Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 28 May 2021
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 13 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2021.670166

Can Exergames Be Improved to Better Enhance Behavioral Adaptability in Older Adults? An Ecological Dynamics Perspective

  • Aix-Marseille Université & CNRS, ISM UMR 7287, Institut des Sciences du Mouvement, Marseille, France

Finding effective training solutions to attenuate the alterations of behavior and cognition in the growing number of older adults is an important challenge for Science and Society. By offering 3D computer-simulated environments to combine perceptual-motor and cognitive exercise, exergames are promising in this respect. However, a careful analysis of meta-analytic reviews suggests that they failed to be more effective than conventional motor-cognitive training. We analyzed the reasons for this situation, and we proposed new directions to design new, conceptually grounded, exergames. Consistent with the evolutionary neuroscience approach, we contend that new solutions should better combine high level of metabolic activity with (neuro)muscular, physical, perceptual-motor, and cognitive stimulations. According to the Ecological Dynamics rationale, we assume that new exergames should act at the agent–environment scale to allow individuals to explore, discover, and adapt to immersive and informationally rich environments that should include cognitively challenging tasks, while being representative of daily living situations.

Striving for Healthy Active Aging: A Scientific and Public Health Challenge

With the increase in life expectancy, the number of older adults suffering from declines in brain, cognitive, physical, and perceptual-motor functions also increases. Thus, an important challenge for cognitive and behavioral science is to identify the most attractive and effective solutions for attenuating the deleterious effects of aging on the brain–mind–body system (BMBS). By combining physical and cognitive exercises thanks to virtual environments and game scenarios, exergames are promising in this respect and constitute a growing market exploited by the video games and fitness industries to penetrate the field of geriatrics and rehabilitation. Introduced by the big players of the video games industry, exergames are considered as innovative solutions to improve brain functions and cognitive performance. This hypothesis remains, however, to be unequivocally demonstrated in older adults. In this prospective paper, on the basis of available reviews and meta-analyses, we address this issue. Our conclusion is that, until now, exergames did not keep their promises, in particular in comparison with conventional cognitive-motor training. We contend that, beyond the heterogeneity and methodological weakness of exergames studies, a more fundamental reason is that most commercial products are implicitly based on the theoretical commitments of the classic cognitive science, which considers dual-task training as the gold standard for targeting brain plasticity and cognitive functioning (Wollesen and Voelcker-Rehage, 2014; Gallou-Guyot et al., 2020; Wollesen et al., 2020). This has two important consequences. On the one hand, exergames do not fully exploit the functional relationships between perceptual-motor, physical, and cognitive domains resulting from our evolutionary history (Raichlen and Alexander, 2017). On the other hand, they do not take advantage of the resources offered by virtual reality to improve adaptive perceptual-motor behaviors, through (inter)active exploration of immersive environments. To remedy this situation, we suggest some avenues, grounded on the theoretical frameworks of evolutionary neuroscience and ecological dynamics that could inspire the design of Ecological-Exergames (E-EG).

Exercise and Cognition: From Separate to Combined Training

For a long time, cognitive training was considered the only way to improve brain functions and cognitive performance and make older adults smarter and more able to learn faster and better (Simons et al., 2016). However, although positive training effects were observed in some studies [e.g. (Green and Bavelier, 2003, 2006; Anguera et al., 2013)], inconsistent results were also frequently reported [for meta-analyses, see (Lampit et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2014)], which has led to vivid controversies [for an overview (Simons et al., 2016)]. Progressively, they died down, thanks to the studies that have identified the effective specifications of cognitively demanding digital environments, which make video games effective to stimulate brain and cognition [for details, see (Bediou et al., 2018; Dale et al., 2020)].

In this context, the meta-analysis conducted by Colcombe and Kramer (2003) has considerably renewed the view of the relationships between exercise and cognition by showing that physical activity may also improve neuroplasticity and cognitive functioning. Then, subsequent studies contributed to elucidate the mechanisms underlying the benefits of endurance training and muscular resistance training on brain and cognitive functioning [for overviews (Voelcker-Rehage and Niemann, 2013; Herold et al., 2019; Netz, 2019)]. More recently, it has been demonstrated that complex motor skills training also improved brain plasticity and cognitive functioning [e.g. (Voelcker-Rehage et al., 2010, 2011; Niemann et al., 2014; Netz, 2019)]. For instance, available data suggested that the effects of aerobic training were magnified when aerobic effort was supported by complex motor skills [e.g. (Pesce, 2012; Diamond and Ling, 2016, 2019)]. In addition, physical-cognitive training (Fissler et al., 2013; Bamidis et al., 2015) and motor-cognitive training (Gallou-Guyot et al., 2020; Gavelin et al., 2020) were found to be more effective than separate training to improve cognitive functions [but see (Zhu et al., 2016)], for a different conclusion]. In view of these findings, exergames are expected to be more effective than conventional training (i.e., interventions supervised by a coach and not assisted by technologies) to improve behavior and cognition in older adults (Stanmore et al., 2017; Martin-Niedecken and Mekler, 2018). However, this optimistic hypothesis needs to be confirmed, in particular with respect to conventional motor-cognitive training and physical-cognitive training. This issue is addressed in the following sections.

Are Exergames Effective for Enhancing Brain Plasticity and Cognition?

