Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 20 September 2022
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 14 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2022.999288

Metabolic syndrome predictors of brain gray matter volume in an age-stratified community sample of 776 Mexican- American adults: Results from the genetics of brain structure image archive

Eithan Kotkowski1* Larry R. Price2 Ralph A. DeFronzo3 Crystal G. Franklin1 Maximino Salazar1 Amy S. Garrett1 Mary Woolsey1 John Blangero4 Ravindranath Duggirala4 David C. Glahn5,6 Peter T. Fox1,7
  • 1Research Imaging Institute, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX, United States
  • 2Departments of Mathematics and Education, Texas State University, San Marcos, TX, United States
  • 3Diabetes Research Unit and Diabetes Division, Texas Diabetes Institute, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX, United States
  • 4Genomics Computing Center, South Texas Diabetes and Obesity Institute, University of Texas Rio Grande Valley, Brownsville, TX, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 6Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Institute of Living, Hartford Hospital, Hartford, CT, United States
  • 7South Texas Veterans Health Care System, San Antonio, TX, United States

Introduction: This project aimed to investigate the association between biometric components of metabolic syndrome (MetS) with gray matter volume (GMV) obtained with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) from a large cohort of community-based adults (n = 776) subdivided by age and sex and employing brain regions of interest defined previously as the “Neural Signature of MetS” (NS-MetS).

Methods: Lipid profiles, biometrics, and regional brain GMV were obtained from the Genetics of Brain Structure (GOBS) image archive. Participants underwent T1-weighted MR imaging. MetS components (waist circumference, fasting plasma glucose, triglycerides, HDL cholesterol, and blood pressure) were defined using the National Cholesterol Education Program Adult Treatment Panel III. Subjects were grouped by age: early adult (18–25 years), young adult (26–45 years), and middle-aged adult (46–65 years). Linear regression modeling was used to investigate associations between MetS components and GMV in five brain regions comprising the NS-MetS: cerebellum, brainstem, orbitofrontal cortex, right insular/limbic cluster and caudate.

Results: In both men and women of each age group, waist circumference was the single component most strongly correlated with decreased GMV across all NS-MetS regions. The brain region most strongly correlated to all MetS components was the posterior cerebellum.

Conclusion: The posterior cerebellum emerged as the region most significantly associated with MetS individual components, as the only region to show decreased GMV in young adults, and the region with the greatest variance between men and women. We propose that future studies investigating neurological effects of MetS and its comorbidities—namely diabetes and obesity—should consider the NS-MetS and the differential effects of age and sex.

Introduction

Metabolic syndrome (MetS), also known as the insulin resistance syndrome, systemic metabolic dysfunction, or syndrome X (Reaven, 1988), is a cluster of clinical risk factors for cardiovascular disease (CVD) and type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM). These risk factors include central obesity [increased waist circumference (WC)], elevated fasting plasma glucose (FPG), triglycerides (TG), blood pressure (BP), and reduced high density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol (Grundy et al., 2005). MetS and its associated cardiometabolic diseases—CVD, T2DM, and obesity—are also known to be significantly associated with cognitive dysfunction (Geijselaers et al., 2014; Gonzalez et al., 2015) and neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s and vascular dementia (Vanhanen et al., 2006; Lee and Mattson, 2014; Morris et al., 2014). Indeed, there are measurable significant negative correlations seen between both the number and severity of MetS components when measured against cognition metrics, specifically for tests of executive function (Yates et al., 2012), processing speed (Reijmer et al., 2012), reward perception (Cornier et al., 2010; Cameron et al., 2017), and affect regulation (Hu et al., 2015; Wolf et al., 2016).

Despite numerous studies examining the cognitive effects of MetS, neuroimaging studies aimed at identifying gray matter neuroanatomical correlates of MetS are sparse. Existing neuroimaging literature has focused primarily on white matter integrity and hyperintensities (Van Bloemendaal et al., 2016), structures associated with processing speed (Kochunov et al., 2016), but not with behavior or executive function (Lansley et al., 2013). Furthermore, there is a sizeable neuroimaging literature investigating correlations between obesity markers such as body mass index (BMI), waist circumference and gray matter volume (Kurth et al., 2013; Janowitz et al., 2015). Many such studies have often excluded individuals possessing MetS components as confounders, in effect eliminating subjects with a higher disease burden who are more likely to experience neurovascular events and cognitive decline. Furthermore, most large cohort studies looking at diabetes and obesity have primarily been conducted in older (>60 years) individuals, Caucasian and East Asian ethnic cohorts, and have often employed relatively small sample sizes (30–50 subjects) (Moulton et al., 2015; Wu et al., 2017).

Age and sex are important sources of variance in any analysis involving MetS and neuroimaging. It is widely understood that sex hormones significantly influence fat metabolism and adipocyte distribution, an influence that also changes with age. Indeed, metabolic studies have shown that men tend to exhibit higher levels of triglycerides and blood pressure, but lower age-adjusted levels of waist circumference and HDL cholesterol as compared to women (Ervin, 2009). Effects of MetS also differ between men and women physiologically (Regitz-Zagrosek et al., 2006) and cognitively (Laudisio et al., 2008). In one example, Cavalieri et al. (2010) found that men with increasing number of MetS components performed worse on memory and executive function tests than women. Another more recent study showed that higher waist circumference and mental health symptoms had a stronger association with dementia in women than in men (Gong et al., 2021). For this reason, investigating the effects of MetS on the brain using separate sex-stratified cohorts is important. Assessing similarities and differences between the sexes can be accomplished using regression analyses that adjust for the effects of age and sex. However, we believe that stratifying groups as independent analyses can be more helpful in providing valuable insight for clinicians to be mindful of the differential effects of MetS and its neurocognitive underpinnings on men vs. women.

