Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 09 February 2012
Sec. Learning and Memory

Conflict processing in the rat brain: behavioral analysis and functional μPET imaging using [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose


      Christine Marx* Christine Marx1*Bjrn Lex Björn Lex2Carsten Calaminus, Carsten Calaminus2,3Wolfgang Hauber Wolfgang Hauber2Heiko Backes Heiko Backes1Bernd Neumaier Bernd Neumaier1Günter Mies Günter Mies1Rudolf Graf Rudolf Graf1Heike Endepols
   Heike Endepols1
  • 1 Max Planck Institute for Neurological Research, Cologne, Germany
  • 2 Department of Animal Physiology, Institute of Biology, University of Stuttgart, Stuttgart, Germany
  • 3 Laboratory for Preclinical Imaging and Imaging Technology of the Werner Siemens-Foundation, Department of Preclinical Imaging and Radiopharmacy, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany

Conflicts in spatial stimulus–response tasks occur when the task-relevant feature of a stimulus implies a response toward a certain location which does not match the location of stimulus presentation. This conflict leads to increased error rates and longer reaction times, which has been termed Simon effect. A model of dual route processing (automatic and intentional) of stimulus features has been proposed, predicting response conflicts if the two routes are incongruent. Although there is evidence that the prefrontal cortex, notably the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), plays a crucial role in conflict processing, the neuronal basis of dual route architecture is still unknown. In this study, we pursue a novel approach using positron emission tomography (PET) to identify relevant brain areas in a rat model of an auditory Simon task, a neuropsychological interference task, which is commonly used to study conflict processing in humans. For combination with PET we used the metabolic tracer [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose, which accumulates in metabolically active brain cells during the behavioral task. Brain areas involved in conflict processing are supposed to be activated when automatic and intentional route processing lead to different responses (dual route model). Analysis of PET data revealed specific activation patterns for different task settings applicable to the dual route model as established for response conflict processing. The rat motor cortex (M1) may be part of the automatic route or involved in its facilitation, while premotor (M2), prelimbic, and ACC seemed to be essential for inhibiting the incorrect, automatic response, indicating conflict monitoring functions. Our findings and the remarkable similarities to the pattern of activated regions reported during conflict processing in humans demonstrate that our rodent model opens novel opportunities to investigate the anatomical basis of conflict processing and dual route architecture.

Introduction

The limits of cognitive capacity are especially evident during attempts to perform simultaneously several tasks, such as speaking and typing. This leads to conflicts in the cognitive system and hence, to slower activity and in the worst case, to action slips, as typing errors. An established method to analyze such conflicts is the so called Simon task (Simon et al., 1970). For example, when a stimulus indicates that a left button press is required, a response conflict arises if the stimulus is presented on the right-hand side. The Simon effect is a neuropsychological interference effect in which reaction times (RTs) are longer and errors more frequent when spatial features of the stimulus (although task-irrelevant) are inconsistent with spatial attributes of the response (Simon et al., 1970). Contemporary models assume that the Simon effect reflects basic cognitive mechanisms in which sensorimotor processing occurs via two different channels (De Jong et al., 1994; “dual route model,” Figure 1D). One route automatically activates the response spatially congruent to stimulus position (“automatic route”), whereas the other route is based on intentional selection of the response (“intentional route”). If stimulus and response position do not concur (incompatible condition), two competing responses are activated, resulting in a delayed response onset and an increased error rate (ER). While the neuronal bases of automatic and intentional pathways are unknown, human fMRI and EEG studies have suggested that the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) monitors conflicts arising during incompatible dual route processing, and signals the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex to improve performance in subsequent conflict trials (conflict resolution; Botvinick et al., 1999, 2001, 2004; Kerns, 2006). Particularly the right inferior frontal cortex seems to participate in response inhibition as one mechanism of conflict resolution (Forstmann et al., 2008). It should be noted that there are other approaches besides the dual route hypothesis to explain the Simon effect. For example, the binding hypothesis proposes the formation of event files (Hommel et al., 2004), i.e., temporary association of cognitive representations (“codes”) containing features of stimuli and response. The speed of event file formation is thought to account for variations of RTs. Another approach, the tectonic theory (Melara et al., 2008), suggests that inappropriate attention to the irrelevant spatial stimulus dimension disrupts selective attention to the relevant non-spatial stimulus dimension. We sought in the present study to use [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose in order to identify the pattern of metabolic activation in the brain of rats concomitant with performance of a Simon task. As the complex manner of the Simon task requires multiple testing and imaging, an established animal model is needed. The Simon effect has already been described in rats (Courtière et al., 2007), and it was shown that metabolic behavioral positron emission tomography (PET) in animal models is a suitable method to detect activation in focal brain regions (Jang et al., 2009; Sung et al., 2009; Endepols et al., 2010). The purpose of the current study therefore was to develop an animal model that links behavior with metabolic brain activity to investigate the anatomical and functional basis of conflict processing.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. General Setup of the auditory Simon task and schematic illustration of the dual route model. (A) The rat starts each trial with a nose poke of t > 1.5 s, which results in playback of an acoustic stimulus from one of the speakers. (B) Reaction time (RT) is measured from start of the stimulus to withdrawal from the nose poke unit. (C) According to stimulus pitch (10/15 kHz), the rat moves to one of the pellet feeders (left/right). The time from withdrawal from the nose poke unit to entering of the pellet receptacle is measured as movement time (MT). (D) Dual route model, after Botvinick et al. (2001), Hommel et al. (2004), Kerns et al. (2004), and Zhang et al. (1999).

Materials and Methods

Animals

All animal procedures adhered to German Welfare Act and were approved by the local animal care committee and regional government authorities.