Although encouraging results were observed in several studies [e.g. (Anderson-Hanley et al., 2012; Maillot et al., 2012; Barcelos et al., 2015; Eggenberger et al., 2015; Schättin et al., 2016; Ballesteros et al., 2018)], controversial findings have also been reported as exergames were found to be effective [e.g. (Stanmore et al., 2017)], ineffective (Ordnung et al., 2017; Sala et al., 2019), or unclear (Stojan and Voelcker-Rehage, 2019), in comparison with conventional interventions. Several reasons may explain these inconsistencies. The first reason lies on the low methodological quality and the great heterogeneity of the available studies, which is reported by most reviews and meta-analyses reported (Stojan and Voelcker-Rehage, 2019; Gallou-Guyot et al., 2020; Gavelin et al., 2020). A second reason is that most studies were carried out with off-the-shelf products, which (i) lacked proper theoretical founding concepts, (ii) disregarded basic learning and training principles provided by Movement and Sport Sciences (Caserman et al., 2020) and, (iii) were neither suited for research purposes nor adapted to the specific psychological, physical, and cognitive characteristics of older adults [e.g. (Ordnung et al., 2017)]. A third reason is that the contents of digital environments and game scenarios provided by the different products were scarcely analyzed in the literature [for a noticeable exception, see (Fronza et al., 2019)], though research on action video games has demonstrated that they significantly affect the level of cognitive demands (Bediou et al., 2018; Dale et al., 2020) and that they could have more weight in the observed benefits on cognition than the physical components (Maillot et al., 2012). Thus, whether the virtual environments of the current commercial exergames (more or less) heavily load cognitive processes remains unknown. Similarly, whether the movements requested by gameplays were effectively performed by the players was never analyzed in the different studies [for a noticeable exception, see (Anders et al., 2020)]. In addition, in most studies interested in the effects of exergaming on cognition, changes in cardio-respiratory fitness and muscle strength were not analyzed. However, this should be a prerequisite for the analysis of cognitive modifications, which are presumably mediated, at least in part, by the improvement of aerobic capacities and muscle strength. Inconsistent findings have been reported in this respect in the literature. In their meta-analysis, Peng et al. (2011) concluded that exergames facilitate light-to-moderate physical activity [see (Maillot et al., 2012) for confirming evidence], but they also mentioned that participants were often below the moderate level. However, it might depend on the type of used exergame. In support of this hypothesis, using a biking exergame, Moholt et al. (2017) showed that Exergaming can be an innovative way of high-intensity training. With respect to muscle strength, although Monteiro-Junior et al. (2016) argued that, due to weight-bearing exercises, exergames might lead to effects comparable to those observed during muscular resistance training, this hypothesis was challenged by DeVries et al. (2020), who showed reported low muscular activation in balance games. A fourth reason is that, while a wide range of exergames was used to support training interventions across the different studies, their potential differences in effectiveness were scarcely assessed in the literature. This was also the case for lab-customized exergames [e.g. (Ben-Sadoun et al., 2016; Anderson-Hanley et al., 2017; Wall et al., 2018)], whose effects were never systematically compared with those of commercial products or of conventional training [for a noticeable exception, see (Willaert et al., 2020)]. Thus, the conclusions reported in most reviews on exergames were generally based on the assumption that the different exergames were all comparable in their effectiveness, which was certainly wrong.

In summary, studies on exergames predominantly aimed at describing their benefits on cognition and behavior [scarcely on brain functions (Stojan and Voelcker-Rehage, 2019)] by comparing them to a control group of more or less active individuals [e.g. (Maillot et al., 2012; Karssemeijer et al., 2019; Adcock et al., 2020)]. Few studies aimed at determining whether their benefits were larger than those of physical or cognitive training [e.g. (Martin-Niedecken and Schättin, 2020)]. On the other hand, whether exergames are equally or more effective than conventional motor-cognitive training has been scarcely addressed until now and available findings are inconsistent in this respect [e.g. see (Eggenberger et al., 2015; Htut et al., 2018) versus (Bacha et al., 2018; Schättin et al., 2018)]. A plausible reason is that the differences between the mechanisms at work in conventional motor-cognitive training and exergaming are still unclear. Monteiro-Junior et al. (2016) argued that these mechanisms could be, at least in part, similar. Specifically, neuroplasticity (i.e., neurogenesis, synaptogenesis, and angiogenesis) could be facilitated by the stimulation of neurotrophic factors (e.g. BDNF) resulting from light to moderate aerobic physical activity, while guidance effects might result from interactions with 3D virtual environments, which are hypothesized to increase the activation of the brain areas that are related to involved cognitive functions. With respect to cognitive content of commercial exergames, several authors considered that they allow delivering motor-cognitive dual-task training (MCDT) [e.g. (Wollesen and Voelcker-Rehage, 2014; Monteiro-Junior et al., 2016; Anders et al., 2018; Gallou-Guyot et al., 2020; Gavelin et al., 2020; Wollesen et al., 2020)] since additional challenges, in the form of extra tasks, such as counting, matching objects, or avoiding obstacles, are often used to add cognitive training to the physical exercise (Anders et al., 2018). These elements create dual task situations [i.e., Thinking while moving situations, (Herold et al., 2018)], in which the player needs to focus on two or more things simultaneously. Strikingly, few studies investigated the effects of exergames on dual-task performance [for noticeable exceptions, see (Schättin et al., 2016; Peng et al., 2020)] but, recently, Gallou-Guyot et al. (2020) compared (indirectly) conventional MCDT training and exergaming. They did not observe any superiority of the latter over the former, though some studies suggested the opposite [e.g. (Eggenberger et al., 2015)]. In addition, they reported that most of the tested exergames weakly improved physical performance, suggesting that cognitive-motor dual-task training delivered through exergames moderately loaded metabolic capacities [e.g. (Larsen et al., 2013; Anderson-Hanley et al., 2017; Levin et al., 2017; Martin-Niedecken and Mekler, 2018; Taylor et al., 2018), but see Peng et al., 2011; Maillot et al., 2012], for a different conclusion]. Consequently, one can conclude that, in most cases, exergames did not fully exploit the synergy between facilitation and guidance effects, which is considered a basic condition for observing strong benefits on brain structures and cognitive functions, when aerobic and cognitive exercise are associated (Fabel et al., 2009; Fissler et al., 2013; Raichlen and Alexander, 2017). In summary, the available literature did not demonstrate unequivocally the superiority of exergames over the other forms of training (see Table 1). These findings suggest that new exergames should be designed to improve their effectiveness relative to conventional motor-cognitive training. In the following, we propose some avenues in this respect.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary table of the main findings of the literature for (i) cognitive training alone, (ii) physical/motor training alone, (iii) combined motor-cognitive and physical-cognitive training, and (iv) simultaneous, dual-task training.

