Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Cell. Neurosci., 31 July 2025

Sec. Cellular Neurophysiology

Volume 19 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2025.1629492

This article is part of the Research TopicMemory processing in health and disease: linking behavioral, circuits, and molecular scales.View all articles

Memory reconsolidation impairment by amyloid beta (1–42) and its prevention by non-competitive antagonists of NMDA receptors

  • 1Institute for Advanced Brain Studies, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia
  • 2Department of Biology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia

In a healthy brain, the reactivation of memories under conditions of novelty leads to their labilization and subsequent reconsolidation. However, if plasticity of the nervous system is reduced reconsolidation mechanisms may be disrupted, leading to weakening and loss of existing memory. We hypothesize that such self-degradation of old memory due to its reactivation in the compromised brain may lead to progressive memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease. Preventing memory lability when accessing it, may slow down such engram degradation. To test these hypotheses, we first examined whether beta-amyloid peptide Aβ1–42 can impair reconsolidation of memory in one-trial passive avoidance task in young chicks. Next, we examined the possibility to prevent such reminder-associated amnesia by administering a non-competitive N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist MK-801 prior to memory reactivation. Finally, we compared the memory protecting effects of two non-competitive NMDA antagonists, MK-801 and memantine which is a clinically used medication for treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. We found that administration of Aβ1–42 prior to memory reactivation in passive avoidance task in chicks impaired its subsequent reconsolidation. Concurrent systemic injection of MK-801 or memantine prevented this impairment. Our data thus support the hypothesis about the possible role of impaired reconsolidation in the progressive deterioration of old memories in neurodegenerative diseases, particularly in Alzheimer’s disease. This hypothesis offers a new explanation for the protective effects of memantine and suggests the possibility of similar effects with other NMDA receptor antagonists.

1 Introduction

Animal models of Alzheimer’s disease primarily study impairments in acquiring new experiences and consolidation of acquired memories (Puzzo et al., 2014; Webster et al., 2014). In this study, we test the hypothesis that β-amyloid (Aβ1–42) may affect memory not only by impairing memory consolidation, but also by interfering with its reconsolidation, a process that occurs when a previously consolidated memory is reactivated and becomes temporarily labile (Dudai and Eisenberg, 2004; Dudai, 2006; Nader, 2015; Bellfy and Kwapis, 2020). This hypothesis has important implications for the brain pathology: if memory reconsolidation is vulnerable to Aβ pathology, this may result in gradual degradation of already formed memories in Alzheimer’s disease. If so, preventing the transition of memory into a labile form during its retrieval can be used to slow down its self-degradation in the brain which has diminished reconsolidation mechanisms. This hypothesis can be directly tested in animals with amyloid pathology by pharmacologically preventing memory destabilization during its reactivation.

Pharmacological regulation of memory reconsolidation has been well-studied using learning models in different animal species (Przybyslawski and Sara, 1997; Kida et al., 2002; Kelly et al., 2003; Koh and Bernstein, 2003; Ben Mamou et al., 2006; Crowe et al., 2008). Memory reactivated by a reminder along with blockade of protein synthesis is unable to reconsolidate, resulting in reminder-associated amnesia (Nader et al., 2000; Anokhin et al., 2002; Lopez et al., 2015; Bonin and De Koninck, 2015; Bellfy and Kwapis, 2020). Such amnesia can be prevented by antagonists of NMDA receptors or their subunits injected before memory reactivation (Ben Mamou et al., 2006; Balaban et al., 2014; Bal et al., 2017; Nikitin et al., 2021; Rossato et al., 2023). We recently showed that in young chicks trained in a passive avoidance model, amnesia induced by memory reactivation paired with protein synthesis blockade can be prevented by the non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 (Tiunova et al., 2024).

An important advantage of the passive avoidance learning model in chicks is that it allows for precise timing of training, reactivation, and testing of memory. This temporal resolution is critical for studying phase-specific pharmacological effects on consolidation or reconsolidation. The neural mechanisms underlying memory consolidation and reconsolidation in this model have been extensively studied (Rose, 2000; Crowe et al., 2008). In addition, this model has been proposed as a valuable tool for Alzheimer’s disease research due to the close homology between the amyloid precursor protein (APP) of chick and humans. In chicks, APP plays a critical role in memory consolidation, and disruption of its synthesis results in amnesia (Mileusnic and Rose, 2010).

A single administration of β-amyloid peptides (Aβ1–42 or Aβ12–28) has previously been used as a model of amyloid pathology in chicks (Mileusnic et al., 2004, 2005; Gibbs et al., 2010; Gibbs, 2015). Administration of Aβ fragments from 24 h before to 15 min after training in the passive avoidance model impaired recall when tested from 35 min to 24 h after training (Gibbs et al., 2010). However, the effects of β-amyloid fragments on memory reconsolidation in chicks has not been previously studied. Accordingly, the first objective of this study was to investigate the effect of β-amyloid on memory reconsolidation. In the second stage, we tested the possibility of preventing reminder-associated retrograde amnesia by administering the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 prior to memory reactivation. Finally, we compared the effects of MK-801 and memantine, a drug that is also a non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist and is used clinically to treat Alzheimer’s disease (Peng et al., 2023; Tang et al., 2023; Karimi Tari et al., 2024).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Subjects

The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council of the European Union issued 22 September 2010, on the protection of animals, used for scientific purposes (Section 27). The protocol was approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the Lomonosov Moscow State University.

Domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus, Panzirevskaya Black strain) of both sexes were delivered from the Research and Technological Poultry Institute, Moscow Region, on the next day after hatching. The chicks were placed in pairs in metal pens (20 × 25 × 20 cm) with access to food and water and allowed to acclimatize overnight. The experimental room was maintained at 30°C and 12:12 h dark/light cycle. The chicks were taken in the experiments on the following morning, i.e., at the age of 2 days.

In total, 906 chicks were used for the experiments and 823 of them were used in the data analysis. The withdrawal (9.2%) was applied to the chicks that did not peck in the pre-training or training trials and to those which pecked at the aversive bead during the reminder session (see below). Each chick was used only once for training, reminder, and testing.

All behavioral procedures were carried out by a researcher blind to the injected solutions and to which experimental group each chick belonged.

