Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 18 May 2022
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Measurable Brain and Cognitive Reserve: The Implication of Neuroimaging Biomarkers in the Normal Aging Process View all 11 articles

Mapping of Structure-Function Age-Related Connectivity Changes on Cognition Using Multimodal MRI

  • 1Schulich School of Medicine & Dentistry, Western University, London, ON, Canada
  • 2CIBM Center for Biomedical Imaging, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 3Institute of Bioengineering, Center for Neuroprosthetics, EPFL, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Radiology and Medical Informatics, University of Geneva (UNIGE), Geneva, Switzerland
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 6Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Western University, London, ON, Canada
  • 7The Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON, Canada
  • 8Department of Clinical Neurological Sciences, Schulich School of Medicine, Western University, London, ON, Canada

The relationship between age-related changes in brain structural connectivity (SC) and functional connectivity (FC) with cognition is not well understood. Furthermore, it is not clear whether cognition is represented via a similar spatial pattern of FC and SC or instead is mapped by distinct sets of distributed connectivity patterns. To this end, we used a longitudinal, within-subject, multimodal approach aiming to combine brain data from diffusion-weighted MRI (DW-MRI), and functional MRI (fMRI) with behavioral evaluation, to better understand how changes in FC and SC correlate with changes in cognition in a sample of older adults. FC and SC measures were derived from the multimodal scans acquired at two time points. Change in FC and SC was correlated with 13 behavioral measures of cognitive function using Partial Least Squares Correlation (PLSC). Two of the measures indicate an age-related change in cognition and the rest indicate baseline cognitive performance. FC and SC—cognition correlations were expressed across several cognitive measures, and numerous structural and functional cortical connections, mainly cingulo-opercular, dorsolateral prefrontal, somatosensory and motor, and temporo-parieto-occipital, contributed both positively and negatively to the brain-behavior relationship. Whole-brain FC and SC captured distinct and independent connections related to the cognitive measures. Overall, we examined age-related function-structure associations of the brain in a comprehensive and integrated manner, using a multimodal approach. We pointed out the behavioral relevance of age-related changes in FC and SC. Taken together, our results highlight that the heterogeneity in distributed FC and SC connectivity patterns provide unique information about the variable nature of healthy cognitive aging.

Introduction

Aging is associated with heterogenous changes in cognition and in the structure and function of the brain (Kennedy and Raz, 2015; Damoiseaux, 2017). Research over the past decades has yet to elucidate the effect of age on the association between functional and structural brain networks. Various “disruptive” or “disconnection” theories suggest that cognitive decline in normal aging stems from alterations in the integration of functional properties of brain networks and/or from subtle anatomical disconnection between brain regions, possible due to microstructural white matter loss or demyelination (O’Sullivan et al., 2001; Salat et al., 2005; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007).

Longitudinal studies of aging indicate that changes in cognition vary considerably across different cognitive domains and behavioral tasks (i.e., visuo-spatial abilities, reaction time, processing speed, planning, decision making) at the inter-individual and even intra-individual level (Kliegel and Sliwinski, 2004; Salthouse, 2009; Goh et al., 2013; Mella et al., 2015). For example, some individuals may experience a general cognitive decline across multiple behavioral tasks (i.e., homogenous decline), while others may decline in one cognitive ability but experience preservation or even improvement of others (i.e., heterogenous change). A hallmark of cognitive aging is decreased processing speed, or an inferior performance on perceptual, motor, and decision-making tasks (Salthouse, 2000). Processing speed involves coordinated activity across multiple neural networks, and so engages perception, decision making, planning, motor performance, and task evaluation (Salthouse, 1996; Eckert et al., 2010). Therefore, processing speed tasks could be evaluating any of these abilities.

The small number of longitudinal studies that investigated changes in functional connectivity (FC) in healthy older adults were inconsistent, some indicate stability (Persson et al., 2014), while others reported a decline in intra-network FC in the executive control network and default mode network (DMN) as well as an initial increase in inter-network FC between the executive control network and DMN followed by a subsequent decline with older age (Ng et al., 2016) On the other hand, some studies suggest a pattern of initial decrease and subsequent temporary increase in FC (Cao et al., 2014; Damoiseaux, 2017). These results are generally attributed to the variability of compensatory or over-recruitment mechanisms across different cognitive domains (reviewed by Betzel et al., 2014; Damoiseaux, 2017). For example, the initial increase in FC can be attributed to an attempt to compensate for declining function, a strategy that cannot be sustained over time. Studies on structural changes with aging are more consistent, with most results pointing to widespread decreases in fractional anisotropy, particularly in frontal brain regions (Betzel et al., 2014; Zhao et al., 2015; Damoiseaux, 2017).

Most studies on the topic of FC and SC changes in aging are cross-sectional, focusing on group mean differences rather than longitudinal individual-level age-related changes (i.e., the aging process itself; Damoiseaux, 2017). Zimmermann et al. (2016) examined the effect of age on the correlation of FC and structural connectivity (SC) in a small, adult lifespan sample, and found that age-related changes in FC and SC coupling are region-dependent.

The current study examined the effect of aging on the change in the structural and functional integrity of the brain while also considering individual differences in cognition. Specifically, the main objective of the present study was to investigate how changes in FC and SC derived from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), over a span of 2 and a half years, correlate with changes in different cognitive measures using a multivariate framework. Partial least squares (PLS) were used to map orthogonal patterns of brain-cognition relationships (McIntosh and Lobaugh, 2004; Krishnan et al., 2011).

Overall, we expected FC and SC—cognition correlations to be captured by variations in the pattern and amplitude of change across various cognitive measures (i.e., heterogeneity and amplitude of change). Additionally, it was expected that the brain-behavior relationship was positively and negatively associated with numerous structural and functional connections, mainly relating to regions susceptible to age-related change such as cingulo-opercular, dorsolateral prefrontal, somatosensory and motor, and temporo-parieto-occipital. One of the novel aspects of the current study is the use of the measure of change in brain connectivity as well as of behavioral change, which should better reflect changes related to the aging process rather than just age-related differences.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The participants of the current study came from the longitudinal Geneva Aging Study. They all provided written informed consent, and approval was obtained from the ethics committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the University of Geneva and the Swiss Ethics Committee.