Age is also an important risk factor for MetS. One large epidemiological study of the United States reported that 18.3% of individuals aged 20–39 years met the International Diabetes Federation’s criteria for MetS with the prevalence rising sharply with age. Indeed, 46.7% of those older than 60 years were identified as meeting the diagnostic criteria for MetS. Importantly, the age of any individual with MetS can be confounded by the length of time they have met the diagnostic criteria, and whether meeting such criteria occurred earlier vs. later in life. The prevalence of MetS is also higher among minority populations, with Hispanic-Americans and Native Americans most widely affected (Aguilar et al., 2015). As with sex, linear regression modeling can adjust for the effects of age. In fact, it is often considered the sine qua non of such analyses. However, independent age-stratified analyses may shed light on whether there are neuroanatomical differences seen in cross sections of early vs. young vs. middle-aged adults, and whether age is still a significant co-factor within the stratified age-groups.

Previously, our lab characterized the neural signature of MetS (NS-MetS) using a large age- and sex-matched cohort (n = 208; 37.3 ± 13.2 years, 56.7% women) of Mexican-American participants, 104 meeting the International Diabetes Federation criteria for MetS and 104 healthy controls that did not meet any criteria. Those with MetS were observed to have lower gray matter volume in specific brain regions as compared to their age- and sex-matched metabolically healthy controls. These five chief brain regions included the posterior cerebellum, brainstem, orbitofrontal cortex, right insula/limbic structures, and caudate nuclei (Kotkowski et al., 2019; Figure 1). Interestingly, decreased GMV within the hippocampus was conspicuously absent from the findings. This was surprising because the hippocampus is the structure whose degeneration is touted as the primary feature in the pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s disease. It is also often the most often alleged culprit in much of the diabetes and obesity-related dementia literature (Biessels and Reagan, 2015; Alford et al., 2018).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The neural signature of metabolic syndrome (NS-MetS) as defined by Kotkowski et al. (2019). Each indicated region represents one of the five main clusters that comprise the NS-MetS. Control regions include gray matter densities from the whole brain and hippocampal, neither of which are part of the NS-MetS.

For the present study, we sought to identify the extent to which brain regions making up the neural signature of MetS are related to the biometric components that define MetS. This will allow us to specify the metabolic underpinnings associated with reduced gray matter volume in individuals with MetS and identify how they differ between age-groups and by sex. We therefore hypothesized that gray matter volume will exhibit an inverse linear relationship with each MetS components across our community-based population (with the exception of HDL cholesterol), implying that the relationship between NS-MetS GMVs in these regions and individual MetS components lie on a continuum. We also hypothesized that sex and age would show variations in the relationship between gray matter and MetS, crucial factors necessary for the design of future studies and treatment plans. Moreover, we included whole brain gray matter and hippocampal gray matter as regions of interest in our analyses due to their high research interest with the hypothesis that they will show weaker correlations with MetS components than NS-MetS regions.

Materials and methods

Participants

The Genetics of Brain Structure (GOBS) dataset is a cross-sectional imaging archive aimed at localizing, identifying, and characterizing genes and quantitative trait loci associated with variations in brain structure and function. To minimize selection bias, participants were drawn from extended pedigrees of Mexican American families recruited at random from the San Antonio, Texas community. Demographic and data acquisition details have been previously described (Winkler et al., 2010; Curran et al., 2013; Kotkowski et al., 2019). Briefly, 1,911 individuals were recruited in three data acquisition blocks over the course of 15 years. The sample in the current manuscript was selected from the GOBS cohort based on strict inclusion criteria. Participants must have completed structural MRI scans and acquired all MetS component metrics: fasting plasma glucose, lipid panel, blood pressure, and waist circumference. A notable limitation of this cohort is its lack of more precise glucose measures in the form of hemoglobin A1c.

Of the 1,911 participants in the GOBS archive, 519 were excluded due to lack of or inadequate, incomplete, or absent T1-weighted MRI scans. An additional 557 participants from the first phase of acquisition were excluded due to a lack relevant biometric, blood chemistry, and lipid data relevant to MetS. Thirty Five subsequent participants with a history of stroke, neurosurgery, dementia, or substance use disorder also were excluded from the analysis. Finally, 24 individuals 66-years and older, were excluded because the cohort size, which would have been termed “older adults,” was not large enough to produce statistically significant data. This left a total of 776 individuals in the final sample. These individuals were further sub-divided arbitrarily into three age-groups of roughly similar size and by sex as follows: early adult (18–25 years) men (n = 137) and women (n = 148), young adult (26–45 years) men (n = 137) and women (n = 183), and middle-age (46–65 years) men (n = 67) and women (n = 103).