2.2 Passive avoidance learning

Passive avoidance learning is based on the innate predisposition of young chicks to try and peck at novel small objects. To stimulate their pecking activity, the chicks were first pre-trained with two 10 s presentations of a 3 mm dry metal bead on a rod, with 5 min interval between the presentations. Only chicks that pecked at the bead (normally over 90% of the total) were included in the experiment. Twenty minutes after the second pre-training, the chicks underwent training with a 2 mm white plastic bead on a rod coated with a bitter substance, methyl anthranilate (Sigma). After pecking at the bead, the chicks exhibited a species-specific disgust reaction (head shaking, beak wiping, distress calls) and afterward avoided pecking an identical but dry bead during subsequent presentations.

2.3 Reminder procedure

Two hours post-training, the chicks were presented for 10 s with a dry white bead identical to the training bead. Normally, the chicks did not peck at the bead and displayed typical noticeable avoidance behavior, such as backing away and distress calls (> 80% of the total). This procedure served as reminder that reactivated the memory formed after the training (Anokhin et al., 2002). The response of pecking or avoiding the dry bead was recorded and those that pecked were excluded from further experiments.

2.4 Testing

The retention test was given either 4 or 24 h after the training. The testing procedure was identical to the reminder, involving presentation of the same dry white bead. Responses (peck or avoid) were recorded, and a percentage avoidance score was calculated for each experimental group as a proportion of avoiding animals (Tiunova et al., 2024).

To exclude the possibility of a generalized avoidance behavior, 20 min after testing memory for an aversive bead, a neutral bead of a different color was presented. Only the chicks that peck at it and thus distinguished an aversive bead from a neutral one, were included in the analysis.

Avoidance levels between groups of chicks were compared using the two-sided prop.test in R statistics and post hoc χ2 test of independence. Differences were considered significant at P < 0.05.

2.5 Drugs and injections

Amyloid: β-amyloid (Aβ1–42, Bachem) was dissolved in DMSO (1 mg/100 μl) and stored at −80°C in aliquots. Immediately before use the aliquots were diluted with physiological saline to the final concentration of Aβ1–42 1 μg/μl. The solution was used within 30 min after preparation to avoid aggregation. The control peptide, scrambled Aβ1–42 (Bachem), was prepared in the same way.

Bilateral intracranial injections of Aβ1–42 and scrambled Aβ1–42 were performed 45 min prior to training or to reminder using a 10 μl Hamilton syringe and a specialized headholder to target the lateral ventricles and adjacent brain areas (Davis et al., 1979). In the dose-dependency experiment each chick of the experimental groups received 0.5–4 μg of the peptide in the volume of 2 μl/hemisphere. Chicks from the control groups received 4 μg of scrambled peptide or physiological saline in the same volume.

MK-801: MK-801 [(+)-MK-801 hydrogen maleate, Sigma] was administered intraperitoneally at a dose of 0.25 mg/kg in 0.1 ml saline, 30 min before the reminder.

Memantine: Memantine hydrochloride (Sigma) was administered intraperitoneally at a dose of 0.73 mg/kg in 0.1 ml saline, 30 min before the reminder.

In chicks, 0.1 ml of 0.1 mM memantine solution was shown to improve memory in weak learning and reactivation of weak memory (Samartgis et al., 2012). A 10-fold higher dose (0.1 ml of 1 mM solution) prevented scopolamine-induced amnesia (Barber and Haggarty, 2010) and enhanced memory in observational learning (Barber and Kimbrough, 2015). Memory was also improved in isolation-induced stress by 0.1 ml of 5 mM solution (Barber et al., 2010). Based on these data, we chose a 0.1 ml of 1 mM memantine (0.73 mg/kg) to test the possibility of preventing reminder-associated memory deficit modeled by Aβ1–42.

2.6 Experimental groups

In each experiment with memory reactivation there was a control group that received no injections and no reminder (Control). The other controls included groups receiving memory reactivation along with physiological saline or scrambled peptide injection, and groups with β-amyloid injection without the reminder. In the experimental groups the memory was reactivated along with administration of Aβ1–42, MK-801, memantine, or their combinations.

3 Results

3.1 Aβ1–42 impairs memory consolidation

In the first experiment, we tested the effect of a single administration of β-amyloid on memory during its formation (consolidation). β-amyloid Aβ1–42 was administered 45 min before passive avoidance training in doses ranging from 0.5 to 4 μg. Control groups were administered with scrambled peptide (Scrmb, 4 μg) or saline. Testing 24 h later showed that Aβ1–42 at a dose of 4 μg significantly reduced the level of avoidance (Figure 1A, group Aβ/4μg, χ2 = 10.1, P < 0.001 compared with the Saline group; χ2 = 11.91, P < 0.001 compared with the Scrmb group). These results are consistent with data previously obtained in the same learning model (Mileusnic et al., 2005).

FIGURE 1
Flowchart and bar graphs depict a behavioral study on chicks. The flowchart outlines experiment phases: pretraining, training, and testing using saline, Ab, and scrambled sequences. Bar graph A shows the percentage of chicks avoiding in test 24h after the training with higher avoidance at Ab 0.5 and lower at Ab 4. Bar graph B displays the percentage of chicks avoiding in test 4h or 24 h after the training. Numerals represent sample sizes, and asterisks denote statistical significance.

Figure 1. Administration of β-amyloid (1–42) prior to training impairs memory retention. Data are shown as the percentage of chicks showing avoidance. Numbers of chicks in each group are shown in the bars. (A) Experimental groups: Saline, pre-training saline injection; Aβ, pre-training injection of Aβ1–42 (0.5, 1 or 4 μg); Scrmb, pre-training injection of scrambled peptide (4 μg). Avoidance levels in the retention test 24 h after the training. Injections 45 min pre-training. **P < 0.01 compared to Saline; +++P < 0.001compared to Scrmb group. (B) Experimental groups: Saline, pre-training saline injection; Aβ_4 h, pre-training injection of Aβ1–42 (4 μg), test 4 h after the training; Aβ_24 h, pre-training injection of Aβ1–42 (4 μg), test 24 h after the training; Scrmb_24 h, pre-training injection of scrambled peptide (4 μg), test 24 h after the training. ***P < 0.001 compared to Saline; +++P < 0.001 compared to Scrmb_24 h group.

