Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 30 April 2019
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 11 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00087

Decreased Cerebrospinal Fluid Flow Is Associated With Cognitive Deficit in Elderly Patients

Jadwiga Attier-Zmudka1,2 Jean-Marie Sérot1 Jeremy Valluy3 Mo Saffarini3* Anne-Sophie Macaret4 Momar Diouf5 Salif Dao6 Youcef Douadi4 Krzysztof Piotr Malinowski7 Olivier Balédent2,8
  • 1Department of Gerontology, Centre Hospitalier de Saint-Quentin, Saint-Quentin, France
  • 2CHIMERE, EA 7516 Head and Neck Research Group, University of Picardie Jules Verne, Amiens, France
  • 3ReSurg SA, Nyon, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Neurology, Centre Hospitalier de Saint-Quentin, Saint-Quentin, France
  • 5Department of Research, Amiens University Hospital, Amiens, France
  • 6Department of Radiology, Centre Hospitalier de Saint-Quentin, Saint-Quentin, France
  • 7Faculty of Health Sciences, Institute of Public Health, Jagiellonian University Medical College, Kraków, Poland
  • 8BioFlowImage, Image Processing Unit, University Hospital of Amiens, Amiens, France

Background: Disruptions in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) flow during aging could compromise protein clearance from the brain and contribute to the etiology of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD).

Objective: To determine whether CSF flow is associated with cognitive deficit in elderly patients (>70 years).

Methods: We studied 92 patients admitted to our geriatric unit for non-acute reasons using phase-contrast magnetic resonance imaging (PC-MRI) to calculate their ventricular and spinal CSF flow, and assessed their global cognitive status, memory, executive functions, and praxis. Multivariable regressions with backward selection (criterion p < 0.15) were performed to determine associations between cognitive tests and ventricular and spinal CSF flow, adjusting for depression, anxiety, and cardiovascular risk factors.

Results: The cohort comprised 71 women (77%) and 21 (33%) men, aged 84.1 ± 5.2 years (range, 73–96). Net ventricular CSF flow was 52 ± 40 μL/cc (range, 0–210), and net spinal CSF flow was 500 ± 295 μL/cc (range, 0–1420). Ventricular CSF flow was associated with the number of BEC96 figures recognized (β = 0.18, CI, 0.02–0.33; p = 0.025). Spinal CSF flow was associated with the WAIS Digit Span Backward test (β = 0.06, CI, 0.01–0.12; p = 0.034), and categoric verbal fluency (β = 0.53, CI, 0.07–0.98; p = 0.024) and semantic verbal fluency (β = 0.55, CI, 0.07–1.02; p = 0.024).

Conclusion: Patients with lower CSF flow had significantly worse memory, visuo-constructive capacities, and verbal fluency. Alterations in CSF flow could contribute to some of the cognitive deficit observed in patients with AD. Diagnosis and treatment of CSF flow alterations in geriatric patients with neurocognitive disorders could contribute to the prevention of their cognitive decline.

Introduction

The cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) is an important part of the central nervous system, as it allows exchange of water, small molecules and proteins between the brain parenchyma and arterial and venous blood (Oreskovic and Klarica, 2010; Brinker et al., 2014), by either passive diffusion or active transport (Oreskovic and Klarica, 2014; Oreskovic et al., 2017b). The CSF therefore plays an important role in regulating brain homeostasis, waste clearance (Puy et al., 2016), as well as intracranial pressure and blood supply (Baledent et al., 2004). During aging, CSF turnover can be disrupted (Rubenstein, 1998; Stoquart-ElSankari et al., 2007) which could contribute to the etiology of age-related neurocognitive disorders (Rubenstein, 1998; Weller et al., 2000; Launer, 2002; Silverberg et al., 2003; Chakravarty, 2004). Several studies revealed that patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) have disrupted CSF pressure (Silverberg et al., 2006), turnover (Henry-Feugeas and Intracranial, 2009; Serot et al., 2012), and oscillations (Silverberg et al., 2006; Stoquart-ElSankari et al., 2007). Moreover, biomarkers for AD are found in the CSF, and their abundance was shown to have predictive value for clinical progression (Wolfsgruber et al., 2017).

