economic and demographic factors. To this end, we investigated global and regional differences in BOLDSD in a group of older adults scanned twice at an interval of approximately 2.5 years, and regressed the effect of cortical morphology by introducing CT, CA, and GM as covariates. We hypothesized that cortical morphology metrics show an interaction with BOLDSD. We predict that age-related neuroanatomical alterations in CT, CA, GM are physiological confounds to BOLDSD measures, and that consequently adjusting for these metrics may help “unmask” the functional relevance of BOLDSD.

Materials and Methods

Participants

All data obtained for the present study were obtained from the longitudinal Geneva Aging Study, after approval by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the University of Geneva and the Swiss Ethic committee. Older subjects were initially recruited either from the University of the Third Age of Geneva or through newspaper and association advertisements for pensioners, as part of a larger longitudinal study. All participants gave written informed consent and older adults received a small amount of money as a compensation for their transportation fees.

Our initial sample consisted of 31 older adults scanned twice (mean age at first scan = 71.65 ± 6.03 years, mean age at second scan = 74.06 ± 5.99 years; 9 males). These subjects were chosen, within our pool, because they were the only ones that had undergone two T1 structural images and task fMRI scans, as well as other cognitive tests. Participants were screened for health problems with a questionnaire. The structural MRIs were inspected to rule out severe abnormalities (white matter changes, ventricular enlargement, tumors etc.). Three of the participants showed signs of Parkinson or lesion on their anatomical MRI, so they were excluded from the final sample (n = 28). All models and results in this current paper thus reflect 28 older adults (mean age at first scan = 71.61 ± 6.21 years, mean age at second scan = 74.07 ± 6.15 years; 7 males). The scans were 2.46 ± 0.69 years apart.

MRI Data Acquisition

Participants were scanned in a Siemens Trio 3T magnet. A BOLD fMRI task-rest sequence was administered using a reaction time paradigm, where the participant had to indicate on which side a cross was changing into a square, as fast as possible (Mella et al., 2013). The task consisted of eight experimental blocks, interspersed with eight resting blocks (respectively, 52–20 s). The BOLD activity was obtained using an echo planar imaging acquisition (echo time, TE = 30 ms, time repetition, TR = 2100 ms, flip angle = 80°, field of view, FOV = 205 mm). Then, a structural T1-weighted MRI was acquired (TE = 2.27 ms, TR = 1900 ms, FOV = 256 mm, voxel size 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm).

Structural Data Processing

Structural T1-weighted MR images were analyzed using Freesurfer version 6.0, a widely used and freely available automated processing pipeline1, which allows surface-based three dimensional reconstruction and quantification of cortical morphology. The standard steps for analysis were implemented (using “recon-all” pipeline with the default set of parameters). Regional measures of GM, CT, and CA for each hemisphere were obtained using the automated anatomic parcellation procedure. Technical details are found in prior publications (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999a,b, 2001; Fischl and Dale, 2000; Zhang et al., 2001; Salat et al., 2004; Winkler et al., 2012). In brief, T1-weighted images underwent preprocessing steps including motion correction, brain extraction, intensity normalization, and Talairach transformation (Sled et al., 1998; Smith, 2002; Ségonne et al., 2004; Reuter et al., 2010). GM and WM surface boundaries were reconstructed to estimate the distance between them across the cortex (Fischl and Dale, 2000). The generated cortical models were inflated into spheres to be registered to a spherical atlas and parcellated into regions of interest using Destrieux atlas (Fischl and Dale, 2000; Segonne et al., 2007; Destrieux et al., 2010).

Functional Data Processing

Processing of the functional data were performed using FSL version 5.0 (Analysis Group, FMRIB, Oxford, United Kingdom (Smith et al., 2004). Standard preprocessing were followed using FSL’s FEAT and FSL’s Melodic for functional data (Jenkinson et al., 2012). Briefly, for each participant preprocessing steps included motion correction, slice timing, spatial normalization, highpass temporal filtering (100 s), smoothing (kernel 5 mm FWHM), and linear registration (12 degrees of freedom) of the functional data to the high-resolution T1 structural image, and from T1 to 1 mm standard space (MNI 152). Additionally, FSL’s Melodic was used to correct each functional image for artifacts using automatic dimensionality estimation via independent component analysis (ICA) (Beckmann and Smith, 2004). Change in cortical morphology between the two scanning sessions was determined as cortical morphology metrics GM (delta.volume), CT (delta.thickness), CA (delta.area) at timepoint 2–at timepoint 1.

