Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 06 March 2020
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 12 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2020.00051

A Neurovisceral Integrative Study on Cognition, Heart Rate Variability, and Fitness in the Elderly

  • 1Departament of Physical Education, Health Sciences Center, State University of Maringá (UEM), Maringá, Brazil
  • 2Institute of Technology, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil

The association between physical fitness and cognitive performance has been widely investigated in the literature. However, the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying this relationship are not yet clear. Here, we aim to evaluate the interactions between executive function measures, heart rate variability (HRV), and physical fitness in the context of the neurovisceral integration (NVI) theory. Twenty-eight healthy elderly subjects (>60 years) were submitted to evaluation of executive performance with three computerized tests: the N-back test measured working memory capacity, the Stroop Color test evaluated inhibitory control and selective attention, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) evaluated abstract reasoning and cognitive flexibility. We also used the Physical Testing Battery for the Elderly to measure aerobic capacity, dynamic balance, upper body flexibility, and handgrip strength. Our results confirm the relationship between executive function and physical fitness, particularly between working memory, cardiorespiratory fitness, and dynamic balance. We also demonstrate an association between executive performance and HRV in older people, corroborating previous results from other groups obtained in young adults. However, our regression models did not indicate that HRV mediates the relationship between cognition and physical fitness in the elderly, suggesting that age-related degeneration of autonomic control can affect aspects of NVI in this population.

Introduction

The neurovisceral integration (NVI) model (Thayer and Lane, 2000; Thayer et al., 2009) proposes that adaptive behavior depends on the integration of neural networks spanning both the central (CNS) and autonomic nervous systems (ANS) tasked with regulating cardiovascular function. Brain metabolism critically depends on the cardiovascular supply of cerebral blood flow (CBF) due to the limited availability of this organ’s intracellular energy substrates. The crosstalk between CNS and ANS structures, necessary to provide the brain with adequate levels of oxygen and energy sources, is mediated by a network of brain areas known as the “central autonomic network” (CAN; Benarroch, 1993; Valenza et al., 2019), which includes the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). Outputs from these regions eventually reach premotor neurons located in the lower brain stem and nucleus ambiguus in the medulla oblongata which contributes to the sympathetic and parasympathetic modulation of the heart (Thayer and Lane, 2000; Benarroch, 2012).

The heart rate variability (HRV) reflects the variation in the time interval between consecutive heartbeats (Saul, 1990; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996) and has been considered a surrogate parameter for the complex interaction between brain and cardiovascular system (Ernst, 2017). Both systems are simultaneously affected by the normal process of aging (Peters, 2006; Jandackova et al., 2016) and the result is a decline in the capacity of the CAN to adjust the CBF in response to environmental challenges while negatively influencing emotion control (Mather and Thayer, 2018) and cognitive performance (Thayer et al., 2009; Elias and Torres, 2017; Forte et al., 2019). Jandackova et al. (2016) showed a marked decline in HRV in middle-aged individuals which seems to be secondary to other pathological conditions. Cognitive dementia is the result of multiple age-related neuropathies (Power et al., 2018) associated with cardiovascular risk factors (Oishi et al., 2017), such as chronic brain hypoperfusion (Zheng et al., 2019), which particularly affects the frontal cortex (Sherwood et al., 2011).

Over the past decade, a large number of studies have investigated the role lifestyle activities play in decreasing the risk of cognitive decline in the elderly (for review, see Christie et al., 2017). Among these activities, physical exercise has gained prominence due to its neuroprotective effects against dementia and other neurodegenerative diseases (Phillips et al., 2014; Vecchio et al., 2018). For instance, research has shown that regular exercise promoted hippocampal neurogenesis (Erickson et al., 2009, 2011), local increases on the concentration of neurotrophins (such as BDNF; Leckie et al., 2014; Håkansson et al., 2017), neuroplasticity (Erickson and Kramer, 2009; Voss et al., 2013a,b; Erickson et al., 2014), angiogenesis (Bloor, 2005; Al-Jarrah et al., 2010), and adaptive changes in CBF (Dupuy et al., 2015; Jennings et al., 2015). Electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies have provided evidence that cardiorespiratory fitness is positively correlated with brain function, particularly in brain regions associated with the CAN, which has shown increased neuroplasticity after physical exercise interventions, improving both cardiovascular and cognitive control (Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman, 2013). However, relatively few studies have focused on the relationship between physical fitness and effects on NVI biomarkers such as HRV, particularly in the elderly (see Albinet et al., 2010, 2016; Dupuy et al., 2018). While Alderman and Olson (2014) demonstrated the role of physical fitness in improving autonomic and neurocognitive health in young adults, these authors failed to show HRV-mediated influences between cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive performance, suggesting that other mediators may be more relevant in this population, which is at the apex of cognitive performance and with not much individual difference in cardiorespiratory fitness, compared to the elderly. In addition, other studies that investigated the relationship between cognition, HRV, and fitness from an NVI perspective have focused exclusively on aerobic capacity as an independent variable (Alderman and Olson, 2014; Dupuy et al., 2015).