Eleven male Lister hooded rats (Harlan-Winkelmann, Borchen, Germany) were used, weighing 250 g at the start of the training. Animals were housed in pairs under an inverted 12:12 h light-dark cycle (lights out at 8 am) in a temperature- and humidity-controlled facility room (20 ± 2°C, 50–60%) and restricted to 15 g food per animal per day. Water was available ad libitum.

Apparatus for Behavioral Testing

Animals were tested in an operant chamber (30.5 cm × 24.1 cm × 21.0 cm; Med Associates, Inc., Georgia, VM, USA) with a central nose poke unit and two trough-like food receptacles on either side, equipped with light barriers for measuring reaction and movement times (Robbins et al., 1993). Food receptacles were connected to a motor-driven pellet dispenser, delivering 45-mg precision pellets (Bioserv) as reward. Two loudspeakers (Med Associates “cage tweeter,” range: 5–15 kHz) were placed above the pellet receptacles (Figure 1). The acoustic stimuli consisted of two 300 ms (rise/fall time 5 ms) pure tones with carrier frequencies of 10 and 15 kHz, and a sound pressure level of 60 dB. All experiments took place during the animals’ dark phase under red light.

Operant Conditioning of the Basic Simon Task

On the first day of training rats were allowed to become accustomed to the operant chamber, the auditory stimuli, and the food reward (45-mg precision pellets, Bioserv).The two auditory stimuli alternated in a pseudorandomized fashion every 10 s, and were associated with a food reward from the pellet trough on the side associated with the stimulus. From the second day on, the rats had to learn to initiate a trial by themselves with a nose poke. The nose poke should last at least 1.5 s. Pokes under 1.5 s were indicated by diode illumination in the nose poke unit and punished with 2 s in which the rat could not start a new trial. A correct nose poke resulted in a bilateral tone presentation (i.e., one of the two stimuli was played back simultaneously from the two speakers) and immediate delivery of a pellet at the side associated with the stimulus. In the last training step, the reward was no longer delivered automatically after stimulus presentation. Instead, the rat had to choose one side according to the auditory stimulus and enter the pellet trough. Five of the eleven rats were trained to go to the left food receptacle after a 10-kHz stimulus and to the right food receptacle after a 15-kHz stimulus. The other six rats were trained to make the opposite association between frequency and side of reward. If the choice was correct, a pellet was delivered. If the rat chose the wrong side, the nose poke diode illuminated for 2 s during which the rat could not start a new trial. The procedure of this training step was identical with the control task consisting of 100% neutral trials (TN) in behavioral PET (see below). In all other tests following the training stage, the acoustic stimulus was delivered unilaterally (i.e., one of the two stimuli was played back from one of the speakers, either left or right). Rats were trained for several weeks, always on five consecutive days and then rested for 2 days. Each training session lasted 15 min or was terminated if the rat accomplished 60 correct responses in less than 15 min. Rats advanced to the next training step after reaching a performance level of 85% correct responses.

Basic Simon Task

The basic Simon task resembled the last training step, only with unilateral stimulus presentation, and a total of 120 trials. During the basic Simon task, rats had to initialize every trial with a nose poke >1.5 s in the central nose poke unit, which lead to playback of one single auditory stimulus (300 ms; tone pitch 10 or 15 kHz in pseudorandomized order). According to pitch, rats had to choose the left or right food receptacle, and were rewarded after correct choice. If the sides of stimulus presentation and correct response concurred, this was recorded as a compatible condition (C). During incompatible conditions (I), stimulus and response occurred on different sides. Only in neutral trials (N) tone stimuli were emitted from both speakers simultaneously. Conditions were presented in a pseudorandomized sequence. The reaction time (RT; Figure 1) was taken as the time between start of the auditory stimulus and withdrawal from the nose poke unit, while movement time was taken as the time from nose withdrawal until entrance of the food receptacle. Because movement time was similar under all conditions, it will not be mentioned further in this study. The trial was terminated if the nose was withdrawn before the end of the required nose poke-time of 1.5 s, while trials with RT >1 s or <130 ms were discarded off-line. Error rate (ER) was taken as the percentage of wrong choices, and was arc-sine square root transformed before statistical analysis.

Test Schedule

Two experimental blocks were conducted: one behavioral PET imaging block with five different tests, followed by one purely behavioral block with three different tests. All tests, except the resting state control, were variants of the basic Simon task (see below). Each rat had to perform all tests within a block.

Behavioral PET Imaging Block

In the behavioral PET block we wanted to see focal metabolic brain activation associated with Simon-like conflicts. Each rat underwent five behavioral PET sessions. We took the task with 50% compatible and 50% incompatible trials as a basis, because a balanced number of compatible and incompatible trials avoids biasing the metabolic response by one type of trials.

(1) Basic Simon task with 50% incompatible and 50% compatible trials in randomized order (TR). The Simon task had to be compared to several control conditions:

(2) No-conflict control with neutral trials only (TN). Here, the rats had to do exactly the same as during TR. However, conflicts did not occur because bilateral stimulus presentation prevented spatial information.

Because of the cumulative nature of PET it is not possible to separate metabolic responses to compatible and incompatible trials. We therefore conducted two additional controls with 100% compatible and 100% incompatible trials, respectively:

(3) Basic Simon task with compatible trials only (TC; side of stimulus presentation and required response side always matched);

(4) Basic Simon task with incompatible trials only (TI; stimulus and required response were always on opposite sides); Finally, we wanted to compare the Simon task with the naive situation before training:

(5) Resting state control (RS), with naive rats before operant conditioning. In RS there were no cognitive requirements, the rats only heard the sound stimuli in random order with 10 s interstimulus intervals and food pellets accessible ad libitum in the food receptacle.

Tasks (1)–(4) took place after successful operant conditioning. They were presented on average 6 days apart in the order of increasing complexity, i.e., TN, TC, TI, TR. During the days in between PET sessions, the rats repeated the last training step.