Older subjects that underwent two T1-weighted structural images, diffusion-weighted images, and fMRI scans, as well as a battery of cognitive tests were selected from a larger pool of the Geneva Aging Study, a lifespan study of 219 older adults (Fagot et al., 2018). The current analyses focused on a subset of individuals that completed longitudinal follow-up at ~2.5 years including behavioral testing and scanning. Subjects missing any one of the behavioral tests or scans were not included. At this stage, 31 subjects were selected. Two subjects were excluded due to additional missing behavioral data and one because of a lesion on the anatomical MRI. The final sample included 28 older subjects (mean age at first scan = 72 ± 6 years, mean age at second scan = 74 ± 6 years; eight males). Their demographic information is presented in Table 1. Participants were screened for health problems with a health questionnaire and their structural MRIs were inspected by a neurosurgeon with significant experience evaluating MRIs (SR) to rule out abnormalities. The battery of the behavioral tests was systematically administered before the scanning sessions (mean interval between testing and MRI = 57 days, SD = 39). Image quality was additionally ensured by examining head motion: absolute head motion at a single time point >2 mm and relative head motion >2.5 mm were criteria for participant removal. No participants were removed.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographic information.

Behavioral Measures

Participants underwent a battery of cognitive tests including 11 different measures which are briefly synthesized in Table 2 and reported in detail in other manuscripts (Mella et al., 2013, 2015; Fagot et al., 2018). Two of the measures, heterogeneity and amplitude of change, indicate an age-related change in cognition and the rest indicate baseline cognitive performance (i.e., measured at one timepoint). These measures are a composite score summarizing spatial and spatial-verbal working memory scores (Mat_span, MDA), crystallized abilities (MH), fluid intelligence (Raven), cognitive inhibition (Stroop), as well as mean performance and intraindividual variability in three reaction time tasks (a simple reaction time task, SRT) and a two-conditions complex processing speed task (CL6 and CL9). Intraindividual variability and mean performance have been shown to reflect different cognitive processes, intraindividual variability being linked to sustained attention abilities, while mean performance reflects the general level of processing speed. The composite scores of amplitude and heterogeneity of change in cognitive abilities at the individual level have been computed using individual analyses of variance of all tasks assessing simple reaction times, complex processing speed, and inhibition. This allowed assessing both the general amplitude of change and the heterogeneity of individual change across these tasks independently of the sample characteristics (see Mella et al., 2018 for more details). Briefly, heterogeneity of change refers to variations in the patterns of change across various cognitive measures. The homogenous decline is said to reflect a global decline in attentional resources or of mental resources, while strong heterogeneity of change may indicate a decline in one ability but stability or improvement in another (i.e., heterogenous pattern). Mean amplitude of change refers to both the direction and amplitude of the change in performance across the tasks considered. A smaller, negative change does not necessarily indicate decline but rather stability or less improvement. The higher the absolute value the stronger the change (explained in detail in Mella et al., 2018).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Behavioral measures and corresponding cognitive function.

The 11 single measures of cognition, as well as the general amplitude and heterogeneity of change were correlated with change in SC and FC (described below) using Partial Least Square Correlation (PLSC; Mella et al., 2013, 2015; Fagot et al., 2018).

MRI Acquisition and Preprocessing

Participants were scanned in a Siemens Trio 3T magnet. The following imaging sets were analyzed in the current study: resting-state functional MRI (fMRI), structural T1-weighted MR image (T1-w), and DWI sequences. Two sequences of 30 directions DWI were acquired (TR = 8,400 ms, TE = 88 ms, b value = 1,000 s/mm2, and voxel size 2.0 mm3). The two DWI acquisitions were concatenated using FSL 5.081 (Smith et al., 2004) to increase the signal-to-noise ratio during the postprocessing. Then, a structural T1-w MR image was acquired (TE = 2.27 ms, TR = 1,900 ms, FOV = 256 mm, voxel size 1.0 mm3). Finally, the resting-state fMRI was obtained using an echo planar imaging acquisition (echo time, TE = 30 ms, time repetition, TR = 2,100 ms, flip angle = 80°, field of view, FOV = 205 mm, voxel size = 3.2 mm3, 140 volumes).

The diffusion data were corrected for eddy currents and movements using FSL (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016). Structural T1-w MR images were preprocessed using Freesurfer version 6.02 (Fischl, 2012) with a standard automated preprocessing pipeline (i.e., “recon-all” with the default set of parameters). Tissue-segmented images were obtained (i.e., white matter, gray matter, and cerebrospinal fluid), and used at the diffusion data analysis stage. For each participant, the T1-w image was registered to the diffusion data with cross-modal registration (i.e., rigid with six degrees of freedom) using a boundary-based cost function (Greve and Fischl, 2009). The diffusion data were further processed using MRtrix33 (Tournier et al., 2019) following the following standard structural connectome construction steps. Briefly, to estimate white matter fiber orientation distributions the white matter response function was estimated and used to perform single-shell, single-tissue constrained spherical deconvolution (MRtrix command “dwi2 response Tournier”; Tournier et al., 2004, 2007, et al., 2013). Whole-brain tractography was generated using anatomical-constrained tractography (MRtrix command “tckgen”, 50 million streamlines, maximum tract length = 250, fractional anisotropy cutoff = 0.06; Smith et al., 2012). Spherical-deconvolution informed filtering of tractograms (SIFT2, command “tcksift2”) algorithm was used to estimate structural connection density (Smith et al., 2015; Tournier et al., 2015). Finally, the structural connectome was produced by mapping a multi-modal parcellation atlas with 360 regions (180 per hemisphere) described in detail in Glasser et al. (2017), to the streamlines obtained from SIFT2. A 360 × 360 connectivity matrix was created, representing the number of white matter reconstructed pathways for each pair of regions, normalized by the sum of the volumes of the two regions.