The increase of intracranial pressure during the cardiac cycle causes a flow from the blood and brain interstitial fluid to the CSF, and a net CSF flow toward its extracerebral compartment and venous blood (Oreskovic et al., 2017a). Since this CSF flow is important for protein clearance from the brain (Puy et al., 2016), it is possible that impaired CSF flow could be associated with cognitive decline (Coblentz et al., 1973; Sohn et al., 1973; Rubenstein, 1998). Moreover, CSF flow is linked with brain perfusion (Egnor et al., 2002; Baledent et al., 2004), defects of which are known causes of neurocognitive disorders in the elderly (O’Brien and Thomas, 2015). A number of studies suggested that the choroid plexus and the ventricular walls degenerate with the progression of AD (Serot et al., 2000; Balusu et al., 2016; Daouk et al., 2016), but none could determine whether disrupted CSF flow causes cognitive decline, or whether it is a by-product of AD or normal aging.

To the authors’ knowledge, there are no published studies that investigated the relationship between CSF flow alterations and cognitive deficit in the elderly, adjusting for cardiovascular risk factors for the development of neurocognitive disorders. The purpose of this study was therefore to evaluate the association of CSF flow in the brain ventricles and cervical spine with cognitive deficit (assessed using neurocognitive tests in clinical settings) in a cohort of elderly patients (>70 years) admitted to our geriatric unit for non-acute reasons. The hypothesis was that reduced CSF flow would be associated with cognitive deficit. Improved knowledge of such associations could guide the development of medical or surgical treatments to limit or prevent cognitive decline in the elderly (Nakajima et al., 2018).

Materials and Methods

We enrolled 115 consecutive patients admitted to our geriatric unit for non-acute reasons between October 2015 and March 2018. The inclusion criterion was patients aged over 70 years. Twenty-one patients were excluded because of contraindications to phase-contrast magnetic resonance imaging (PC-MRI) for analysis of brain fluid motion, and two patients died before completing neurocognitive assessments. This left a study cohort of 92 patients (Figure 1) who underwent PC-MRI that enabled calculation of ventricular (aqueduct) CSF flow and spinal (C2-C3) CSF flow.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of patient inclusion.

Data Acquisition

The PC-MRI was performed using a 1.5-T machine. Conventional morphologic image sequences were first acquired in the sagittal and axial planes. The CSF flow acquisition planes were then selected perpendicular to the presumed direction of flow through the Sylvius aqueduct [representing the ventricular flow (Jacobson et al., 1996)] and the spinal C2-C3 sub-arachnoid spaces (representing the spinal flow). Flow images were acquired using a velocity-encoded phase-contrast pulse sequence with peripheral gating, as previously described (Baledent et al., 2001, 2004). Velocity sensitization was set at 10 cm/s for the ventricular flow and 5 cm/s for the spinal flow.

Data Analysis

Phase-contrast magnetic resonance imaging data were transferred to a Sparc 10 workstation (SUN Microsystems) and analyzed using an in-house image processing software with Interactive Data Language (Baledent et al., 2001). This software automatically measures the CSF flow curve over the cardiac cycle for a given region of interest. Cranial–caudal flows were positive (CSF flush), whereas caudal–cranial flows were negative (CSF fill). The difference between CSF fill and flush flows is the net CSF flow, which reflects the volume of CSF produced (Nilsson et al., 1994). For technical reasons, intracranial sub-arachnoid CSF flow was not investigated.

Neurocognitive Assessment

All patients underwent a battery of neurocognitive tests that assessed global cognitive status, memory, executive functions, praxis, as well as depression and anxiety (Table 2). Global cognitive efficiency was assessed by the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975), standard of Kalafat et al. (2003), following the GRECO standardization and calibration, and the Mattis Dementia Rating scale (MDRS) (Hersch, 1979; Gardner et al., 1981). The memory domain was assessed using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) – Digit Span task (Iverson and Tulsky, 2003; Hill et al., 2010) and the Grober-Buschke (GB) test (French version of the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test) (Buschke, 1984; Grober et al., 1988; Grober and Kawas, 1997; Van der Linden and Juillerat, 2004). Instrumental cognition was assessed with Signoret’s Battery of Cognitive Efficacy (BEC 96) (visuo-constructive subscale) (Jacus et al., 2001). Attention and executive domains were assessed using the Stroop Color and Words Test (Comalli et al., 1962), as well as two categoric and semantic verbal fluency tests, as impaired semantic fluency is a predictor of progression to AD (Vaughan et al., 2018) and categoric fluency may be impaired in amnesic MCI (Balthazar et al., 2007). In addition, the patients were evaluated with the Montgomery-Asberg Depression rating scale (MADRS) (Montgomery and Asberg, 1979), on 60 points, and the Goldberg anxiety scale (Goldberg et al., 1987; Huber et al., 1999) on 9 points, to rule out effects of depression or anxiety on cognitive test results.