For the combination of behavior with metabolic PET imaging, rats were briefly anesthetized for intraperitoneal injections of [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG; 1.7–2.1 mCi; 500 μl injection volume; stock solution in 228 mM Na-phosphate buffer, diluted as needed with 0.9% NaCl). Five minutes after tracer injection, rats started to perform one of the five tasks in the operant chamber for 30 min. As a glucose analog, FDG is incorporated by active brain cells and is subsequently phosphorylated by hexokinase, but cannot be further metabolized because of the missing hydroxyl group (Wienhard, 2002). The process of trapping is an indicator of the state of metabolic activity of tissue, which can be measured during a scan under anesthesia after the behavioral task. Fifty minutes after FDG administration (i.e., 15 min after the end of the behavioral task), animals were anesthetized by inhalation of isoflurane (5%, delivered in 70% N2O and 30% O2), and placed in the animal holder of a Focus 220 micro PET scanner (CTI/Siemens Knoxville, TN, USA; resolution at center of field of view: 1.4 mm). Breathing rate was kept at 50–70 per min by adjusting isoflurane concentration (1.5–2.5%). Body temperature was held at 37°C with a feedback-controlled flow of warm water through the animal holder. Glucose concentration was measured in a blood sample collected from the tail vein at 60 min after FDG injection, using a blood glucose level meter (One Touch Ultra). Emission data were recorded over 30 min in list mode, starting 60 min after FDG injection. Following Fourier rebinning, data were reconstructed using the iterative OSEM3D/MAP procedure (Qi et al., 1998), resulting in voxel sizes of 0.38 mm × 0.38 mm × 0.82 mm. RTs and ERs measured during FDG accumulation were compared over tasks (1)–(4) using one-way repeated measures ANOVA with post hoc comparison and Holm–Sidak correction. Each task provided one factor level, except task (1)TR, which yielded two factor levels, one for compatible and the other for incompatible trials.

Behavioral Block

In the behavioral block we wanted to see if the Simon effect occurred in a test situation undisturbed from PET procedures, e.g., tracer injection, and if the Simon effect would be influenced by the probability of incompatible trials. All rats conducted three basic Simon tests, each with different relative probabilities of incompatible trials: (1) 20% I; (2) 50% I; identical to TR in the PET block; (3) 80% I. For example, in the 80% I condition a rat trained to associate the 15 kHz tone with the left response side and the 10 kHz tone with the right response side heard the following in randomized order: 15 kHz – right speaker (I) – 40% of trials; 10 kHz – left speaker (I) – 40% of trials; 15 kHz – left speaker (C) – 10% of trials; 10 kHz – right speaker (C) – 10% of trials. A test condition with 100% incompatible trials, as used in the PET block, is not suitable for behavioral analysis, because a certain number of compatible trials is required within the same task for comparison of RT and ER.

Rats received one test session per day, in randomized order balanced between animals. Effects of conflict probability and condition on RT and ER were estimated with two-way repeated measures ANOVA (see results for factorial design) and Holm–Sidak corrected post hoc comparison. Statistical computations were conducted with Sigma Plot (version 11.0, Systat Software, Inc.). Significance level (α-level) was set at p < 0.05.

MRI Scans

To rule out gross structural brain anomalies and to provide individual templates for co-registration of the PET images, T2-weighted structural MR images were acquired. MRI scans were performed in a 4.7-T BioSpec animal scanner (Bruker BioSpin, Ettlingen, Germany) using a quadrature transmit/receive birdcage coil (Rapid Biomedical, Rimpar, Germany) with an inner diameter of 38 mm. A relaxation enhancement (RARE) sequence was used: RARE factor = 8, TR/TE = 5000/14.0 ms, averages = 2, matrix size = 256 × 256, FOV = 4.6 cm × 4.6 cm, 21 slices, slice thickness = 1.3 mm, interslice interval = 1.8 mm. Inhalation anesthesia procedures were the same as those used for μPET scans.

Imaging Data Analysis and Statistics

MRI and PET data were analyzed using the imaging tool VINCI (Vollmar et al., 2007). MR images were manually co-registered on a master brain derived from the atlas of Swanson (2003) and examined for structural abnormalities. PET images were then manually co-registered on the corresponding MR images. With the help of the master brain, individual MR images, and the brain atlas of Paxinos and Watson (2005), three-dimensional volumes of interest (VOIs) corresponding to defined brain areas (Table 1; Figure 6) were drawn section by section in the transverse/coronal plane. Section thickness was identical with z-dimension of voxels, i.e., 0.815 mm. For intensity normalization, every image was divided by the respective mean value of a whole brain VOI (ratio normalization; Arndt et al., 1996). Normalized metabolic activity was then assessed in the VOIs of individual brain areas. Because of chewing food reward pellets during operant behavior, high FDG uptake of temporal muscles was unavailable, and substantial spillover of radioactivity obscured some parts of the lateral cerebral cortex (Figure 2). With a threshold function we determined the outline of muscle activity and used this to draw a muscle artifact mask and to adjust VOIs, if necessary.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Volumes of interest analysis of relative metabolic activity during the Simon task (TR) and control tasks (TC and TI).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Example of a structural MRI and (B) the corresponding PET image. (C) Fusion of the two images shows muscle artifacts on the lateral aspects of the brain (arrowheads). This leads to covering of the lateral cortical regions in the atlas (D). Cg1, Cg2, anterior cingulate cortex area 1 and 2; LS, lateral septum; lStr, dorsolateral striatum; M1, motor cortex; M2, premotor cortex; MS/DB, medial septum and diagonal band of Broca; mStr, mediodorsal striatum. Scale bars: 1 cm.