Resting-state fMRI was preprocessed using SPM84 and functions of the data processing assistant for resting- state fMRI (Yan, 2010) and individual brain atlases using statistical parametric mapping (Alemán-Gómez, 2006) toolboxes. Standard preprocessing steps were followed: realignment of the functional scans, spatial smoothing with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 5-mm full width at half maximum, co-registration of the T1-w image to the functional mean, and segmentation of the structural images (white matter, gray matter, cerebrospinal fluid). The average signal from cerebrospinal fluid and white matter then was regressed out from functional time courses, together with six motion parameters (translation and rotation along the three dimensions) and linear/quadratic trends. Next, Glasser’s multimodal parcellation atlas (the same used for the structural connectome) was resliced to functional resolution and applied to the fMRI data to estimate regional average time courses. For each participant, functional connectivity was calculated as Pearson correlation between each pair of time series. These were then Fischer z-transformed and stored in a 360 × 360 FC matrix.

For each subject, the change in SC and FC was calculated as the difference in connectivity values between time 2 and time 1, after taking the absolute values of the matrices for FC. The absolute change in connectivity at the individual level, reflecting therefore a change in connectivity strength (regardless of the sign, for FC), was then stored as final connectivity difference matrices.

Age and gender were regressed from the SC and Fischer’s z-transformed FC, and residuals were used for the PLSC analysis.

Statistical Analysis

The behavioral measures (n = 13) were correlated (Pearson’s) with each other to better understand the relationships between them, resulting in a 13 × 13 cross-correlation matrix of correlation coefficients r. The p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using a false discovery rate (FDR; Benjamini et al., 2006). MATLAB version 2019a5 was used for all statistical analyses.

PLSC for neuroimaging (McIntosh and Lobaugh, 2004; Krishnan et al., 2011) was used to assess the multivariate patterns of correlations between behavioral variables (i.e., cognition) and SC and FC measures. The performed analysis relates to that described in Zimmermann et al. (2018). Briefly, the upper triangle of the symmetric connectivity difference matrices (360 × 360 regions of interest) was vectorized for each of the subjects. Structural connections that were 0 at both timepoints, for 66% of subjects or more, were excluded from the analysis to avoid issues with resampling statistics (Zimmermann et al., 2018). Vectorized brain connectomes were then stacked across subjects. The behavioral variables were stored in a matrix of subjects*13 behavioral measures. Next, to start the PLSC, we computed a correlation matrix between the brain and behavioral matrices (i.e., a cross-correlation matrix between two data matrices). The cross-correlation matrix representing the brain-behavior correlation was submitted to singular value decomposition (SVD), which yields orthogonal latent variables (LVs) that capture the covariance between the variables from the behavioral and brain datasets (McIntosh and Lobaugh, 2004; Ziegler et al., 2013). The significance of the LVs was determined via permutation tests (1,000 iterations) of the singular values from the SVD, and the stability of the brain and behavior weights (also called saliences) was assessed using bootstrapping (500 bootstrap samples). For each significant LV, bootstrap ratios (BSR) for brain and behavior weights were calculated as the ratio of the weight over its estimated standard error. The stability of each connection in brain weights and behavioral weights was assessed based on its BSR: a positive high BSR >2.5 contributed positively and reliably to the brain-cognition correlations while a negative high BSR <−2.5 contributed negatively and reliably to the brain-cognition correlations. The positive and negative dimensions reflect distributions of connections that covary in a similar pattern to one another. Therefore, a BSR with a larger magnitude indicates that the connection with which it is associated has a large singular vector weight (i.e., contributes to the LV) and a small standard error (i.e., stable across participants).

To explore the contributions of each imaging modality to behavior, a matrix of the pairwise linear correlation coefficients (Pearson correlation coefficient) between each pair of regions was calculated. Next, each matrix was vectorized and correlated using Pearson correlation.

The PLSC analysis was conducted with the 13 cognitive measures (see Table 2) as behavioral variables and with SC and FC change as brain variables.

Results

Cognitive Measures

The cognitive measures were mostly weakly correlated with each other. Only measures CL9 MRIM1 and CL6MRI1M, both measures of processing speed involving mean performance on letters comparison task with nine letters and six letters, respectively, were positively and significantly correlated after FDR correction (0.83, p < 0.00001; Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Pearson’s Correlation among the cognitive measures. The color bar indicates the strength of the correlation (r). Abbreviations: MH MR1, Mill Hill Vocabulary Test; SRT MRI1M, mean performance of Simple Reaction Time; MDA MRI1, Matrices Task with position recall; MAT MRI1, Matrices Task with word position recall; CL6 MRI1, mean performance on Letters Comparison Task with six letters; CL9 MRI1, mean performance on Letters Comparison task with nine letters; RAVEN MRI1, Raven Progressive Matrices 38; CL6 MRICV, Coefficient of variation of Letters Comparison task with six letters; CL9_MRI1CV, Coefficient of variation of letters comparison task with nine letters; Stroop MRI1, Color Stroop Task. *Indicates significance at p < 0.00001.

Brain Connectivity and Behavioral Interpretation

The relationship between the change in FC and SC and cognitive function (resulting from the PLS analysis) was captured by one LV (17% of total covariance; singular value = 2,406.87; p = 0.0070), showing a significant contribution to the covariance. The LV revealed functional and structural correlations expressed across an array of behavioral measures, that optimally covaried with each other, and contributed negatively or positively to the FC and SC—cognition relationship. The contribution of the connections was expressed as BSRs, which indicates how robustly each connection contributed to the weighted pattern of the SC and FC—cognition matrix.

