Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 04 September 2019
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias
Volume 11 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00245

Differential Associations Between Volumes of Atrophic Cortical Brain Regions and Memory Performances in Early and Late Mild Cognitive Impairment

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Seoul St. Mary’s Hospital, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Yeouido St. Mary’s Hospital, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, South Korea

Background: Early and late mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients have been reported to have a distinctive prognosis of converting to Alzheimer’s disease.

Objective: To evaluate the difference in gray matter volume and assess the association between cognitive function evaluated by comprehensive cognitive function test, and cortical thickness across healthy controls (HCs) (n = 37), early (n = 30), and late MCI patients (n = 35).

Methods: Differences in gray matter volume were evaluated by whole brain voxel-based morphometry across the groups. Multiple regression analysis was used to analyze group by memory performance interactions for the normalized gray matter volume.

Results: The early MCI group showed reduced gray matter volume in the right middle temporal gyrus in comparison to the HC group. The late MCI group displayed atrophy in the left parahippocampal gyrus in comparison to the HC group. Late MCI patients exhibited a decreased gray matter volume in the left fusiform gyrus in comparison to patients in the early MCI group (Monte Carlo simulation corrected p < 0.01, Tukey post hoc tests). Furthermore, there was a significant group (HC vs. early MCI) by memory performance interaction for the normalized cortical volume of the right middle temporal gyrus. Additionally, a significant group (early MCI vs. late MCI) by memory performance interaction was found for the normalized gray matter volume of the left fusiform gyrus (p < 0.001).

Conclusion: Early and late MCI patients showed distinctive associations of gray matter volumes in compensatory brain regions with memory performances. The findings can contribute to a better understanding of the structural changes in compensatory brain regions to elucidate memory decline in the trajectory of the subdivided prodromal stages of the Alzheimer’s disease (AD).

Introduction

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a feature of the transitional stage from normal aging to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Petersen et al., 2001), which has been reported to progress to dementia at a rate of 10–15% per year (Gauthier et al., 2006). Moreover, when memory is deteriorated in consideration of age, sex, and years of education, it is classified as an amnestic subtype of MCI (Petersen and Negash, 2008), which has been shown to be associated with beta-amyloid (Aβ) retention (Markesbery et al., 2006). Amnestic MCI is classified into early and late MCI based on the degree of deterioration in memory performances evaluated by detailed neuropsychological tests (Jessen et al., 2014). In addition, the early MCI has been suggested to reflect individuals at an earlier point in the clinical spectrum of AD compared with the late MCI (Aisen et al., 2010). Furthermore, recent studies have demonstrated that the risk of AD increases in patients with late MCI in comparison to patients with early MCI (Jessen et al., 2014).

In the trajectory of AD, cortical brain atrophy is preceded by Aβ accumulation and tauopathy (Jack et al., 2013). Moreover, atrophy has been described to progress at different rates depending on the region of the brain (Schuff et al., 2012) and to follow a sigmoidal shape as a function of time (Sabuncu et al., 2011). Given this AD pathogenesis, cortical changes have been documented to have an association with functional changes in brain and cognitive changes (Buckner et al., 2005).

Previous research has reported the structural change in the brain at the prodromal stage of the AD. Amnestic MCI subjects have shown the atrophy in hippocampal region, cingulate gyri, temporoparietal, and frontal neocortex in comparison to cognitively normal elderly and have displayed the preserved cortical volume in posterior association cortex in comparison to AD patients (Chetelat et al., 2002; Pennanen et al., 2005; Hämäläinen et al., 2007; Yao et al., 2011). In one study evaluating cortical changes in early and late MCI, a late MCI group showed atrophy in the entorhinal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, and inferior temporal gyrus in comparison to an early MCI group. The early MCI group exhibited increased volume in the precuneus, supramarginal, and superior parietal gyrus in comparison to healthy elderly subjects and the late MCI group (Li et al., 2017). These structural changes have been documented to be related to memory, attention, and executive function in several regions of interest including the middle frontal gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, and fusiform gyrus (Bonekamp et al., 2010). However, in this previous study, the results should be interpreted considering that the mean age of early MCI patients was profoundly lower than late MCI patients and controls. In contrary to this previous result, another previous study has reported the temporal atrophy, which is modulated by APOE ε4 genotype, in early MCI patients compared with healthy controls (HCs) (Risacher et al., 2013).

In addition, the other previous study has differentiated MCI converters from non-converters by evaluating hemispheric asymmetry of morphological biomarker including the cortical thickness in the medial temporal lobe (Long et al., 2018). Although the differentiation by MCI converter and non-converter is more clinically meaningful than that by early and late MCI, the former differs from the latter that can be examined at specific time points in that long-term follow-up observations are required for evaluating MCI converter and non-converter.