Thus, the main purpose of the current study is to fill the gap in the literature regarding the association of physical fitness with cognitive performance and HRV in elderly subjects. Besides, we aim to verify how this relationship is associated with different physical abilities, such as strength, flexibility, dynamic balance, and aerobic capacity.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-eight (28) subjects (24 women) aged 60 years and over (mean age 66.71 ± 7.64 years old) participated in the research. The volunteers were screened using the following inclusion criteria: being over 60 years old, be literate, possess normal or corrected visual acuity, possess familiarity with computers, and have medical clearance to perform physical activities.

The exclusion criteria were: smoking (<6 months), surgery (<6 months), official medical diagnosis of psychiatric, psychological or cardiac disease, use of medication that may impair cardiac autonomic control and/or cognitive functions, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score <24 (or <21 for subjects with lower schooling levels; Almeida, 1998), Geriatric Depression Scale-Short Form (GDS-SF) score >5.

All experimental procedures performed in this study were approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the State University of Maringá (1,161,402).

Outcome Measures

Cognitive Measurements

We used three tests to evaluate different dimensions of executive function, namely the N-back test (working memory), the Stroop task (selective attention and inhibitory control), and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; abstract reasoning and cognitive flexibility). All tests were performed in a computerized setup created with the software Presentation Version 20.2 (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Berkeley, CA, USA).

The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)

The WCST (Grant and Berg, 1948) is a problem-solving task for cognitive performance assessment. The test consists of matching test cards one by one with stimulus cards, following a rule that must be deduced by the subjects themselves. We used 64 test cards which had to be associated with four stimulus cards (1-one red triangle; 2- two green stars; 3- three yellow crosses; and 4- four blue circles). The match is made by pressing on the keyboard the number corresponding to the desired stimulus card. After each attempt, the participant received positive or negative feedback on their performance through the words “right” or “wrong” presented on the screen. Blocks of 10 or 11 stimuli were presented and at the end of each one, the rule changed. We used as a parameter to evaluate performance the total number of errors and the number of perseverative errors. These scores were chosen because they are sensitive to the effects of aging (MacPherson et al., 2002; Guarino et al., 2019).

Stroop Color

We used the Victoria version of the Stroop test (Spreen and Strauss, 1998), which consists of 72 stimuli, distributed in three tasks with 24 items each. The task is divided into three blocks, starting after an instruction presented on the screen explains the task to be performed in each step. The stimuli are presented for 2 s and if no response is given within 5 s another stimulus is presented.

The first block was composed of colored rectangles in the colors green, pink, blue and brown; the second block consisted of neutral words (each, never, today, everything) written with the colors of the previous rectangles; the third block had stimuli with a word-color conflict. At each stimulus presented the subjects should press a key with a color corresponding to the stimulus presented on the screen.

We used response latency as a measure of performance.

N-back

We used a spatial n-back task adapted from Vermeij et al. (2014). The test has three levels, 0-back (control condition), 1-back (low working memory load) and 2-back (high working memory load). The test consisted of black square-shaped stimuli, which could appear at any one of 14 fixed locations on a monitor screen. Before each stimulus, a fixation point was presented at the center of the screen. Each trial lasted 2,500 ms and consisted of 2,000 ms of fixation and 500 ms of stimulus presentation. The task began after the subjects pressed a key indicating they had read the instructions on the screen. Each level consisted of 60 trials, with 17 target stimuli that should be identified by the subject. During instruction a screen containing the black squares in all possible positions appeared for 30 s, then a quick presentation of each stimulus was made for 500 ms in a sequence from left to right and bottom to top on the screen.

Test performance was evaluated by the percentages of 2-back hits, total hits, and misses.

Heart Rate Variability

Heart Rate was recorded with the subjects at rest in a comfortable and quiet room for 10 min. R-wave peaks from the ECG were recorded continuously using a Polar HRM V800 heart rate monitor (Polar OY, Finland) with a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz for subsequent analysis of HRV. R-R intervals (the interval between two successive R waves of the ECG’s QRS signal) were visually inspected for ectopic beats and replaced by interpolated data from adjacent normal to normal (N-N) intervals. A period of 5 min with lowest variance was selected for analysis. We performed both time and frequency domain analysis of the ECG signal with the Kubios software (Kubios Oy). In the time domain, we calculated the mean HR, the standard deviation of the N-N intervals (SDNN) and the square root of the successive quadratic mean interval differences (RMSSD), which are the most common measures representing parasympathetic activity (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996). In the frequency domain, we used Fourier transform to quantify spectral density power of low frequency (LF; 0.04–0.15 Hz) and high frequency (HF; 0.15–0.40 Hz) bands. Additional calculations included LF + HF, LF and HF expressed in normalized units and the LF/HF ratio. We used the Poincaré graph to quantify SD1 and SD2 (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996).