In order to assess task-relevant regional brain activation, we compared PET sessions with each other. For analyzing brain activation associated with the task condition in general, we compared TI (Simon task with 100% incompatible trials) with the resting state control RS by calculating 100 × TI/RS (i.e., percent normalized metabolic activity of TI versus RS). Metabolic activation associated solely with conflict processing was assessed by displaying percent metabolic activity of TR, TI, and TC versus the no-conflict control TN (100 × TR/TN; 100 × TI/TN; 100 × TC/TN). TN was chosen as reference condition, because the bilateral stimulus presentation provides ambiguous spatial information, and therefore no-conflict occurs. In TC, on the other hand, there is no-conflict, either, but it cannot be ruled out that facilitatory processes (e.g., facilitation of the automatic route) may take place. For this reason, normalized metabolic activity during the incompatible control task TI was also compared to the compatible control task TC as well. Using the one-sample t-test, we compared the resulting relative VOI activities with μ = 100% (i.e., no change relative to TN or TC). In addition to the VOI analysis, we compared matched voxels of the four tests using one-way repeated measures ANOVA. For post hoc comparison, the Holm–Sidak method was used with TN serving as control. Voxels from TI were also compared to TC with the help of the paired t-test. Finally, correlation analyses were run between task related activity changes on the one hand, and RT and ER on the other hand, using the Pearson product moment correlation test. As the rat brain comprises approx. 19,000 voxels, voxel-based statistical calculations include multiple comparisons associated with a considerable increase in the type I error rate. P-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini–Hochberg control of false discovery rate. However, as in previous PET studies with low degrees of freedom (e.g., Nichols and Hayasaka, 2003; Rocke et al., 2005), all individual voxel comparisons missed significance if using the false discovery rate procedure. Uncorrected significant p-values were between 0.01 and 0.05, therefore we set a threshold of p = 0.02 [corresponding to F(3,9) = 5.51], as proposed by Genovese et al. (2002).

Results

Test Schedule

Seven animals were used for behavioral PET, and successfully conducted RS and TI. One rat died during the PET block, and two refused to work reliably in the PET situation, so that four rats completed all five PET tests. For the subsequent behavioral block, these four plus four additional animals were used.

Behavioral Block

Behavioral data. We start with reporting the behavioral data, because occurrence of a stable Simon effect in the behavioral experiments undisturbed from PET procedures is the prerequisite for all further analyses. The different probabilities of incompatible trials are important for the following PET block as well, since a pronounced Simon effect with a high rate of incompatible trials would suggest a high conflict level in the PET TI condition (100% incompatible trials). The analysis of the Simon tasks with three different frequencies of incompatible trials revealed a Simon effect for both [RT; (n = 8); mean values calculated for all three Simon tasks: RT[C] = 315 ms; RT[I] = 351 ms] and [ER; mean values for all three Simon tasks: ER[C] = 7%; ER[I] = 14%; Figure 3]. This was confirmed by two-way repeated measures ANOVA with the factors “condition” (factor levels: I, C) and “probability of incompatible trials” (factor levels: 20% I, 50% I, 80% I), indicating a significant main effect of the factor “condition” on RT [F(1,14) = 16.8, p = 0.005] and ER [F(1,14) = 10.5, p = 0.014]. Post hoc comparison showed that RT[C] was significantly shorter than RT[I] in the 20% I (p = 0.003) and 80% I task (p = 0.029), while ER[C] was significantly lower than ER[I] in 20% I (p = 0.001), 50% I (p = 0.016) and 80% I task (p = 0.029). Conflict probability had no significant main effect on RT and ER.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Reaction times (A) and error rates (B) in compatible (black) versus incompatible (gray) trials at different conflict probabilities, measured in the behavioral block. Values shown are means ± SEM (n = 8).

Behavioral PET Imaging Block

Behavioral data. Here we evaluated if rats showed a Simon effect also in the PET condition, where tracer injection, scanner noise, etc., may have compromised conflict processing. Most important are results of TR, where compatible and incompatible trials can be compared directly. Four animals were tested repetitively in combination with PET in all conditions. Rats conducted 128–246 trials per session. Blood glucose levels at the start of the scan (123–192 mg/dl) did not correlate significantly with trial numbers (R = 0.17, p = 0.53, Pearson product moment correlation test), suggesting that the number of consumed food pellets during the task did not bias global cerebral FDG uptake. TR yielded results comparable to those of the behavioral block (Figure 4). RTs were on average 33 ms shorter in compatible compared to incompatible trials (t = 2.17, p = 0.059; one-tailed paired t-test). In the control tasks consisting of only one type of condition (TC, TI, and TN), RTs were similar to those in incompatible trials of TR and did not differ significantly across tests [F(4,12) = 1.82, p = 0.19, one-way repeated measures ANOVA]. To further assess whether TI is a valid control with high conflict level we compared average RTs during the first, middle, and last third (10 min each) of the task. During the first third, RTs were higher than in the other tasks, but decreased significantly during the following 20 min (Table 2). This indicates a high conflict level at least in the first 10 min of the task.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Reaction times (A) and error rates (B) in the Simon tasks combined with PET imaging. Each dot represents the result from one animal (n = 4). Mean values are indicated by a short line. In the classical Simon task TR, where compatible and incompatible trials were presented in randomized order, the results for compatible and incompatible trials were analyzed separately [TR(C) and TR(I)]. There was a significant Simon effect on error rate, but not on reaction times. TR(C) and TR(I) correspond to black and gray bars, respectively, of the condition “50% incompatible trials” in Figure 2. Control tasks: TC, compatible trials only; TI, incompatible trials only; TN, neutral trials only.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Reaction times during PET sessions (mean ± SEM).

Error rates were on average 5.8% lower in compatible compared to incompatible trials in TR, leading to a significant main effect across tests [F(4,12) = 4.58, p = 0.0178] and a significant difference between compatible and incompatible trials of TR after post hoc comparison.