In these previous studies, however, atrophy of the cortex has been assessed only for specific brain regions of interest, without evaluation across the whole brain in early and late MCI. In addition, although the deterioration of cognitive function in various domains was observed in the MCI stage (Mistridis et al., 2015), most of the previous studies have mainly focused on a domain of memory. Additionally, although early MCI and late MCI reflect different stages in the clinical spectrum of AD, few studies have examined a differential relationship between cortical atrophy and cognitive decline in early and late MCI.

The aim of this study is to evaluate the difference in gray matter volume by whole brain voxel-based morphometry and to assess the association between cognitive function evaluated by comprehensive cognitive function test, and cortical thickness across cognitively normal older adults, early, and late MCI patients. As previous studies have reported the different prognosis and the difference in cortical atrophy, and cognitive decline in the subdivided prodromal stages of the AD, we hypothesized that there would be a significant difference in cortical thickness between these diagnoses in the medial temporal lobe and that there would be a distinctive association between cortical atrophy and cognitive decline among controls, early, and late MCI.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

One hundred two subjects were included in this study [37 subjects in the healthy control (HC) group (age range: 72–78 years), 30 subjects with early MCI (age range: 71–82 years), and 35 subjects with late MCI (age range: 69–82 years)]. Subjects were recruited from the Catholic Geriatric Brain MRI database, which was built through the outpatient psycho-geriatric clinic of Seoul Saint Mary’s Hospital located in Seoul, Republic of Korea, from October 2016 to July 2018. The cognitive functions of all subjects were assessed with the Korean version of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for AD (CERAD-K) (Lee et al., 2002). Measures included assessment in verbal fluency (VF), the 15-item Boston Naming Test (BNT), the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE-K) (Park, 1989), Word List Memory (WLM), Word List Recall (WLR), Word List Recognition (WLRc), Constructional Praxis (CP), and Constructional Recall (CR). In addition, total memory (TM) domain scores were obtained by summing scores from the CERAD-K, WLM, WLR, and WLRc. Patients with MCI met Peterson’s criteria of (1) memory complaint corroborated by an informant, (2) objective memory impairment for age, level of education, and sex, (3) essentially preserved general cognitive function, (4) mostly intact functional activities, and (5) no dementia. All MCI patients had an overall Clinical Dementia Rating of 0.5 (Morris, 1993). Classifications of late MCI and early MCI were made as follows. Subjects classified with late MCI reported memory impairment (memory has become worse) and had performance scores greater than 1.5 standard deviations (SDs) below the respective age-specific, education-specific, and sex-specific normative mean on the CERAD-K WLR. On the other hand, subjects classified with early MCI had performance scores between 1.5 and 1.0 SDs below the normative mean. Concise descriptions of the tests and the review process are described in the Supplementary Material. We excluded participants with any history of alcoholism, drug abuse, head trauma, or psychiatric disorders; those taking any psychotropic medications (e.g., cholinesterase inhibitors, antidepressants, benzodiazepines, and antipsychotics); those with multiple vascular risk factors; and those with extensive cerebrovascular disease.

The inclusion criteria of elderly HCs were as follows: (1) subjects were older than 60 years of age; (2) subjects had a performance on the CERAD-K WLR less than 1.0 SDs and on the other domains of the CERAD-K less than 1.5 SDs below the normative mean, and (3) subjects had a Clinical Dementia Rating score of 0. Subjects with any history of alcoholism, drug abuse, head trauma, or psychiatric disorders, as well as those taking any psychotropic medications, were excluded. The study was conducted under the ethical and safety guidelines set forth by the Institutional Review Board of The Catholic University of Korea, which approved all study procedures. Informed and written consent was obtained from all participants.

MRI Acquisition and Pre-processing

Imaging data were collected in the Department of Radiology of Seoul Saint Mary’s Hospital at The Catholic University of Korea, using a 3T Siemens Verio machine and an eight-channel Siemens head coil (Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany). The parameters used for T1-weighted volumetric magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo scan sequences were TE = 2.5 ms, TR = 1,900 ms, inversion time = 900 ms, FOV = 250 mm, matrix = 256 × 256, and voxel size = 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm.

We processed the data using the VBM8 toolbox1, which was part of the SPM8 software package2, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, United Kingdom). Data pre-processing and analysis were performed using SPM83. Data pre-processing involved visual inspection of the T1-weighted images to control for imaging artifacts and the consecutive segmentation into gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), building a customized template for GM and WM through an iteratively non-linear registration algorithm (DARTEL Toolbox for SPM8) and a normalization of this template to the Montreal Neurological Institute template. The Jacobian determinants resulting from the normalization procedure were used to obtain modulated VBM data preserving regional volumes. Individual GM and WM images were smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 6 mm full-width at half-maximum prior to statistical analyses. Global volumes of GM, WM, and CSF were assessed from segmented images using the VBM8 toolbox for SPM8 and summed to generate an estimate for total intracranial volume (TIV).