Mild cognitive impairment was diagnosed based on the criteria of Petersen et al. (Petersen et al., 2001), while AD and AD-like diseases were diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV) and the recommendations of the National Institute on Aging – Alzheimer’s Association workgroups (McKhann et al., 2011).

Vascular risk factors were diagnosed based on a comprehensive geriatric assessment performed at our geriatric unit. Blood samples were obtained after a minimum of 10 h of fasting. The diagnosis of diabetes was attributed with blood glucose levels above 7 mmol/L, anemia with hemoglobin levels below 12 (women) or 13 (men) g/dL and inflammation with CRP levels above 10 mg/L. Likewise, the reference range was 4.1–6.5 mmol/L for total cholesterol, 0.6–1.8 mmol/L for triglycerides and 35–50 g/L for Albumin. Malnutrition was defined by Albumin levels <35 g/L.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to summarize the data. Shapiro–Wilk tests were used to assess the normality of distributions. Pearson’s or Spearman’s coefficients were calculated to identify significant correlations between CSF flow (ventricular and spinal) and all cognitive test results. The ventricular CSF flow was correlated with the BEC96 visuo-constructive subscale (r = 0.231, p = 0.037), while the spinal CSF flow was correlated with the backward WAIS digit span task (r = 0.251, p = 0.023) and the GB recall test (r = 0.230, p = 0.054). Multivariable regressions with backward selection (criterion p < 0.15) were performed to determine associations between the aforementioned cognitive tests and thirteen independent variables, to adjust for depression, anxiety, and cardiovascular risk factors (Barnes and Yaffe, 2011) [gender, age, BMI, CSF flow (ventricular), CSF flow (spinal), MADRS, and Goldberg scores, as well as presence of diabetes, inflammation, hypercholesterolemia, hypertriglyceridemia, malnutrition or anemia]. Statistical analyses were performed using R version 3.3.2 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). P-values <0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

The final cohort comprised 71 women (77%) and 21 (33%) men, aged 84.1 ± 5.2 years (range, 73–96), with BMI 25.2 ± 4.8 kg/m2 (range, 15.6–40.0) (Table 1). The MADRS score was 7.3 ± 6.6 (range, 0–31), and 4 patients (4.3%) had scores >20, indicating moderate to severe depression. The Goldberg score was 2.8 ± 2.4 (range, 0–9), and 23 patients (25%) had scores ≥5, indicating possible anxiety. Net ventricular CSF flow was calculated at 52 ± 40 μL/cc (range, 0–210), and net spinal CSF flow at 500 ± 295 μL/cc (range, 0–1420). The majority of patients reported cognitive impairment and overall diagnosis revealed mild cognitive impairment in 41 patients (46%), vascular dementia in 11 (12%), AD in 5 (6%), Lewy-body dementia in 1 (1%), and mixed dementia in 32 (36%). The cognitive assessment echoed these findings with an MMSE score of 22 ± 4.8 (range, 7–30), and a MDRS or 117.7 ± 17.8 (range, 60–143) (Table 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patient demographics.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Cognitive assessment.

Multivariable analysis of factors affecting the memory domain revealed that (i) the WAIS Digit Span Backward results improved with increasing spinal CSF flow (β = 0.06, CI, 0.01–0.12; p = 0.034) and were worse for patients with anemia (β = -0.54, CI, -0.90 to -0.18; p = 0.004); (ii) the GB recall results increased with spinal CSF flow (β = 0.70, CI, -0.01–1.14; p = 0.054) and were worse in patients with malnutrition (β = -6.30, CI, -10.88 to -1.73; p = 0.008) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multi-variable regressions to identify factors associated with the memory domain (Backward selection p < 0.15).