How Can Effectiveness of Exergames Be Improved?

According to the above considerations, new exergames should be designed to allow better combining high level of metabolic (aerobic) activity with (neuro)muscular, perceptual-motor, and cognitive stimulations, to provide the whole BMBS the multiple and synergically combined inputs that are necessary for its plastic adaptation. This hypothesis is consistent with the Adaptive Capacity Model (ACM) developed by Raichlen and Alexander (2017) in the context of the evolutionary neuroscience approach. However, a limitation of the ACM is that it endorses the mainstream approach of cognitive (neuro)science, without deeply considering the potential of virtual environments to elicit other mechanisms, at the scale of the performer–environment relationship, which underlie the way individuals perceive, explore, navigate, and, finally, act upon their surroundings in dynamic environments (Raja and Calvo, 2017). This hypothesis is consistent with the framework of ecological dynamics (ED), which may help to formalize these mechanisms and provide guidelines for designing new products.

The Ecological Dynamics Approach

Ecological dynamics focuses on the whole BMBS, considered as a complex dynamical system (Kelso, 1995; Seifert et al., 2015) that is embedded and informationally coupled to the continuously evolving environment (Chiel and Beer, 1997). Thus, agent–environment relationship is considered the appropriate level of analysis of adaptation mechanisms. In the BMBS, the perceptual, cognitive, cardiovascular, respiratory, and motor subsystems are deeply intertwined by virtue of mutual couplings, which give rise to stable and flexible behavioral (motor) patterns that are shaped by the coalition of organismic, task, and environmental constraints (Newell, 1985; Bingham et al., 1991; Chiel and Beer, 1997). These flexible patterns make the BMBS able to adapt to the dynamic environment.

In this perspective, perception and action cannot be understood one without the other. On the perceptual side, it is assumed that information is directly available and can be picked up by individuals to attune action (Gibson, 1979). Thus, the agent would perceive affordances, i.e., action opportunities (e.g. reaching, grasping, sitting, walking, jumping, etc.) provided to the agent by the substances, surfaces, objects, and other living creatures that surround it (i.e., the environment) (Fajen et al., 2008). That means that the environment is perceived in terms of properties scaled to the performer’s motor abilities, and action, considered as an expression of cognition, is the behavioral realization of an affordance. Thus, behavioral dynamics emerge from the continuous exploration of the perceptual-motor workspace that links the performer’s perception of task constraints to the opportunities for action, given the state of the agent–environment system at each moment in time (Pacheco et al., 2019).

The idea that perception and cognition are mutually embedded and embodied in action leads to consider cognition as a virtual supervisor, distributed over the brain, mind, body, and environment, whose function is twofold. On the one hand, cognition modulates functional interactions between the different components of the BMBS, thereby allowing to shape the emerging behavioral patterns [e.g. (Zanone and Kelso, 1992; Monno et al., 2002; Taylor et al., 2018; Temprado et al., 2020)]. On the other hand, cognition supports the attunement of behavior to the perceptual-motor workspace by supervising how the individual does something somewhere (Araujo, 2009), that is, how it attains a functional relationship with the world by virtue of changes in information-movement coupling [e.g. (Davids et al., 2015)].