Magnetic resonance imaging

Scans were obtained on a 3T Siemens Tim Trio MRI scanner at the University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio’s Research Imaging Institute with the aid of an 8-element high-resolution phase array head coil. A total of seven high quality T1-weighted 3D structural images were collected per participant via a TurboFLASH sequence with an adiabatic inversion recovery pulse (TE = 3.04 ms, TR = 2,000 ms, TI = 795 ms, flip angle = 8°, NEX = 6; FOV = 200 mm) optimized to achieve a gray/white contrast of approximately 25% with signal to noise ratio of 25. Each image contained 0.8 mm3 isotropic voxels and covered the entire brain and cerebellum. Scan time per participant for the anatomical T1-weighted 3D scan totaled 60 min. Afterward, each of the seven MPRAGE T1-weighted images was motion corrected using a retrospective motion-correction protocol and averaged to generate a single high-resolution anatomical image per participant. This method of acquisition produces high quality images because it breaks down a long acquisition time into several shorter acquisitions that are then corrected using inter-scan motion. This is done by registering images to the third image and forming a single image that is both averaged and motion-corrected, thus improving contrast-to-noise ratio and boundary detail (Kochunov et al., 2006).

Gray matter volume

Brain volume was calculated using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) voxel-based morphometry (VBM) pipeline (Good et al., 2001; Smith et al., 2004). We used Freesurfer for initial processing with the autorecon1 command that corrects head motion, normalizes intensities, and skull-strips the brain for further processing (Fischl, 2012). Skull-stripped brains were then processed with FSL-VBM (Douaud et al., 2007). The next step entailed segmenting gray-matter by also eliminating the influence of white matter and cerebrospinal fluid and performing a non-linear registration to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)-152 1 × 1 × 1 standardized brain space (Andersson et al., 2007). A study-specific template was then created to which all images were subsequently registered. Images were then modulated to correct for expansion (or contraction) resulting from the spatial transformation from native space to the standard MNI-125 template space. Finally, gray matter images were smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel (sigma = 3 mm).

Once processed, normalized gray matter volumes were extracted from each individual brain by applying region of interest (ROI) masks for the entire neural signature of metabolic syndrome (NS-MetS), and each of the five largest NS-MetS sub-regions, including: the posterior cerebellum, brainstem, orbitofrontal cortex, right insular/limbic cluster and caudate (Figure 1). Additionally, we also measured hippocampal and whole brain ROIs, due to the high interest in these regions in the MetS, diabetes, and obesity literature. Normalized values of gray matter density were calculated by taking the total gray matter signal from each ROI in each normalized image and dividing it by the total ROI volume for each relevant ROI.

Group analysis

All group analyses were performed using SPSS software (version 25; SPSS Inc., Cary, Chicago, United States). Two-tailed p-values < 0.008 were considered statistically significant after correcting for multiple comparisons between the six groups in our study (early adult men and women, young adult men and women, and middle-aged men and women). Because fasting plasma glucose (FPG) and triglyceride (TG) levels are not normally distributed and have a positive skew, we applied a log-transform to these values to fit the linear regression framework. We then assessed the differences between the three age groups within each sex using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by a separate one-way ANOVA comparing men and women. Finally, we performed two separate (for each sex) age-adjusted partial Pearson’s correlations to probe the association between MetS’s component values, gray matter densities, and other related biometric values of interest (LDL cholesterol, total cholesterol, body mass index, and NCEP-ATP III MetS score).

A total of six univariate linear regression analyses were performed independently within in each of the six age/sex groups. We chose this approach in order to identify the MetS components (independent variables) that would most strongly correlate with GMV within select regions of interest (dependent variables). All MetS variables plus age were entered in a single linear regression step. This method allowed us to correct for each variable’s influence on every other variable and subsequently identify the most significant variable or variables accounting for the observed correlations.

Results

Demographics

The demographic and MetS component characteristics of our cohort can be found in Table 1 where the most appreciable between-sex differences (p≤ 0.001) were observed in triglycerides (TG), high density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, diastolic blood pressure (BPD), and National Cholesterol Education Program Adult Treatment Panel III (NCEP-ATP III) scores for MetS. Notably, there were no significant differences seen between the sexes with respect to BMI, total cholesterol, low density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol, and global assessment of functioning (GAF) scores. There were also no significant differences in the percentage of participants with type 2 diabetes, hypercholesterolemia, hypertriglyceridemia, and hypertension between the sexes or in individuals taking prescribed medications for each of these disorders. As hypothesized, every within-sex and between-age measure was significantly different in men with the exception of HDL cholesterol and Global Assessment of Function (GAF) scores. Similarly, each within-sex and between-age measure was significantly different in women with the exception of GAF scores.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Group demographic data and health characteristics (mean ± SD).

Table 2 lists gray matter densities (GMV) for each region of interest obtained from voxel-based morphometry (VBM) processing arranged by sex- and age-group, previously described. As with Table 1, the results highlight the differences between men and women and differences between age groups. The brain regions whose gray matter densities were significantly different between men and women (p ≤ 0.001) included the whole brain, neural signature of metabolic syndrome (NS-MetS) regions as a whole, orbitofrontal cortex, right insular/limbic cluster, the caudate nuclei (p = 0.003), and brainstem (p = 0.013). Notable between sex difference exceptions included the posterior cerebellum (p = 0.254) and hippocampus (p = 0.829). Importantly, results did not change when covaried for total brain volume. Interestingly, differences between age-groups and within sex-groups in all brain regions’ GMVs were significantly different (p ≤ 0.001) from one another with the notable exception of the hippocampus (p = 0.079 for men and p = 0.222 for women).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain gray matter volume statistics by age, sex, and brain regions.