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses for demographic data were performed with R software (version 2.15.3). Normality assumptions were tested for all continuous variables using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. All variables were normally distributed. One-way ANOVA and chi-square (χ2) tests were used to assess potential differences between the HC, early, and late MCI groups for all demographic variables. All statistical analyses used a two-tailed level of 0.05 for defining statistical significance. We conducted a whole brain voxel-wise analysis of between-group differences in GM volume with a general linear model (GLM) using SPM8, controlling for age, sex, level of education, and APOEε4 genotype, which are known to affect cortical atrophy (Sowell et al., 2006; Gutiérrez-Galve et al., 2009; Arenaza-Urquijo et al., 2013; Hurtz et al., 2014), and TIV. Multiple corrections were performed using a cluster-extent correction (AlphaSim), as implemented through data processing and analysis for brain imaging (DPABI) (Yan et al., 2016), and the parameters were set as follows: individual voxel threshold p < 0.01, number of Monte Carlo simulations = 1000, and p < 0.05 as the effective threshold for cluster-extent correction. In post hoc procedures, corrected p values under a given control procedure for comparing group means of any pairs were calculated (through Studentized range statistics for Turkey-Kramer corrections). The p maps were then converted to Z maps according to the normal inverse cumulative distribution function, with the sign of group mean differences applied.

The GM volumes from brain regions with significant group differences were used for further region of interest (ROI) analysis. Resultant raw volumetric measures were normalized to the TIV to compensate for inter-individual variability in head size. To assess the main effect of the interaction between group and cognitive performance on the normalized cortical volume in ROIs, we used multiple regression analyses after adjusting for demographic covariates with a significant difference across the groups. Moreover, we examined the main effect of APOE ε4 genotype, cognitive performance, and interaction between two factors on the normalized cortical atrophy within and across the groups.

Results

Baseline Demographic and Clinical Data

Table 1 shows the baseline demographic data for the different subject groups. There were no significant differences in sex, number of years of education, and APOEε4 genotype between the control, early, and late MCI groups. However, there was a significant difference in age across groups (p < 0.001). As expected based on the inclusion criteria, the groups differed in memory performance test scores (MMSE-K, CERAD-K WLM, WLR, WLRc, and TM) across groups (p < 0.001). Concerning non-amnestic cognitive functions, there were also differences in CERAD-K VF, BNT, CR (p < 0.001), and CP scores across groups (p = 0.024) (see Supplementary Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study participants.

Group Differences in Normalized Gray Matter Volume

There was a significant group effect on the normalized gray matter volume of the left parahippocampal gyrus and inferior temporal gyrus (Table 2 and Figure 1A, AlphaSim corrected p < 0.01). Post hoc analysis indicated that the normalized gray matter volumes of the right middle temporal gyrus and left parahippocampal gyrus were significantly lower in early and late MCI groups, respectively, in comparison to the control group (Table 2 and Figures 1B,C), Monte Carlo simulation corrected p < 0.01, Tukey post hoc tests). In addition, the left fusiform gyrus displayed reduced normalized gray matter volume in the late MCI group in comparison to the early MCI group (Table 2 and Figure 1D), Monte Carlo simulation corrected p < 0.01, Tukey post hoc tests). Distribution of normalized gray matter volume across groups is demonstrated in Supplementary Figure 1.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Anatomical location of regions showing differences in VBM-based gray matter volume.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Regions of gray matter where normalized volume shows significant differences among the control subjects, early MCI patients, and late MCI patients (Monte Carlo simulation corrected p < 0.01). (B) Regions of gray matter where normalized volume shows significant reductions in the early MCI group in comparison to the control group. (C) In the late MCI group in comparison to the control group. (D) In the late MCI group in comparison to the early MCI group (Tukey post hoc tests, Monte Carlo simulation corrected p < 0.01).

A Significant Group by Cognitive Performance Interaction for Normalized Gray Matter Volumes in Regions of Interest