Physical Assessment

To assess physical abilities we used a functional fitness battery test for elderly people (Rikli and Jones, 1999a). Aerobic capacity was measured by the distance covered in meters during a 6-min walk test (6MWT). For measures of agility, speed, and dynamic balance, we used the Timed Up-and-Go test (TUG), in which the subjects should sit in a chair, stand up, walk to a cone 2.44 meters away, return, and sit again in the shortest time possible (time was measured in seconds). Flexibility was evaluated with the sit and reach test, measured in centimeters. Additionally, Handgrip strength was measured using a hand dynamometer with the dominant hand (North Coast Medical, Inc., Morgan Hill, CA, USA). Three trials from the dominant hand were calculated and the median was used for the analysis (Innes, 1999). Handgrip strength is expressed in kilograms (kg).

Data Collection Procedures

The evaluations were performed in three moments along 2 days. In moment 1, the participants signed the Informed Consent Form and answered the MMSE and GDS-SF questionnaires. Then, they went through a familiarization period with the computerized cognitive tests. Moments 2 and 3 occurred 1 week later to minimize learning effects on the performance of cognitive tests. In moment 2 we recorded the subjects’ resting heart rate (HR) before they undertook tests of executive function. The moment 3 occurred soon after and consisted of physical fitness measures.

Statistical Analysis

We performed an exploratory analysis with the Shapiro–Wilks test. For variables with normal distribution, we used the Pearson correlation test and, for non-normal distributions, we used the Spearman correlation test. Finally, multiple linear regressions with the hierarchical method were used for the independent variables, in which the researchers chose the order of the variables inserted in the model. We used the Durbin–Watson test for the independance of residuals. We also tested for multicollinearity and no strong correlation coefficients were found between the independent variables. There were no outliers in the residual statistics (with standard deviations below −3 and +3). We used the SPSS 21.0 Software (IBM Corp.) for analysis.

Results

Table 1 presents the results of the descriptive analysis of cognitive, HRV and physical variables. Correlations between cognitive measures and HRV parameters are shown in Table 2. We found significant correlations between the number of misses in the working memory task (N-back) and both mean HR (r = 0.406, p = 0.032) and mean RR intervals (r = − 0.383, p = 0.044). As for the WCST results, the number of misses correlated significantly with the following HRV indexes, RMSSD (r = 0.476, p = 0.012), LF (n.u.; r = −386, p = 0.046), HF (%; r = 0.426, p = 0.027), HF (n.u.; r = 0.389, p = 0.045), SD1 (r = 0.473, p = 0.013), and SD2/SD1 ratio (r = −0.465, p = 0.014). The performance on the Stroop test did not correlate with any of the HRV variables.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive values of cognitive performance, heart rate variability (HRV), and fitness.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between HRV and cognitive parameters.

Working memory performance measured by the N-back correlated positively with aerobic performance (6MWT) and dynamic balance (TUG; Figure 1). Only the number of hits on the 2-back test did not show a significant correlation with the TUG (r = −332, p = 0.084). No significant correlation was found between working memory and either handgrip strength or flexibility. The other cognitive tests also showed no significant correlation (p < 0.05) with physical fitness variables.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Correlations between working memory, aerobic capacity, and dynamic balance. (A) Hits in 2-back level and walk, (B) total hits in N-back test and walk, (C) misses in N-back and walk, (D) total hits in N-back test and TUG, and (E) misses in N-back and walk.

Table 3 shows the correlations measures between HRV indexes and physical fitness parameters. No significant correlation (p < 0.05) was found between these variables in the present study.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation between HRV and physical parameters.

Multiple Regression Model for Mean HR

We analyzed the impact of cognition variables (independent variables) N-back misses and C-A Latency on the HRV index and the Mean HR (dependent variables). Regarding the correlation coefficients between the independent variables, an acceptable variation was verified regarding the absence of multicollinearity (correlation coefficient ranging from −0.327 to 0.406). The Durbin-Watson’s diagnosis of independent residue verification was 2.458 and confirmed the quality of the model.

We observed that both models had moderate correlation coefficients (0.406 < r < 0.530), and the coefficients values increased by including more independent variables to the model (Model 2). Similarly, R2 indicates that the percentage change in the dependent variable explained by the independent variables increased as they were input into the model (0.165 < R2 < 0.281), suggesting that model 2, associated with N-back misses and AC latency in the Stroop test account for approximately 28% of the variation in mean HR. The change statistics revealed that the construction of the two models sequentially improved the model prediction, indicating statistical significance for the insertion of an independent variable in relation to a model without predictors (p = 0.032), improving the model by approximately 16% (p = 0.016; Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Impact analysis, change statistics and analysis of variance for models involving mean HR and cognition.

Finally, ANOVA confirms that the fit of the model with predictors is better than the fit without predictors (p < 0.032).