BOLD Signal Variability Calculation

As part of the Geneva Dataset, the subjects were performing different cognitive tasks, and for the current study, only the fixation/rest blocks from the block design task fMRI were selected to calculate BOLDSD, as previously described (Garrett et al., 2010). BOLDSD analysis was restricted to the GM using participant specific GM mask obtained from FSL’s FAST. First, fixation blocks were normalized so that the overall four-dimensional mean across brain and block was 100. Next, for each voxel, the block mean was subtracted to remove block-wise drift, followed by concatenation of all blocks. The standard deviation of the normalized mean of the concatenated fixation blocks was used to obtain BOLDSD values for each brain region (n = 148) of each subject, as defined by the Destrieux Atlas (Destrieux et al., 2010), using in-house MATLAB code. Change (delta.variability) in variability between the two scanning sessions was calculated as variability at timepoint 2–variability at timepoint 1. BOLDSD encompassing both timepoints was introduced as “variability” (see section “Regional Model”).

Statistical Analysis

Linear mixed effects models (LMMs) were used to investigate the potential confounding effect of cortical morphology, GM, CT, CA on BOLDSD (Bates, 2005; Zuur et al., 2011).The LMMs allow estimation of the effects of explanatory variables (“fixed effects”) and their interactions on the dependent variable (i.e., BOLDSD), while statistically controlling for the effects of randomly selected participants (“random effects”) on the dependent variable (BOLDSD). Multiple models were run and the likelihood-ratio test was used to (1) investigate if introducing subjects as random effects improves the fit of the model (2) to select the optimal combination of fixed effects fitted with maximum likelihood, while keeping the random effects structure the same. Therefore, the likelihood-ratio test via ANOVA was used to compare the goodness of fit of different models. R statistical software package (R Core Team, 2013)2 was used for all statistical analyses. Correlation between cortical morphology measures were computed using cor function in R. All models were fitted using the “lmer” or “lm” function in R. “lmerTest” R package was used to obtain summary table and p-values for linear mixed models via Satterthwaite’s degrees of freedom method (Kuznetsova et al., 2017). A spatio-temporal approach to LMMs allowed characterization of regionally specific variation across the brain (Bernal-Rusiel et al., 2013). This approach was implemented to investigate if there is a significant change in BOLD variability across time (1) all cortical regions (2) region specific. Random effect structure with subjects varying in their “baseline” BOLD variability was retained (random intercept, 1| ID). Additionally, to model a different rate of change in the expected response levels, time varying predictors were introduced by random slopes (i.e., thickness, time) (“Regional Model”). To reduce spatial correlation issues an LMM model with the described structure was applied at each spatial location (i.e., region of interest) independently. Each model produced a parameter which quantifies the mean change in BOLD variability (delta.variability) for that region. P-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate (FDR) at q = 0.05 (Benjamini and Yekutieli, 2001).

Models

Model 1 < lm (delta.variability∼ region).

Model 2 < - lmer [delta.variability∼ region+(1| ID), REML = F].

Model 3 < - lmer [delta.variability ∼ delta.thickness + region+(1| ID), REML = F].

Model 4 < - lmer [delta.variability ∼ delta.area + region + (1| ID), REML = F].

Model 5 < - lmer [delta.variability ∼ delta.thickness + delta.area + region+(1| ID), REML = F].

Model 6 < - delta.variability ∼ delta.volume + region + (1 | ID).

Final Model

lmer [delta.variability∼ delta.thickness + region+(1| ID)].

Regional Model

lmer [variability∼ time + thickness + (1+thickness + time| ID)].