Metabolic imaging. Subtractive approach. We first describe metabolic patterns associated with the Simon task in general, by comparing metabolic activity in one of the basic Simon task variants (TI) with resting state RS (n = 7; Figure 5). We found a significant task related decrease of metabolic activity in the left prefrontal cortex (VOI statistics: one-sample t-test; t = −3.33, p = 0.0157), right prelimbic cortex (t = −2.74, p = 0.0338), right posterior cingulate region Cg1 (t = −3.66, p = 0.0106) and decreased metabolic activity bilaterally in the posterior Cg2 (left: t = −3.41, p = 0.0144; right: t = −3.24, p = 0.0176). Furthermore, metabolic activity was decreased bilaterally in the retrosplenial granular cortex (left: t = −2.65, p = 0.0382; right: t = 2.54, p = 0.0442), medial septum (t = −4.22, p = 0.0055), bilaterally in the lateral septum (left: t = −4.71, p = 0.0033; right: t = −3.86, p = 0.0084), and in the hypothalamus (t < −3.0, p < 0.022). A task related increase of metabolic activity was also found in the left lateral striatum (t = 3.22, p = 0.0182).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Subtractive approach: percent change of metabolic activity in Simon tasks with 100% incompatible trials (TI) relative to resting state (RS). Grand average from n = 7 animals with mean changes projected onto transverse and horizontal sections of a master brain. Coordinates are mm from Bregma. Areas obscured by muscle artifacts are masked and significant VOIs are indicated by asterisks.

Next, we report metabolic activity changes related to conflict processing. We looked for metabolic activity changes relative to TN, which were visible in TI and TR, but not in TC (Figure 6, columns 1–3; Table 1). In the right prelimbic cortex FDG uptake was significantly increased during TR at the VOI level (t = 15.26, p = 0.0006) as well as during TI and TR at voxel level [F(3,9) > 4.3, p < 0.0378]. FDG uptake also increased significantly in the right ventrolateral striatum, on the border to the entorhinal cortex, during TR [F(3,9) > 4.98, p < 0.0264 for voxels]. In TI, there was a non-significant average increase of FDG uptake of more than 10% in the same region. Metabolic activity decreased in voxels of the left dorsocentral striatum during TI and TR [F(3,9) > 4.5, p < 0.0349].

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Subtractive approach: percent change of metabolic activity in Simon tasks with randomized presentation of compatible and incompatible trials (50% each; TR), 100% compatible trials (TC), and 100% incompatible trials (TI). Values are relative to TN (column 1–3) or TC (column 4). Grand average from n = 4 animals with mean changes projected onto transverse sections of a master brain. Column 5: coordinates (mm from Bregma) and analyzed volumes of interest (VOIs). Areas obscured by muscle artifacts are masked. Significant voxels (uncorrected p < 0.05) are shown in green and (uncorrected p < 0.02) in yellow, significant VOIs are indicated by asterisks.

Brain activity changes related to potential automatic route facilitation caused by compatible trials should be visible in TC and TR, but not in TI. We found decreased metabolic activity in right olfactory tubercle voxels during TC and TR [F(3,9) > 4.6, p < 0.0334].

The cerebrometabolic correlates of automatic route suppression may become most obvious if we analyze TI relative to TC (Figure 6, column 4). Here, we found increased FDG uptake in the right Cg1 region of the ACC, (t > −3.39, p < 0.0428 for voxels), right orbitofrontal cortex (t > −3.29; p < 0.0462 for voxels), right basal forebrain and nucleus accumbens (t > −3.50, p < 0.0393 for voxels), right dorsolateral striatum (t = −3.57, p = 0.0374 for one voxel; t = 13.4711, p = 0.0006 for VOI), right amygdala (t > −3.35, p < 0.0440 for voxels), right subthalamic region (t > −4.11, p < 0.0261 for voxels), right mediodorsal thalamus (t > −3.37, p < 0.0433 for voxels), and left lateral hippocampus (t > −5.22, p < 0.0137 for voxels). FDG uptake decreased in the left tenia tecta (t > 3.19, p < 0.0497 for voxels), right lateral septum (t > 4.06, p < 0.0270 for voxels), left dorsocentral striatum (t = 5.45, p = 0.0122 for voxels), left hippocampus (t = 3.75, p = 0.0331 for voxels), and left mediodorsal thalamus (t > 3.47, p < 0.0403 for voxels).

Correlative approach. Here the search was for areas where metabolic activity correlated with RT and/or ER during TI and TR, but not during TC and TN. The only area satisfying this precondition was the anterior premotor cortex aM2 (Figure 7), where metabolic activity was correlated to ER during TI (left aM2: R = 0.99, p = 0.0021; right aM2: R = 0.99, p = 0.0146) and inversely correlated to RT during TR (left aM2: R = −0.96, p = 0.0440; right aM2: R = −0.86, p = 0.1366). This indicates that with a higher aM2 activity animals will respond faster during conflicting situations but at the cost of a higher error probability.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Correlative approach: error rates and reaction times in Simon tasks containing incompatible trials (TI and TR), plotted over metabolic activity in the left (A,C) and right (B,D) anterior premotor cortex (aM2) of four animals. Significant correlations were found in (A–C).

Discussion

The purpose of the current study was twofold, namely (1) to identify and study brain regions involved in conflict processing in rodents, and (2) to demonstrate that behavioral FDG–PET is a suitable tool to analyze behavior on a metabolic level in a complex cognitive test like the Simon task.