Multivariable analysis also revealed that (i) the number of BEC96 figures recognized increased with ventricular CSF flow (β = 0.18, CI, 0.02–0.33; p = 0.025) and decreased in patients with malnutrition (β = -1.48, CI, -2.81 to -0.15; p = 0.029) (Table 4); (ii) categoric verbal fluency increased with spinal CSF flow (β = 0.53, CI, 0.07–0.98; p = 0.024) and in men (β = 3.88, CI, 0.68–7.08; p = 0.018), and was decreased in patients with diabetes (β = -7.48, CI, -11.92 to -3.05; p = 0.001); (iii) semantic verbal fluency increased with spinal CSF flow (×100) (β = 0.55, CI, 0.07–1.02; p = 0.024) and was decreased in patients with diabetes (β = -5.08, CI, -9.66 to -0.51; p = 0.030).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Multi-variable regressions to identify factors associated cognitive tests (Backward selection p < 0.15).

Discussion

The main finding of this study is that CSF flow was associated with cognitive test results. Ventricular CSF flow was associated with the number of recognized BEC 96 figures, while spinal CSF flow was associated with the WAIS digit-span backward test, categoric and semantic verbal fluencies, as well as the Grober-Buschke recall test (borderline). Taken together, these results suggest that CSF flow influences memory, visuo-constructive capacities, and verbal fluency. Interestingly, our findings are coherent with CSF flow values reported in recent literature. Our measures of ventricular CSF flow were comparable to values reported for healthy adults, but higher than those reported for healthy elderly volunteers, while our measures of spinal CSF flow were slightly higher than those reported for all elderly patients, except those with amnesic mild cognitive impairment (Table 5; Baledent et al., 2004; El Sankari et al., 2011; Puy et al., 2016).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. CSF flow measures in our cohort and reported in the recent literature.

It is unclear how CSF flow alterations could affect cognition. While CSF flow could be influenced by blood perfusion (Baledent et al., 2004), the cognitive picture of altered CSF flow offered in this study is more reminiscent of AD (Chakravarty, 2004) rather than vascular dementias, in which memory deficits are less constant (O’Brien and Thomas, 2015). Impaired CSF flow was shown to affect the local biochemical composition of CSF (Puy et al., 2016), and thus, could affect protein clearance. This could lead to amyloid or phosphorylated tau accumulation in the brain parenchyma, which are thought to be important elements in the etiology of AD, and whose concentration in the CSF was shown to predict clinical development (Wolfsgruber et al., 2017). This is consistent with ongoing research into CSF biology suggesting that CSF and interstitial fluid exchange along a brain-wide perivascular network, linking glial and vascular function (Simon and Iliff, 2016). This “glymphatic system” is hypothesized to contribute to neuroinflammation and neurodegeneration, especially in the aging brain. CSF flow disruptions are common in AD (Silverberg et al., 2006; Stoquart-ElSankari et al., 2007; Henry-Feugeas and Intracranial, 2009; Serot et al., 2012), confirming an etiological role for CSF flow disturbance in the development of late-onset AD.

Normal pressure hydrocephalus (NPH) is a rare form of neurocognitive disorder, in the elderly – characterized by the triad of dementia, gait apraxia and incontinence – thought to be caused by impaired CSF circulation (Leinonen et al., 2017). Its effects on cognition are similar to AD, and several studies (Del Bigio et al., 1997; Bech et al., 1999; Savolainen et al., 1999; Silverberg et al., 2006) underlined the high prevalence of AD in patients meeting clinical criteria for hydrocephalus. Results of hydrocephalus treatment by ventriculoperitoneal shunting are promising (Silverberg et al., 2002; Nakajima et al., 2018), but a trial to apply this technique in AD patients showed no benefits (Silverberg et al., 2008). Thus, it is likely that AD and NPH are separate disorders, with some clinical similarity. However, due to its rarity and to partial overlap of symptoms, NPH is often misdiagnosed as AD. Unlike in AD, where ventricular CSF flow is normal or decreased (Silverberg et al., 2006), in NPH, ventricular CSF flow is increased (El Sankari et al., 2011). Measurement of CSF flow as performed in this study thus presents the opportunity to better diagnose neurocognitive disorders of the elderly and offer appropriate treatment. Further, a proportion of AD patients is characterized by symptoms resembling NPH (Silverberg et al., 2002; Chakravarty, 2004), and in light of our findings, it is likely that CSF flow impairment is responsible for some cognitive impairment in these patients, who could therefore benefit from treatment (Attier-Zmudka et al., 2016).