The two imaging modalities were overall weakly correlated (−0.072, p < 0.0001). The strength of correlations varied based on region as depicted in Figure 2. For example, regions in the posterior cortex (e.g., temporo-parieto-occipital junction, superior parietal, inferior parietal, posterior cingulate) were positively correlated with medial temporal and lateral temporal cortices. The anterior cortex and sensorimotor cortices largely showed negative correlations with the rest of the brain. Positive correlations indicate that both FC and SC change contribute in the same direction towards behavior, while negative correlations indicate that FC and SC change have opposite contributions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Correlation matrix between SC and FC modalities. The first 180 regions pertain to the left hemisphere and the next 180 (i.e., 181–360) to the right hemisphere (i.e., homologs). Their full description can be found in Table 1 in Supplementary Neuroanatomical Results in Glasser et al. (2017). The color bar refers to the strength of the correlations.

The contributions to behavior can be better appreciated in Figure 3, which depicts the association of each region (i.e., node) to the brain-behavior relationship.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Brain plots for FC (A) and SC (B) brain saliences. Brain regions are represented by nodes whose size and color code are proportional to their node strength. The node strength essentially represents the degree to which each connection contributes to the covariance pattern of behavior (red is higher strength while green is lower strength).

Specifically, several sets of functional and structural connections were found to have stable weights by bootstrapping (Figure 4). The behavioral weights corresponding to the significant LV are shown in Figure 5. In brief, Figure 5 indicates the behavioral pattern determined by the LV. PLS selects patterns of connections whose signal change covaries with the behavioral pattern across subjects identified by the LV. Those behaviors with higher BSR are said to modulate or contribute to a greater extent to the functional or structural connections than behaviors with lower BSR. The behavioral interpretation is presented in the following subsections (i.e., “Structural connections”, “Functional connections”) in conjunction with the brain weights.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Bootstrap connectivity weights for FC (A) and SC (B) change. The first 180 regions pertain to the left hemisphere and the next 180 (i.e., 181–360) to the right hemisphere (i.e., homologs). Their full description can be found in Table 1 in Supplementary Neuroanatomical Results in Glasser et al., 2017. The color bar refers to the bootstrap ratios. Connections with positive bootstrap ratios are positively associated with the FC or SC—cognition correlation, and negative bootstrap ratios are negatively associated with the FC or SC—cognition correlation.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Behavioral saliences, with confidence intervals from bootstrap resampling. Error bars indicate bootstrapping 5th to 95th percentiles. Abbreviations: MRI1, at first MRI scan; MH MR1, Mill Hill Vocabulary Test; SRT MRI1M, mean performance of Simple Reaction Time; MDA MRI1, Matrices Task with position recall; MAT MRI1, Matrices Task with word position recall; CL6 MRI1, mean performance on Letters Comparison Task with six letters; CL9 MRI1, mean performance on Letters Comparison Task with nine letters; RAVEN MRI1, Raven Progressive Matrices 38; CL6 MRICV, Coefficient of variation of Letters Comparison Task with six letters; CL9_MRI1CV, Coefficient of variation of letters comparison Task with nine letters; Stroop MRI1, Color Stroop Task.

Several sets of overlapping and non-overlapping functional and structural connections, specified in detail in the following subsections, were found to uniquely and independently contribute both negatively and positively to the association between change in brain connectivity and cognition in aging.

To facilitate interpretation of the data, in each hemisphere the 180 regions were separated into 22 larger partitions or “cortices” based on their topological proximity, common properties, based on architecture, task-fMRI profiles, and/or FC. Therefore, each of the 360 regions occupies one of the 22 cortices. The assignment is described in detail in Supplementary Neuroanatomical Results in Glasser et al. (2017).

Structural Connections

Structural connections captured by the LV loaded mostly positively onto the brain-behavior relationship (Figures 3, 6). Some notable exceptions were several interhemispheric connections, mainly between the prefrontal cortex and several other regions (i.e., anterior cingulate, inferior frontal cortex, etc.), that loaded negatively. Specifically, interhemispheric structural connections between right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and several other regions: left anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex (BSR = −5.01), left inferior frontal cortex (BSR = −4.32), left premotor cortex (BSR = −5.01), dorsal visual stream (BSR = −5.01), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BSR = −5.00), and intra-hemispheric structural connections between right insular and frontal opercular cortex—right lateral temporal cortex (BSR = −4.128), left superior parietal cortex (including intra-parietal sulcus) and left premotor (BSR = −4.16), and right dorsal stream visual cortex and left temporo-parieto-occipital-junction (BSR = −3.46), etc. Parieto-occipital-junction was defined as a strip of cortex bounded by auditory, lateral temporal, inferior parietal, and occipital (visual MT+ complexes) regions (Glasser et al., 2017). Therefore, decreased SC between the above-mentioned regions was positively correlated with cognitive functioning (i.e., measured by heterogeneity and amplitude of change), and baseline performance on crystallized intelligence, verbal-spatial memory, and processing speed. Decreased SC was negatively correlated with baseline performance on spatial working memory, and fluid intelligence.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Bootstrap connectivity weights for SC change. Each of the 360 Glasser regions occupies one of the 22 cortices, which are listed on the y-axis. Each cortex was further labeled to a larger partition depicted on the matrix. The assignment is described in detail in Supplementary Neuroanatomical Results in Glasser et al. (2017).

Functional Connections

There was variation in the contribution of FC connections to the brain-behavior relationship (Figures 3, 7). Some connections showed a strong, positive contribution including left premotor cortex and right early visual cortex (BSR = 8.57), left lateral temporal cortex and left temporo-parieto-occipital junction (BSR = 8.60), somatosensory and motor—MT+ complex and neighboring visual areas (BSR = 5.13). Thus, increased FC in these regions was correlated with inferior baseline performance on crystallized intelligence, verbal-spatial memory, and processing speed. Increased FC was positively correlated with higher scores in baseline spatial working memory, and fluid intelligence.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Bootstrap connectivity weights for FC change. Each of the 360 Glasser regions occupies one of the 22 cortices, which are listed on the y-axis. Each cortex was further labeled to a larger partition depicted on the matrix. The assignment is described in detail in Supplementary Neuroanatomical Results in Glasser et al. (2017).