We also created an age-adjusted figure, which incorporates the demographic characteristics from Tables 1, 2 for each sex group using partial correlation with the effect of age regressed out (Figure 2). This figure is helpful for visualizing age-adjusted correlations in our sample cohort between GMV in the NS-MetS brain regions (with the addition of whole brain and hippocampus), correlations between MetS values, and correlations between other lipid measures of interest (NCEP-ATP III scores, body mass index, total cholesterol, and LDL cholesterol). The figure also indicates the strength and significance of each correlation between the NS-MetS regions and MetS factors most relevant for each sex. For example, after adjusting for age, we can appreciate that the relationship between waist circumference (WC) and posterior cerebellar GMV is strong in both men (r = –0.50) and women (r = –0.40), p ≤ 0.001.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Age-adjusted partial Pearson’s correlation tables in men (A) and women (B) for gray matter densities of the neural signature of metabolic syndrome (NS-MetS); its substructures: posterior cerebellum (P. CB), brainstem (BS), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), right insular/limbic cluster (R. IN/LC), caudate nuclei (CN); control brain regions: whole brain (WB) and hippocampus (HPC); components of metabolic syndrome: waist circumference (WC), log-transformed fasting plasma glucose (FPG), log-transformed triglycerides (TG), high-density lipoprotein cholesterol (HDL), systolic blood pressure (BPS), and diastolic blood pressure (BPD); other biometric values: national cholesterol education program adult treatment plan III metabolic syndrome index score (NCEP-ATP), body mass index (BMI), total cholesterol (TC), and low density lipoprotein cholesterol (LPL); and cognitive/psychometric function scores: global assessment of functioning scale (GAF), and latent variable of GOBS psychometric tests composite (Psy). ***Significant at p ≤ 0.001, **significant at p ≤ 0.01, *significant at p < 0.05.

Table 3 (men) and 4 (women) represent the results from our linear regression analyses with each table sub-divided by age-group. A total of eight linear regression analyses were conducted for each brain structure within each age-group. This allowed us to identify which of MetS’s five components (WC, FPG, TG, HDL, BP, with the addition of age) most significantly predicted GMV in each region after adjusting for every other covariate.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. MetS components’ relationship with gray matter volume in neural signature of metabolic syndrome substructures, whole brain, and hippocampus in men.

In Table 3, we can appreciate that the MetS components that most strongly predict GMV in the NS-MetS. In early adult men, these are the combined values of WC and triglycerides (TG), accounting for an effect size of r2 = 0.372 (also Figure 3). This indicates that 37.2% of the variance in GMV in the NS-MetS can be explained by the combined effects of WC and TG. In young adult men, WC was the single strongest predictor (r2 = 0.269). In middle-aged men, age and WC were the two strongest predictors (r2 = 0.161). Overall, WC was the most significant predictor of GMV in the NS-MetS and posterior cerebellum in early, young, and middle-aged adult men (Figure 4). TGs were only seen as significant in early adult men, albeit with small effect sizes (r2 = 0.050 for the right insular/limbic cluster and r2 = 0.061 for the caudate). Further, age—within the age-group variance—appeared as the most significant predictor of GMV in the orbitofrontal cortex, caudate, brainstem, right insular/limbic cluster and in the whole brain for young and middle-aged adult men.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Combined neural signature of metabolic syndrome gray matter volume vs. waist circumference scatterplot series.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Posterior cerebellar gray matter volume vs. waist circumference scatterplot series.

In Table 4, we can appreciate that WC was the single strongest predictor of GMV in the NS-MetS in early adult women (r2 = 0.193) (Figure 3). The combined association of WC and age were the strongest predictors of GMV in the NS-MetS for young adult women (r2 = 0.254), and WC and FPG were the strongest predictors of GMV in middle-aged women (r2 = 0.226). Overall, WC was the most significant of the MetS components in predicting GMV in women of all age groups and in almost every region. Our data indicate that each MetS component contributed to GMV in more brain regions in women as compared to men, but the effects of MetS components appeared larger in men than in women, predominantly in the posterior cerebellum. Secondary to WC in women were age, FPG, and TG, with the latter two expressing the largest correlation with GMV in middle-aged women.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. MetS components’ relationship with gray matter volume in neural signature of metabolic syndrome substructures, whole brain, and hippocampus in women.

For men and women in general, the primary MetS components exhibiting any degree of significance were WC, TG, age (within age groups), FPG and to a lesser extent, HDL. This indicates that most of the neuroanatomical effects driven by MetS are weighted primarily by these components. Moreover, MetS component’s effects on the whole brain’s gray matter were minimal, with age (within age-groups) emerging as the most significant predictor of whole brain GMV in both men and women within age groups. Importantly, none of the MetS components exerted any noteworthy predictive value on hippocampal gray matter volume.

Discussion

Our first study found that the greatest differences in gray matter volume (GMV) between individuals with MetS and metabolically healthy controls were observed in the posterior cerebellum, brainstem, orbitofrontal cortex, right insular/limbic cluster (involving the right posterior insula and right amygdala), and caudate nuclei (Figure 1). Other groups have reported similar findings, including an inverse relationship between WC and cerebellar GMV (Kurth et al., 2013) and decreased GMV density of the right insula in individuals with MetS associated with allostatic load (Zsoldos et al., 2018).