In this context, behavioral adaptability is the primary criterion for assessing individual functional states and training benefits. It can be directly or indirectly assessed through dynamical markers (Sleimen-Malkoun et al., 2014), as, for instance, directly through (i) the repertoire of behavioral patterns (Sleimen-Malkoun et al., 2013, 2014; Temprado et al., 2020) and/or of execution strategies [e.g. (Poletti et al., 2016)] that are available and effectively used by the performer; and (ii) the dynamics of switching between behavioral patterns and strategies that are observed when task constraints are increased [e.g. (Kelso, 1984; Temprado et al., 2020)]. Moreover, adaptability can also be indirectly assessed through the complex structure of the non-linear fluctuations of the output of the system under consideration [e.g. (Lipsitz, 2002; Vaillancourt and Newell, 2002)]. It has been suggested that complexity markers can be used to assess the training benefits of behavioral adaptability [e.g. (Manor and Lipsitz, 2013; Wayne et al., 2013)], but to our knowledge, this approach has not been yet applied to the study of the consequences of cognitive-motor training with exergames.

Designing New Exergames: An Ecological Dynamics Perspective

Two main assumptions should guide the design of exergames inspired by the ecological dynamics framework (hereafter E-Exergames). First of all, the agent–environment relationship is the appropriate level at which E-Exergames must act to transform behavior and cognition. Secondly, to develop the capacity to produce adaptive behaviors, E-Exergames should allow older adults to refine or enlarge their behavioral repertoire through repeated stimulation of the synergistic relationships between cardiovascular, muscular, perceptual-motor, and cognitive subsystems, thanks to performing complex motor tasks in immersive environments. In sum, E-Exergames should be designed to allow the players learning by exploring, discovering, and adapting to dynamic environments (Rudd et al., 2020), while producing moderate to high level of cardiovascular effort and muscular force (Wall et al., 2018).

To achieve these general objectives, E-Exergames should confront older adults with novel, challenging, immersive, and interactive 3D environments to facilitate the transfer of training benefits to daily living tasks. This issue is crucial and might lead to a superiority of E-Exergames over classic commercial products. Specifically, game scenarios provided by virtual environments should simulate natural activities, in emotionally salient contexts, to afford goal-oriented, exploratory motor behaviors (e.g. spatial navigation, catching, hitting, aiming, interception of mobile objects, aperture crossing, jumping, coordinating multi-limb, or controlling dynamic balance). In addition, training situations should facilitate the detection of action possibilities, to make quick and accurate decisions under time pressure, in a context of rapidly changing visual scenes [e.g. (Bediou et al., 2018; Dale et al., 2020)]. In this aim, virtual environments should enhance affordances (walkability, reachability, graspability, throwability, etc.) and allow manipulating task constraints, to afford different action possibilities and lead the exer-players to either exploit the existing patterns of their repertoire or insert new (learned) patterns (Zanone and Kelso, 1992). These guidelines are consistent with the Moving while thinking training situations, which have been recently hypothesized to be optimal training conditions since, in these situations, cognition is “incorporated” into motor tasks, making them more closely related to real daily life situations (Herold et al., 2018; see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Thinking while Moving and Moving while Thinking training situations, which are hypothesized to be two different kinds training conditions. In the former, the cognitive task is simply added to the motor/physical task, while in the latter, cognition is “incorporated” into motor tasks, making them more closely related to real daily life situations [modified from Herold et al. (2018)].

To be more effective than commercial products, E-Exergames should combine the principles of effective cardiovascular and muscular training [e.g. (Niemann et al., 2014; Wall et al., 2018; Herold et al., 2019)], with those of perceptual and motor learning proposed by movement and training sciences [e.g. (Papegaaij and Steenbrink, 2017; Stone et al., 2019)]. In this aim, game scenarios should also be adapted to allow task simplification, progressive increase in tasks difficulty, manipulation of information, modulation of constraints, variable conditions of practice, and feedback of information about successful behaviors and goal achievements (Vaillancourt and Newell, 2002; Hristovski and Araujo, 2009; Rudd et al., 2020). Task simplification aims at making the tasks easier to achieve, while keeping present all the components of the perceptual-motor behavior (perception, decision-making, and movement patterns). Then, difficulty of the tasks must be progressively increased over the training program, in order to keep the players in the optimal flow zone. Information manipulation allows enhancing the perceptual saliency of existing affordances that invite specific functional actions needed to reach intended task goals (Raja and Calvo, 2017). It can be achieved by making some aspects of the environment more salient, while blocking others (Raja and Calvo, 2017) and/or by including additional cues, thanks to Augmented Reality (AR). In this aim, the most important information to be picked up must be identified to be subsequently used to direct the attentional focus in order to facilitate affordance perception and learning (Wulf, 2013; Becker and Fairbrother, 2019). Manipulation of constraints related to tasks, individual, or environment (Newell, 1985) allows providing variables conditions that force the exer-player to explore and discover the optimal solution (Rudd et al., 2020). Indeed, variable conditions of practice allow the exer-player to explore the perceptual-motor workspace without imposing movement solutions, until the behavioral patterns become stable and reproductible (Pacheco et al., 2019). Information feedback provided to the players will be helpful in this respect, allowing them to either reinforce their behavioral responses or explore new solutions.