In this study, multivariable analysis revealed negative associations between diabetes and executive functions (as indicated by the verbal fluency tests), between malnutrition and episodic memory and executive function (indicated by the GB recall test and the categoric verbal fluency test, respectively), and between anemia and working memory (indicated by the WAIS digit span backward test). In addition, men had greater categoric fluency, which could be explained by the fact that they were on average younger than the women (80 ± 4.8 vs. 85 ± 4.9 years, respectively). Our findings are consistent with the literature: Diabetes is an important predictor of cognitive decline in the elderly and is associated with deficits in attention and executive functions (Ryan et al., 2016). Malnutrition is a broad disorder, but folate levels have been associated with reduced episodic memory (Hassing et al., 1999). Similarly, low hemoglobin levels are known to affect memory in the elderly (Shah et al., 2009).

This study is the first to report direct associations between CSF flow and cognition, and could improve our understanding of the complex etiologies and symptoms of neurocognitive disorders in the elderly. It shows that altered CSF flow is associated with cognitive deficits in elderly patients, and thus suggests that treatments aiming at restoring normal CSF flow could reduce cognitive deficits. This is in accordance with a clinical observation in our practice, of a patient who recovered from debilitating neurocognitive disorders after sleep apnea treatment and ventriculoperitoneal shunting (Attier-Zmudka et al., 2016). However, this study has certain limitations. First, hydrocephalus diagnosis could not be performed in these patients, though it was suspected in several cases. Advanced hydrocephalus presents a complex etiological picture, and the presence of these patients in our cohort could affect the results of the study. Second, patients with more severe cognitive deficit had difficulty in performing the tests, and could not provide data, so that the multivariable models do not make use of the full cohort. Third, the scope of the study is limited by the lack of control group of age-matched patients with no cognitive impairment, so we cannot extrapolate our findings to the general population. Finally, our list of competing risk factors was incomplete, and we were unable to correctly or systematically assess the patients’ smoking habits, alcohol consumption, or lack of physical activity.

In conclusion, this study revealed direct associations of ventricular and spinal CSF flows with cognitive scores in a geriatric population with cognitive impairment. CSF flow was associated with memory, visuo-constructive ability and verbal fluency. It is therefore possible that CSF flow alterations are responsible for at least a part of the cognitive deficit observed in our patients. Better diagnosis and treatment of CSF flow alterations in geriatric patients suffering from neurocognitive disorders is therefore recommended.

Ethics Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all patients for their participation and confirmed by their next-of-kin if necessary. The study protocol was approved by an independent Ethical Review Board (CPP Amiens: 2015/6) and the National Data Protection Authority (CNIL:150075B-31). The study was registered in clinicaltrials.gov (NCT02578303). All procedures were performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

JA-Z and OB designed the study, collected the data, and edited the manuscript. J-MS designed the study and edited the manuscript. JV wrote and edited the manuscript and performed the statistics. MS wrote and edited the manuscript. A-SM, SD, and YD collected the data. MD and KM validated the statistics.

Conflict of Interest Statement

MS and JV are employed by ReSurg SA (research consulting firm that was paid by St Quentin Hospital to perform statistical analyses and write the manuscript).