Others showed a strong negative contribution, including interhemispheric functional connections between right insular and frontal opercular cortex and several other regions: left paracentral lobular and mid cingulate cortex (BSR = −5.47), left anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex (BSR = −3.32), left inferior parietal cortex (BSR = −5.72), posterior cingulate (BSR = −3.24), left somatosensory and motor cortex (BSR = −3.03), left superior parietal cortex and intra-parietal sulcus (BSR = −4.23). Also, intra-hemispheric functional connections between right between posterior opercular cortex and right posterior cingulate (BSR = −3.14), and right anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex—insular and frontal opercular cortex (BSR = −4.23).

Therefore, decreased FC in these regions was positively correlated with cognitive functioning (i.e., measured by heterogeneity and amplitude of change), and baseline performance on crystallized intelligence, verbal-spatial memory, and processing speed. Decreased FC was negatively correlated with baseline spatial working memory and fluid intelligence.

Discussion

In the current study, we characterized the mapping of age-related changes in FC and SC on 13 behavioral measures of cognitive function such as spatial working memory (MAT_MRI1_SPAN), crystallized intelligence (MH_MRI1), reaction time (SRT_MRI1M), and resistance to interference (i.e., STROOP_MRI1), in a sample of older adults.

We used PLSC, a multivariate method increasingly used for neuroimaging analysis (McIntosh and Lobaugh, 2004), to characterize brain-behavior relationships.

Structural and Functional Connections Vary in Their Contribution to the Brain-Behavior Relationship in Aging

Several distinct sets of functional and structural connections were identified and assigned to 22 larger partitions (see “Materials and Methods” Section). This can be visualized in Figure 2. Areas that contribute to behavior in the same way (either both negatively or both positively) are depicted in yellow (i.e., positive correlations), while areas that have opposing contributions are in dark blue. Visual, auditory, and temporal cortices mostly present positive correlations between FC and SC contributions indicating that in these areas FC and SC change contribute in the same direction to behavior. Anterior cortices had largely a negative correlation with other cortices. For example, FC change (Figure 7) indicates negative loading onto behavior while SC change (Figure 6) indicates positive loading. This indicates that there is preservation of structural connections but decline in functional ones.

The majority of the structural connections loaded positively onto the latent variables identified by PLSC, indicating stability or increased connectivity. Given the relatively short time of 2.5 years between the scans, and that our study sample is of healthy older adults we did not expect an accelerated global decrease in structural connectivity. Although the majority of structural connections contributed strongly and positively to the brain-behavior relationship, there were some exceptions with some areas contributing strongly and negatively (see Figure 4). Notably, interhemispheric structural connections between the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and several other regions (frontal, sensory, motor) in the left hemisphere: anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal, premotor, dorsal visual stream, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The dorsal visual stream contains higher visual areas implicated in perceiving where visual stimuli are located and in planning visually guided actions (Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982; Goodale and Milner, 1992). Other negatively contributing regions were left intra-hemispheric fronto-parietal and temporo-parieto-occipital structural connections. Decreased SC in these regions was associated with the decline in cognitive functioning (i.e., measured by heterogeneity and amplitude of change), and inferior baseline performance on crystallized intelligence, verbal-spatial memory, and processing speed. Our results are congruent with other studies that indicate that normal aging involves decreasing myelination and reduced microstructural integrity of the white matter in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as well as in the corpus callosum (i.e., reduced interhemispheric connections), leading to reduced conduction of nerve fibers and cognitive decline (Peters and Sethares, 2002; Bowley et al., 2010; Kaller et al., 2015). Such changes are commonly seen in other prefrontal areas with aging and correlated with reduced cognitive ability especially involving processing speed, planning, and distractibility/inhibition (Eckert et al., 2010; Luebke et al., 2010; Kaller et al., 2015). Our measures of cognitive functioning, heterogeneity, and amplitude of change, were derived from tests assessing simple reaction times, complex processing speed, and inhibition. The findings of a decline in these measures correlating with decreased SC in prefrontal, motor, and sensory regions are in line with the existing literature. Studies indicate the decline in white matter fractional anisotropy is associated with lower processing speed, reflecting a regionally specific structural decline in attention-related regions in the prefrontal cortex, and a structural decline in the motor and sensory cortex, as these last are essential for performing processing speed tasks (Salthouse, 2000; Coffey et al., 2001; Charlton et al., 2006; Kennedy and Raz, 2009; Eckert et al., 2010).

There was variation in the contribution of functional connections to the brain-behavior relationship, which is in line with the existing literature, which indicates that age-related functional reorganization does not follow a global, whole-brain pattern rather is region-dependent (Goh et al., 2013; Cao et al., 2014; Zimmermann et al., 2016). Some connections showed a strong, positive contribution including somatosensory, motor, and temporo-occipital connections. Thus, preserved or increased FC in these regions was correlated with higher scores in baseline spatial working memory and fluid intelligence, decline in cognitive functioning, inferior baseline performance on crystallized intelligence, verbal-spatial memory, and processing speed. These observations are largely consistent with other studies reporting age-related increases in FC within motor cortices (Meier et al., 2012; Tomasi and Volkow, 2012), somato-motor connections (Betzel et al., 2014), and visual networks (Betzel et al., 2014; Geerligs et al., 2015).