After controlling for age covariate, which showed a significant difference across the groups, there was a main effect of the interaction between group (HC vs. early MCI) and memory performance (CERAD-K WLM, WLR, and TM) on the normalized cortical volume of the right middle temporal gyrus (Figure 2A and Supplementary Results 2.1; WLM, Standardized β coefficient (std. β) = 1.14, adjusted R2 (adj. R2) = 0.2933, F = 7.85, p < 0.001; WLR, std. β = 0.55, adj. R2 = 0.2372, F = 7.84, p < 0.001; TM, std. β = 1.07, adj. R2 = 0.2704, F = 7.12, p < 0.001). There was also a main effect of the interaction between group (early MCI vs. late MCI) and memory performance (CERAD-K WLM) on the normalized gray matter volume of the left fusiform gyrus (Figure 2B and Supplementary Results 2.2; WLM, Standardized β coefficient (std. β) = −0.89, adjusted R2 (adj. R2) = 0.2438, F = 7.88, p < 0.001). Additionally, there was a trend toward a main effect of interaction between group (early MCI vs. late MCI) and visuospatial function (CERAD-K CP) on the normalized gray matter volume of the left fusiform gyrus (Supplementary Results 2.2; CP, Standardized β coefficient (std. β) = 1.62, adjusted R2 (adj. R2) = 0.1828, F = 4.58, p = 0.003).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Significant group (healthy control vs. early MCI) by memory performance interactions for normalized gray matter volumes of the right middle temporal gyrus (RMTG). (B) Significant group (early MCI vs. late MCI) by memory performance interactions for normalized gray matter volumes of the left fusiform gyrus (LFG).CERAD-K, the Korean version of the consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease; WLM, word list memory; WLR, word list recall; TM, total scores of memory domains including the CERAD-K WLM, WLR, and WLRc; RMTG, right middle temporal gyrus; LFG, left fusiform gyrus.

However, there was no main effect of the interaction between group (HC vs. early MCI vs. late MCI) and memory performance on the normalized cortical volume of the left parahippocampal gyrus and inferior temporal gyrus. In addition, there was no main effect of the interaction between group (HC vs. late MCI) and cognitive performance on the normalized gray matter volume of the left parahippocampal gyrus.

The Main Effect of APOE ε4 Genotype, Cognitive Performance, and Interaction Between Two Factors on the Normalized Gray Matter Volume Within and Across the Groups

When analyzed, including all of the controls and the early MCI group, there was a trend toward a main effect of APOE ε4 genotype on the normalized gray matter volume of the right middle temporal gyrus. In addition, there was a main effect of the memory performance (CERAD-K WLM) on the normalized cortical volume of the left fusiform gyrus only in case of including all of the controls and the early MCI group. However, there was no main effect of the interaction between APOE ε4 genotype and cognitive performance on the normalized gray matter volume in regions of interest (see Supplementary Table 2).

Discussion

The present study was designed to examine differences in gray matter volume by whole brain voxel-based morphometry and to assess the association between cognitive function evaluated by comprehensive cognitive function test, and cortical thickness in the subdivided trajectory of the prodromal stage in AD.

With respect to the first research question, it was found that the normalized cortical volume of the left parahippocampal gyrus and inferior temporal gyrus showed significant differences across the three groups. In accordance with the present results, previous studies have demonstrated that the left parahippocampal gyrus shows significant atrophy in MCI subjects who later progress to AD (Hämäläinen et al., 2007; Waragai et al., 2009). These results also support previous research indicating more greatly reduced gray matter volume in the left parahippocampal gyrus of AD and late MCI subjects than in healthy elderly subjects and early MCI subjects (Li et al., 2017). Concerning the inferior temporal gyrus, a previous study showed greater atrophy of this region in early and late MCI subjects than in HCs (Li et al., 2017). In addition, another study has reported decreased cortical volumes in the bilateral inferior temporal gyrus of amnestic MCI patients than in healthy elderly subjects (Whitwell et al., 2007).

Post hoc analysis of the current study also found that the early MCI group showed greater atrophy in the right middle temporal gyrus in comparison to the HC group. In prior studies, the gray matter volume of this region has been reported to be more greatly decreased in progressive and amnestic MCI patients than in HC subjects (Chetelat et al., 2002; Hämäläinen et al., 2007). Moreover, the cortical volume of this region has been shown to be more greatly reduced in AD patients than in MCI patients (Yao et al., 2011). In another previous, early MCI patients have displayed more temporal lobe atrophy than HCs (Risacher et al., 2013). Additionally, in the current study, greater levels of atrophy in the left fusiform gyrus were found in late MCI subjects in comparison to early MCI subjects. In a previous study, changes in this region have been displayed to predict conversion from MCI to AD (Convit et al., 2000). Furthermore, Aβ retention in hippocampus has been exhibited to be associated with the atrophy of the fusiform gyrus in early stage AD patients (Chang et al., 2016). Finally, it was found that the late MCI group showed reduced gray matter volume in the left parahippocampal gyrus in comparison to the HC group. This result is consistent with that of comparable studies (Hämäläinen et al., 2007; Yao et al., 2011).