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Attier-Zmudka, J., Serot, J. M., Douadi, Y., Capel, C., Bouzerar, R., and Baledent, O. (2016). Normal-pressure hydrocephalus, sleep apnea syndrome, and quality of life in an octagenarian. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 64, 1922–1923. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baledent, O., Gondry-Jouet, C., Meyer, M. E., De Marco, G., Le Gars, D., Henry-Feugeas, M. C., et al. (2004). Relationship between cerebrospinal fluid and blood dynamics in healthy volunteers and patients with communicating hydrocephalus. Invest. Radiol. 39, 45–55. doi: 10.1097/01.rli.0000100892.87214.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baledent, O., Henry-Feugeas, M. C., and Idy-Peretti, I. (2001). Cerebrospinal fluid dynamics and relation with blood flow: a magnetic resonance study with semiautomated cerebrospinal fluid segmentation. Investig. Radiol. 36, 368–377. doi: 10.1097/00004424-200107000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balthazar, M. L. F., Cendes, F., and Damasceno, B. P. (2007). Category verbal fluency performance may be impaired in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement. Neuropsychol. 1, 161–165. doi: 10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balusu, S., Brkic, M., Libert, C., and Vandenbroucke, R. E. (2016). The choroid plexus-cerebrospinal fluid interface in Alzheimer’s disease: more than just a barrier. Neural Regen. Res. 11, 534–537. doi: 10.4103/1673-5374.180372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, D. E., and Yaffe, K. (2011). The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 10, 819–828. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70072-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bech, R. A., Waldemar, G., Gjerris, F., Klinken, L., and Juhler, M. (1999). Shunting effects in patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus; correlation with cerebral and leptomeningeal biopsy findings. Acta Neurochir. 141,633–639.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Brinker, T., Stopa, E., Morrison, J., and Klinge, P. (2014). A new look at cerebrospinal fluid circulation. Fluids Barriers CNS 11:10. doi: 10.1186/2045-8118-11-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschke, H. (1984). Cued recall in amnesia. J. Clin. Neuropsychol. 6, 433–440. doi: 10.1080/01688638408401233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chakravarty, A. (2004). Unifying concept for Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, and normal pressure hydrocephalus - a hypothesis. Med. Hypotheses 63, 827–833. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2004.03.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coblentz, J. M., Mattis, S., Zingesser, L. H., Kasoff, S. S., Wisniewski, H. M., and Katzman, R. (1973). Presenile dementia. Clinical aspects and evaluation of cerebrospinal fluid dynamics. Arch. Neurol. 29, 299–308.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Comalli, P. E. Jr., Wapner, S., and Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. J. Genet. Psychol. 100, 47–53. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1962.10533572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daouk, J., Bouzerar, R., Chaarani, B., Zmudka, J., Meyer, M. E., and Baledent, O. (2016). Use of dynamic (18)F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography to investigate choroid plexus function in Alzheimer’s disease. Exp. Gerontol. 77, 62–68. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2016.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Bigio, M. R., Cardoso, E. R., and Halliday, W. C. (1997). Neuropathological changes in chronic adult hydrocephalus: cortical biopsies and autopsy findings. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 24, 121–126. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100021442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egnor, M., Zheng, L., Rosiello, A., Gutman, F., and Davis, R. (2002). A model of pulsations in communicating hydrocephalus. Pediatr. Neurosurg. 36, 281–303. doi: 10.1159/000063533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Sankari, S., Gondry-Jouet, C., Fichten, A., Godefroy, O., Serot, J. M., Deramond, H., et al. (2011). Cerebrospinal fluid and blood flow in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: a differential diagnosis from idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus. Fluids Barriers CNS 8:12. doi: 10.1186/2045-8118-8-12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198.

Google Scholar

Gardner, R. Jr., Oliver-Munoz, S., Fisher, L., and Empting, L. (1981). Mattis dementia rating scale: internal reliability study using a diffusely impaired population. J. Clin. Neuropsychol. 3, 271–275. doi: 10.1080/01688638108403130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, D. P., Bridges, K., Duncan-Jones, P., and Grayson, D. (1987). Dimensions of neuroses seen in primary-care settings. Psychol. Med. 17,461–470.

Google Scholar

Grober, E., Buschke, H., Crystal, H., Bang, S., and Dresner, R. (1988). Screening for dementia by memory testing. Neurology 38, 900–903.