In all subsequent experiments, β-amyloid was administered at a dose of 4 μg. Since the main objective of our work was to investigate the effects of β-amyloid on reactivated memory, we examined whether the effect of Aβ1–42 administration would be evident when tested 4 h after training. We found that the avoidance level in the Aβ1–42 group was significantly reduced compared with the saline and scrambled peptide groups and was not different from the avoidance behavior in the Aβ group tested 24 h later (Figure 1B, χ2 = 51.06, P < 0.001; groups Aβ_4 h and Aβ_24 h: P < 0.001 compared with the Saline group and the Scrmb _24 h group). These results are consistent with those previously reported in a passive avoidance model (Gibbs et al., 2010). As shown in this study, administration of Aβ1–42 45 min before training impaired memory when tested as early as 35 min after training, and the memory deficit persisted for at least 24 h.

3.2 Aβ1–42 impairs memory reconsolidation

β-Amyloid was administered 45 min before memory reactivation by a reminder, i.e., 75 min after training. The Control (no memory reactivation and no injections) and Reminder (Sal/Rem, memory reactivation with saline) groups were used as controls. To exclude the effect of Aβ1–42 itself, without association with memory reactivation, an additional control group was administered Aβ1–42 75 min after training, but no reminder was presented (Aβ/NoRem group). To test the specificity of the Aβ1–42 peptide effects, another group received scrambled peptide (Scrmb) 45 min before memory reactivation. An additional group of animals was administered scrambled peptide 75 min after training and no reminder was presented.

Administration of Aβ1–42 45 min before memory reactivation by the reminder resulted in a decrease in the level of avoidance when tested 4 h after training (Figure 2A, Aβ/Rem group: P < 0.001 compared with Control, χ2 = 8.96, and Sal/Rem, χ2 = 8.7, groups). Administration of Aβ1–42 without reminder did not affect memory (Figure 2A, χ2 = 11.19, P < 0.001 between Aβ/Rem and Aβ/NoRem groups). Administration of scrambled peptide either with or without reactivation did not affect the level of recall when tested (Figure 2A, Scrmb/Rem and Scrmb/NoRem groups).

FIGURE 2
Flowchart and bar graphs depicting a chick aversion study. The top flowchart shows six experimental conditions involving pretraining, training, and testing phases with interventions such as Sal, Ab, and Scrmb, with or without reminders. Bar graphs A and B illustrate the percentage of chicks avoiding a stimulus across these conditions, with different significance levels indicated by asterisks. Each bar graph includes sample sizes at the bottom.

Figure 2. Administration of β-amyloid (1–42) prior to reminder impairs memory retention. Data are shown as the percentage of chicks showing avoidance. Numbers of chicks in each group are shown in the bars. (A) Experimental groups: Control, no reminder, no injections; Sal/Rem, reminder coupled with vehicle injection; Aβ/NoRem, injection of Aβ1–42, no reminder; Aβ/Rem, injection of Aβ1–42 prior to reminder; Scrmb/NoRem, injection of scrambled peptide, no reminder; Scrmb/Rem, injection of scrambled peptide prior to reminder. Avoidance levels in the retention test 4 h after the training. Injections 45 min pre-reminder. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001 compared to Aβ/Rem group. (B) Experimental groups: Control, no reminder, no injections; Sal/Rem, reminder coupled with vehicle injection; Aβ/NoRem, injection of Aβ1–42, no reminder; Aβ/Rem, injection of Aβ1–42 prior to reminder; Scrmb/NoRem, injection of scrambled peptide, no reminder; Scrmb/Rem, injection of scrambled peptide prior to reminder. Avoidance levels in the retention test 24 h after the training. Injections 45 min pre-reminder. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001 compared to Aβ/Rem group.

Thus, our results show that memory reactivation combined with administration of β-amyloid results in memory impairment, suggesting that Aβ is capable of not only disrupting memory consolidation but also damaging previously acquired memory.

In the next experiment, we tested whether the amnestic effect of β-amyloid persisted when tested 24 h after training. The same groups were used in the experiment as in the previous one. Testing after 24 h showed that the level of avoidance in the group receiving a reminder in the presence of β-amyloid was significantly reduced compared to the control groups that did not receive a reminder or received a reminder in the presence of saline or scrambled peptide (Figure 2B, χ2 = 29.23, P < 0.001).

3.3 MK-801 prevents amnesia produced by pre-reminder administration of β-amyloid

We recently found that administration of a non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 prevents the reminder-associated amnestic effect of protein synthesis inhibitors on reactivated memory in chicks trained in passive avoidance task (Tiunova et al., 2024).

Therefore, in the next experiment, we tested whether MK-801 could prevent the retention deficit produced by administration of Aβ1–42 coupled with memory reactivation.

As in the previous experiments, β-Amyloid was administered 45 min before reminder-induced memory reactivation. MK-801 was administered intraperitoneally at a dose of 0.25 mg/kg in 0.1 ml saline, 30 min before the reminder (Tiunova et al., 2024).

Testing 4 h after training showed that the administration of Aβ1–42 in combination with memory reactivation impaired retrieval (Fig. 3, group Aβ/Rem, χ2 = 14.53, P < 0.001 compared to the Control group). At the same time, the administration of Aβ1–42 alone, without memory reactivation, did not lead to any notable decrease in the level of avoidance (Figure 3, group Aβ/NoRem). Memory reactivation along with the introduction of a scrambled peptide also had no significant effect on the level of avoidance during testing (Figure 3, group Scrmb/Rem). The administration of MK-801 produced an effect similar to the effect of β-amyloid. In combination with memory reactivation, MK-801 significantly impaired memory (Figure 3, group MK/Rem, χ2 = 13.17, P < 0.001 compared to the Control group). Administration of MK-801 without memory reactivation did not affect the level of avoidance (Figure 3, MK/NoRem group, χ2 = 6.7, P < 0.01 compared with MK/Rem group).

FIGURE 3
A diagram and bar chart illustrate an experimental timeline and data on chick avoidance behavior. The top diagram outlines seven experimental conditions involving pretraining, training, treatments (Ab, Sal, Scrmb, MK), and testing 4 h after training. The bottom bar chart displays percentages of chicks avoiding a stimulus across conditions, with significant differences marked by asterisks and symbols. Each bar graph includes sample sizes at the bottom.