Results

Relations Between Cortical Thickness and BOLD Signal Variability Age-Related Changes

The functional and structural data of 28 participants was assessed. As expected, our study showed that CA and GM are highly correlated r = 0.904, p < 0.001 (95% Cl 0.901–0.908) and consequently collinearity was suspected. Multiple LMMs were utilized to assess the effect of neuroanatomical metrics CT, CA, and GM on BOLDSD age-related changes. Results from the linear mixed effect models run with likelihood-ratio test via ANOVA, are presented in Table 1. (A) indicates that subject intercept should be introduced as a random effect, while (B–E) show the steps that have led to the final model. Specifically, Table 1. (B,C) indicate that introducing CT or CA as covariates, separately, each significantly improve the fit of the model p < 0.0001, p < 0.05, respectively. However, from (D) it is apparent that adding CA to a model that already has CT as a covariate does not improve the fit of the model, meaning that CT only should be included in the final model (see section “Final Model”). (E) indicates that introducing GM as a covariate does not improve the fit of the model. Neither CT, GM, nor CA mean changes were significant as tested with LMMs. Regional Model LMM indicated that neither overall nor regionally specific mean BOLDSD change was significant after FDR correction.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results from likelihood-ratio test via ANOVA for model comparison (fit with “lmer” function in R).

Discussion

Cortical Thickness and Its Association With BOLDSD

In this study we aimed at determining the contribution of cortical morphology to BOLDSD in order to better understand the physiological nature of brain function capture by BOLDSD. BOLDSD changes across the lifespan have been shown to be robust to certain vascular factor such as cerebral blood flow, BOLD cerebrovascular reactivity, maximal BOLD signal change (Garrett et al., 2017), as well as GM volume changes (Nomi et al., 2017). However, using LMMs, we found that functional alterations in aging as captured by BOLDSD are confounded by the structural metric CT. This is not surprising, considering that it is well known that BOLD signal change/activation is dependent on the laminar organization of the cortex, and consequently is influenced by its depth or thickness (Koopmans et al., 2010; Tian et al., 2010; Bandettini, 2012). In fact, neurovascular coupling is reported to vary by cortical depth and layer (Goense et al., 2012).

In a longitudinal study investigating CT, GM volume, CA across the lifespan, Storsve et al. (2014) reported cortical morphology metric specific and region specific rates of mean annual percentage change (APC) in healthy adults aged 23–87 years. In most regions, GM volume has a mean APC of –0.51, CT of –0.35, and CA of –0.19. Other longitudinal studies in older adults, report similar reductions: in GM volume, mean APC ranging from –0.5 to –2.1 ± 1.6% (Tang et al., 2001; Fjell et al., 2009), in CT, mean APC –0.3% (Shaw et al., 2016), in WM tracts, mean APC ranging from –0.20 to –0.65 depending on the tract location (Storsve et al., 2016). In our sample, the change in cortical morphology from scan 1 to scan 2 (2.5 years apart) was not statistically significant. However, the findings reported by these longitudinal studies suggest that while the neuroanatomical alterations may be too subtle to reach statistically significance in the investigated timespan of 2.5 years, they may still contribute to BOLDSD age-related findings. Particularly, accounting for CT age-related changes may help “unmask” the functional value of BOLDSD.

While the relationship between cortical morphology metrics in aging is dynamic, GM volume changes were largely accounted by changes in CT rather than CA, highlighting the importance of tracking changes in CT (Storsve et al., 2014). In fact, studies show that CT and CA are genetically independent (Panizzon et al., 2009). Their neurodevelopment in the lifespan is largely independent of each other suggesting that they should be considered as separate metrics with different contributions to cortical volume (Im et al., 2008; Winkler et al., 2010; Eyler et al., 2011; Lemaitre et al., 2012; Storsve et al., 2014). Furthermore, studies show that that some cortices experience decrease in cortical morphology metrics at a higher rate than others (Storsve et al., 2014).

The hemodynamic properties of each brain region are highly correlated with its cortical structure (Ulrich and Yablonskiy, 2016; Wen et al., 2018). Studies based on quantitative gradient recalled echo (qGRE) analysis use the GRE signal decay rate parameter (R2t) to gather information on tissue-specific contributions to the fMRI signal. Regional R2t metric variations are associated with variations in neuronal density and myelination (positive correlation), as well as glial cells and synapses concentration (i.e., negative correlation) (Wen et al., 2018). In normal aging R2t increases in all cortical regions (Zhao et al., 2016). Thicker regions show the opposite pattern, respectively, decreased neuronal density, and higher concentration of glial cells and synapses relative to neurons. Thicker areas also tend to extract less oxygen from the blood, as measured by oxygen extraction ratios (i.e., expressed as local-to-global ratio). These findings indicate that laminar differences in cellular content impact neurovascular coupling mechanisms, which in turn may compromise the power of BOLDSD measurements to detect “real” changes in neuronal variability processing (Harris et al., 2011). Although, laminar differences in BOLDSD remain rather elusive, our study suggests the that CT should be considered in BOLD variability studies.