Google Scholar

Grober, E., and Kawas, C. (1997). Learning and retention in preclinical and early Alzheimer’s disease. Psychol. Aging. 12, 183–188. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.1.183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassing, L., Wahlin, A., Winblad, B., and Backman, L. (1999). Further evidence on the effects of vitamin B12 and folate levels on episodic memory functioning: a population-based study of healthy very old adults. Biol. Psychiatr. 45,1472–1480. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00234-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry-Feugeas, M. C., and Intracranial, M. R. (2009). dynamics in clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease: the emerging concept of ”pulse wave encephalopathy”. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 6, 488–502. doi: 10.2174/156720509790147124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hersch, E. L. (1979). Development and application of the extended scale for dementia. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 27, 348–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1979.tb06056.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, B. D., Elliott, E. M., Shelton, J. T., Pella, R. D., O’Jile, J. R., and Gouvier, W. D. (2010). Can we improve the clinical assessment of working memory? An evaluation of the wechsler adult intelligence scale-third edition using a working memory criterion construct. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 32, 315–323. doi: 10.1080/13803390903032529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, P., Mulligan, R., Mackinnon, A., Nebuloni-French, T., and Michel, J. P. (1999). Detecting anxiety and depression in hospitalised elderly patients using a brief inventory. Eur. Psychiatr. 14, 11–16. doi: 10.1016/s0924-9338(99)80710-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, G. L., and Tulsky, D. S. (2003). Detecting malingering on the WAIS-III. Unusual digit span performance patterns in the normal population and in clinical groups. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 18, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6177(01)00176-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, E. E., Fletcher, D. F., Morgan, M. K., and Johnston, I. H. (1996). Fluid dynamics of the cerebral aqueduct. Pediatr. Neurosurg. 24, 229–236. doi: 10.1159/000121044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacus, J. P., Martin, C., Ailleret-Jean, C., Courcet, L., Delmotte-Tsocanakis, G., Faraldi, O., et al. (2001). [Cognitive aging in chronic psychotic patients]. Presse Med. 30(25 Pt 1), 1246–1252.

Google Scholar

Kalafat, M., Poitrenaud, J., and Hugonot-Diener, L. (2003). The Mini Mental State (MMS): french standardization and normative data [Standardisation et étalonnage français du ”Mini Mental State” (MMS) version GRÉCO]. Rev. Neuropsychol. 13, 209–236.

Google Scholar

Launer, L. J. (2002). Demonstrating the case that AD is a vascular disease: epidemiologic evidence. Ageing Res. Rev. 1, 61–77. doi: 10.1016/s0047-6374(01)00364-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leinonen, V., Vanninen, R., and Rauramaa, T. (2017). Cerebrospinal fluid circulation and hydrocephalus. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 145, 39–50. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-802395-2.00005-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr., Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, S. A., and Asberg, M. A. (1979). new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br. J. Psychiatry 134, 382–389. doi: 10.1192/bjp.134.4.382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakajima, M., Miyajima, M., Ogino, I., Akiba, C., Kawamura, K., Kurosawa, M., et al. (2018). Shunt intervention for possible idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus improves patient outcomes: a nationwide hospital-based survey in japan. Front. Neurol. 9:421. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, C., Stahlberg, F., Gideon, P., Thomsen, C., and Henriksen, O. (1994). The nocturnal increase in human cerebrospinal fluid production is inhibited by a beta 1-receptor antagonist. Am. J. Physiol. 267(6 Pt 2), R1445–R1448.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

O’Brien, J. T., and Thomas, A. (2015). Vascular dementia. Lancet 386, 1698–1706. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00463-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oreskovic, D., and Klarica, M. (2010). The formation of cerebrospinal fluid: nearly a hundred years of interpretations and misinterpretations. Brain Res. Rev. 64, 241–262. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oreskovic, D., and Klarica, M. (2014). A new look at cerebrospinal fluid movement. Fluids Barriers CNS 11:16. doi: 10.1186/2045-8118-11-16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oreskovic, D., Rados, M., and Klarica, M. (2017a). New concepts of cerebrospinal fluid physiology and development of hydrocephalus. Pediatr. Neurosurg. 52, 417–425. doi: 10.1159/000452169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oreskovic, D., Rados, M., and Klarica, M. (2017b). Role of choroid plexus in cerebrospinal fluid hydrodynamics. Neuroscience 354, 69–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., et al. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 58, 1985–1992.