Though prominently acting at the agent–environment level, E-Exergames will target cognition through the “explore–discover–adapt” process, which requires a cognitive supervision and, specifically, the involvement of executive functions, through intention and attention (Kramer and Erickson, 2007). In this context, Augmented Reality included in E-Exergames may allow educating intention (through instructions, rules, etc.) and the direction of attentional focus to the more useful variables (through the salience of some features), which, in turn, influences exploration of the environment and the detection of affordances. E-Exergames will also contribute to stimulate brain plasticity and self-repairs of the altered functional components of the BMBS, thanks to the dissipation of flows of energy and information generated by exploratory motor behavior produced at moderate-to high-intensity levels of cardiovascular effort (Kramer and Erickson, 2007; Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014; Raichlen and Alexander, 2017). Based on these mechanisms, extensive practice of E-Exergames could contribute to restore age-related loss of complexity of the whole brain–mind–body–behavior system that is currently associated with behavioral adaptability (Vaillancourt and Newell, 2002; Manor et al., 2010).

Conclusion and Perspectives

During the last two decades, a number of studies aimed to assess the effects of exergaming on behavior and cognition. However, due to its great heterogeneity and methodological weaknesses, poorly informative or inconsistent findings piled up across studies, which precluded researchers to firmly conclude its superiority over conventional training.

In the present paper, we proposed new directions to design (hopefully more effective) E-Exergames, which should be based on two main pillars: on the one hand, exploiting the functional relationships between perceptual-motor, physical, and cognitive domains resulting from our evolutionary history, and on the other hand, acting at the agent–environment scale to allow individuals to explore the dynamic perceptual-motor workspace offered by each game situation and to discover and select appropriate actions in order to adapt to complex environments. In addition, to improve engagement and enjoyment, they should to create a positive flow experience for the players (Martin-Niedecken and Mekler, 2018). Then, E-Exergames would be both attractive, to encourage further meaningful engagement in active live habits, and effective to develop a wide range of complex and flexible perceptual-motor skills (Rudd et al., 2020).

To verify the hypotheses developed in the present paper, further studies are necessary, in particular to determine whether and under which conditions they are more effective, attractive, and acceptable than currently available commercial products and conventional training interventions.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The author thanks Gilles Montagne and Marta Torre for reading the preliminary versions of the manuscript.

References

Adcock, M., Sonder, F., Schättin, A., Gennaro, F., and de Bruin, E. D. (2020). A usability study of a multicomponent video game-based training for older adults. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act 17:3.

Google Scholar

Anders, P., Bengtson, E. I., Grønvik, K. B., Skjæret-Maroni, N., and Vereijken, B. (2020). Balance training in older adults using exergames: game speed and cognitive elements affect how seniors play. Front. Sports Act Living 2:54. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2020.00054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anders, P., Lehmann, T., Müller, H., Grønvik, K. B., Skjæret-Maroni, N., Baumeister, J., et al. (2018). Exergames inherently contain cognitive elements as indicated by cortical processing. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:102. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson-Hanley, C., Arciero, P. J., Brickman, A. M., Nimon, J. P., Okuma, N., Westen, S. C., et al. (2012). Exergaming and older adult cognition: a cluster randomized clinical trial. Am. J. Prev. Med. 42, 109–119.

Google Scholar

Anderson-Hanley, C., Maloney, M., Barcelos, N., Striegnitz, K., and Kramer, A. (2017). Neuropsychological benefits of neuro-exergaming for older adults: a pilot study of an interactive physical and cognitive exercise system (iPACES). J. Aging Phys. Act 25, 73–83. doi: 10.1123/japa.2015-0261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., et al. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501, 97–101. doi: 10.1038/nature12486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Araujo, D. (2009). Introduction-Preface to” Ecological approaches to cognition in sport and exercise”. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 40:1215.

Google Scholar

Bacha, J. M. R., Gomes, G. C. V., de Freitas, T. B., Viveiro, L. A. P., da Silva, K. G., Bueno, G. C., et al. (2018). Effects of kinect adventures games versus conventional physical therapy on postural control in elderly people: a randomized controlled trial. Games Health J. 7, 24–36. doi: 10.1089/g4h.2017.0065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., Voelcker-Rehage, C., and Bherer, L. (2018). Editorial: cognitive and brain plasticity induced by physical exercise, cognitive training, video games, and combined interventions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:169. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamidis, P. D., Fissler, P., Küster, O. C., Wischniowski, J., Thurm, F., Elbert, T., et al. (2015). Gains in cognition through combined cognitive and physical training: the role of training dosage and severity of neurocognitive disorder. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:152. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barcelos, N., Shah, N., Cohen, K., Hogan, M. J., Mulkerrin, E., Arciero, P. J., et al. (2015). Aerobic and cognitive exercise (ACE) pilot study for older adults: executive function improves with cognitive challenge while exergaming. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 21, 768–779. doi: 10.1017/s1355617715001083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, K. A., and Fairbrother, J. T. (2019). The use of multiple externally directed attentional focus cues facilitates motor learning. J. Sports Sci. 14, 651–657. doi: 10.1177/1747954119870172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bediou, B., Adams, D. M., Mayer, R. E., Tipton, E., Green, C. S., Bavelier, D., et al. (2018). Meta-analysis of action video game impact on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills. Psychol. Bull. 144, 77–110. doi: 10.1037/bul0000130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Sadoun, G., Sacco, G., Manera, V., Bourgeois, J., König, A., Foulon, P., et al. (2016). Physical and cognitive stimulation using an exergame in subjects with normal aging, mild and moderate cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 53, 1299–1314. doi: 10.3233/jad-160268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bingham, G. P., Schmidt, R. C., Turvey, M. T., and Rosenblum, L. D. (1991). Task dynamics and resource dynamics in the assembly of a coordinated rhythmic activity. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept Perform. 17, 359–381. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.17.2.359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caserman, P., Hoffmann, K., Müller, P., Schaub, M., Straßburg, K., Wiemeyer, J., et al. (2020). Quality criteria for serious games: serious part, game part, and balance. JMIR Ser. Games 8:e19037. doi: 10.2196/19037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiel, H. J., and Beer, R. D. (1997). The brain has a body: adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment. Trends Neurosci. 20, 553–557. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(97)01149-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, G., Joessel, A., Bavelier, D., and Green, C. S. (2020). A new look at the cognitive neuroscience of video game play. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1464, 192–203. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davids, K., Araújo, D., Seifert, L., and Orth, D. (2015). “Expert performance in sport,” in An Ecological Dynamics Perspective. Routledge Handbook of Sport Expertise, eds J. Baker and D. Farrow (London: Routledge), 130–135.