Age-Related Changes in BOLDSD

In our study, we did not find a significant change in BOLDSD, likely due to the low timespan between scans (2.5 years), and relatively low sample (n = 28). Overall, most studies reporting a decrease in BOLDSD suggest that this finding may indicate structural reductions in synaptic complexity and integrity, as well as functional decline in neural optimization and flexibility (Garrett et al., 2010). Burzynska et al. (2015) reported a positive correlation between increased microstructural integrity of WM and BOLDSD in healthy adults, consequently warranting the consideration of structural alterations in variability studies. Nomi et al. (2017) found increases in variability in bilateral anterior insula, anterior cingulate and ventral temporal cortex, and decreases in BOLDSD in sensory brain regions and other subcortical areas, in participants with ages ranging from 6 to 86 years. In their study they accounted for GM volume but not CT or CA. Two seminal studies investigated BOLDSD differences in aging alone (Garrett et al., 2010) and in relation to performance on cognitive tasks (Garrett et al., 2011) between a young group of participants (20–30 years) and an older one (56–85 years) and found both increases and decreases in regional variability with younger age alone, and younger age and better performance (Garrett et al., 2011) making it rather difficult to isolate specific key contributing regions. Most regions in these two studies showed the same trend but there were some inconsistencies. For example, superior frontal gyrus was reported to show greater variability with younger age and better performance (Garrett et al., 2011) in one study, while another one reported that its variability increases with age (Garrett et al., 2010). CT was not considered in any of these studies. The relationship between cognitive performance and/or flexibility and BOLDSD is definitely complex and task dependent. Behavioral studies indicate age-related differences in intra-individual variability on cognitive performance tests involving reaction time, and working memory. Older adults showing higher intra-individual variability on reaction time tests than younger adults, while the opposite is observed on working memory tests (Fagot et al., 2018). Additionally, the relationship between BOLDSD change, aging and cognitive health has not been a consistent finding. Some studies have found increased BOLD in healthy older adults (Baum and Beauchamp, 2014), Alzheimer’s (Scarapicchia et al., 2018), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Nomi et al., 2018), and other neurological disorders (Zöller et al., 2017).

Other Possible Confounds in BOLDSD Studies

Most studies investigating BOLD variability utilized a cross-sectional design while we used a within-subject design. This design is a highlight of our study because it allows for the investigation of normative age-related differences, specifically intra-individual effects of the processing of aging on cortical morphology and BOLDSD, rather than process of aging simply age differences across groups. Importantly, the within-subject design helps account for numerous sources of individual differences that may affect BOLD variability such as differences in dopaminergic neurotransmission (Guitart-Masip et al., 2016; Alavash et al., 2018), significant differences in vascular features, pain sensitivity and coping (Rogachov et al., 2016), clinical symptomology (Ke et al., 2015; Nomi et al., 2018; Zöller et al., 2018), and even differences in tendency for financial risk taking (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2010). Given that in our study the participants were scanned at an interval of approximately 2.5 years, we were able to at least partially account for such factors. Furthermore, it is clear from the literature that there are numerous environmental factors [i.e., socioeconomic status, education (Chan et al., 2018)] that may contribute to individual neuroanatomical alterations, which in turn may confound BOLD variability studies. This further supports the findings of our study, specifically that CT is a neuroanatomical metric that should be accounted for.

Lastly, there may be inconsistency in results between studies due to: (1) using task-fMRI and resting-state fMRI, (2) calculation of BOLD signal variability using standard deviation vs. mean-square successive difference) (for review, Garrett et al., 2013b), (3) type of statistical analysis performed (e.g., partial least squares method, LMM, general linear model), and (4) not accounting for contribution of CT and the other mentioned sources of individual differences.

Study Limitations

The main limitations, as discussed above and further addressed by Garrett et al. (2013b); Scarapicchia et al. (2018), are the lack of standardization in acquiring and analyzing the fMRI data. Additionally, the sample used in our study consists of a relatively small sample of older adults and rather short scan-rescan time, consequently this limits our ability to make strong generalization to other age groups or longer time spans, respectively. Nevertheless, since studies show that the brain undergoes extensive annual structural alterations across the lifespan at different rates, our finding that CT contributes to BOLDSD alterations, remains an important consideration.