Figure 3. MK-801 prevents reminder-associated amnesia produced by β-amyloid. Experimental groups: Control, no reminder, no injections; Aβ/NoRem, injection of Aβ1–42, no reminder; Aβ/Rem, injection of Aβ1–42 prior to reminder; MK/NoRem, injection of MK-801, no reminder; MK/Rem, injection of MK-801prior reminder; Aβ/MK/Rem, injection of Aβ1–42 and MK-801 prior to reminder; Scrmb/Rem, injection of scrambled peptide prior to reminder. Avoidance levels in the retention test 4 h after the training. Injections of Aβ1–42 and scrambled peptide 45 min pre-reminder, MK-801 30 min pre-reminder. ***P < 0.001 compared to the Control group; +P < 0.05, ++P < 0.01 compared to Aβ/MK/Rem group; ××P < 0.01 compared to MK/NoRem group.

In the group that received a reminder combined with both β-amyloid and MK-801, the level of avoidance was significantly higher than in the groups that received a reminder in combination with β-amyloid alone or MK-801 alone (Figure 3, Aβ/MK/Rem group, P < 0.01 compared with Aβ/Rem group, P < 0.05 compared with MK/Rem group).

Thus, while β-amyloid and MK-801 alone impaired the retrieval of reactivated memory, no memory impairment was observed when both substances were administered.

3.4 Memantine prevents amnesia produced by pre-reminder administration of β-amyloid

To test the ability of memantine to prevent amnesia caused by memory reactivation combined with β-amyloid, memantine was administered 30 min before the reminder. Testing 4 h after training showed that memantine administration alone or paired with memory reactivation had no effect on memory. The level of avoidance in the memantine-treated groups did not differ significantly from the level of avoidance in the control group, which did not receive either a reminder or injections (Figure 4A, Mem/Rem and Mem/NoRem groups).

FIGURE 4
Flowchart and bar graphs showing experimental procedures and results for chick avoidance behavior. Different groups undergo pretraining, training, and test phases with varying intervals and treatments like Ab, Mem, and Sal. Bar Graph A and B display the percentage of chicks avoiding certain stimuli across these groups, with sample sizes indicated at the bottom.

Figure 4. (A) Memantine prevents reminder-associated amnesia produced by β-amyloid. (A) Experimental groups: Control, no reminder, no injections; Aβ/NoRem, injection of Aβ1–42, no reminder; Aβ/Rem, injection of Aβ1–42 prior to reminder; Mem/NoRem, injection of memantine, no reminder; Mem/Rem, injection of memantine prior reminder; Aβ/Mem/Rem, injection of Aβ1–42 and memantine prior to reminder; Scrmb/Rem, injection of scrambled peptide prior to reminder. Avoidance levels in the retention test 4 h after the training. Injections of Aβ1–42 and scrambled peptide 45 min pre-reminder, memantine 30 min pre-reminder. +P < 0.05 compared to Aβ/Mem/Rem group; ×P < 0.05 compared to Aβ/NoRem group. (B) Experimental groups: Control, no reminder, no injections; Aβ/NoRem, injection of Aβ1–42, no reminder; Aβ/Rem, injection of Aβ1–42 prior to reminder; Mem/NoRem, injection of memantine, no reminder; Mem/Rem, injection of memantine prior reminder; Aβ/Mem/Rem, injection of Aβ1–42 and memantine prior to reminder; Scrmb/Rem, injection of scrambled peptide prior to reminder. Avoidance levels in the retention test 24 h after the training. Injections of Aβ1–42 and scrambled peptide 45 min pre-reminder, memantine 30 min pre-reminder. *P < 0.05 compared to the Control group; ×P < 0.05 compared to Aβ/NoRem group; +++P < 0.001 compared to Aβ/Mem/Rem group; ooP < 0.01 compared to Scrmb/Rem group.

As in previous experiments, β-amyloid administration 75 min after training did not affect memory recall during testing, and the level of avoidance in this group did not differ from the control (Figure 4A, Aβ/NoRem group). At the same time, memory reactivation along with Aβ1–42 administration significantly reduced the level of avoidance during testing (Figure 4A, Aβ/Rem group, χ2 = 4.91, P < 0.05 compared with Aβ/NoRem group). Reminder in combination with scrambled peptide did not affect the level of avoidance during testing (Figure 4A, Scrmb/Rem group).

Administration of memantine before memory reactivation paired with β-amyloid prevented the amnestic effect: the level of avoidance in the group receiving the reminder together with both β-amyloid and memantine was significantly higher than in the group receiving the reminder in combination with β-amyloid only, and did not differ from the level of avoidance in the control groups (Figure 4A, Aβ/Mem/Rem group, χ2 = 5.12, P < 0.05 compared with Aβ/Rem group).

The next experiment tested whether the protective effects of memantine would persist when tested 24 h after training. As with testing 4 h after training, administration of β-amyloid in combination with memory reactivation significantly reduced the level of avoidance (Figure 4B, Aβ/Rem group, χ2 = 4.25, P < 0.05 compared with the Control group, χ2 = 10.9, P < 0.01 compared with the Scrmb/Rem group). The level of avoidance after administration of Aβ1–42 without memory reactivation did not differ from the level of avoidance in the Control group and was significantly higher than in the group receiving Aβ1–42 in combination with a reminder (Figure 4B, Aβ/NoRem group, χ2 = 5.04, P < 0.05 compared with the Aβ/Rem group). Memantine administration did not affect memory either by itself or in combination with memory reactivation (Figure 4B, Mem/Rem and Mem/NoRem groups). At the same time, memantine administration prevented memory impairment caused by combination of its reactivation with Aβ administration (Figure 4B, Aβ/Mem/Rem group, χ2 = 13.58, P < 0.001 compared with Aβ/Rem group).

4 Discussion

The present study was based on the hypothesis of self-degradation of already consolidated memory in Alzheimer’s disease due to impairment of its reconsolidation mechanisms in the brain damaged by amyloid pathology. If this hypothesis is correct, then it is possible to slow down the progressive memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease by preventing the destabilization of old memories during their reactivation.

To test these two hypotheses, we first investigated the effect of a single intraventricular injection of β-amyloid Aβ1–42 on reactivated memory in the passive avoidance model in young chicks. The ability of this peptide to disrupt memory consolidation in this model has been already shown (Mileusnic et al., 2004, 2005; Gibbs et al., 2009, 2010; Gibbs, 2015). We also studied the effects of different doses of Aβ 45 min before training, and our results showed that Aβ1–42 at a dose of 4 μg disrupted memory when tested after 4 and 24 h, which is consistent with previously obtained data. This dose and time of administration were used in further experiments to test the effect of β-amyloid on reactivated memory.