Google Scholar

DeVries, A. W., Willaert, J., Jonkers, I., van Dieën, J. H., and Verschueren, S. M. P. (2020). Virutal reality balance games provide little muscular challenge to prevent muscle weakness in healthy older adults. Game Health J. 9:3.

Google Scholar

Diamond, A., and Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A., and Ling, D. S. (2019). “Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory,” in Cognitive and Working Memory Training: Perspectives From Psychology, Neuroscience, and Human Development, eds J. M. Novick, M. F. Bunting, M. R. Dougherty, and R. W. Engle (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 145–389.

Google Scholar

Eggenberger, P., Schumacher, V., Angst, M., Theill, N., and de Bruin, E. D. (2015). Does multicomponent physical exercise with simultaneous cognitive training boost cognitive performance in older adults? A 6-month randomized controlled trial with a 1-year follow-up. Clin. Interv. Aging 10, 1335–1349. doi: 10.2147/cia.s87732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabel, K., Wolf, S. A., Ehninger, D., Babu, H., Leal-Galicia, P., Kempermann, G., et al. (2009). Additive effects of physical exercise and environmental enrichment on adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. Front. Neurosci. 3:50. doi: 10.3389/neuro.22.002.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., and Turvey, M. T. (2008). Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 40, 79–107.

Google Scholar

Fissler, P., Küster, O., Schlee, W., and Kolassa, I. T. (2013). Novelty interventions to enhance broad cognitive abilities and prevent dementia: synergistic approaches for the facilitation of positive plastic change. Prog. Brain Res. 207, 403–434. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-444-63327-9.00017-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fronza, F. C. A. C., Ferrari, E. P., Ramos, D. K., Sartori, A. S., and Cardoso, F. L. (2019). Cognitive content of commercial exergames. Intern. J. Game Based Learn. 9, 23–37. doi: 10.4018/ijgbl.2019070102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallou-Guyot, M., Mandigout, S., Bherer, L., and Perrochon, A. (2020). Effects of exergames and cognitive-motor dual-task training on cognitive, physical and dual-task functions in cognitively healthy older adults: an overview. Age. Res. Rev. 63:101135. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2020.101135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gavelin, H. M., Dong, C., Minkov, R., Bahar-Fuchs, A., Ellis, K. A., Lautenschlager, N. T., et al. (2020). Combined physical and cognitive training for older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Age. Res. Rev. 66:101232. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2020.101232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1979). “The theory of affordances,” in The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception, (Boston: Houghton Mifflin), 127–143.

Google Scholar

Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423, 534–537. doi: 10.1038/nature01647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2006). “The cognitive neuroscience of video games,” in Digital Media: Transformations in Human Communication, Vol. 1, eds P. Messaris and L. Humphreys (New York, NY: Peter Lang), 211–223.

Google Scholar

Herold, F., Hamacher, D., Schega, L., and Müller, N. G. (2018). Thinking While moving or moving while thinking - concepts of motor-cognitive training for cognitive performance enhancement. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:228. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herold, F., Törpel, A., Schega, L., and Müller, N. G. (2019). Functional and/or structural brain changes in response to resistance exercises and resistance training lead to cognitive improvements - a systematic review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act 16:10.

Google Scholar

Hristovski, R., and Araujo, D. (2009). “Information for regulating action in sport: metastability and emergence of tactical solutions under,” in Perspectives on Cognition and Action in Sport, eds M. Raab, D. Araujo, and H. Ripoll (New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 43–57.

Google Scholar

Htut, T. Z. C., Hiengkaew, V., Jalayondeja, C., and Vongsirinavarat, M. (2018). Effects of physical, virtual reality-based, and brain exercise on physical, cognition, and preference in older persons: a randomized controlled trial. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act 15:10.

Google Scholar

Karssemeijer, E. G. A., Aaronson, J. A., Bossers, W. J. R., Donders, R., Olde Rikkert, M. G. M., Kessels, R. P. C., et al. (2019). The quest for synergy between physical exercise and cognitive stimulation via exergaming in people with dementia: a randomized controlled trial. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 11:3.

Google Scholar

Kelso, J. A. (1984). Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am. J. Physiol. 246, R1000–R1004.