The cerebellum has long been known to be involved in motor control and coordination, but recent studies have shown that it also plays an important role in cognition (Schmahmann, 2018; Kotkowski et al., 2020). The cerebellum serves to integrate information from the cortex to produce an output that is coordinated, precise, and accurate on a millisecond timescale in both motor and cognitive domains. While this is obvious in the setting of motor coordination, the behavioral and cognitive aspects are not readily apparent. In fact, these two functions exhibit a clear anatomical partition in the cerebellum with an anterior lobe that modulates motor function and a posterior lobe that modulates behavioral and cognitive function (Stoodley and Schmahmann, 2009). As posited by Jeremy Schmahmann in 1991: “In the same way as the cerebellum regulates the rate, force, rhythm and accuracy of movements, so may it regulate the speed, capacity, consistency and appropriateness of mental or cognitive processes (Schmahmann, 1991).” For this reason, gray matter impairment in the posterior cerebellum might be responsible for observed cognitive impairment in individuals with longstanding MetS and associated comorbidities such as type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM) and obesity.

The nodal stress hypothesis might help explain the link between MetS and the caudate nuclei, orbitofrontal cortex, and amygdala in the context of the reward (or appetitive) network. This hypothesis stipulates that regions with high levels of connectivity, which require higher metabolic demand and blood flow, are preferentially susceptible to damage from oxidative stress (Zhou et al., 2012; Crossley et al., 2014). Thus, the over-stimulation—or chronic stimulation of the reward/appetitive network (orbitofrontal cortex, caudate nuclei, insula)—by over-consuming highly palatable food might partially explain our GMV findings. Alternatively, it is possible that genetically-determined predispositions, such as those discussed by Curran et al. (2013) in chromosome 17 associated with leptin signaling, might account for both brain morphology and obesity findings (Winkler et al., 2010). The effects in these regions may thus be components of vulnerability, outcome, or a combination of the two. Nevertheless, a more sophisticated explanation is warranted to explain the pronounced sex differences observed in our study, where we find that certain MetS components appear to correlate more strongly with more regions of the NS-MetS in women than in men, with the notable exception of the posterior cerebellum, which appears to have a stronger correlation with MetS components in men than in women.

Our most surprising finding in this study was the strong negative association between GMV in the posterior cerebellum and WC. We know that in Mexican American populations, elevated waist circumference is independently associated with insulin resistance and risk for developing T2DM (Mamtani et al., 2013). Thus, it could be postulated that the posterior cerebellum is more susceptible to chronically elevated plasma insulin levels. Insulin in the brain plays an important role in modulating synaptic transmission, maintaining neuronal and glial metabolism, and modulating the neuroinflammatory response (Kleinridders et al., 2014). Insulin resistance in the brain might therefore manifest itself as neuronal degeneration detected as decreased GMV. Insulin-like growth factor (IGF) receptors are also heavily expressed in both the cerebellum and the hippocampus (Verdile et al., 2015) and insulin can cross-react with the IGF receptors. This may explain the neuronal degeneration in the cerebellum, but it does not explain why the hippocampus is spared from decreased GMV under the same conditions. Alternatively, the cerebellum is the only brain region, aside from the hypothalamus, with leptin receptors. This has led some investigators to speculate that leptin resistance might also play a role in cerebellar degeneration (Berman et al., 2012), though our current understanding of the mechanistic processes by which this occurs is still in its infancy.

Insulin in the brain modulates synaptic plasticity, specifically of dopamine neurons involved in the reward and appetitive network (Stouffer et al., 2015). In cases of diet-induced obesity, insulin resistance in the hippocampus—a significant brain structure in the pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s dementia (AD), which happens to densely express insulin receptors—has a negative impact on learning and memory. For example, when insulin is elevated, increased glucose metabolism was observed in the appetitive network, prefrontal cortex, insula, and anterior cingulate cortex, but decreased in the hippocampus and cerebellum (Anthony et al., 2006). Findings such as these that identify common structures to those of AD have generated increased interest in identifying hippocampal pathology mechanisms that relate to MetS, type 2 diabetes, obesity and AD. What our research has shown is that when it comes to gray matter atrophy, the pattern observed with the NS-MetS is very distinct from that of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD. Whereas MCI and AD begin demonstrating gray matter loss in the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe structures prior to symptom onset, the same mechanism cannot be suggested to explain why individuals with MetS end up developing cognitive decline. Instead, other brain regions that have not been as extensively studied but are nonetheless important to cognition, namely the posterior cerebellum, should be given more attention in future research.

Our study’s key strength is that it represents a population-based and heterogenous community sample, as opposed to a disease-based sample, allowing us to investigate relationships between normally distributed characteristics. Additionally, the T1-weighted MRI images available for each of the 776 subjects were of exceptionally high quality, as highlighted in the methods, using a research-dedicated 3T scanner and 1-h long, multi-faceted anatomical brain scans. However, our study also had notable limitations. The largest limitation is its cross-sectional and retrospective design. This method limited our ability to obtain biomarkers which would have made our subject stratification more ideal. For example, using hemoglobin A1c instead of fasting plasma glucose as a metric to define blood glucose levels. The cross-sectional nature of our sample allowed us to calculate correlations but prevented us from deducing causal relationships. Furthermore, we were limited by the number of participants in older age-groups that could have yielded valuable information regarding the effects of MetS factors on older individuals who manifest cognitive decline in higher numbers. Finally, were also limited by a lack of data on when each individual first met diagnostic criteria for MetS and related diseases like diabetes and hypertension along with how long they carried the disease burden. This last limitation is the key in understanding why there could be a lower variance in the correlation data between NS-MetS GMV and WC and the posterior cerebellar GMV and WC in older men (Figures 3, 4). For example, it is likely that men who meet the diagnostic criteria for MetS or have increased WC early in life suffer earlier instances of morbidity and mortality, thus experiencing deleterious brain effects earlier. These same individuals might be less likely to be represented in the older cohort who may have developed increased waist circumference and MetS biomarkers later in life after decades of relative health.