Other functional connections showed a strong negative contribution to the brain-behavior relationship. Largely, these were composed of interhemispheric cingulo-opercular, and insular and opercular connections with parietal, paracentral lobule, medial prefrontal cortex, somatosensory, motor, anterior, mid, and posterior cingulate functional connections. To provide behavioral context to the above results, decreased FC in these regions was associated with the decline or less improvement in cognitive functioning (i.e., measured by heterogeneity and amplitude of change), inferior baseline performance on crystallized intelligence, verbal-spatial memory, processing speed, and higher scores in baseline spatial working memory, fluid intelligence.

Interestingly, decreased cingulo-opercular network (including the insula) FC has been reported to correlate with age-related reductions in visual processing speed (Ruiz-Rizzo et al., 2019), and so in our study, this may explain the lower scores on heterogeneity and amplitude of change since these were derived using processing speed tasks with visual stimuli. Some of the other functional connections mentioned (posterior cingulate, prefrontal, parietal cortex, etc.) are part of the default mode network (DMN; Alves et al., 2019), and lower DMN FC has been reported to be decreased in age-related decline in processed speed (Damoiseaux et al., 2008). We found that decreased FC between right insular and frontal opercular cortex and several other regions: left paracentral lobular and midcingulate cortex, left anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex, left inferior parietal cortex, and posterior cingulate was associated with decreased cognitive functioning (i.e., measured by heterogeneity and amplitude of change), inferior baseline performance on crystallized intelligence, verbal-spatial memory, processing speed, and higher scores in baseline spatial working memory, fluid intelligence. Disruption of coordinated functional activity of key network regions such as the ones mentioned (posterior cingulate, prefrontal, parietal cortex), has been strongly linked to cognitive decline in advancing age without an overt disease (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007). Some studies found that the relationship between decreased DMN and reduced processing speed is attenuated by controlling for whole-brain FC and white matter hyperintensities (Staffaroni et al., 2018) and reduced functional anisotropy between DMN areas (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007). Therefore, there is a proposed vulnerability of these connections with cognitive aging. Similarly, reduced FC was reported for the insular and cingulated cortex with aging and cognitive decline (Onoda et al., 2012). Our study did not focus on within and between network analysis, however, that would be an interesting direction to explore. For example, the anticorrelation between the DMN and dorsal attention network is well-studied metric for cognitive functioning in aging. However, multi-modal longitudinal studies are limited in this area (Zhu et al., 2016).

Moreover, right intra-hemispheric functional connections involving cingulo-opercular, fronto-parietal, and prefrontal-opercular (including insula) connections, were also found to be negatively contributing to the brain-behavior relationship. In a comprehensive review by Robertson (2014), cognitive reserve (known to be protective in cognitive aging) is linked to the functional integrity of the frontoparietal connections in the right hemisphere, meaning that less FC in these regions may indicate a decline in cognitive functioning involving arousal, sustained attention, response to novelty, and awareness (Robertson, 2014; Haupt et al., 2019). Thereby, corroborating our results of decreased FC in these regions associated with a decline or less improvement in cognitive functioning.

Future Directions and Limitations

A significant strength of our study is our longitudinal design, as only longitudinal studies can measure change. It allowed the investigation of individual brain-behavior developmental trajectories in aging, rather than just general tendencies in age differences, an important approach considering that aging is a heterogenous process. The within-subject approach minimizes cohort and period effects typical of cross-sectional designs and may help disentangle to what degree change in FC and SC ultimately drives aging-related changes in cognition, as well as investigate the behavioral relevance of FC and SC metrics.

Further longitudinal studies investigating age-related change in FC and SC while considering cognition are necessary to elucidate the underlying brain mechanisms behind cognitive decline in normal aging. It would be valuable to investigate the change in FC within and between brain networks (i.e., DMN, salience network, etc) and correlate it with white matter integrity and SC, with various brain parcellations, and across large longitudinal samples of healthy older adults and in different pathological states (i.e., Alzheimer’s).

Some weaknesses of our study are our relatively small sample size (n = 28) and the small number of cognitive measures that represent change (i.e., heterogeneity and amplitude of change). We also had only two timepoints at a relatively short interval (2.5 years), which may limit the generalizability of our study to the lifespan scale. Additional time points and a larger time frame would allow a more robust assessment of where on the trajectory of age-related change an individual’s brain is.

Conclusions

In summary, we have shown that age-related changes in the brain-behavior relationship are supported by distinct positive and negative contributions from structural and functional connections distributed across the brain. From a behavioral perspective, we found that subjects with a decline in cognitive functioning (as derived from processing speed tasks), had decreased SC in fronto-parietal, prefrontal, and frontal connections, and decreased FC in cingulo-opercular, and DMN associated regions. Globally, we found a tendency for whole-brain SC preservation and mixed FC changes.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The participants of the current study came from the longitudinal Geneva Aging Study. They all provided written informed consent, and approval was obtained from the ethics committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the University of Geneva and the Swiss Ethic Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the design and implementation of the research, to the analysis of the results, and to the writing of the manuscripts. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the SNF (Swiss National Foundation, Grant Nos. 100014_135410, PMPDP1 171335, and PMPDP1 158319). Additionally, DP received funding from Mitacs Globalink Research Award while conducting the relevant research abroad. MP was supported by the CIBM Center for Biomedical Imaging, a Swiss research center of excellence founded and supported by Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV), University of Lausanne (UNIL), Ecole polytechnique fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), University of Geneva (UNIGE), and Geneva University Hospitals (HUG).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants of the Geneva Aging Study. Thank you to the reviewers for their thorough read and edits, which have significantly improved the quality of the manuscript. Thank you to Renee-Marie Virginia Ragguett and Liam Emerson Bilbie for technical help with figures and matrices.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl
  2. ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/
  3. ^ www.mrtrix.org/
  4. ^ www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/
  5. ^ https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html

References

Alemán-Gómez, Y. (2006). “IBASPM: toolbox for automatic parcellation of brain structures,” in 12th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (Florence, Italy).