Google Scholar

Kelso, J. A. (1995). Dynamic Pattern: the self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., and Erickson, K. I. (2007). Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 342–348. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lampit, A., Hallock, H., and Valenzuela, M. (2014). Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. 11:e1001756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen, L. H., Schou, L., Lund, H. H., and Langberg, H. (2013). The physical effect of exergames in healthy elderly-a systematic review. Games Health J. 2, 205–212. doi: 10.1089/g4h.2013.0036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, O., Netz, Y., and Ziv, G. (2017). The beneficial effects of different types of exercise interventions on motor and cognitive functions in older age: a systematic review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act 14:20.

Google Scholar

Lipsitz, L. A. (2002). Dynamics of stability: the physiologic basis of functional health and frailty. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 57, B115–B125.

Google Scholar

Maillot, P., Perrot, A., and Hartley, A. (2012). Effects of interactive physical-activity video-game training on physical and cognitive function in older adults. Psychol. Aging 27, 589–600. doi: 10.1037/a0026268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manor, B., Costa, M. D., Hu, K., Newton, E., Starobinets, O., Kang, H. G., et al. (2010). Physiological complexity and system adaptability: evidence from postural control dynamics of older adults. J. Appl. Physiol. 109, 1786–1791. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00390.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manor, B., and Lipsitz, L. A. (2013). Physiologic complexity and aging: implications for physical function and rehabilitation. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 287–293. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin-Niedecken, A. L., and Mekler, E. D. (2018). “The ExerCube: participatory design of an immersive fitness game environment,” in Proceedings of the Joint International Conference on Serious Games, ed. S. Göbel (Cham: Springer), 263–275. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-02762-9_28

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin-Niedecken, A. L., and Schättin, A. (2020). Let the Body’n’Brain games begin: toward innovative training approaches in eSports athletes. Front. Psychol. 11:138. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moholt, T., Weie, S., Chorianopoulos, K., Wang, A. I., and Hagen, K. (2017). Exergaming can be an innovative way of enjoyable high-intensity interval training. BMJ Open Sport Exerc. Med. 3:e000258. doi: 10.1136/bmjsem-2017-000258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monno, A., Temprado, J. J., Zanone, P. G., and Laurent, M. (2002). The interplay of attention and bimanual coordination dynamics. Acta Psychol. 110, 187–211. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(02)00033-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monteiro-Junior, R. S., Vaghetti, C. A. O., and Camas Deslandes, A. (2016). Exergames: neuroplastic hypothesis about cognitive improvement and biological effects on physical function of institutionalized older persons. Neural Regen. Res. 11, 201–204. doi: 10.4103/1673-5374.177709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Netz, Y. (2019). Is there a preferred mode of exercise for cognition enhancement in older age?-A narrative review. Front. Med. 6:57. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2019.00057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newell, K. M. (1985). Coordination, control and skill. Adv. Psychol. 27, 295–317. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4115(08)62541-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemann, C., Godde, B., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2014). Not only cardiovascular, but also coordinative exercise increases hippocampal volume in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:170. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ordnung, M., Hoff, M., Kaminski, E., Villringer, A., and Ragert, P. (2017). No overt effects of a 6-week exergame training on sensorimotor and cognitive function in older adults. a preliminary investigation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:160. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco, M. M., Lafe, C. W., and Newell, K. M. (2019). Search strategies in the perceptual-motor workspace and the acquisition of coordination, control, and skill. Front. Psychol. 10:1874. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papegaaij, S., and Steenbrink, F. (2017). Clinical Gait Analysis: Treadmill-Based vs Overground. Seattle, WA: Motek Inc.

Google Scholar

Peng, H. T., Tien, C. W., Lin, P. S., Peng, H. Y., and Song, C. Y. (2020). Novel mat exergaming to improve the physical performance, cognitive function, and dual-task walking and decrease the fall risk of community-dwelling older adults. Front. Psychol. 11:1620. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, W., Lin, J. H., and Crouse, J. (2011). Is playing exergames really exercising? A meta-analysis of energy expenditure in active video games. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 681–688. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesce, C. (2012). Shifting the focus from quantitative to qualitative exercise characteristics in exercise and cognition research. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 34, 766–786. doi: 10.1123/jsep.34.6.766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poletti, C., Malkoun, R. S., Decker, L. M., Retornaz, F., Lemaire, P., Temprado, J.-J., et al. (2016). Strategic variations in Fitts’ task: comparison of healthy older adults and cognitively impaired patients. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:334. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichlen, D. A., and Alexander, G. E. (2017). Adaptive capacity: an evolutionary neuroscience model linking exercise, cognition, and brain health. Trends Neurosci. 40, 408–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raja, V., and Calvo, P. (2017). Augmented reality: an ecological blend. Cogn. Syst. Res. 42, 58–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2016.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Park, D. C. (2014). How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 355–370. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudd, J. R., Pesce, C., Strafford, B. W., and Davids, K. (2020). Physical literacy - a journey of individual enrichment: an ecological dynamics rationale for enhancing performance and physical activity in all. Front. Psychol. 11:1904. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sala, G., Tatlidil, K. S., and Gobet, F. (2019). Still no evidence that exergames improve cognitive ability: a commentary on Stanmore et al. (2017). Neurosci. Biobehavl. Rev. 123, 352–353. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schättin, A., Arner, R., Gennaro, F., and de Bruin, E. D. (2016). Adaptations of prefrontal brain activity, executive functions, and gait in healthy elderly following exergame and balance training: a randomized-controlled study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:278. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schättin, A., Gennaro, F., Egloff, M., Vogt, S., and de Bruin, E. D. (2018). Physical activity, nutrition, cognition, neurophysiology, and short-time synaptic plasticity in healthy older adults: a cross-sectional study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:242. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seifert, L., Boulanger, J., Orth, D., and Davids, K. (2015). Environmental design shapes perceptual-motor exploration, learning, and transfer in climbing. Front. Psychol. 6:1819. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2016). Do “Brain-Training” programs work. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest. 17, 103–186.