The brain regions mentioned above show some overlap with the Braak tau stage III/IV, which is understood to be the primary substrate for amnestic MCI (Markesbery, 2010). These findings may be partly explained by the relationship of tauopathy in Braak-related brain regions with cortical thickness (Wang et al., 2016). Furthermore, these Braak-related regions in stage III/IV have been indicated to predict progression from MCI to AD (Convit et al., 2000). However, in the present study, for brain regions showing more atrophy in early MCI patients than in HCs and for brain regions displaying more greatly reduced cortical volumes in late MCI patients than in early MCI patients and HCs, the observed cortical spreading patterns are mismatched with known cortical spreading patterns in Braak stage III/IV. This intriguing finding might be explained by the fact that the current research was a cross-sectional study and that memory decline does not necessarily represent the progression of the AD pathology, given that early and late MCI are classified based on delayed memory recall scores. Furthermore, the current study did not evaluate tauopathy in any group. In this regard, these findings need to be interpreted with caution, and further longitudinal research evaluating tau pathology needs to be pursued. With respect to atrophy in the middle temporal gyrus, this region of the brain has been shown to constitute part of the default mode network (DMN) (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010) that has been reported to be associated with Aβ retention and cortical atrophy in the trajectory of AD (Buckner et al., 2005, 2008; Sepulcre et al., 2016).

However, these results differ from some published studies, which have described cortical atrophy in the frontal, parietal cortex, precuneus, hippocampus, and cingulate gyrus (Chetelat et al., 2002; Bozzali et al., 2006; Hämäläinen et al., 2007). This inconsistency in findings may be explained by the fact that previous studies do not classify MCI groups or control for all of the factors affecting atrophy, including age, sex, level of education, and APOE ε4 genotype. In addition, it is possible that differences in methodology to examine atrophy levels may account for some observed discrepancies. In other research comparing gray matter volumes in early and late MCI subjects, early MCI patients showed increased volumes in comparison healthy elderly subjects and late MCI patients in the precuneus, supramarginal, and superior parietal gyrus (Li et al., 2017). Given that age affects gray matter volumes (Hurtz et al., 2014), this finding could have been generated by the fact that the mean age of the early MCI group was below 70 years old and significantly younger than the other groups in this previous study.

In addition, the current study showed that the significant interaction between group and memory performance was exhibited in Braak-related brain regions such as the right middle temporal gyrus and left fusiform gyrus. Given the positive relationship between normalized cortical volumes and memory performances in the early MCI group of the present study, a possible explanation for this interaction may be that the gray matter volume of the right middle temporal gyrus decreases more in early MCI subjects than in healthy elderly subjects, and that the cortical volume of the left fusiform gyrus is more greatly reduced in early MCI subjects than in late MCI subjects, when the memory function decreases during progression of AD. Cortical changes in the right middle temporal gyrus, constituting the brain regions of the DMN (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010), have been reported to be associated with changes in memory (Buckner et al., 2005). Moreover, previous studies evaluating functional changes in this region have suggested that this region might have a compensatory role for AD pathogenesis (Mormino et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2011). In regards to the left fusiform gyrus, the gray matter volume of this brain region has been documented to have a relationship with verbal memory (Bonekamp et al., 2010). In addition, recent study has demonstrated a significant association between the functional connectivity of the fusiform gyrus and MMSE scores in the amnestic MCI patients (Cai et al., 2015a). Furthermore, the existing body of research suggests a compensatory function in the fusiform gyrus by showing increased functional connectivity between the bilateral fusiform gyrus and right thalamus in the early MCI patients rather than in the late MCI patients (Cai et al., 2015b). Although this result might serve as one of the bases for the relatively clear relationship between cortical atrophy of the left fusiform gyrus and cognitive decline in the early MCI group compared with the late MCI group, observed in this study, how the functional brain change contributes to the structural brain change in this brain region is not yet fully understood. Therefore, further studies, which take these variables into account, will need to be undertaken. In addition, there was a trend toward the main effect of interaction between the group and CP on the cortical volume of the left fusiform gyrus in the current study. Previous study has established that AD patients with constructional apraxia have lower volume of the fusiform gyrus than those without constructional apraxia and that AD patients with constructional apraxia show worse performance in other cognitive functions including working memory (Serra et al., 2014).

Moreover, these Braak-related brain regions have been demonstrated to mediate the association between neurodegeneration and cognitive function (Bejanin et al., 2017). Because the current study did not evaluate tau pathology, however, further studies should be undertaken in conjunction with evaluating tau deposition and performing mediation analysis. Moreover, in consideration of previous research reporting that white matter is more sensitive to changes in cognitive function than gray matter (Salat et al., 2010; Meyer et al., 2013), further studies will need to be undertaken to take these variables into account.

Considering the impact of the APOE ε4 genotype on the cognition, and cortical atrophy in the MCI (Farlow et al., 2004; Hämäläinen et al., 2008; Risacher et al., 2013), we evaluated the main effect of APOE ε4 genotype on the cortical atrophy within and across the groups. In these results, we found a trend toward a main effect of APOE ε4 genotype on the cortical volume of the right middle temporal gyrus in case of including both controls and early MCI group, being consistent with those found in the earlier study (Risacher et al., 2013). However, more research including factors such as Aβ and tau protein retention, which affects cortical atrophy, is needed to better understand the possible effect of APOE ε4 genotype on the cortical atrophy in the subdivided trajectory of the MCI.