Behavior

Our study confirmed the results of Courtière et al. (2007) that rats performing a Simon task displayed longer RTs and produced more errors in incompatible compared to compatible trials. This was statistically significant in the behavioral block, for both reaction times and ERs, while in the PET block only ERs were significantly elevated in incompatible trials. Taken together, behavioral results indicate that a cognitive conflict was present in incompatible trials, which was sufficient to produce metabolic changes in tasks with mixed compatible and incompatible trials in the PET situation. However, it is not possible to disentangle metabolic responses to compatible and incompatible trials within one PET session, because FDG–PET is a cumulative method. The uptake measured 1 h after FDG injection is heavily weighted to the total metabolic activity prevailing in the preceding interval when the animals were performing the task. We thus had to conduct control sessions in our PET study consisting of either 100% compatible (TC) or 100% incompatible trials (TI). In humans, the Simon effect is strongly reduced or even reversed in tasks that include 80% or more incompatible trials (“practice effect”; Stürmer et al., 2002; Melara et al., 2008; Iani et al., 2009), raising doubt whether the present experimental design generated a sufficient level of conflict in TI. However, a practice effect was not found in our rats, which displayed a significant Simon effect also in tasks with 80% incompatible trials, leading to the assumption that in TI a high conflict level was present as well. It was therefore surprising that RTs during TC and TN were not reduced compared to TI. This may be explained by an order effect, since PET sessions were performed in the sequence TN, TC, TI, TR, on average 6 days apart with training in between. It may be possible that rats further improved their performance during the PET block, so that a learning-induced decrease of RTs compensated a conflict-induced increase of RTs in TI.

Simon Task versus Resting State

According to numerous studies, energy consumption in a given brain area is mainly determined by input activity (for review see Ritter and Villringer, 2002; Raichle and Mintun, 2006) whereby excitatory and inhibitory inputs cannot be distinguished. Elevated FDG uptake can therefore be interpreted as increased afferent activity, independent of spike rate of the neurons in this area. In order to analyze brain activity changes during the Simon task in general, we compared FDG uptake during one of the Simon task variants with resting state (RS). As Simon task we chose the task with 100% incompatible trials (TI), because it was conducted by the most animals. We found a significant decrease of FDG uptake during the task in brain areas recently assigned to the default mode network (DMN) in rats (Lu et al., 2011), namely the prelimbic and cingulate cortex as well as the retrosplenial cortex. This corroborates human PET and fMRI studies demonstrating that the DMN contains areas with a high resting state activity, e.g., the posterior cingulate and anterior medial prefrontal cortices, which decrease their activity in attention-demanding cognitive tasks (Raichle et al., 2001; Greicius et al., 2003). In addition, FDG uptake decreased in the septum and the hypothalamus during TI compared to RS. This may reflect a stress-induced activation of these areas during RS (Sung et al., 2009), because rats had not yet started training when the resting state scan took place and were therefore not as familiar with the operant chamber as during the Simon task scans.

Comparison of Different Simon Task Variants

Brain areas involved in conflict processing are supposed to be activated when automatic and intentional route processing lead to different responses. These areas should therefore change their metabolic activity during PET sessions involving incompatible trials, i.e., during TR and TI, but not during TC. As reference, a no-conflict control (TN; bilateral stimulus presentation, therefore ambiguous spatial information) was used, and reported activity changes were relative to TN, unless otherwise stated. We found a conflict related increase of FDG uptake in the prelimbic cortex. This is in line with other conflict studies on rats (de Wit et al., 2006; Haddon and Killcross, 2006), where the prelimbic cortex, together with the anterior part of the cingulate cortex, was essential for inhibiting the incorrect, competing response (Chudasama et al., 2003) and therefore seems to be involved in conflict resolution. FDG uptake also changed in the right ventrolateral striatum (increase) and the left dorsocentral striatum (decrease) during TR and TI, indicating that these areas are related to conflict processing as well.

During the PET sessions involving compatible trials (TR and TC), possible automatic route facilitation may be reflected by increased FDG uptake. However, we found no spots of increased FDG uptake visible in TR and TC but not in TI. This indicates that facilitation, if present at all, was not strong enough to increase metabolic demand significantly. We therefore hoped to increase metabolic contrast between maximal conflict associated with automatic route suppression and minimal conflict possibly associated with automatic route facilitation by comparing TI to TC rather than to TN. A decrease of FDG uptake in TI versus TC could then be interpreted as decrease of excitatory activity of serially coupled automatic route areas. An increase of FDG uptake could reflect (1) an increase of inhibitory activity during automatic route suppression, or (2) an increase of excitatory activity reflecting conflict monitoring or resolution which was not strong enough to be visible in TI or TR versus TN. We found an increased FDG uptake in TI relative to TC in the right Cg1 region of the ACC, which was not visible in TI relative to TN. This most likely indicates a conflict monitoring function in analogy to findings in human studies implicating this region as the main conflict monitoring area (Botvinick et al., 1999, 2004; Peterson et al., 2002; Kerns et al., 2004; Kerns, 2006). Furthermore, we found a significant decrease in FDG uptake in the right posterior motor cortex (pM1), suggesting that automatic route suppression may occur during the last stage of audiomotor integration. This is in line with an event-related potential study in humans, reporting “late” automatic route suppression in the frontolateral motor cortex (Stürmer and Leuthold, 2003). The increase of metabolic activity in the right dorsomedial striatum during TI can as well be interpreted as evidence for conflict resolution via automatic route suppression, because this area is involved in behavioral inhibition (Eagle and Baunez, 2010). Furthermore, a small spot of decreased FDG uptake was observed during TI (compared to both TN and TC) and TR in the dorsocentral striatum. This region receives projections from M2 and posterior parietal cortex (PPC; Cheatwood et al., 2005), and the fact that metabolic activity was also found to be lower in the ipsilateral M2 during TI, may therefore indicate blocking of the motor basal ganglia loop (Alexander and Crutcher, 1990; Joel and Weiner, 2000) during automatic route suppression.