Table 5 shows that the regression coefficients of both models have a significant impact on the understanding of the relationships between the independent and dependent variables. Model 2 shows that the significance values are all below 0.049, confirming the relevance of the variables N-back misses and C-A Stroop Latency for the model. It is noteworthy that the standardized coefficients reveal that the variable “N-back miss” has greater relevance to the model (coefficient = 0.496) when compared to “C-A Stroop latency” (coefficient = 0.0352).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Regression coefficient for models involving mean HR variability and cognition variables.

Multiple Regression Model for Mean RR

We evaluated the impact of the cognitive variable “N-back misses” (independent variables) on the HRV variable “Mean RR” (dependent variable). Regarding the correlation coefficients between the independent variables, an acceptable variation was found regarding the absence of multicollinearity (correlation coefficient ranging from −0.383 to 0.045). The Durbin-Watson’s diagnosis was 2.419, confirming the quality of the model.

Table 6 presents the analysis of the correlation coefficients and impact of N-back misses on mean RR, as well as the change coefficients and the ANOVA value for the model. The correlation coefficient of the model was moderate (r = 0.383) and R2 (0.147) indicates that the percentage variation of mean RR explained by N-back was approximately 14%.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Impact analysis, change statistics and analysis of variance for the model involving miss N-back and mean RR.

Table 7 shows that the regression coefficient has a significant impact on the understanding of the relationships between the independent variable and the dependent variable.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Regression coefficient for the model involving miss N-back and mean RR.

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to evaluate how physical fitness contributes to executive performance in the context of the NVI model in elderly subjects. Normal physiological aging is known to increase the probability of the occurrence of pathological changes in the brain (Fjell et al., 2014) which are associated with different trajectories of cognitive performance in the elderly, including dementia (Sorond et al., 2015). Even though recent studies have shown the benefits of physical exercise and the positive influence of increased aerobic capacity on mitigating the deleterious effects of aging on cognitive measures (Kramer et al., 2006; Miller et al., 2012), many questions remain regarding the relative impact of different exercise modalities and the indirect role played by cardiovascular improvements on those measures. While a recent study Alderman and Olson (2014) demonstrated the positive role of aerobic capacity in both autonomic and cognitive measures in young adults, the authors were not able to show a correlation between these outcomes with changes in HRV, suggesting that brain-cardiovascular interactions may not be the most important mediators of change in this population.

The physical fitness of our sample was within the expected values for their age, when compared to normative values available in the literature (for references see: Innes, 1999; Rikli and Jones, 1999b; Mazo et al., 2015). Our subjects also performed comparably to other reports in the literature that used the same working memory tasks (Vermeij et al., 2014). The only exception was the number of hits in the N-back test, which was considerably lower, probably due to cultural specificities and the low educational level of our sample when compared to other studies. Our results also corroborated previous findings on the relationship between cognitive performance and physical fitness (Hansen et al., 2004; Erickson and Kramer, 2009; Erickson et al., 2009; Albinet et al., 2010; Chang et al., 2012; Soares-Miranda et al., 2014; Håkansson et al., 2017; Northey et al., 2018). Specifically, we demonstrated that working memory is associated with the performance in the 6-min walk and TUG tests, which are reliable indicators of aerobic capacity and dynamic balance in the elderly (Figure 1). These results are in agreement with other studies that showed a direct correlation between aerobic capacity and performance on tasks relying on executive functions (Kramer and Erickson, 2007; Alderman and Olson, 2014; Dupuy et al., 2015, 2018). Hansen et al. (2004) found that after a period of 8 weeks of aerobic training individuals not only improved performance on cognitive tasks (working memory, sustained and selective attention), but that this result was accompanied by improvements in HRV. The authors then performed a detraining period and found that this manipulation caused a reduction in both cognitive performance and HRV, indicating that they were associated with physical fitness. However, another study that investigated the cognitive performance of athletes throughout the sport season found that at times of large increased physical demand there was an associated increase in simple reaction time (slower cognitive processing) accompanied by reduced athletic performance, which indicated a dose-response relationship between exercise intensity and cognition (Matos et al., 2014). Surprisingly, these authors showed that changes in physical training did not induce significant changes in HRV, suggesting that other adjacent mechanisms may be involved in this interaction between cognition and physical capacity.

In our study, we also found a correlation between performance on cognitive tests and HRV (Table 2). For instance, there are positive correlations between N-back errors and mean HR and negative correlations between N-back errors and average RR intervals. This result indicates that higher parasympathetic activity is associated with better scores in working memory tasks. We also found a positive correlation between the number of errors in the WCST and heart indices associated with parasympathetic activity (RMSSD, percentage of HF, HF n.u and SD1) and a negative correlation between the number of errors with sympathetic activity (LF n.u. and SD2). This result leads us to consider that higher parasympathetic and lower sympathetic activity are associated with a decrease in behavioral flexibility. Other studies support the hypothesis that better performance in executive functions is associated with a higher parasympathetic activity as measured by HRV (Hansen et al., 2003; Duschek et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2009). Apparently, this relationship between cognition and HRV is important for self-regulation under various behavioral conditions (Blasi et al., 2006; Thayer et al., 2009; Capuana et al., 2014; Mather and Thayer, 2018).