We found that administration of Aβ1–42 45 min before the reminder led to a recall deficit in the test 4 h after memory reactivation. Moreover, the effect of β-amyloid on reactivated memory persisted for 24 h. Our data are consistent with findings from mammalian studies on the vulnerability of memory during reconsolidation in the amyloid-compromised brain. In 5XFAD transgenic mice, a model of Alzheimer’s disease, memory reactivation following fear conditioning resulted in a significant impairment of previously consolidated memory compared to both wild-type controls and 5XFAD mice that were not re-exposed to the memory cue (Ohno, 2009). These results indicate that in the presence of amyloid pathology, the reconsolidation process is impaired, resulting in the weakening of previously established memories. Similarly, in rats, injection of the Aβ25–35 peptide during the reactivation of an object recognition memory, impaired its subsequent retrieval (Álvarez-Ruíz and Carrillo-Mora, 2013), providing further evidence that amyloid interferes with the reconsolidation process. APP/PS1 mice with Alzheimer’s disease trained in a water maze at 3 months of age and tested at 7 months showed worse memory than wild-type animals (Rai et al., 2020). However, if they were “overtrained,” i.e., pre-trained at 2 months of age, their performance in the test at 7 months was as good as that of the control group. The authors argue that this improvement involved reconsolidation processes that were impaired in Alzheimer’s disease but could be enhanced by overtraining. These mammalian results are consistent with our data in the chick model, where memory impairment occurs only if Aβ1–42 is administered at the time of memory reactivation, but not without it.

Memory reactivation combined with administration of amnestic agents in some cases leads only to transient amnesia, and over time the memory is spontaneously recovered (Litvin and Anokhin, 2000; Milekic and Alberini, 2002; Lattal and Abel, 2004; Salinska et al., 2004; Power et al., 2006; Prado-Alcalá et al., 2006). Studies in a passive avoidance model in chicks have shown that memory reactivation following administration of antagonists of glutamate NMDA, AMPA, and metabotropic receptors (AP5, CNQX, MPEP), cyclin-dependent kinase 5 (roscovitine), RNA synthesis inhibitors (DRB), and inhibitors of protein synthesis or glycosylation (anisomycin, cycloheximide, 2-deoxygalactose) resulted in memory deficits when tested up to 4 h after reactivation, and in all cases no deficits were observed when tested 24 h later (Litvin and Anokhin, 2000; Anokhin et al., 2002; Summers et al., 2003; Salinska, 2006; Sherry and Crowe, 2008a,b; Sherry et al., 2010). In contrast, the administration of these agents during training leads to development of permanent amnesia. Thus, the effect of antagonists on consolidation of memory differs from their effect on the reconsolidation of reactivated memory. In contrast to these data, administration of amyloid peptide (Aβ1–42) before memory reactivation in our experiments resulted in retrieval deficits even 24 h later, indicating that its effects differ from those of other amnestic agents.

Next, we investigated the possibility of preventing amnesia produced by administration of β-amyloid paired with memory reactivation. We tested whether the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 would be able to prevent amnesia caused by administration of Aβ1–42 during memory reactivation. This possibility was partly suggested by our previous data on the ability of MK-801 to prevent reminder-associated amnesia caused by protein synthesis inhibitors. Indeed, as our experiments showed, administration of MK-801 to chicks that received Aβ1–42 paired with the reminder had a protective effect, preventing memory deficit.

In addition to the amnesic effects MK-801 in doses above 0.1 mg/kg causes hyperlocomotion, stereotypy, ataxia, and anxiolytic/anxiogenic behavior (Janus et al., 2023). However, in chicks MK-801 was administered intraperitoneally without visible side effects at doses up to 0.4 mg/kg (Burchuladze and Rose, 1992; Bullock et al., 1993; Freeman and Rose, 1995; Tiunova et al., 2020). In the present study, we administered MK-801 in a dose of 0.25 mg/kg without noticeable behavioral changes. In our experimental conditions hyperlocomotion and anxiolytic effects, if present, would have resulted in an increase in the number of chicks pecking during the reminder, which was not observed. Ataxia, if present, would have been noticeable as unsuccessful pecking attempts, but we did not observe such impairments. Anxiogenic effects, if present, would have resulted in more distress and fear calls–no such effects were noticed. Our data also show that the reminder procedure influenced subsequent testing (Figure 3, MK/Rem and MK/NoRem groups), thereby confirming that the chicks were able to pay attention to the bead during the reminder procedure.

Normally, the effects of endogenous β-amyloid and NMDA receptors (NMDARs) appear to interact in several ways. On the one hand, Aβ synthesis requires the activity of extra synaptic NMDA receptors containing the NR2B unit, and, in turn, an increase in Aβ concentration enhances their activity (Li and Selkoe, 2020; Babaei, 2021; Cheng et al., 2021). In addition, soluble Aβ oligomers impair glutamate reuptake by neurons and astrocytes, which also increases the activity of extrasynaptic NMDA receptors (Li and Selkoe, 2020; Li and Stern, 2022). On the other hand, soluble Aβ forms suppress the functions of synaptic NMDA receptors and enhance their internalization, thereby disrupting the balance of the glutamatergic network (Babaei, 2021; Zhang et al., 2022). Hyperactivation of extrasynaptic NMDA receptors leads to excessive Ca2+ entry, which triggers a chain of toxic reactions leading to cell membrane destruction, disruption of synaptic transmission, and cell death. The ability to prevent this chain is usually attributed to the action of the NMDA receptor antagonist memantine, used in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease (Karimi Tari et al., 2024; Nystuen et al., 2024).

However, our work was based on a different hypothesis of the protective effect of NMDAR antagonists on reactivated memory, namely their ability to prevent engram labilization. This hypothesis is supported by the literature (Ben Mamou et al., 2006; Balaban et al., 2014; Bal et al., 2017; Nikitin et al., 2021; Rossato et al., 2023) and our previous results (Tiunova et al., 2024) showing that NMDAR antagonists prevent reminder-associated amnesia regardless of the agent that caused the impairment of reactivated memory. Accordingly, we tested the ability of memantine to prevent amnesia induced by memory reactivation in the presence of Aβ1–42.