Conclusion

In conclusion, contrary to a view that BOLD variability is just “noise,” we consider it to be emerging as an important metric of normal aging. Keeping in mind that across the lifespan there are considerable cortical morphometry alterations and that cortical depth affects the BOLD signal, we reported that CT contributes to BOLDSD changes, in an older sample of health adults. An increasing number of studies are considering healthy regional BOLDSD changes in the context of functional networks (Rogachov et al., 2016; Nomi et al., 2017). Pursuing this direction while accounting for CT and other possible confounding factors such as dopaminergic neurotransmission, socioeconomic background etc., should reveal new insights into the mechanisms behind age-related neural processes.

Data Availability

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript and/or the supplementary files.

Author Contributions

DP, RE, and SR conceived and designed this specific project on the data collected by NM and AR. DP analyzed the data. All co-authors participated in writing of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the SNF (Swiss National Foundation, Grant Nos. 100014_135410, PMPDP1 171335, and PMPDP1 158319).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants of the Geneva Aging Study. We also thank the Department of Statistical and Actuarial Sciences at Western University for helpful discussions and advice on data analysis and statistical modeling.

Footnotes

  1. ^http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/
  2. ^www.R-project.org

References

Achiron, A., Chapman, J., Tal, S., Bercovich, E., Gil, H., and Achiron, A. (2013). Superior temporal gyrus thickness correlates with cognitive performance in multiple sclerosis. Brain Struct. Funct. 218, 943–950. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0440-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alavash, M., Lim, S. J., Thiel, C., Sehm, B., Deserno, L., and Obleser, J. (2018). Dopaminergic modulation of hemodynamic signal variability and the functional connectome during cognitive performance. Neuroimage 172, 341–356. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armbruster-Genc, D. J. N., Ueltzhoffer, K., and Fiebach, C. J. (2016). Brain signal variability differentially affects cognitive flexibility and cognitive stability. J. Neurosci. 36, 3978–3987. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2517-14.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandettini, P. A. (2012). The BOLD plot thickens: sign- and layer-dependent hemodynamic changes with activation. Neuron 76, 468–469. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D. (2005). Fitting linear mixed models in R. R News 5, 27–30. doi: 10.1159/000323281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baum, S. H., and Beauchamp, M. S. (2014). Greater BOLD variability in older compared with younger adults during audiovisual speech perception. PLoS One 9:e111121. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, C. F., and Smith, S. M. (2004). Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 23, 137–152. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Yekutieli, D. (2001). The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. Ann. Stat. 29, 1165–1188. doi: 10.1214/aos/1013699998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernal-Rusiel, J. L., Reuter, M., Greve, D. N., Fischl, B., and Sabuncu, M. R. (2013). Spatiotemporal linear mixed effects modeling for the mass-univariate analysis of longitudinal neuroimage data. Neuroimage 81, 358–370. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burzynska, A. Z., Wong, C. N., Voss, M. W., Cooke, G. E., McAuley, E., and Kramer, A. F. (2015). White matter integrity supports BOLD signal variability and cognitive performance in the aging human brain. PLoS One 10:e0120315. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, M. Y., Na, J., Agres, P. F., Savalia, N. K., Park, D. C., and Wig, G. S. (2018). Socioeconomic status moderates age-related differences in the brain’s functional network organization and anatomy across the adult lifespan. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, E5144–E5153. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1714021115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Destrieux, C., Fischl, B., Dale, A., and Halgren, E. (2010). Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. Neuroimage 53, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotson, V. M., Szymkowicz, S. M., Sozda, C. N., Kirton, J. W., Green, M. L., O’Shea, A., et al. (2016). Age differences in prefrontal surface area and thickness in middle aged to older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:250. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Driscoll, I., Davatzikos, C., An, Y., Wu, X., Shen, D., Kraut, M., et al. (2009). Longitudinal pattern of regional brain volume change differentiates normal aging from MCI. Neurology 72, 1906–1913. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a82634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eyler, L. T., Prom-Wormley, E., Panizzon, M. S., Kaup, A. R., Fennema-Notestine, C., Neale, M. C., et al. (2011). Genetic and environmental contributions to regional cortical surface area in humans: a magnetic resonance imaging twin study. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2313–2321. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagot, D., Mella, N., Borella, E., Ghisletta, P., Lecerf, T., and de Ribaupierre, A. (2018). Intra-individual variability from a lifespan perspective: a comparison of latency and accuracy measures. J. Intell. 6, 1–18. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence6010016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., and Dale, A. M. (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Liu, A., and Dale, A. M. (2001). Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 20, 70–80. doi: 10.1109/42.906426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., and Dale, A. M. (1999a). Cortical surface-based analysis. II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B., and Dale, A. M. (1999b). High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 272–284.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Fennema-Notestine, C., McEvoy, L. K., Hagler, D. J., Holland, D., et al. (2009). One-year brain atrophy evident in healthy aging. J. Neurosci. 29, 15223–15231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3252-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Reinvang, I., Lundervold, A., Salat, D., Quinn, B. T., et al. (2006). Selective increase of cortical thickness in high-performing elderly - structural indices of optimal cognitive aging. Neuroimage 29, 984–994. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2010). Blood oxygen level-dependent signal variability is more than just noise. J. Neurosci. 30, 4914–4921. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5166-09.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2011). The importance of being variable. J. Neurosci. 31, 4496–4503. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5641-10.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2013a). The modulation of BOLD variability between cognitive states varies by age and processing speed. Cereb. Cortex 23, 684–693. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., MacDonald, S. W. S., Lindenberger, U., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2013b). Moment-to-moment brain signal variability: a next frontier in human brain mapping? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 610–624. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Lindenberger, U., Hoge, R. D., and Gauthier, C. J. (2017). Age differences in brain signal variability are robust to multiple vascular controls. Sci. Rep. 7:10149. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-09752-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2014). Brain signal variability is parametrically modifiable. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2931–2940. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goense, J., Merkle, H., and Logothetis, N. K. (2012). High-resolution FMRI reveals laminar differences in neurovascular coupling between positive and negative BOLD responses. Neuron 76, 629–639. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. L., and Garrett, D. D. (2014). Understanding variability in the BOLD signal and why it matters for aging. Brain Imaging Behav. 8, 274–283. doi: 10.1007/s11682-013-9253-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guitart-Masip, M., Salami, A., Garrett, D., Rieckmann, A., Lindenberger, U., and Bäckman, L. (2016). BOLD variability is related to dopaminergic neurotransmission and cognitive aging. Cereb. Cortex 26, 2074–2083. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, J. J., Reynell, C., and Attwell, D. (2011). The physiology of developmental changes in BOLD functional imaging signals. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 199–216. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogstrom, L. J., Westlye, L. T., Walhovd, K. B., and Fjell, A. M. (2013). The structure of the cerebral cortex across adult life: age-related patterns of surface area, thickness, and gyrification. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2521–2530. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, L., Goense, J., Kennerley, A. J., Trampel, R., Guidi, M., Reimer, E., et al. (2015). Cortical lamina-dependent blood volume changes in human brain at 7 T. Neuroimage 107, 23–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Im, K., Lee, J. M., Lyttelton, O., Kim, S. H., Evans, A. C., and Kim, S. I. (2008). Brain size and cortical structure in the adult human brain. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2181–2191. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., and Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. Neuroimage 62, 782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Sachdev, P., Lipnicki, D. M., Zhang, H., Liu, T., Zhu, W., et al. (2014). A longitudinal study of brain atrophy over two years in community-dwelling older individuals. Neuroimage 86, 203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ke, J., Zhang, L., Qi, R., Xu, Q., Li, W., Hou, C., et al. (2015). Altered blood oxygen level-dependent signal variability in chronic post-traumatic stress disorder during symptom provocation. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 11, 1805–1815. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S87332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kielar, A., Deschamps, T., Chu, R. K., Jokel, R., Khatamian, Y. B., Chen, J. J., et al. (2016). Identifying dysfunctional cortex: dissociable effects of stroke and aging on resting state dynamics in MEG and fMRI. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:40. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koopmans, P. J., Barth, M., and Norris, D. G. (2010). Layer-specific BOLD activation in human V1. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1297–1304. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., and Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). LmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaitre, H., Goldman, A. L., Sambataro, F., Verchinski, B. A., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Weinberger, D. R., et al. (2012). Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and gray matter volume? Neurobiol. Aging 33, 617.e1–617.e9. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makedonov, I., Black, S. E., and MacIntosh, B. J. (2013). BOLD FMRI in the white matter as a marker of aging and small vessel disease. PLoS One 8:e67652. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makedonov, I., Chen, J. J., Masellis, M., and MacIntosh, B. J. (2016). Physiological fluctuations in white matter are increased in Alzheimer’s disease and correlate with neuroimaging and cognitive biomarkers. Neurobiol. Aging 37, 12–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGinnis, S. M., Brickhouse, M., Pascual, B., and Dickerson, B. C. (2011). Age-related changes in the thickness of cortical zones in humans. Brain Topogr. 24, 279–291. doi: 10.1007/s10548-011-0198-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mella, N., de Ribaupierre, S., Eagleson, R., and de Ribaupierre, A. (2013). Cognitive intraindividual variability and white matter integrity in aging. ScientificWorldJournal 2013:350623. doi: 10.1155/2013/350623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nomi, J. S., Bolt, T. S., Chiemeka Ezie, C. E., Uddin, L. Q., and Heller, A. S. (2017). Moment-to-moment BOLD signal variability reflects regional changes in neural flexibility across the lifespan. J. Neurosci. 37, 5539–5548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nomi, J. S., Schettini, E., Voorhies, W., Bolt, T. S., Heller, A. S., and Uddin, L. Q. (2018). Resting-state brain signal variability in prefrontal cortex is associated with ADHD symptom severity in children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:90. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco, J., Goh, J. O., Kraut, M. A., Ferrucci, L., and Resnick, S. M. (2015). Greater cortical thinning in normal older adults predicts later cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 903–908. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panizzon, M. S., Fennema-Notestine, C., Eyler, L. T., Jernigan, T. L., Prom-Wormley, E., Neale, M., et al. (2009). Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2728–2735. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petracca, M., Saiote, C., Bender, H. A., Arias, F., Farrell, C., Magioncalda, P., et al. (2017). Synchronization and variability imbalance underlie cognitive impairment in primary-progressive multiple sclerosis. Sci. Rep. 7:46411. doi: 10.1038/srep46411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at: https://www.R-project.org/