Regarding the hypothesis that HRV could act as a link between cognition and physical performance, none of our regression models showed a significant relationship between executive function and HRV-mediated physical fitness. These results reinforce the negative findings of a previous study that investigated whether the influence of cardiorespiratory fitness on cognition of young adults would be mediated by HRV (Alderman and Olson, 2014). The researchers attributed their findings to the low variability in the aerobic capacity of their young volunteers, indicating that studies in other age-groups could obtain different results. However, our results corroborated their findings, suggesting that other mechanisms not associated with the NVI model may be responsible for the relationship between cognition and physical fitness in the elderly. The list of possible mediators includes neurotrophin levels, such as BDNF, and changes in CBF due to regular physical exercise (Dinoff et al., 2016).

Cardiovascular adaptations that positively influence HRV are generally attributed in the literature to the practice of aerobic exercises (Carter et al., 2003). However, other training methods such as resistance training, are also shown to promote cognitive improvements in the elderly (Liu-Ambrose et al., 2010; Bherer et al., 2013; Kattenstroth et al., 2013; Gajewski and Falkenstein, 2016; Müller et al., 2017; Northey et al., 2018; Lin et al., 2019). Chang et al. (2012) reviewed the effect of resistance training on elderly cognition and proposed IGF-1-mediated increases in neurogenesis, vascular density and glucose utilization in the brain as possible mediators. In addition, BDNF and VEGF (vascular endothelial growth factor) are also suggested as possible targets for exercise-related brain adaptations, which would explain the absence of correlation between HRV and physical fitness shown in our study. Our results also showed a lack of relationship between strength capacity and cognitive performance, pointing to the need for further studies exploring this association.

Age-related impairments in cognition, especially executive functions, are also associated with reduced HRV, probably due to the detrimental effects of aging on vagal activity (Britton et al., 2008; Frewen et al., 2013; Forte et al., 2019). The vagal tank theory (VTT) proposed by Laborde et al. (2018), uses the metaphor of a “vagal tank,” the level of which is determined by changes in cardiac vagal control activity and determines the efficiency of self-regulatory adaptations to cognitive, social, and emotional challenges. According to the VTT, risk of abnormal HRV, which is a measure of cardiac vagal control, is the impaired ability to cope with physical and emotional demands in daily life activities. One alternative to counteract these effects would be to engage in regular physical exercise, which improves both the basal “tank” volume and vagal reactivity (Hottenrott et al., 2019).

Even though it is widely known that physical activity improves cognitive and brain functions, there is still no consensus on the magnitude of its effects and which are the best strategies and adequate dosage (duration, intensity, and frequency; Kramer et al., 2005; Hillman et al., 2008; Erickson et al., 2019). Physical activity can promote either nonspecific or specific cognitive improvements, depending on the type of exercise, with executive functions being the most benefited (Gajewski and Falkenstein, 2016; Erickson et al., 2019). According to the Physical Activity, Cognition, and Brain Outcomes of the American College of Sports Medicine, training programs involving different components such as endurance, strength and coordination, from moderate to vigorous intensity over the long term, are the most adequate to provide cognitive benefits to older populations (>50 years; Erickson et al., 2019).

In summary, our results are consistent with the literature regarding the relationship between cognition and physical fitness. Our results also support the NVI model, now tested in elderly individuals, demonstrating the association between executive function performance and HR. However, the hypothesis of HRV acting as a link between cognition and physical fitness was not confirmed in our sample, which indicates that age-related degeneration of autonomic control can affect aspects of NVI in this population.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Permanent Committee on Human Research Ethics—COPEP, State University of Maringá. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

FM and AP designed the study and wrote the manuscript. AV and FM collected the data. AV, FM, WG, WL, and AP analyzed the data. All authors read and commented on the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank CREAS (Reference Center Specializing in Social Assistance) from Lidianópolis (PR), in particular to Mrs. Lucia de Jesus Maia Buzato, Coordinator of Social Assistance.