Google Scholar

Alves, P. N., Chris, F., Vyacheslav, K., Danilo, B., Daniel, S. M., Emmanuelle, V., et al. (2019). An improved neuroanatomical model of the default-mode network reconciles previous neuroimaging and neuropathological findings. Commun. Biol. 2:370. doi: 10.1038/s42003-019-0611-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, J. L. R., and Sotiropoulos, S. N. (2016). An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage 125, 1063–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Abraham, Z. S., Justin, L. V., Cindy, L., Denise, H., Marcus, E. E. R., et al. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56, 924–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., Krieger, A. M., and Yekutieli, D. (2006). Adaptive linear step-up procedures that control the false discovery rate. Biometrika 93, 491–507. doi: 10.1093/biomet/93.3.491

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Betzel, R. F., Lisa, B., Ye, H., Joaquín, G., Xi, N. Z., Olaf, S., et al. (2014). Changes in structural and functional connectivity among resting-state networks across the human lifespan. NeuroImage 102, 345–357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowley, M. P., Howard, C., Douglas, L. R., and Alan, P. (2010). Age changes in myelinated nerve fibers of the cingulate bundle and corpus callosum in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 3046–3064. doi: 10.1002/cne.22379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, M., Jin, H. W., Zheng, J. D., Xiao, Y. C., Li, L. J., Feng, M. F., et al. (2014). Topological organization of the human brain functional connectome across the lifespan. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 76–93. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charlton, R. A., Barrick, T. R., McIntyre, D. J., Shen, Y., O’Sullivan, M., Howe, F. A., et al. (2006). White matter damage on diffusion tensor imaging correlates with age-related cognitive decline. Neurology 66, 217–222. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000194256.15247.83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coffey, C. E., Ratcliff, G., Saxton, J. A., Bryan, R. N., Fried, L. P., Lucke, J. F., et al. (2001). Cognitive correlates of human brain aging: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging investigation. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 13, 471–485. doi: 10.1176/jnp.13.4.471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S. (2017). Effects of aging on functional and structural brain connectivity. NeuroImage 160, 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damoiseaux, J. S., Beckmann, C. F., Sanz Arigita, E. J., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P. H., Stam, C. J., et al. (2008). Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1856–1864. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckert, M. A., Noam, I. K., Donna, R. R., Vince, D. C., and Kelly, C. H. (2010). Age-related changes in processing speed: unique contributions of cerebellar and prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:10. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.010.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagot, D., Nathalie, M., Erika, B., Paolo, G., Thierry, L., Anik, D. R., et al. (2018). Intra-individual variability from a lifespan perspective: a comparison of latency and accuracy measures. J. Intell. 6:16. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence6010016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B. (2012). FreeSurfer. NeuroImage 62, 774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerligs, L., Remco, J. R., Emi, S., Natasha, M. M., and Monicque, M. L. (2015). A brain-wide study of age-related changes in functional connectivity. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1987–1999. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glasser, M. F., Timothy, S. C., Emma, C. R., Carl, D. H., Essa, Y., Kamil, U., et al. (2017). Europe pmc funders group europe pmc funders author manuscripts a multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536, 171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature18933