In this investigation, the demographic characteristics of the three groups do not precisely correspond. Notably, the difference in ages across groups is a possible source of error affecting our results. Therefore, these data must be interpreted with caution, and it is necessary to perform additional validation using samples in which the demographic data across groups correspond. Additionally, although the education years were used as a proxy of cognitive reserve in this study, premorbid IQ, a more sensitive marker for cognitive reserve (Schmand et al., 1997), was not evaluated. Moreover, given that this premorbid IQ can affect degree of presentation of memory deficit in the course of the AD (Tuokko et al., 2003), more research using more valid proxy for the cognitive reserve is needed. Finally, with a small sample size, caution must be applied, as the findings might not have sufficient statistical power for evaluating the hypothesis of the present study. Therefore, further research including a larger sample size is needed to be carried out.

In conclusion, this study has explored differences in gray matter volumes and the relationship between cognitive function and cortical thickness across the subdivided prodromal stages of the AD, which show the different prognosis for the AD. We found that Braak-related regions—regions that are vulnerable to tau pathology (Cho et al., 2016; Johnson et al., 2016) — showed significant differences in the normalized cortical volumes across the three groups. Additionally, we found that there was a main effect of the interaction between the subdivided stages of the MCI and memory performance on the cortical volumes in the compensatory regions, which decreased more in early MCI subjects than in HCs and late MCI subjects as memory performance scores declined. The current study has gone some way toward enhancing our understanding of the structural changes in compensatory brain regions to elucidate memory decline in the trajectory of the subdivided prodromal stages of the AD.

Ethics Statement

This study was conducted under the ethical and safety guidelines set forth by the Institutional Review Board of The Catholic University of Korea, which approved all study procedures. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Author Contributions

DK, HL, and CL conceived and designed the research. DK, HL, SJ, and CL recruited the subjects and followed them to obtain clinical results. DK, NL, and HL performed the in vivo MRI experiments. DK and HL performed the image preprocessing and image analysis. NL and SJ performed the statistical analysis. DK wrote the manuscript. CL provided scientific mentorship throughout the project. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the Ministry of Trade, Industry and Energy (MOTIE, South Korea) under the Industrial Technology Innovation Program No. 10062378.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00245/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm8/
  2. ^ http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/
  3. ^ https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8/

References

Aisen, P. S., Petersen, R. C., Donohue, M. C., Gamst, A., Raman, R., Thomas, R. G., et al. (2010). Clinical core of the alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative: progress and plans. Alzheimer’s Dement. 6, 239–246.

Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65, 550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Landeau, B., La Joie, R., Mevel, K., Mézenge, F., Perrotin, A., et al. (2013). Relationships between years of education and gray matter volume, metabolism and functional connectivity in healthy elders. Neuroimage 83, 450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bejanin, A., Schonhaut, D. R., La Joie, R., Kramer, J. H., Baker, S. L., Sosa, N., et al. (2017). Tau pathology and neurodegeneration contribute to cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 140, 3286–3300. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonekamp, D., Yassa, M. A., Munro, C. A., Geckle, R. J., Yousem, D. M., Barker, P. B., et al. (2010). Gray matter in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: voxel-based morphometry. Neuroreport 21:259. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328335642a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozzali, M., Filippi, M., Magnani, G., Cercignani, M., Franceschi, M., Schiatti, E., et al. (2006). The contribution of voxel-based morphometry in staging patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 67, 453–460. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000228243.56665.c2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R., Andrews-Hanna, J., and Schacter, D. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Shannon, B. J., LaRossa, G., Sachs, R., Fotenos, A. F., et al. (2005). Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J. Neurosci. 25, 7709–7717. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2177-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, S., Chong, T., Zhang, Y., Li, J., von Deneen, K. M., Ren, J., et al. (2015a). Altered functional connectivity of fusiform gyrus in subjects with amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a resting-state fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:471. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, S., Huang, L., Zou, J., Jing, L., Zhai, B., Ji, G., et al. (2015b). Changes in thalamic connectivity in the early and late stages of amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance study from ADNI. PLoS One 10:e0115573. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Y.-T., Huang, C.-W., Chen, N.-C., Lin, K.-J., Huang, S.-H., Chang, W.-N., et al. (2016). Hippocampal amyloid burden with downstream fusiform gyrus atrophy correlate with face matching task scores in early stage Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:145. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chetelat, G., Desgranges, B., De La Sayette, V., Viader, F., Eustache, F., and Baron, J.-C. (2002). Mapping gray matter loss with voxel-based morphometry in mild cognitive impairment. Neuroreport 13, 1939–1943. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200210280-00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, H., Choi, J. Y., Hwang, M. S., Kim, Y. J., Lee, H. M., Lee, H. S., et al. (2016). In vivo cortical spreading pattern of tau and amyloid in the Alzheimer disease spectrum. Ann. Neurol. 80, 247–258. doi: 10.1002/ana.24711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Convit, A., De Asis, J., De Leon, M., Tarshish, C., De Santi, S., and Rusinek, H. (2000). Atrophy of the medial occipitotemporal, inferior, and middle temporal gyri in non-demented elderly predict decline to Alzheimer’s disease? Neurobiol. Aging 21, 19–26. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(99)00107-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farlow, M., He, Y., Tekin, S., Xu, J., Lane, R., and Charles, H. J. N. (2004). Impact of APOE in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 63, 1898–1901. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000144279.21502.b7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, S., Reisberg, B., Zaudig, M., Petersen, R. C., Ritchie, K., Broich, K., et al. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet 367, 1262–1270.