So far we have not explained activity changes occurring in both TI and TC but not in the other tasks, such as the decrease of activity found in the left aM2 and right aM1. We preferred 100% incompatible (TI) or 100% compatible trials (TC) over a mixed design, because metabolic patterns should be determined by one type of trials only. However, one could argue that in the homogeneous tasks TC and TI it could be possible for the animal to ignore the relevant stimulus dimension (pitch) and choose the correct response side with the help of the irrelevant stimulus dimension (side). In TC, the animal has to respond always toward the stimulus side, and in TI always toward the opposite side. Because the rats have never encountered homogeneous tasks before, this new stimulus side – response side association, if established at all, must have been developed during the PET session itself. Especially in the purely incompatible TI task, we would then expect RTs to decrease continuously during the session, if the new rule based on stimulus side is easier for the animal than that based on pitch information. Indeed, we found a steady decrease of RTs during the TI session. If the nature of audiomotor integration changes during the task, metabolic activity is supposed to be decreased in pathways related to the pitch-based intentional processing route compared to the neutral task TN, where animals have to rely solely on pitch information. In such a scenario, the decrease of activity found in the left aM2 and right aM1 may therefore be interpreted as linked to learning a new rule rather than caused by automatic route suppression. Although the correlative analysis (see below) supports the role of aM2 (but not aM1) in conflict processing, further studies should rather use a high conflict control with 10% compatible trials. The conflict level would still be high enough to determine metabolic patterns, but the rats will have to use the tone pitch – response side association throughout the whole task. Interpreting metabolic patterns as associated with learning a new association would then be ruled out completely.

Correlation of Imaging and Behavior

Conflict processing may not always be accompanied by profound changes of neuronal activity, which alter the average glucose metabolism by more than 5%. Instead, activity changes may differ between animals, due to individual variations in conflict processing capacity, or may remain below our detection threshold. Thus, a correlative approach might provide further insights: Metabolic activity of a brain area involved in conflict processing may be correlated with behavioral parameters during TI and/or TR, but not during TC and TN. Consistent with this account, the activity of the left aM2 was inversely correlated to RTs during TR, indicating that fast animals had high premotor activities. Furthermore, these findings correspond well with data from humans suggesting that the premotor cortex is involved in conflict resolution (Egner et al., 2007).

Synopsis

So far we have discussed activation patterns emerging in the Simon task versus resting state and comparisons between different variants of the Simon task independently from each other. But how can we interpret the finding that an area of the DMN, the prelimbic cortex, shows reduced metabolic activity during the Simon task with 100% conflict trials when compared to resting state, but increased activity when compared to the no-conflict Simon task control with 100% neutral trials (i.e., metabolic activity of prelimbic cortex: RS > TI > TN)? Reduction of resting activity in the DMN during cognitive tasks is currently interpreted as attenuation of the brain’s self-referential (excitatory) activity as a means of more effectively focusing on a task (Sheline et al., 2009). Our finding can therefore be explained in three different ways: (1) Metabolic activity changes comprise mainly alterations of excitatory input. DMN activity during TN is therefore more strongly attenuated than during TI. (2) DMN activity is attenuated likewise during TI and TN, but the prelimbic cortex is engaged in conflict processing during TI, leading to a higher net activity. (3) Metabolic activity changes reflect also inhibitory input activity. The prelimbic cortex may receive strong inhibitory input during TI, but not during TN, which would lead to an even more effective attenuation of resting activity during TI. Further studies are needed to decide which alternative may account for the observed activation patterns.

Our results demonstrate that spatial response conflicts occur in rats just as in humans. Our imaging results show remarkable similarities to the pattern of activated regions reported during conflict processing in human fMRI studies. The rat motor cortex (pM1) may be part of the automatic route or involved in its facilitation, while premotor (aM2), prelimbic, and ACC may play a role in conflict resolution and/or monitoring. Moreover, conflict-induced automatic route suppression presumably occurs in M2 and M1 as well as in the dorsocentral striatum, i.e., on the motor side of audiomotor integration. Our data shows interesting insights in cross species comparison of conflict processing, and opens novel opportunities to investigate the anatomical basis of conflict processing in a rodent model.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Alexander, G. E., and Crutcher, M. D. (1990). Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci. 13, 266–271.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arndt, S., Cizadlo, T., O’Leary, D., Gold, S., and Andreasen, N. C. (1996). Normalizing counts and cerebral blood flow intensity in functional imaging studies of the human brain. Neuroimage 3, 175–184.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature 402, 179–181.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 8, 539–546.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cheatwood, J. L., Corwin, J. V., and Reep, R. L. (2005). Overlap and interdigitation of cortical and thalamic afferents to dorsocentral striatum in the rat. Brain Res. 1036, 90–100.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chudasama, Y., Passetti, F., Rhodes, S. E. V., Lopian, D., Desai, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2003). Dissociable aspects of performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following lesions of the dorsal anterior cingulate, infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat: differential effects on selectivity, impulsivity and compulsivity. Behav. Brain Res. 146, 105–119.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Courtière, A., Hardouin, J., Burle, B., Vidal, F., and Hasbroucq, T. (2007). Simon effect in the rat: a new model for studying the neural bases of the dual-route architecture. Behav. Brain Res. 179, 69–75.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., and Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 731–750.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Wit, S., Kosaki, Y., Balleine, B. W., and Dickinson, A. (2006). Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex resolves response conflict in rats. J. Neurosci. 26, 5224–5229.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eagle, D. M., and Baunez, C. (2010). Is there an inhibitory-response-control system in the rat? Evidence from anatomical and pharmacological studies of behavioral inhibition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 50–72.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Egner, T., Delano, M., and Hirsch, J. (2007). Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain. Neuroimage 35, 940–948.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Endepols, H., Sommer, S., Backes, H., Wiedermann, D., Graf, R., and Hauber, W. (2010). Effort-based decision making in the rat: an [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose micro positron emission tomography study. J. Neurosci. 30, 9708–9714.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Forstmann, B. U., van den Wildenberg, W. P., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2008). Neural mechanisms, temporal dynamics, and individual differences in interference control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1854–1865.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Genovese, C. R., Lazar, N. A., and Nichols, T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15, 870–878.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., and Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 253–258.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haddon, J. E., and Killcross, S. (2006). Prefrontal cortex lesions disrupt the contextual control of response conflict. J. Neurosci. 26, 2933–2940.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., and Vu, K. P. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychol. Res. 68, 1–17.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Iani, C., Rubichi, S., Gherri, E., and Nicoletti, R. (2009). Co-occurrence of sequential and practice effects in the Simon task: evidence for two independent mechanisms affecting response selection. Mem. Cognit. 37, 358–367.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jang, D. P., Lee, S. H., Lee, S. Y., Park, C. W., Cho, Z. H., and Kim, Y. B. (2009). Neural responses of rats in the forced swimming test: A [F-18]FDG micro PET study. Behav. Brain Res. 203, 43–47.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Joel, D., and Weiner, I. (2000). The connections of the dopaminergic system with the striatum in rats and primates: an analysis with respect to the functional and compartmental organization of the striatum. Neuroscience 96, 451–474.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kerns, J. G. (2006). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an FMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task. Neuroimage 33, 399–405.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303, 1023–1026.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lu, H., Zou, Q., Rea, W., Stein, E. A., and Yang, Y. (2011). Rat brain possesses a default mode network. Proc. Int. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. 19, 3684.