Systemic administration of memantine 30 min before a reminder prevented the amnesia induced by β-amyloid associated with memory reactivation when tested 4 or 24 h after training. The effect of memantine was comparable to that of MK-801, except that memantine itself, unlike MK-801, did not impair reactivated memory. A possible explanation could be that memantine has lower affinity to NMDAR compared to MK-801 (Cheng et al., 2021). In addition, the effect of memantine on NMDARs demonstrates an order of magnitude faster kinetics than the effect of MK-801 (Rogawski and Wenk, 2003). Additionally, although we could not directly compare the doses of these two antagonists on NMDARs, the doses of memantine we used were significantly lower than those used in clinical settings (Danysz and Parsons, 2003; Parsons et al., 2007; Karimi Tari et al., 2024).

In addition to NMDA receptor antagonist properties, memantine has been shown to enhance cholinergic signaling (Tang et al., 2023). This ability may also contribute to its protective effect on memory by preventing acetylcholine deficiency - one of the main neuropathological features of Alzheimer’s disease. In rats, memantine prevented the loss of cholinergic innervation to the neocortex induced by Aβ1–42 (Nyakas et al., 2011). It also reverses scopolamine-induced learning deficits in mice, chicks, and rats (Drever et al., 2007; Barber and Haggarty, 2010; Bali et al., 2019). Since cholinergic transmission is involved in passive avoidance learning in chicks (Rose et al., 1980; Zhao et al., 1997; Mezey et al., 1999), the effects of memantine on the cholinergic system may also have contributed to memory protection in our experiments.

Thus, our results demonstrate a reconsolidation-related mechanism of Aβ-induced amnesia. The fact that memory impairment occurred only when Aβ was administered before the reminder, but not without such reactivation, strongly suggests that Aβ targets the destabilization phase that precedes memory reconsolidation. This is consistent with the notion that memories become transiently unstable after reactivation and require NMDA-dependent restabilization, a process vulnerable to pathological interference. In contrast to consolidation deficits that affect newly formed memories, our data indicate that even well-established memories can be disrupted by reconsolidation impairment in Aβ-compromised brains.

Overall, our data are consistent with the hypothesis that impaired reconsolidation may play a role in the weakening of old consolidated memory in neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease. In addition to providing another explanation for the therapeutic effect of memantine, this hypothesis also suggests the possibility of a similar effect of other NMDA receptor antagonists, including those used or tested clinically for other purposes (e.g., riluzole, amantadine, neramexane). Testing the possibility of protecting old memory from reactivation-associated impairment with these and other NMDA receptor antagonists may open up a promising new approach for the treatment of neurodegenerative diseases.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The animal study was approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the Lomonosov Moscow State University. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

AT: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Formal Analysis, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing. ED: Investigation, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing. KA: Conceptualization, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review and editing, Funding acquisition, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The study was supported by the Russian Science Foundation Grant No. 20-15-00283.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Álvarez-Ruíz, Y., and Carrillo-Mora, P. (2013). Amyloid beta 25-35 impairs reconsolidation of object recognition memory in rats and this effect is prevented by lithium carbonate. Neurosci. Lett. 548, 79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anokhin, K. V., Tiunova, A. A., and Rose, S. P. R. (2002). Reminder effects - reconsolidation or retrieval deficit? Pharmacological dissection with protein synthesis inhibitors following reminder for a passive-avoidance task in young chicks. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1759–1765. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02023.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Babaei, P. (2021). NMDA and AMPA receptors dysregulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 908:174310. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2021.174310

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bal, N. V., Rysakova, M. P., Vinarskaya, A. K., Ivanova, V., Zuzina, A. B., and Balaban, P. M. (2017). Cued memory reconsolidation in rats requires nitric oxide. Eur. J. Neurosci. 45, 643–647. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13503

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balaban, P. M., Roshchin, M., Timoshenko, A. K., Gainutdinov, K. L., Bogodvid, T. K., Muranova, L. N., et al. (2014). Nitric oxide is necessary for labilization of a consolidated context memory during reconsolidation in terrestrial snails. Eur. J Neurosci. 40, 2963–2970. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12642

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bali, Z. K., Bruszt, N., Tadepalli, S. A., Csurgyók, R., Nagy, L. V., Tompa, M., et al. (2019). Cognitive enhancer effects of low memantine doses are facilitated by an alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonist in scopolamine-induced amnesia in rats. Front. Pharmacol. 10:73. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.00073

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, T. A., and Haggarty, M. K. (2010). Memantine ameliorates scopolamine-induced amnesia in chicks trained on taste-avoidance learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 93, 540–545. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.02.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, T. A., and Kimbrough, T. N. (2015). Memantine improves observational learning in day-old chicks. Behav. Pharmacol. 26, 407–410. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000130

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, T. A., Meyers, R. A., and McGettigan, B. F. (2010). Memantine improves memory for taste-avoidance learning in day-old chicks exposed to isolation stress. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 95, 203–208. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellfy, L., and Kwapis, J. L. (2020). Molecular mechanisms of reconsolidation-dependent memory updating. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 21:6580. doi: 10.3390/ijms21186580

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben Mamou, C., Gamache, K., and Nader, K. (2006). NMDA receptors are critical for unleashing consolidated auditory fear memories. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1237–1239. doi: 10.1038/nn1778

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonin, R. P., and De Koninck, Y. (2015). Reconsolidation and the regulation of plasticity: Moving beyond memory. Trends Neurosci. 38, 336–344. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.04.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullock, S., Rose, S. P., Pearce, B., and Potter, J. (1993). Training chicks on a passive avoidance task modulates glutamate-stimulated inositol phosphate accumulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 5, 43–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1993.tb00203.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burchuladze, A. A., and Rose, S. P. R. (1992). Memory formation in day-old chicks requires NMDA but not non-NMDA glutamate receptors. Eur. J. Neurosci. 4, 533–538. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1992.tb00903.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, Y. J., Lin, C. H., and Lane, H. Y. (2021). Involvement of cholinergic, adrenergic, and glutamatergic network modulation with cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 22:2283. doi: 10.3390/ijms22052283

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowe, S. F., Sherry, J. M., and Hale, M. W. (2008). Remembering that things have changed: A review of the cellular mechanisms of memory re-consolidation in the day-old chick. Brain Res. Bull. 76, 192–197. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.02.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Danysz, W., and Parsons, C. G. (2003). The NMDA receptor antagonist memantine as a symptomatological and neuroprotective treatment for Alzheimer’s disease: Preclinical evidence. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, S23–S32. doi: 10.1002/gps.938