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goh, J. O., Lori, L. B.-H., Yang, A., Michael, A. K., and Susan, M. R. (2013). Frontal function and executive processing in older adults: process and region specific age-related longitudinal functional changes. NeuroImage 69, 43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale, M. A., and Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. 15, 20–25. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greve, D. N., and Fischl, B. (2009). Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage 48, 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haupt, M., Adriana, L. R.-R., Christian, S., and Kathrin, F. (2019). Phasic alerting effects on visual processing speed are associated with intrinsic functional connectivity in the cingulo-opercular network. NeuroImage 196, 216–226. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaller, C. P., Marco, R., Michael, K., Roza, U., Irina, M., Jürgen, H., et al. (2015). Predicting planning performance from structural connectivity between left and right mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: moderating effects of age during postadolescence and midadulthood. Cereb. Cortex 25, 869–883. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, K. M., and Raz, N. (2009). Aging white matter and cognition: differential effects of regional variations in diffusion properties on memory, executive functions and speed. Neuropsychologia 50, 39–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, K. M., and Raz, N. (2015). “Normal aging of the brain,” in Brain Mapping (Elsevier), 603–617. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1.00068-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kliegel, M., and Sliwinski, M. (2004). MMSE cross-domain variability predicts cognitive decline in centenarians. Gerontology 50, 39–43. doi: 10.1159/000074388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnan, A., Lynne, J. W., Anthony, R. M., and Hervé, A. (2011). Partial least squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: a tutorial and review. NeuroImage 56, 455–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luebke, J., Helen, B., and Alan, P. (2010). Effects of normal aging on prefrontal area 46 in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. Rev. 62, 212–232. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, A. R., and Lobaugh, N. J. (2004). Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: applications and advances. NeuroImage 23, 250–263. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, T. B., Alok, S. D., Svyatoslav, V., Veena, A. N., Jie, S., Bharat, B. B., et al. (2012). Support vector machine classification and characterization of age-related reorganization of functional brain networks. NeuroImage 60, 601–613. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mella, N., de Ribaupierre, S., Eagleson, R., and de Ribaupierre, A. (2013). Cognitive intraindividual variability and white matter integrity in aging. ScientificWorldJournal 2013:350623. doi: 10.1155/2013/350623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mella, N., Fagot, D., Lecerf, T., and de Ribaupierre, A. (2015). Working memory and intraindividual variability in processing speed: a lifespan developmental and individual-differences study. Mem. Cognit. 43, 340–356. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0491-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mella, N., Fagot, D., Renaud, O., Kliegel, M., and De Ribaupierre, A. (2018). Individual differences in developmental change: quantifying the amplitude and heterogeneity in cognitive change across old age. J. Intell. 6:10. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence6010010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, K. K., June, C. L., Joseph, K. W. L., Michael, W. L. C., and Juan, Z. (2016). Reduced functional segregation between the default mode network and the executive control network in healthy older adults: a longitudinal study. NeuroImage 133, 321–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Sullivan, M., Jones, D. K., Summers, P. E., Morris, R. G., Williams, S. C. R., Markus, H. S., et al. (2001). Evidence for cortical “disconnection” as a mechanism of age-related cognitive decline. Neurology 57, 632–638. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.4.632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onoda, K., Masaki, I., and Shuhei, Y. (2012). Decreased functional connectivity by aging is associated with cognitive decline. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2186–2198. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persson, J., Sara, P., Lars-Göran, N., and Lars, N. (2014). Longitudinal assessment of default-mode brain function in aging. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 2107–2117. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, A., and Sethares, C. (2002). Aging and the myelinated fibers in prefrontal cortex and corpus callosum of the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 442, 277–291. doi: 10.1002/cne.10099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, I. H. (2014). A right hemisphere role in cognitive reserve. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 1375–1385. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz-Rizzo, A. L., Christian, S., Natan, N., Julia, N., Aurore, M., Hermann, J. M., et al. (2019). Decreased cingulo-opercular network functional connectivity mediates the impact of aging on visual processing speed. Neurobiol. Aging 73, 50–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salat, D. H., Tuch, D. S., Greve, D. N., van der Kouwe, A. J., Hevelone, N. D., Zaleta, A. K., et al. (2005). Age-related alterations in white matter microstructure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 1215–1227. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol. Rev. 103, 403–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2000). Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol. Psychol. 54, 35–54. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00052-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2009). Decomposing age correlations on neuropsychological and cognitive variables. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 650–661. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R. E., Jacques, D. T., Fernando, C., and Alan, C. (2012). Anatomically-constrained tractography: improved diffusion mri streamlines tractography through effective use of anatomical information. NeuroImage 62, 1924–1938. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23, S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R. E., Tournier, J. D., Calamante, F., and Connelly, A. (2015). SIFT2: enabling dense quantitative assessment of brain white matter connectivity using streamlines tractography. Neuroimage 119, 338–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staffaroni, A. M., Jesse, A. B., Kaitlin, B. C., Fanny, M. E., Jersey, D., John, N., et al. (2018). The longitudinal trajectory of default mode network connectivity in healthy older adults varies as a function of age and is associated with changes in episodic memory and processing speed. J. Neurosci. 38, 2809–2817. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3067-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasi, D., and Volkow, N. D. (2012). Aging and functional brain networks. Mol. Psychiatry 17, 549–558. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J. D., Fernando, C., and Alan, C. (2007). Robust determination of the fibre orientation distribution in diffusion mri: non-negativity constrained super-resolved spherical deconvolution. Neuroimage 35, 1459–1472. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J. D., Fernando, C., and Alan, C. (2013). Determination of the appropriate b value and number of gradient directions for high-angular-resolution diffusion-weighted imaging. NMR Biomed. 26, 1775–1786. doi: 10.1002/nbm.3017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J. D., Fernando, C., and Alan, C. (2015). SIFT2: enabling dense quantitative assessment of brain white matter connectivity using streamlines tractography. NeuroImage 13, 612–632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J. D., Fernando, C., David, G. G., and Alan, C. (2004). Direct estimation of the fiber orientation density function from diffusion-weighted MRI data using spherical deconvolution. NeuroImage 202:116137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tournier, J. D., Robert, S., David, R., Rami, T., Thijs, D., Maximilian, P., et al. (2019). MRtrix3: a fast, flexible and open software framework for medical image processing and visualisation. NeuroImage 202:116137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ungerleider, L. G., and Mishkin, M. (1982). “Two cortical visual systems,” in Analysis of Visual Behavior, eds D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, and R. J. W. Mansfield (Cambridge: MIT Press), 549–586.

Google Scholar

Yan, C. G. (2010). DPARSF: a matlab toolbox for “Pipeline” data analysis of resting-state FMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:13. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, T., Miao, C., Haijing, N., Xi, N. Z., Alan, E., Yong, H., et al. (2015). Age-related changes in the topological organization of the white matter structural connectome across the human lifespan. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3777–3792. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, H., Peng, Z., Sarael, A., Yuanyuan, C., Hongbao, C., Miao, T., et al. (2016). Changes of intranetwork and internetwork functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. J. Neural Eng. 13:46008. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/13/4/046008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, G., Dahnke, R., Winkler, A. D., and Gaser, C. (2013). Partial least squares correlation of multivariate cognitive abilities and local brain structure in children and adolescents. NeuroImage 82, 284–294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, J., John, D. G., and Anthony, R. M. (2018). Unique mapping of structural and functional connectivity on cognition. J. Neurosci. 38, 9658–9667. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0900-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, J., Petra, R., Kelly, S., Simon, R., Michael, S., Anthony, R. M., et al. (2016). Structural architecture supports functional organization in the human aging brain at a regionwise and network level. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 2645–2661. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fMRI, functional connectivity, structural connectivity, healthy aging, variability, neuroimaging, structure-function coupling

Citation: Pur DR, Preti MG, de Ribaupierre A, Van De Ville D, Eagleson R, Mella N and de Ribaupierre S (2022) Mapping of Structure-Function Age-Related Connectivity Changes on Cognition Using Multimodal MRI. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:757861. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.757861

Received: 12 August 2021; Accepted: 21 April 2022;
Published: 18 May 2022.

Edited by:

Ching-Po Lin, National Yang-Ming University, Taiwan

Reviewed by:

Adriana L. Ruiz-Rizzo, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany
Mario Torso, Oxford Brain Diagnostics Ltd, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Pur, Preti, de Ribaupierre, Van De Ville, Eagleson, Mella and de Ribaupierre. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daiana Roxana Pur, dpur@uwo.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.