In summary, our results provide a detailed breakdown of important brain regions implicated in the pathophysiology of MetS while identifying the sub-components that are most associated with GMV in these regions. The results also highlight the importance of age and sex in determining the effects of MetS on the brain. Future neurocognitive, structural, and functional analyses in future studies involving participants with MetS, obesity, and diabetes can stand to benefit from our findings in their experimental designs. These data can also aid in generating hypotheses that merit further inquiry, such as probing why the observed sex differences exist, specifically in questions where sex differences might play a role in metabolic signaling, such as in fat distribution differences, menopause, and other potential hormonal effects. Anticipated investigations will also be focused at understanding the mechanistic nature of why certain MetS components correlate with GMV, specifically in the posterior cerebellum.

Conclusion

This study examined the associations between age groups and between the sexes with each of the five components of metabolic syndrome (MetS) and five brain regions comprising the previously defined neural signature of MetS (NS-MetS). We found that waist circumference (WC) was the strongest predictor of decreased posterior cerebellar gray matter volume (GMV), regardless of age or sex. In men, associations between WC and posterior cerebellum were most pronounced in early adults, an effect that appeared to decrease with increasing age. The inverse was true in women, where the association between WC, (along with TG, and FPG) on the brain’s gray matter volume appeared to be slightly weaker in early adult women but increased with increasing age. Between men and women, the potential of MetS components to predict GMV particularly strong in men but confined primarily to the posterior cerebellum. In women, the correlation between MetS components on GMV was more diffuse across most NS-MetS regions.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from DG and JB. Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for this study.

Ethics statement

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional committee, NIH, and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants involved in this study. The patient data utilized in this research project has been acquired and is currently archived at the Research Imaging Institute as part of the Genetics of Brain Structure and Function (GOBS) study. The GOBS database constitutes a large (n = 1911) series of magnetic resonance and genetics data gathered by DG and JB. This project was funded by a series of NIMH grants (MH078111; MH0708143; MH083824). Part of the resource-sharing agreement for these grants is that GOBS data be de-identified and accessible for future studies.

Author contributions

EK and PF devised the project and its main conceptual ideas with help from RAD. EK wrote the manuscript with support from PF, RAD, CF, and AG. CF was instrumental in the design of the methods involving data curation and image processing with help from MS. LP assisted in the design of statistical methods and analyses used in the study. MW, JB, DG, RD, and PF obtained the individual subject data and MRI scans used in this study. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by funds from the National Institutes of Health (NIH/NIGMS: South Texas Medical Scientist Training Program, T32 GM113898; NIH/NIMH; R01 Diversity Supplement Grant, R01 MH074457-11S1; NIH/NCATS: Translational Scientist Training Program, TL1 TR002647-01; NIH/NIA: South Texas Alzheimer’s Disease Center P30 AG066546). The content was solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Drs. Cheasequah J. Blevins and Tom Vanasse for their valuable edits, insight, and data analysis guidance. We would also like to thank the UT Health San Antonio Medical Scientist Training Program for their Institutional and Academic support.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aguilar, M., Bhuket, T., Torres, S., Liu, B., and Wong, R. J. (2015). Prevalence of the metabolic syndrome in the United States, 2003-2012. JAMA 313, 1973–1974. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.4260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alford, S., Patel, D., Perakakis, N., and Mantzoros, C. S. (2018). Obesity as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease: Weighing the evidence. Obes. Rev. 19, 269–280. doi: 10.1111/obr.12629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, J. L. R., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. (2007). Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalization. FMRIB technical report TR07JA2. Available online at: www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep (accessed April 15, 2019).