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, J. L., Masouka, D. T., Gerbrandt, D. T., and Cherkin, A. (1979). Autoradiographic distribution of L-proline in chick after intracerebral injection. Physiol. Behav. 22, 177–184. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(79)90233-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Drever, B. D., Anderson, W. G., Johnson, H., O’Callaghan, M., Seo, S., Choi, D. Y., et al. (2007). Memantine acts as a cholinergic stimulant in the mouse hippocampus. J. Alzheimers Dis. 12, 319–333. doi: 10.3233/jad-2007-12405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudai, Y. (2006). Reconsolidation: The advantage of being refocused. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 174–178. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudai, Y., and Eisenberg, M. (2004). Rites of passage of the engram: Reconsolidation and the lingering consolidation hypothesis. Neuron 44, 93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, F. M., and Rose, S. P. (1995). MK-801 blockade of Fos and Jun expression following passive avoidance training in the chick. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7, 563–569. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1995.tb00661.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, M. (2015). Reflections on glycogen and β-amyloid: Why does glycogenolytic β2-adrenoceptor stimulation not rescue memory after β-amyloid? Metab. Brain Dis. 30, 345–352. doi: 10.1007/s11011-014-9563-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, M. E., Gibbs, Z., and Hertz, L. (2009). Rescue of Abeta(1-42)-induced memory impairment in day-old chick by facilitation of astrocytic oxidative metabolism: Implications for Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurochem. 109, 230–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.05800.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, M. E., Maksel, D., Gibbs, Z., Hou, X., Summers, R. J., and Small, D. H. (2010). Memory loss caused by beta-amyloid protein is rescued by a beta(3)-adrenoceptor agonist. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 614–624. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.05.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Janus, A., Lustyk, K., and Pytka, K. (2023). MK-801 and cognitive functions: Investigating the behavioral effects of a non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist. Psychopharmacology 240, 2435–2457. doi: 10.1007/s00213-023-06454-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karimi Tari, P., Parsons, C. G., Collingridge, G. L., and Rammes, G. (2024). Memantine: Updating a rare success story in pro-cognitive therapeutics. Neuropharmacology 244:109737. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2023.109737

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, A., Laroche, S., and Davis, S. (2003). Activation of mitogen-activated protein kinase/extracellular signal-regulated kinase in hippocampal circuitry is required for consolidation and reconsolidation of recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 23, 5354–5360. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05354.2003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kida, S., Josselyn, S. A., de Ortiz, S. P., Kogan, J. H., Chevere, I., Masushige, S., et al. (2002). CREB required for the stability of new and reactivated fear memories. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 348–355. doi: 10.1038/nn819

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koh, M. T., and Bernstein, I. L. (2003). Inhibition of protein kinase A activity during conditioned taste aversion retrieval: Interference with extinction or reconsolidation of a memory? Neuroreport 14, 405–407. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200303030-00021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lattal, K. M., and Abel, T. (2004). Behavioral impairments caused by injections of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin after contextual retrieval reverse with time. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 101, 4667–4672. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0306546101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S., and Selkoe, D. J. (2020). A mechanistic hypothesis for the impairment of synaptic plasticity by soluble Aβ oligomers from Alzheimer’s brain. J. Neurochem. 154, 583–597. doi: 10.1111/jnc.15007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S., and Stern, A. M. (2022). Bioactive human Alzheimer brain soluble Aβ: Pathophysiology and therapeutic opportunities. Mol. Psychiatry 27, 3182–3191. doi: 10.1038/s41380-022-01589-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Litvin, O. O., and Anokhin, K. V. (2000). Mechanisms of memory reorganization during retrieval of acquired behavioral experience in chicks: The effects of protein synthesis inhibition in the brain. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 30, 671–678. doi: 10.1023/a:1026698700139

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopez, J., Gamache, K., Schneider, R., and Nader, K. (2015). Memory retrieval requires ongoing protein synthesis and NMDA receptor activity-mediated AMPA receptor trafficking. J. Neurosci. 35, 2465–2475. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0735-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mezey, S., Székely, A. D., Bourne, R. C., Kabai, P., and Csillag, A. (1999). Changes in binding to muscarinic and nicotinic cholinergic receptors in the chick telencephalon, following passive avoidance learning. Neurosci. Lett. 270, 75–78. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(99)00472-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milekic, M. H., and Alberini, C. M. (2002). Temporally graded requirement for protein synthesis following memory reactivation. Neuron 36, 521–525. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00976-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mileusnic, R., and Rose, S. (2010). The chick as a model for the study of the cellular mechanisms and potential therapies for Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2010:180734. doi: 10.4061/2010/180734

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mileusnic, R., Lancashire, C. L., and Rose, S. P. (2004). The peptide sequence Arg-Glu-Arg, present in the amyloid precursor protein, protects against memory loss caused by A beta and acts as a cognitive enhancer. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 1933–1938. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03276.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mileusnic, R., Lancashire, C. L., and Rose, S. P. (2005). Amyloid precursor protein: From synaptic plasticity to Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1048, 149–165. doi: 10.1196/annals.1342.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, K. (2015). Reconsolidation and the dynamic nature of memory. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 7:a021782. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021782

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., and Le Doux, J. E. (2000). Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406, 722–726. doi: 10.1038/35021052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikitin, V. P., Kozyrev, S. A., Solntseva, S. V., and Nikitin, P. V. (2021). Protein synthesis inhibitor administration before a reminder caused recovery from amnesia induced by memory reconsolidation impairment with NMDA glutamate receptor antagonist. Brain Res. Bull. 171, 44–55. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2021.03.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyakas, C., Granic, I., Halmy, L. G., Banerjee, P., and Luiten, P. G. (2011). The basal forebrain cholinergic system in aging and dementia. Rescuing cholinergic neurons from neurotoxic amyloid-β42 with memantine. Behav. Brain Res. 221, 594–603. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nystuen, K. L., McNamee, S. M., Akula, M., Holton, K. M., DeAngelis, M. M., and Haider, N. B. (2024). Alzheimer’s disease: Models and molecular mechanisms informing disease and treatments. Bioengineering (Basel) 11:45. doi: 10.3390/bioengineering11010045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohno, M. (2009). Failures to reconsolidate memory in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 92, 455–459. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, C. G., Stöffler, A., and Danysz, W. (2007). Memantine: A NMDA receptor antagonist that improves memory by restoration of homeostasis in the glutamatergic system - too little activation is bad, too much is even worse. Neuropharmacology 53, 699–723. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.07.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, Y., Jin, H., Xue, Y. H., Chen, Q., Yao, S. Y., Du, M. Q., et al. (2023). Current and future therapeutic strategies for Alzheimer’s disease: An overview of drug development bottlenecks. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:1206572. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1206572