Melara, R. D., Wang, H. J., Vu, K. P. L., and Proctor, R. W. (2008). Attentional origins of the Simon effect: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Brain. Res. 1215, 147–159.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nichols, T., and Hayasaka, S. (2003). Controlling the familywise error rate in functional neuroimaging: a comparative review. Stat. Methods Med. Res. 12, 419–446.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paxinos, G., and Watson, C. (2005). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates: The New Coronal Set, 5th Edn. San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press, 209.

Peterson, B. S., Kane, M. J., Alexander, G. M., Lacadie, C., Skudlarski, P., Leung, H. C., May, J., and Gore, J. C. (2002). An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 427–440.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Qi, J., Leahy, R. M., Cherry, S. R., Chatziioannou, A., and Farquhar, T. H. (1998). High-resolution 3D Bayesian image reconstruction using the microPET small-animal scanner. Phys. Med. Biol. 43, 1001–1013.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raichle, M. E., and Mintun, M. A. (2006). Brain work and brain imaging. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 449–476.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ritter, P., and Villringer, A. (2002). Inhibition and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Int. Congr. 1235, 213–222.

CrossRef Full Text

Robbins, T. W., Muir, J. L., Killcross, A. S., and Pretsell, D. (1993). “Methods for assessing attention and stimulus control in the rat,” in Behavioural Neuroscience: A Practical Approach, ed. A. Sahgal (Oxford: IRL Press).

Rocke, D. M., Goldberg, Z., Schweitert, C., and Santana, A. (2005). A method for detection of differential gene expression in the presence of inter-individual variability in response. Bioinformatics 21, 3990–3992.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Price, J. L., Rundle, M. M., Vaishnavi, S. N., Snyder, A. Z., Mintun, M. A., Wang, S., Coalson, R. S., and Raichle, M. E. (2009). The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1942–1947.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simon, J. R., Hinrichs, J. V., and Craft, J. L. (1970). Auditory S-R compatibility: reaction time as a function of ear–hand correspondence, and ear-response location correspondence. J. Exp. Psychol. 86, 197–202.

CrossRef Full Text

Stürmer, B., and Leuthold, H. (2003). Control over response priming in visuomotor processing: a lateralized event-related potential study. Exp. Brain Res. 153, 35–44.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schroter, H., and Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 28, 1345–1363.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sung, K. K., Jang, D. P., Lee, S., Kim, M., Lee, S. Y., Kim, Y. B., Park, C. W., and Cho, Z. H. (2009). Neural responses in rat brain during acute immobilization stress: a F-18 FDG micro PET imaging study. Neuroimage 44, 1074–1080.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Swanson, L. (2003). Brain Maps: Structure of the Rat Brain. 3rd edn. San Diego: Academic Press.

Vollmar, S., Hampl, J. A., Kracht, L., and Herholz, K. (2007). “Integration of functional data (PET) into brain surgery planning and neuronavigation,” in Advances in Medical Engineering, eds T. M. Buzug, D. Holz, J. Bongartz, M. KohlBareis, and U. Hartmann (Berlin: Springer-Verlag), 98–103.

CrossRef Full Text

Wienhard, K. (2002). Measurement of glucose consumption using [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose. Methods 27, 218–225.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhang, H. H., Zhang, J., and Kornblum, S. (1999). A parallel distributed processing model of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response compatibility. Cogn. Psychol. 38, 386–432.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: prefrontal cortex, rodent model, cognitive conflict, Simon task

Citation: Marx C, Lex B, Calaminus C, Hauber W, Backes H, Neumaier B, Mies G, Graf R and Endepols H (2012) Conflict processing in the rat brain: behavioral analysis and functional μPET imaging using [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 6:4. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00004

Received: 12 October 2011; Accepted: 23 January 2012;
Published online: 09 February 2012.

Edited by:

Jeff Dalley, University of Cambridge, UK

Reviewed by:

Irini Skaliora, Biomedical Research Foundation of the Academy of Athens, Greece
Paul Cumming, Ludwig-Maximilians University of Munich, Germany
Daniele Caprioli, University of Cambridge, UK

Copyright: © 2012 Marx, Lex, Calaminus, Hauber, Backes, Neumaier, Mies, Graf and Endepols. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial License, which permits non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited.

*Correspondence: Christine Marx, Max Planck Institute for Neurological Research, Gleueler Street 50, 50931 Cologne, Germany. e-mail: christine.marx@nf.mpg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.