References

Albinet, C. T., Abou-Dest, A., André, N., and Audiffren, M. (2016). Executive functions improvement following a 5-month aquaerobics program in older adults: role of cardiac vagal control in inhibition performance. Biol. Psychol. 115, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albinet, C. T., Boucard, G., Bouquet, C. A., and Audiffren, M. (2010). Increased heart rate variability and executive performance after aerobic training in the elderly. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 109, 617–624. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1393-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alderman, B. L., and Olson, R. L. (2014). The relation of aerobic fitness to cognitive control and heart rate variability: a neurovisceral integration study. Biol. Psychol. 99, 26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Jarrah, M., Jamous, M., Al Zailaey, K., and Bweir, S. O. (2010). Endurance exercise training promotes angiogenesis in the brain of chronic/progressive mouse model of Parkinson’s disease. NeuroRehabilitation 26, 369–373. doi: 10.3233/nre-2010-0574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almeida, O. P. (1998). Mini exame dos estado mental e o diagnóstico de demência no brasil. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 56, 605–612. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x1998000400014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benarroch, E. E. (1993). The central autonomic network: functional organization, dysfunction, and perspective. Mayo Clinic Proceedings 68, 988–1001. doi: 10.1016/s0025-6196(12)62272-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benarroch, E. E. (2012). “Central autonomic control,” in Primer on the Autonomic Nervous System, 3rd Edn. eds D. Robertson, I. Bioggioni, G. Burnstock, P. A. Low and J. F. R. Paton (Cambridge: Academic press), 9–12.