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, A. E., Berlau, D. J., McGaugh, J. L., and Steward, O. (2006). Anisomycin infused into the hippocampus fails to block “reconsolidation” but impairs extinction: The role of re-exposure duration. Learn. Mem. 13, 27–34. doi: 10.1101/lm.91206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prado-Alcalá, R. A., Díaz, del Guante, M. A., Garín-Aguilar, M. E., Díaz-Trujillo, A., Quirarte, G. L., et al. (2006). Amygdala or hippocampus inactivation after retrieval induces temporary memory deficit. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 86, 144–149. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Przybyslawski, J., and Sara, S. J. (1997). Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behav. Brain Res. 84, 241–246. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(96)00153-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Puzzo, D., Lee, L., Palmeri, A., Calabrese, G., and Arancio, O. (2014). Behavioral assays with mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease: Practical considerations and guidelines. Biochem. Pharmacol. 88, 450–467. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2014.01.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rai, S. P., Krohn, M., and Pahnke, J. (2020). Early cognitive training rescues remote spatial memory but reduces cognitive flexibility in Alzheimer’s disease mice. J. Alzheimers Dis. 75, 1301–1317. doi: 10.3233/JAD-200161

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogawski, M. A., and Wenk, G. L. (2003). The neuropharmacological basis for the use of memantine in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. CNS Drug Rev. 9, 275–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1527-3458.2003.tb00254.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, S. P. (2000). God’s organism? The chick as a model system for memory studies. Learn Mem. 7, 1–17. doi: 10.1101/lm.7.1.1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, S. P., Gibbs, M. E., and Hambley, J. (1980). Transient increase in forebrain muscarinic cholinergic receptor binding following passive avoidance learning in the young chick. Neuroscience 5, 169–178. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(80)90083-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossato, J. I., Radiske, A., Gonzalez, M. C., Apolinário, G., de Araújo, R. L. S., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., et al. (2023). NMDARs control object recognition memory destabilization and reconsolidation. Brain Res. Bull. 197, 42–48. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2023.03.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salinska, E. (2006). The role of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors in memory consolidation and reconsolidation in the passive avoidance task in 1-day-old chicks. Neurochem. Int. 48, 447–452. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2005.11.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salinska, E., Bourne, R. C., and Rose, S. P. (2004). Reminder effects: The molecular cascade following a reminder in young chicks does not recapitulate that following training on a passive avoidance task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 3042–3047. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03407.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Samartgis, J. R., Schachte, L., Hazi, A., and Crowe, S. F. (2012). Memantine facilitates memory consolidation and reconsolidation in the day-old chick. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 97, 380–385. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.02.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherry, J. M., and Crowe, S. F. (2008a). Inhibition of cyclin-dependent kinase 5 by roscovitine impairs memory consolidation and reconsolidation in the day-old chick. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 91, 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.06.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherry, J. M., and Crowe, S. F. (2008b). The non-NMDA receptor antagonist 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX) impairs late reconsolidation of passive avoidance learning in the day-old chick. Neurosci. Lett. 442, 244–248. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherry, J. M., Milsome, S. L., and Crowe, S. F. (2010). The roles of RNA synthesis and protein translation during reconsolidation of passive-avoidance learning in the day-old chick. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 94, 438–446. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.10.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, M. J., Crowe, S. F., and Ng, K. T. (2003). Memory retrieval in the day-old chick: A psychobiological approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 219–231. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00032-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, B. C., Wang, Y. T., and Ren, J. (2023). Basic information about memantine and its treatment of Alzheimer’s disease and other clinical applications. Ibrain 9, 340–348. doi: 10.1002/ibra.12098

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiunova, A. A., Bezriadnov, D. V., and Anokhin, K. V. (2024). Non-competitive NMDA antagonist MK-801 prevents memory reconsolidation impairment caused by protein synthesis inhibitors in young chicks. Front. Pharmacol. 15:1378612. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2024.1378612

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiunova, A. A., Bezryadnov, D. V., Gaeva, D. R., Solodovnikov, V. S., and Anokhin, K. V. (2020). Memory reacquisition deficit: Chicks fail to relearn pharmacologically disrupted associative response. Behav. Brain Res. 390:112695. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112695

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Webster, S. J., Bachstetter, A. D., Nelson, P. T., Schmitt, F. A., and Van Eldik, L. J. (2014). Using mice to model Alzheimer’s dementia: An overview of the clinical disease and the preclinical behavioral changes in 10 mouse models. Front. Genet. 5:88. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2014.00088

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., Jiang, X., Ma, L., Wei, W., Li, Z., Chang, S., et al. (2022). Role of Aβ in Alzheimer’s-related synaptic dysfunction. Front. Cell. Dev. Biol. 10:964075. doi: 10.3389/fcell.2022.964075

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, W. Q., Feng, H., Bennett, P., and Ng, K. T. (1997). Inhibition of intermediate-term memory following passive avoidance training in neonate chicks by a presynaptic cholinergic blocker. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 67, 207–213. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1997.3767

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: memory, reconsolidation, chicks, β-amyloid, NMDA antagonists, memantine

Citation: Tiunova AA, Diffine EA and Anokhin KV (2025) Memory reconsolidation impairment by amyloid beta (1–42) and its prevention by non-competitive antagonists of NMDA receptors. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 19:1629492. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2025.1629492

Received: 15 May 2025; Accepted: 09 July 2025;
Published: 31 July 2025.

Edited by:

Magdalena Pereyra, Institut Pasteur, France

Reviewed by:

Mauricio Martins Oliveira, New York University, United States
Lucia (Lucy) Privitera, Queen Mary University of London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Tiunova, Diffine and Anokhin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: K. V. Anokhin, ay5hbm9raGluQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.