Google Scholar

Sleimen-Malkoun, R., Temprado, J. J., and Berton, E. (2013). Age-related changes of movement patterns in discrete Fitts’ task. BMC Neurosci. 14:145. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-14-145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sleimen-Malkoun, R., Temprado, J. J., and Hong, S. L. (2014). Aging induced loss of complexity and dedifferentiation: consequences for coordination dynamics within and between brain, muscular and behavioral levels. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:140. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanmore, E., Stubbs, B., Vancampfort, D., de Bruin, E. D., and Firth, J. (2017). The effect of active video games on cognitive functioning in clinical and non-clinical populations: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78, 34–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stojan, R., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2019). A systematic review on the cognitive benefits and neurophysiological correlates of exergaming in healthy older adults. J. Clin. Med. 8:E734.

Google Scholar

Stone, A. E., Terza, M. J., Raffegeau, T. E., and Hass, C. J. (2019). Walking through the looking glass: adapting gait patterns with mirror feedback. J. Biomech. 83, 104–109. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, L., Kerse, N., Klenk, J., Borotkanics, R., and Maddison, R. (2018). Exergames to improve the mobility of long-term care residents: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Games Health J. 7, 37–42. doi: 10.1089/g4h.2017.0084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Temprado, J. J., Torre, M. M., Langeard, A., Julien-Vintrou, M., Devillers-Réolon, L., Sleimen-Malkoun, R., et al. (2020). Intentional switching between bimanual coordination patterns in older adults: is it mediated by inhibition processes. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:29. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toril, P., Reales, J. M., and Ballesteros, S. (2014). Video game training enhances cognition of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Aging 29, 706–716. doi: 10.1037/a0037507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaillancourt, D. E., and Newell, K. M. (2002). Changing complexity in human behavior and physiology through aging and disease. Neurobiol. Aging 23, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00247-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voelcker-Rehage, C., Godde, B., and Staudinger, U. M. (2010). Physical and motor fitness are both related to cognition in old age. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 167–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07014.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voelcker-Rehage, C., Godde, B., and Staudinger, U. M. (2011). Cardiovascular and coordination training differentially improve cognitive performance and neural processing in older adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:26. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voelcker-Rehage, C., and Niemann, C. (2013). Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2268–2295. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wall, K., Stark, J., Schillaci, A., Saulnier, E. T., McLaren, E., Striegnitz, K., et al. (2018). The Enhanced interactive physical and cognitive exercise system (iPACESTM v2.0): pilot clinical trial of an in-home ipad-based neuro-exergame for mild cognitive impairment (MCI). J. Clin. Med. 7:E249.

Google Scholar

Wayne, P. M., Manor, B., Novak, V., Costa, M. D., Hausdorff, J. M., Goldberger, A. L., et al. (2013). A systems biology approach to studying Tai Chi, physiological complexity and healthy aging: design and rationale of a pragmatic randomized controlled trial. Contemp. Clin. Trials 34, 21–34. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2012.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willaert, J., De Vries, A. W., Tavernier, J., Van Dieen, J. H., Jonkers, I., Verschueren, S., et al. (2020). Does a novel exergame challenge balance and activate muscles more than existing off-the-shelf exergames. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 17:6.

Google Scholar

Wollesen, B., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2014). Training effects on motor–cognitive dual-task performance in older adults. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act 11, 5–24.

Google Scholar

Wollesen, B., Wildbredt, A., van Schooten, K. S., Lim, M. L., and Delbaere, K. (2020). The effects of cognitive-motor training interventions on executive functions in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act 17:9.

Google Scholar

Wulf, G. (2013). Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 6, 77–104. doi: 10.1080/1750984x.2012.723728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanone, P. G., and Kelso, J. A. (1992). Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 18, 403–421. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.2.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, X., Yin, S., Lang, M., He, R., and Li, J. (2016). The more the better? A meta-analysis on effects of combined cognitive and physical intervention on cognition in healthy older adults. Age. Res. Rev. 31, 67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: exergames, cognition, brain, behavioral adaptability, ecological dynamics, complex systems

Citation: Temprado J-J (2021) Can Exergames Be Improved to Better Enhance Behavioral Adaptability in Older Adults? An Ecological Dynamics Perspective. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:670166. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.670166

Received: 20 February 2021; Accepted: 20 April 2021;
Published: 28 May 2021.

Edited by:

Eling D. de Bruin, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Federico Gennaro, Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Italy
Renato Sobral Monteiro-Junior, Unimontes, Brazil

Copyright © 2021 Temprado. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jean-Jacques Temprado, jean-jacques.temprado@univ-amu.fr

Download