Google Scholar

Bherer, L., Erickson, K. I., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2013). A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. J. Aging Res. 2013:657508. doi: 10.1155/2013/657508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasi, G., Goldberg, T. E., Weickert, T., Das, S., Kohn, P., Zoltick, B., et al. (2006). Brain regions underlying response inhibition and interference monitoring and suppression. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 1658–1664. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04680.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloor, C. M. (2005). Angiogenesis during exercise and training. Angiogenesis 8, 263–271. doi: 10.1007/s10456-005-9013-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britton, A., Singh-Manoux, A., Hnatkova, K., Malik, M., Marmot, M. G., and Shipley, M. (2008). The association between heart rate variability and cognitive impairment in middle-aged men and women. Neuroepidemiology 31, 115–121. doi: 10.1159/000148257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capuana, L. J., Dywan, J., Tays, W. J., Elmers, J. L., Witherspoon, R., and Segalowitz, S. J. (2014). Factors influencing the role of cardiac autonomic regulation in the service of cognitive control. Biol. Psychol. 102, 88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, J. B., Banister, E. W., and Blaber, A. P. (2003). Effect of endurance exercise on autonomic control of heart rate. Sports Med. 33, 33–46. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200333010-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Y.-K., Pan, C.-Y., Chen, F.-T., Tsai, C.-L., and Huang, C.-C. (2012). Effect of resistance-exercise training on cognitive function in healthy older adults: a review. J. Aging Phys. Act. 20, 497–517. doi: 10.1123/japa.20.4.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christie, G. J., Hamilton, T., Manor, B. D., Farb, N. A. S., Farzan, F., Sixsmith, A., et al. (2017). Do lifestyle activities protect against cognitive decline in aging? A review. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:381. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinoff, A., Herrmann, N., Swardfager, W., Liu, C. S., Sherman, C., Chan, S., et al. (2016). The effect of exercise training on resting concentrations of peripheral brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF): a meta-analysis. PLoS One 11:e0163037. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dupuy, O., Billaut, F., Raymond, F., Benraiss, A., Theurot, D., Bosquet, L., et al. (2018). Effect of acute intermittent exercise on cognitive flexibility: the role of exercise intensity. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2, 146–156. doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0078-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dupuy, O., Gauthier, C. J., Fraser, S. A., Desjardins-Crèpeau, L., Desjardins, M., Mekary, S., et al. (2015). Higher levels of cardiovascular fitness are associated with better executive function and prefrontal oxygenation in younger and older women. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:66. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duschek, S., Muckenthaler, M., Werner, N., and del Paso, G. A. R. (2009). Relationships between features of autonomic cardiovascular control and cognitive performance. Biol. Psychol. 81, 110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., and Kramer, A. F. (2009). Aerobic exercise effects on cognitive and neural plasticity in older adults. Br. J. Sports Med. 43, 22–24. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2008.052498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elias, M. F., and Torres, R. V. (2017). The renaissance of heart rate variability as a predictor of cognitive functioning. Am. J. Hypertens. 31, 21–23. doi: 10.1093/ajh/hpx150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Hillman, C., Stillman, C. M., Ballard, R. M., Bloodgood, B., Conroy, D. E., et al. (2019). Physical activity, cognition, and brain outcomes: a review of the 2018 physical activity guidelines. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 51, 1242–1251. doi: 10.1249/mss.0000000000001936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Leckie, R. L., and Weinstein, A. M. (2014). Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume. Neurobiol. Aging 35, S20–S28. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Hu, L., Morris, K. S., et al. (2009). Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus 19, 1030–1039. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst, G. (2017). Heart-rate variability—more than heart beats? Front. Public Health 5:240. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fjell, A. M., McEvoy, L., Holland, D., Dale, A. M., and Walhovd, K. B. (2014). What is normal in normal aging? Effects of aging, amyloid and Alzheimer’s disease on the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus. Prog. Neurobiol. 117, 20–40. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forte, G., Favieri, F., and Casagrande, M. (2019). Heart rate variability and cognitive function: a systematic review. Front. Neurosci. 13:710. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frewen, J., Finucane, C., Savva, G. M., Boyle, G., Coen, R. F., and Kenny, R. A. (2013). Cognitive function is associated with impaired heart rate variability in ageing adults: the Irish longitudinal study on ageing wave one results. Clin. Auton. Res. 23, 313–323. doi: 10.1007/s10286-013-0214-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gajewski, P. D., and Falkenstein, M. (2016). Physical activity and neurocognitive functioning in aging—a condensed updated review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 13:1. doi: 10.1186/s11556-016-0161-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Pinilla, F., and Hillman, C. (2013). The influence of exercise on cognitive abilities. Compr. Physiol. 3, 403–428. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c110063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, D. A., and Berg, E. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a weigl-type card-sorting problem. J. Exp. Psychol. 38, 404–411. doi: 10.1037/h0059831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guarino, A., Favieri, F., Boncompagni, I., Agostini, F., Cantone, M., and Casagrande, M. (2019). Executive functions in Alzheimer disease: a systematic review. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:437. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Håkansson, K., Ledreux, A., Daffner, K., Terjestam, Y., Bergman, P., Carlsson, R., et al. (2017). BDNF responses in healthy older persons to 35 minutes of physical exercise, cognitive training, and mindfulness: associations with working memory function. J. Alzheimers Dis. 55, 645–657. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., and Thayer, J. F. (2003). Vagal influence on working memory and attention. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 48, 263–274. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(03)00073-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., Sollers, J. J. III., Stenvik, K., and Thayer, J. F. (2004). Heart rate variability and its relation to prefrontal cognitive function: the effects of training and detraining. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 93, 263–272. doi: 10.1007/s00421-004-1208-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hottenrott, L., Ketelhut, S., and Hottenrott, K. (2019). Commentary: Vagal Tank Theory: The Three Rs of Cardiac Vagal Control Functioning–Resting, Reactivity, and Recovery. Front. Neurosci. 13:1300. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innes, E. (1999). Handgrip strength testing: a review of the literature. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 46, 120–140. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1630.1999.00182.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jandackova, V. K., Scholes, S., Britton, A., and Steptoe, A. (2016). Are changes in heart rate variability in middle-aged and older people normative or caused by pathological conditions? Findings from a large population-based longitudinal cohort study. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 5:e002365. doi: 10.1161/jaha.115.002365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jennings, J. R., Allen, B., Gianaros, P. J., Thayer, J. F., and Manuck, S. B. (2015). Focusing neurovisceral integration: cognition, heart rate variability, and cerebral blood flow. Psychophysiology 52, 214–224. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kattenstroth, J.-C., Kalisch, T., Holt, S., Tegenthoff, M., and Dinse, H. R. (2013). Six months of dance intervention enhances postural, sensorimotor, and cognitive performance in elderly without affecting cardio-respiratory functions. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:5. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., Colcombe, S. J., McAuley, E., Scalf, P. E., and Erickson, K. I. (2005). Fitness, aging and neurocognitive function. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 124–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., and Erickson, K. I. (2007). Effects of physical activity on cognition, well-being and brain: human interventions. Alzheimers Dement. 3, S45–S51. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., and Colcombe, S. J. (2006). Exercise, cognition, and the aging brain. J. Appl. Physiol. 101, 1237–1242. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00500.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Mosley, E., and Mertgen, A. (2018). Vagal tank theory: the three rs of cardiac vagal control functioning—resting, reactivity, and recovery. Front. Neurosci. 12:458. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leckie, R. L., Oberlin, L. E., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Szabo-Reed, A., Chaddock-Heyman, L., et al. (2014). BDNF mediates improvements in executive function following a 1-year exercise intervention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:985. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, S., Yang, Y., Qi, Q., Wei, L., Jing, N., Jie, Z., et al. (2019). The beneficial effect of physical exercise on cognitive function in a non-dementia aging chinese population. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:238. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu-Ambrose, T., Nagamatsu, L. S., Graf, P., Beattie, B. L., Ashe, M. C., and Handy, T. C. (2010). Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch. Intern. Med. 170, 170–178. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., and Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function, and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychol. Aging 17, 598–609. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Thayer, J. (2018). How heart rate variability affects emotion regulation brain networks. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 19, 98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matos, F. O., Samulski, D. M., Lima, J. R. P., and Prado, L. S. (2014). Cargas elevadas de treinamento alteram funções cognitivas em jogadores de futebol. Rev. Br. Med. Esporte 20, 388–392. doi: 10.1590/1517-86922014200501274

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazo, G. Z., Petreça, D. R., Sandreschi, P. F., and Benedetti, T. R. B. (2015). Valores normativos da aptidão física para idosas brasileiras de 60 a 69 anos de idade. Rev. Br. Med. Esporte 21, 318–322. doi: 10.1590/1517-869220152104134470