Google Scholar

Gutiérrez-Galve, L., Lehmann, M., Hobbs, N. Z., Clarkson, M. J., Ridgway, G. R., Crutch, S., et al. (2009). Patterns of cortical thickness according to APOE genotype in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 28, 461–470. doi: 10.1159/000258100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hämäläinen, A., Grau-Olivares, M., Tervo, S., Niskanen, E., Pennanen, C., Huuskonen, J., et al. (2008). Apolipoprotein E ε4 allele is associated with increased atrophy in progressive mild cognitive impairment: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neurodegener. Dis. 186–189. doi: 10.1159/000113698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hämäläinen, A., Tervo, S., Grau-Olivares, M., Niskanen, E., Pennanen, C., Huuskonen, J., et al. (2007). Voxel-based morphometry to detect brain atrophy in progressive mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage 37, 1122–1131. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurtz, S., Woo, E., Kebets, V., Green, A. E., Zoumalan, C., Wang, B., et al. (2014). Age effects on cortical thickness in cognitively normal elderly individuals. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. extra 4, 221–227. doi: 10.1159/000362872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R. Jr., Knopman, D. S., Jagust, W. J., Petersen, R. C., Weiner, M. W., Aisen, P. S., et al. (2013). Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 12, 207–216. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Wolfsgruber, S., Wiese, B., Bickel, H., Mösch, E., Kaduszkiewicz, H., et al. (2014). AD dementia risk in late MCI, in early MCI, and in subjective memory impairment. Alzheimer’s Dement. 10, 76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. A., Schultz, A., Betensky, R. A., Becker, J. A., Sepulcre, J., Rentz, D., et al. (2016). Tau positron emission tomographic imaging in aging and early A lzheimer disease. Ann. Neurol. 79, 110–119. doi: 10.1002/ana.24546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. H., Lee, K. U., Lee, D. Y., Kim, K. W., Jhoo, J. H., Kim, J. H., et al. (2002). Development of the korean version of the consortium to establish a registry for alzheimer’s disease assessment packet (CERAD-K) clinical and neuropsychological assessment batteries. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 57, 47–53.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Li, C., Loewenstein, D. A., Duara, R., Cabrerizo, M., Barker, W., and Adjouadi, M. (2017). the relationship of brain amyloid load and APOE status to regional cortical thinning and cognition in the ADNI cohort. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 59, 1269–1282. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, X., Jiang, C., and Zhang, L. (2018). Morphological biomarker differentiating MCI converters from nonconverters: longitudinal evidence based on hemispheric asymmetry. Behav. Neurol. 2018:3954101. doi: 10.1155/2018/3954101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markesbery, W. R. (2010). Neuropathologic alterations in mild cognitive impairment: a review. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 19, 221–228. doi: 10.3233/jad-2010-1220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markesbery, W. R., Schmitt, F. A., Kryscio, R. J., Davis, D. G., Smith, C. D., and Wekstein, D. R. (2006). Neuropathologic substrate of mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 63, 38–46.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Meyer, P., Feldkamp, H., Hoppstädter, M., King, A. V., Frölich, L., Wessa, M., et al. (2013). Using voxel-based morphometry to examine the relationship between regional brain volumes and memory performance in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:89. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mistridis, P., Krumm, S., Monsch, A. U., Berres, M., and Taylor, K. I. (2015). The 12 years preceding mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: the temporal emergence of cognitive decline. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 48, 1095–1107. doi: 10.3233/jad-150137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mormino, E. C., Smiljic, A., Hayenga, A. O., Onami, S. H., Greicius, M. D., Rabinovici, G. D., et al. (2011). Relationships between beta-amyloid and functional connectivity in different components of the default mode network in aging. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2399–2407. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C. (1993). The clinical dementia rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43, 2412–2414.