Google Scholar

Raz, N. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter, M., Rosas, H. D., and Fischl, B. (2010). Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: a robust approach. Neuroimage 53, 1181–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogachov, A., Cheng, J. C., Erpelding, N., Hemington, K. S., Crawley, A. P., and Davis, K. D. (2016). Regional brain signal variability: a novel indicator of pain sensitivity and coping. Pain 157, 2483–2492. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossini, P. M., Rossi, S., Babiloni, C., and Polich, J. (2007). Clinical neurophysiology of aging brain: from normal aging to neurodegeneration. Prog. Neurobiol. 83, 375–400. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salat, D. H., Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Greve, D. N., Desikan, R. S. R., Busa, E., et al. (2004). Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb. Cortex 14, 721–730. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salat, D. H., Lee, S. Y., van der Kouwe, A. J., Greve, D. N., Fischl, B., and Rosas, H. D. (2009). Age-associated alterations in cortical gray and white matter signal intensity and gray to white matter contrast. Neuroimage 48, 21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Kuhnen, C. M., Yoo, D. J., and Knutson, B. (2010). Variability in nucleus accumbens activity mediates age-related suboptimal financial risk taking. J. Neurosci. 30:1426. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4902-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarapicchia, V., Mazerolle, E. L., Fisk, J. D., Ritchie, L. J., and Gawryluk, J. R. (2018). Resting state BOLD variability in Alzheimer’s disease: a marker of cognitive decline or cerebrovascular status? Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:39. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ségonne, F., Dale, A. M., Busa, E., Glessner, M., Salat, D., Hahn, H. K., et al. (2004). A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage 22, 1060–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segonne, F., Pacheco, J., and Fischl, B. (2007). Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 26, 518–529. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2006.887364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, M. E., Sachdev, P. S., Anstey, K. J., and Cherbuin, N. (2016). Age-related cortical thinning in cognitively healthy individuals in their 60s: the path through life study. Neurobiol. Aging 39, 202–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, V., Chertkow, H., Lerch, J. P., Evans, A. C., Dorr, A. E., and Kabani, N. J. (2006). Spatial patterns of cortical thinning in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 129(Pt 11), 2885–2893. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sled, J. G., Zijdenbos, A. P., and Evans, A. C. (1998). A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 17, 87–97. doi: 10.1109/42.668698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl. 1), S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storsve, A. B., Fjell, A. M., Tamnes, C. K., Westlye, L. T., Overbye, K., Aasland, H. W., et al. (2014). Differential longitudinal changes in cortical thickness, surface area and volume across the adult life span: regions of accelerating and decelerating change. J. Neurosci. 34, 8488–8498. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0391-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storsve, A. B., Fjell, A. M., Yendiki, A., and Walhovd, K. B. (2016). Longitudinal changes in white matter tract integrity across the adult lifespan and its relation to cortical thinning. PLoS One 11:e0156770. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0156770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y., Whitman, G. T., Lopez, I., and Baloh, R. W. (2001). Brain volume changes on longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging in normal older people. J. Neuroimaging 11, 393–400. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2001.tb00068.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thambisetty, M., Wan, J., Carass, A., An, Y., Prince, J. L., and Resnick, S. M. (2010). Longitudinal changes in cortical thickness associated with normal aging. Neuroimage 52, 1215–1223. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, P., Teng, I. C., May, L. D., Kurz, R., Lu, K., Scadeng, M., et al. (2010). Cortical depth-specific microvascular dilation underlies laminar differences in blood oxygenation level-dependent functional MRI signal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 15246–15251. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1006735107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, X., and Yablonskiy, D. A. (2016). Separation of cellular and BOLD contributions to T2 signal relaxation. Magn. Reson. Med. 75, 606–615. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wen, J., Goyal, M. S., Astafiev, S. V., Raichle, M. E., and Yablonskiy, D. A. (2018). Genetically defined cellular correlates of the baseline brain MRI signal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, E9727–E9736. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1808121115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, A. M., Kochunov, P., Blangero, J., Almasy, L., Zilles, K., Fox, P. T., et al. (2010). Cortical thickness or grey matter volume? The importance of selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies. Neuroimage 53, 1135–1146. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, A. M., Sabuncu, M. R., Yeo, B. T. T., Fischl, B., Greve, D. N., Kochunov, P., et al. (2012). Measuring and comparing brain cortical surface area and other areal quantities. Neuroimage 61, 1428–1443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2001). Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 20, 45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Y., Wen, J., Cross, A. H., and Yablonskiy, D. A. (2016). On the relationship between cellular and hemodynamic properties of the human brain cortex throughout adult lifespan. Neuroimage 133, 417–429. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, D. A., Piguet, O., Salat, D. H., Prince, K., Connally, E., and Corkin, S. (2010). Cognition in healthy aging is related to regional white matter integrity, but not cortical thickness. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 1912–1926. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zöller, D., Padula, M. C., Sandini, C., Schneider, M., Scariati, E., Van De Ville, D., et al. (2018). Psychotic symptoms influence the development of anterior cingulate BOLD variability in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Schizophr. Res. 193, 319–328. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zöller, D., Schaer, M., Scariati, M., Padula, C. M., Eliez, S., and Van De Ville, D. (2017). Disentangling resting-state BOLD variability and PCC functional connectivity in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Neuroimage 149, 85–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuur, A. F., Leno, E. N., Walker, N. J., Saveliev, A. A., and Smith, G. M. (2011). Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Keywords: signal variability, BOLD fMRI, structural alterations, cortical morphology, aging, cortical thickness, neural processing, biomarker

Citation: Pur DR, Eagleson RA, de Ribaupierre A, Mella N and de Ribaupierre S (2019) Moderating Effect of Cortical Thickness on BOLD Signal Variability Age-Related Changes. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:46. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00046

Received: 04 November 2018; Accepted: 18 February 2019;
Published: 12 March 2019.

Edited by:

Beatrice Arosio, University of Milan, Italy

Reviewed by:

Liang Gong, Chengdu Second People’s Hospital, China
Dejan Jakimovski, University at Buffalo, United States

Copyright © 2019 Pur, Eagleson, de Ribaupierre, Mella and de Ribaupierre. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daiana R. Pur, dpur@uwo.ca

Download