Google Scholar

Puy, V., Zmudka-Attier, J., Capel, C., Bouzerar, R., Serot, J. M., Bourgeois, A. M., et al. (2016). Interactions between flow oscillations and biochemical parameters in the cerebrospinal fluid. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:154. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubenstein, E. (1998). Relationship of senescence of cerebrospinal fluid circulatory system to dementias of the aged. Lancet 351, 283–285. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(97)09234-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, C. M., van Duinkerken, E., and Rosano, C. (2016). Neurocognitive consequences of diabetes. Am. Psychol. 71, 563–576. doi: 10.1037/a0040455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savolainen, S., Paljarvi, L., and Vapalahti, M. (1999). Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in patients investigated for presumed normal pressure hydrocephalus: a clinical and neuropathological study. Acta Neurochir. 141, 849–853. doi: 10.1007/s007010050386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serot, J. M., Bene, M. C., Foliguet, B., and Faure, G. C. (2000). Morphological alterations of the choroid plexus in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 99, 105–108. doi: 10.1007/pl00007412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serot, J. M., Zmudka, J., and Jouanny, P. (2012). A possible role for CSF turnover and choroid plexus in the pathogenesis of late onset Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 30, 17–26. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-111964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, R. C., Wilson, R. S., Tang, Y., Dong, X., Murray, A., and Bennett, D. A. (2009). Relation of hemoglobin to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neuroepidemiology 32, 40–46. doi: 10.1159/000170905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverberg, G., Mayo, M., Saul, T., Fellmann, J., and McGuire, D. (2006). Elevated cerebrospinal fluid pressure in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Cerebrospinal Fluid Res. 3:7.

Google Scholar

Silverberg, G. D., Levinthal, E., Sullivan, E. V., Bloch, D. A., Chang, S. D., Leverenz, J., et al. (2002). Assessment of low-flow CSF drainage as a treatment for AD: results of a randomized pilot study. Neurology 59, 1139–1145. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000031794.42077.a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverberg, G. D., Mayo, M., Saul, T., Fellmann, J., Carvalho, J., and McGuire, D. (2008). Continuous CSF drainage in AD: results of a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study. Neurology 71, 202–209. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000316197.04157.6f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverberg, G. D., Mayo, M., Saul, T., Rubenstein, E., and McGuire, D. (2003). Alzheimer’s disease, normal-pressure hydrocephalus, and senescent changes in CSF circulatory physiology: a hypothesis. Lancet Neurol. 2, 506–511. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(03)00487-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, M. J., and Iliff, J. J. (2016). Regulation of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) flow in neurodegenerative, neurovascular and neuroinflammatory disease. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1862, 442–451. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2015.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sohn, R. S., Siegel, B. A., Gado, M., and Torack, R. M. (1973). Alzheimer’s disease with abnormal cerebrospinal fluid flow. Neurology 23, 1058–1065.

Google Scholar

Stoquart-ElSankari, S., Baledent, O., Gondry-Jouet, C., Makki, M., Godefroy, O., and Meyer, M. E. (2007). Aging effects on cerebral blood and cerebrospinal fluid flows. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 27, 1563–1572. doi: 10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Linden, M., and Juillerat, A. C. (2004). [Neuropsychological rehabilitation in early stage Alzheimer’s disease: principles, methods and perspectives]. Rev. Neurol. 160(4 Pt 2), S64–S70.

Google Scholar

Vaughan, R. M., Coen, R. F., Kenny, R., and Lawlor, B. A. (2018). Semantic and phonemic verbal fluency discrepancy in mild cognitive impairment: potential predictor of progression to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 66, 755–759. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weller, R. O., Massey, A., Kuo, Y. M., and Roher, A. E. (2000). Cerebral amyloid angiopathy: accumulation of A beta in interstitial fluid drainage pathways in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 903, 110–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06356.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolfsgruber, S., Polcher, A., Koppara, A., Kleineidam, L., Frolich, L., Peters, O., et al. (2017). Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers and clinical progression in patients with subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 58, 939–950. doi: 10.3233/jad-161252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cerebrospinal fluid, cognitive dysfunction, aged patients, dementia, magnetic resonance imaging, phase contrast

Citation: Attier-Zmudka J, Sérot J-M, Valluy J, Saffarini M, Macaret A-S, Diouf M, Dao S, Douadi Y, Malinowski KP and Balédent O (2019) Decreased Cerebrospinal Fluid Flow Is Associated With Cognitive Deficit in Elderly Patients. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:87. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00087

Received: 16 July 2018; Accepted: 01 April 2019;
Published: 30 April 2019.

Edited by:

Aurel Popa-Wagner, University Hospital Essen, Germany

Reviewed by:

Julien Rossignol, Central Michigan University, United States
Sabina Capellari, University of Bologna, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Attier-Zmudka, Sérot, Valluy, Saffarini, Macaret, Diouf, Dao, Douadi, Malinowski and Balédent. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mo Saffarini, journals@resurg.eu

Download