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, D. I., Taler, V., Davidson, P. S. R., and Messier, C. (2012). Measuring the impact of exercise on cognitive aging: methodological issues. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 622.e29–622.e43. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, P., Rehfeld, K., Schmicker, M., Hökelmann, A., Dordevic, M., Lessmann, V., et al. (2017). Evolution of neuroplasticity in response to physical activity in old age: the case for dancing. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:56. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northey, J. M., Cherbuin, N., Pumpa, K. L., Smee, D. J., and Rattray, B. (2018). Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 52, 154–160. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oishi, E., Ohara, T., Sakata, S., Fukuhara, M., Hata, J., Yoshida, D., et al. (2017). Day-to-day blood pressure variability and risk of dementia in a general japanese elderly population: the hisayama study. Circulation 136, 516–525. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.116.025667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, R. (2006). Ageing and the brain. Postgrad. Med. J. 82, 84–88. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.2005.036665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, C., Baktir, M. A., Srivatsan, M., and Salehi, A. (2014). Neuroprotective effects of physical activity on the brain: a closer look at trophic factor signaling. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 8:170. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, M. C., Mormino, E., Soldan, A., James, B. D., Yu, L., Armstrong, N. M., et al. (2018). Combined neuropathological pathways account for age-related risk of dementia. Ann. Neurol. 84, 10–22. doi: 10.1002/ana.25246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rikli, R. E., and Jones, C. J. (1999a). Development and validation of a functional fitness test for community-residing older adults. J. Aging Phys. Act. 7, 129–161. doi: 10.1123/japa.7.2.129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rikli, R. E., and Jones, C. J. (1999b). Functional fitness normative scores for community-residing older adults, ages 60–94. J. Aging Phys. Act. 7, 162–181. doi: 10.1123/japa.7.2.162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saul, J. (1990). Beat-To-beat variations of heart rate reflect modulation of cardiac autonomic outflow. Physiology 5, 32–37. doi: 10.1152/physiologyonline.1990.5.1.32

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherwood, C. C., Gordon, A. D., Allen, J. S., Phillips, K. A., Erwin, J. M., Hof, P. R., et al. (2011). Aging of the cerebral cortex differs between humans and chimpanzees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 13029–13034. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016709108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares-Miranda, L., Sattelmair, J., Chaves, P., Duncan, G., Siscovick, D. S., Stein, P. K., et al. (2014). Physical activity and heart rate variability in older adults: the cardiovascular health study. Circulation 129, 2100–2110. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.113.005361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorond, F. A., Cruz-Almeida, Y., Clark, D. J., Viswanathan, A., Scherzer, C. R., De Jager, P., et al. (2015). Aging, the central nervous system and mobility in older adults: neural mechanisms of mobility impairment. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 70, 1526–1532. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreen, O., and Strauss, E. (1998). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. (1996). Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation 93, 1043–1065. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.93.5.1043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., and Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation and health. Ann. Behav. Med. 37, 141–153. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9101-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J. Affect. Disord. 61, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00338-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenza, G., Sclocco, R., Duggento, A., Passamonti, L., Napadow, V., Barbieri, R., et al. (2019). The central autonomic network at rest: uncovering functional MRI correlates of time-varying autonomic outflow. NeuroImage 197, 383–390. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, L. M., Meng, Y., Xhima, K., Lipsman, N., Hamani, C., and Aubert, I. (2018). The neuroprotective effects of exercise: maintaining a healthy brain throughout aging. Brain Plast. 4, 17–52. doi: 10.3233/bpl-180069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermeij, A., van Beek, A. H. E. A., Reijs, B. L. R., Claassen, J. A. H. R., and Kessels, R. P. C. (2014). An exploratory study of the effects of spatial working-memory load on prefrontal activation in low- and high-performing elderly. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:303. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Chaddock, L., Kim, J. S., Alves, H., et al. (2013a). Neurobiological markers of exercise-related brain plasticity in older adults. Brain Behav. Immun. 28, 90–99. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Heo, S., Prakash, R. S., Erickson, K. I., Alves, H., Chaddock, L., et al. (2013b). The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2972–2985. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, W., Cui, B., Han, Y., Song, H., Li, K., He, Y., et al. (2019). Disrupted regional cerebral blood flow, functional activity and connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease: a combined asl perfusion and resting state fMRI study. Front. Neurosci. 13:738. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognition, executive functions, working memory, autonomic nervous system, heart rate variability, aging, physical fitness

Citation: Matos FO, Vido A, Garcia WF, Lopes WA and Pereira A (2020) A Neurovisceral Integrative Study on Cognition, Heart Rate Variability, and Fitness in the Elderly. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:51. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00051

Received: 15 August 2019; Accepted: 17 February 2020;
Published: 06 March 2020.

Edited by:

Mario Bernardo-Filho, Rio de Janeiro State University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Douglas F. Watt, Lesley University, United States
Eddy A. Van Der Zee, University of Groningen, Netherlands

Copyright © 2020 Matos, Vido, Garcia, Lopes and Pereira. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Felipe de Oliveira Matos, fomatos@uem.br

Download