Google Scholar

Anthony, K., Reed, L. J., Dunn, J. T., Bingham, E., Hopkins, D., Marsden, P. K., et al. (2006). Attenuation of insulin-evoked responses in brain networks controlling appetite and reward in insulin resistance: The cerebral basis for impaired control of food intake in metabolic syndrome? Diabetes 55, 2986–2992. doi: 10.2337/db06-0376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, S. M., Paz-Filho, G., Wong, M.-L., Kohno, M., Licinio, J., and London, E. D. (2012). Effects of leptin deficiency and replacement on cerebellar response to food-related cues. Cerebellum 12, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0360-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biessels, G. J., and Reagan, L. P. (2015). Hippocampal insulin resistance and cognitive dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 660–671. doi: 10.1038/nrn4019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, J. D., Chaput, J.-P., Sjödin, A. M., and Goldfield, G. S. (2017). Brain on fire: Incentive salience, hedonic hot spots, dopamine, obesity, and other hunger games. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 37, 183–205. doi: 10.1146/annurev-nutr-071816-064855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavalieri, M., Ropele, S., Petrovic, K., Pluta-Fuerst, A., Homayoon, N., Enzinger, C., et al. (2010). Metabolic syndrome, brain magnetic resonance imaging, and cognition. Diabetes Care 33, 2489–2495. doi: 10.2337/dc10-0851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornier, M. A., Salzberg, A. K., Endly, D. C., Bessesen, D. H., and Tregellas, J. R. (2010). Sex-based differences in the behavioral and neuronal responses to food. Physiol. Behav. 99, 538–543. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crossley, N. A., Mechelli, A., Scott, J., Carletti, F., Fox, P. T., McGuire, P., et al. (2014). The hubs of the human connectome are generally implicated in the anatomy of brain disorders. Brain 137, 2382–2395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran, J. E., McKay, D. R., Winkler, A. M., Olvera, R. L., Carless, M. A., Dyer, T. D., et al. (2013). Identification of pleiotropic genetic effects on obesity and brain anatomy. Hum. Hered. 75, 136–143. doi: 10.1159/000353953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douaud, G., Smith, S., Jenkinson, M., Behrens, T., Johansen-Berg, H., Vickers, J., et al. (2007). Anatomically related grey and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain 130, 2375–2386. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ervin, R. B. (2009). Prevalence of metabolic syndrome among adults 20 years of age and over, by sex, age, race and ethnicity, and body mass index: United States, 2003–2006. Natl. Health Stat. Rep. 13, 1–7.