Google Scholar

Park, J.-H. (1989). Standardization of korean version of the mini-mental state examination (MMSE-K) for use in the elderly. Part II. diagnostic validity. J. Korean Neuropsychiatr. Assoc. 28, 508–513.

Google Scholar

Pennanen, C., Testa, C., Laakso, M., Hallikainen, M., Helkala, E., Hänninen, T., et al. (2005). A voxel based morphometry study on mild cognitive impairment. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 11–14. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.035600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., et al. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 58, 1985–1992.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., and Negash, S. (2008). Mild cognitive impairment: an overview. CNS Spectr. 13, 45–53. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900016151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Risacher, S. L., Kim, S., Shen, L., Nho, K., Foroud, T., Green, R. C., et al. (2013). The role of apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype in early mild cognitive impairment (E-MCI). Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:11. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabuncu, M. R., Desikan, R. S., Sepulcre, J., Yeo, B. T. T., Liu, H., Schmansky, N. J., et al. (2011). The dynamics of cortical and hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 68, 1040–1048. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salat, D., Tuch, D., Van der Kouwe, A., Greve, D., Pappu, V., Lee, S., et al. (2010). White matter pathology isolates the hippocampal formation in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 244–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmand, B., Smit, J. H., Geerlings, M. I., and Lindeboom, J. (1997). The effects of intelligence and education on the development of dementia. a test of the brain reserve hypothesis. Psychol. Med. 27, 1337–1344. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuff, N., Tosun, D., Insel, P. S., Chiang, G. C., Truran, D., Aisen, P. S., et al. (2012). Nonlinear time course of brain volume loss in cognitively normal and impaired elders. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 845–855. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sepulcre, J., Schultz, A. P., Sabuncu, M., Gomez-Isla, T., Chhatwal, J., Becker, A., et al. (2016). In vivo tau, amyloid, and gray matter profiles in the aging brain. J. Neurosci. 36, 7364–7374. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0639-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serra, L., Fadda, L., Perri, R., Spanò, B., Marra, C., Castelli, D., et al. (2014). Constructional apraxia as a distinctive cognitive and structural brain feature of pre-senile Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 38, 391–402. doi: 10.3233/JAD-130656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Peterson, B. S., Kan, E., Woods, R. P., Yoshii, J., Bansal, R., et al. (2006). Sex differences in cortical thickness mapped in 176 healthy individuals between 7 and 87 years of age. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1550–1560. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuokko, H., Garrett, D., McDowell, I., Silverberg, N., and Kristjansson, B. (2003). Cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults: reserve or ascertainment bias? Aging Mental Health 7, 259–270. doi: 10.1080/1360786031000120750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Benzinger, T. L., Su, Y., Christensen, J., Friedrichsen, K., Aldea, P., et al. (2016). Evaluation of tau imaging in staging Alzheimer disease and revealing interactions between β-amyloid and tauopathy. JAMA Neurol. 73, 1070–1077.

Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Yan, C., Zhao, C., Qi, Z., Zhou, W., Lu, J., et al. (2011). Spatial patterns of intrinsic brain activity in mild cognitive impairment and alzheimer’s disease: a resting-state functional MRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1720–1740. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waragai, M., Okamura, N., Furukawa, K., Tashiro, M., Furumoto, S., Funaki, Y., et al. (2009). Comparison study of amyloid PET and voxel-based morphometry analysis in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 285, 100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitwell, J. L., Petersen, R. C., Negash, S., Weigand, S. D., Kantarci, K., Ivnik, R. J., et al. (2007). Patterns of atrophy differ among specific subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 64, 1130–1138.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Yan, C.-G., Wang, X.-D., Zuo, X.-N., and Zang, Y.-F. (2016). DPABI: data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics 14, 339–351. doi: 10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, Z., Hu, B., Zhao, L., and Liang, C. (2011). “Analysis of gray matter in AD Patients and MCI subjects based voxel-based morphometry,” in Proceedings of International Conference on Brain Informatics, Berlin.

Google Scholar

Keywords: voxel-based morphometry, early mild cognitive impairment, late mild cognitive impairment, memory performance, gray matter volume

Citation: Kang DW, Lim HK, Joo S-h, Lee NR and Lee CU (2019) Differential Associations Between Volumes of Atrophic Cortical Brain Regions and Memory Performances in Early and Late Mild Cognitive Impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:245. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00245

Received: 27 September 2018; Accepted: 20 August 2019;
Published: 04 September 2019.

Edited by:

Thomas Wisniewski, New York University, United States

Reviewed by:

John B. Williamson, University of Florida, United States
Liu Jun, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China

Copyright © 2019 Kang, Lim, Joo, Lee and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chang Uk Lee, jihan@catholic.ac.kr

Download