Google Scholar

Fischl, B. (2012). FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62, 774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geijselaers, S. L. C., Sep, S. J. S., Stehouwer, C. D. A., and Biessels, G. J. (2014). Glucose regulation, cognition, and brain MRI in type 2 diabetes: A systematic review. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 3, 75–89. doi: 10.1016/S2213-8587(14)70148-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, J., Harris, K., Hackett, M., Peters, S., Brodaty, H., Cooper, M., et al. (2021). Sex differences in risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia, including death as a competing risk, in individuals with diabetes: Results from the ADVANCE trial. Diabetes Obes. Metab. 23, 1775–1785. doi: 10.1111/dom.14391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, H. M., Tarraf, W., Gouskova, N., Gallo, L. C., Penedo, F. J., Davis, S. M., et al. (2015). Neurocognitive function among middle-aged and older hispanic/latinos: Results from the hispanic community health study/study of latinos. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 30, 68–77. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acu066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, C. D., Johnarude, I. B., Ashburner, J., Hensop, R. N., Riston, K. J., and Frackowiak, R. B. (2001). Voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroimage 14, 21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grundy, S. M., Cleeman, J. I., Daniels, S. R., Donato, K. A., Eckel, R. H., Franklin, B. A., et al. (2005). Diagnosis and management of the metabolic syndrome: An American heart association/national heart, lung, and blood institute scientific statement: Executive summary. Circulation 112, 285–290. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.169405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, H., Huff, C. D., Yamamura, Y., Wu, X., and Strom, S. S. (2015). The relationship between native American ancestry, body mass index and diabetes risk among Mexican-Americans. PLoS One 10:e141260. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janowitz, D., Wittfeld, K., Terock, J., Freyberger, H. J., Hegenscheid, K., Völzke, H., et al. (2015). Association between waist circumference and gray matter volume in 2344 individuals from two adult community-based samples. Neuroimage 122, 149–157. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinridders, A., Ferris, H. A., Cai, W., and Kahn, C. R. (2014). Insulin action in brain regulates systemic metabolism and brain function. Diabetes 63, 2232–2243. doi: 10.2337/db14-0568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochunov, P., Lancaster, J. L., Glahn, D. C., Purdy, D., Laird, A. R., Gao, F., et al. (2006). Retrospective motion correction protocol for high-resolution anatomical MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 957–962. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochunov, P., Thompson, P. M., Winkler, A., Morrissey, M., Fu, M., Coyle, T. R., et al. (2016). The common genetic influence over processing speed and white matter microstructure: Evidence from the Old Order Amish and Human Connectome Projects. Neuroimage 125, 189–197. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotkowski, E., Franklin, C. G., Price, L. R., Salazar, M., Woolsey, M., DeFronzo, R. A., et al. (2019). A neural signature of metabolic syndrome. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 3575–3588. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotkowski, E., Price, L. R., Blevins, C. J., Franklin, C., Woolsey, M., DeFronzo, R. A., et al. (2020). Using the schmahmann syndrome scale to assess cognitive impairment in young adults with metabolic syndrome: A hypothesis-generating report. Cerebellum 20, 295–299. doi: 10.1007/s12311-020-01212-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurth, F., Levitt, J. G., Phillips, O. R., Luders, E., Woods, R. P., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2013). Relationships between gray matter, body mass index, and waist circumference in healthy adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1737–1746. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lansley, J., Mataix-Cols, D., Grau, M., Radua, J., and Sastre-Garriga, J. (2013). Localized grey matter atrophy in multiple sclerosis: A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies and associations with functional disability. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 819–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laudisio, A., Marzetti, E., Pagano, F., Cocchi, A., Franceschi, C., Bernabei, R., et al. (2008). Association of metabolic syndrome with cognitive function: The role of sex and age. Clin. Nutr. 27, 747–754. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, E. B., and Mattson, M. P. (2014). The neuropathology of obesity: Insights from human disease. Acta Neuropathol. 127, 3–28. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1190-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mamtani, M., Kulkarni, H., Dyer, T. D., Almasy, L., Mahaney, M. C., Duggirala, R., et al. (2013). Waist circumference independently associates with the risk of insulin resistance and type 2 diabetes in Mexican American Families. PLoS One 8:e59153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. K., Vidoni, E. D., Perea, R. D., Rada, R., Johnson, D. K., Lyons, K., et al. (2014). Insulin resistance and gray matter volume in neurodegenerative disease. Neuroscience 13, 139–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moulton, C. D., Costafreda, S. G., Horton, P., Ismail, K., and Fu, C. H. Y. (2015). Meta-analyses of structural regional cerebral effects in type 1 and type 2 diabetes. Brain Imaging Behav. 9, 651–662. doi: 10.1007/s11682-014-9348-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reaven, G. M. (1988). Banting lecture 1988: Role of insulin resistance in human disease. Diabetes 37, 1595–1607. doi: 10.2337/diab.37.12.1595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Regitz-Zagrosek, V., Lehmkuhl, E., and Weickert, M. O. (2006). Gender differences in the metabolic syndrome and their role for cardiovascular disease. Clin. Res. Cardiol. 95, 136–147. doi: 10.1007/s00392-006-0351-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reijmer, Y. D., Brundel, M., de Bresser, J., Kappelle, L. J., Leemans, A., and Biessels, G. J. (2012). Microstructural white matter abnormalities and cognitive functioning in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 36, 137–144. doi: 10.2337/dc12-0493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D. (1991). An emerging concept: The cerebellar contribution to higher function. Arch. Neurol. 48, 1178–1187. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1991.00530230086029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D. (2018). The cerebellum and cognition. Neurosci. Lett. 688, 62–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl.), S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoodley, C. J., and Schmahmann, J. D. (2009). Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage 44, 489–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stouffer, M., Woods, C., Patel, J., Lee, C. R., Witkovsky, P., Bao, L., et al. (2015). Insulin enhances striatal dopamine release by activating cholinergic interneurons and thereby signals reward. Nat. Commun. 6:8543. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Bloemendaal, L., Ijzerman, R. G., ten Kulve, J. S., Barkhof, F., Diamant, M., Veltman, D. J., et al. (2016). Alterations in white matter volume and integrity in obesity and type 2 diabetes. Metab. Brain Dis. 31, 621–629. doi: 10.1007/s11011-016-9792-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanhanen, M., Koivisto, K., Moilanen, L., Helkala, E. L., Hänninen, T., Soininen, H., et al. (2006). Association of metabolic syndrome with Alzheimer disease. Neurology 67, 843–847. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000234037.91185.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdile, G., Fuller, S. J., and Martins, R. N. (2015). The role of type 2 diabetes in neurodegeneration. Neurobiol. Dis. 84, 22–38. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, A. M., Kochunov, P., Blangero, J., Almasy, L., Zilles, K., Fox, P. T., et al. (2010). Cortical thickness or gray matter volume? The importance of selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies. Neuroimage 15, 1135–1146. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, E. J., Sadeh, N., Leritz, E. C., Logue, M. W., Stoop, T. B., McGlinchey, R., et al. (2016). Posttraumatic stress disorder as a catalyst for the association between metabolic syndrome and reduced cortical thickness. Biol. Psychiatry 80, 363–371. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.11.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, G., Lin, L., Zhang, Q., and Wu, J. (2017). Brain gray matter changes in type 2 diabetes mellitus: A meta-analysis of whole-brain voxel-based morphometry study. J. Diabetes Complications 31, 1698–1703. doi: 10.1016/j.jdiacomp.2017.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yates, K. F., Sweat, V., Yau, P. L., Turchiano, M. M., and Convit, A. (2012). Impact of metabolic syndrome on cognition and brain: A selected review of the literature. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 32, 2060–2067. doi: 10.1161/ATVBAHA.112.252759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Gennatas, E. D., Kramer, J. H., Miller, B. L., and Seeley, W. W. (2012). Predicting regional neurodegeneration from the healthy brain functional connectome. Neuron 73, 1216–1227. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zsoldos, E., Filippini, N., Mahmood, A., Mackay, C. E., Singh-Manoux, A., Kivimamp, M., et al. (2018). Allostatic load as a predictor of grey matter volume and white matter integrity in old age: The Whitehall II MRI study. Sci. Rep. 8:6411. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24398-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: metabolic syndrome, insulin resistance, central obesity, waist circumference, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), voxel-based morphometry (VBM), posterior cerebellum, cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome (CCAS)

Citation: Kotkowski E, Price LR, DeFronzo RA, Franklin CG, Salazar M, Garrett AS, Woolsey M, Blangero J, Duggirala R, Glahn DC and Fox PT (2022) Metabolic syndrome predictors of brain gray matter volume in an age-stratified community sample of 776 Mexican- American adults: Results from the genetics of brain structure image archive. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:999288. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.999288

Received: 20 July 2022; Accepted: 26 August 2022;
Published: 20 September 2022.

Edited by:

Tao Ma, Wake Forest School of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Xin Wang, Wake Forest School of Medicine, United States
Da Ma, Simon Fraser University, Canada

Copyright © 2022 Kotkowski, Price, DeFronzo, Franklin, Salazar, Garrett, Woolsey, Blangero, Duggirala, Glahn and Fox. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eithan Kotkowski, Kotkowski@uthscsa.edu

Download