Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Comput. Neurosci., 04 April 2017
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2017.00017

Beta Band Corticomuscular Drive Reflects Muscle Coordination Strategies

  • 1Brain-Body Dynamics Lab, Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
  • 2Applied Mathematical Physiology Lab, Division of Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA

During force production, hand muscle activity is known to be coherent with activity in primary motor cortex, specifically in the beta-band (15–30 Hz) frequency range. It is not clear, however, if this coherence reflects the control strategy selected by the nervous system for a given task, or if it instead reflects an intrinsic property of cortico-spinal communication. Here, we measured corticomuscular and intermuscular coherence between muscles of index finger and thumb while a two-finger pinch grip of identical net force was applied to objects which were either stable (allowing synergistic activation of finger muscles) or unstable (requiring individuated finger control). We found that beta-band corticomuscular coherence with the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) and abductor pollicis brevis (APB) muscles, as well as their beta-band coherence with each other, was significantly reduced when individuated control of the thumb and index finger was required. We interpret these findings to show that beta-band coherence is reflective of a synergistic control strategy in which the cortex binds task-related motor neurons into functional units.

Introduction

Both corticomuscular and intermuscular synchronization, as quantified by coherence analysis (Rosenberg et al., 1989; Farmer et al., 1993; Conway et al., 1995), provide an important means of understanding the cortical drive to muscles (Conway et al., 1995; Baker et al., 1997; Brown, 2000; Boonstra et al., 2009a). Corticomotor drive contains a 15–30 Hz (beta-band) oscillatory component (Murthy and Fetz, 1992, 1996a,b; Sanes and Donoghue, 1993; Stancák and Pfurtscheller, 1996; Donoghue et al., 1998; Mima and Hallett, 1999; Lebedev and Wise, 2000; Witham et al., 2010), which entrains targeted motor neurons (Farmer et al., 1993, 1997; Mima and Hallett, 1999) and leads to synchronization between cortical and muscular activities in that frequency range (Murthy and Fetz, 1992; Conway et al., 1995; Baker et al., 1997; Salenius et al., 1997). Functionally-related muscles also share a common intermuscular beta-band input (Kilner et al., 1999; Boonstra and Breakspear, 2012; Boonstra, 2013), which is widely accepted as cortical in origin (Brown et al., 1999), as the motor cortex is the only well-established source for such beta-band drive.

Although beta-band cortical drive has received a great deal of attention, its functional significance for motor control remains unclear. Currently it is suggested that oscillations in this range functions to support a constant motor state (Kilner et al., 2000; Pogosyan et al., 2009; Engel and Fries, 2010). This is consistent with the observation that, during the production of a constant force, beta-band coherence is strengthened with continued sensory feedback and minimal voluntary movement (Gilbertson et al., 2005; Androulidakis et al., 2006, 2007; Lalo et al., 2007; Engel and Fries, 2010; Aumann and Prut, 2015). While the magnitude of beta-band corticomuscular coherence does correlate with force(Conway et al., 1995; Baker et al., 1997; Kilner et al., 1999, 2000, 2004; Baker, 2007; Kristeva et al., 2007; Witte et al., 2007), it disappears during movement (Baker et al., 1997; Kilner et al., 1999, 2000, 2004; Brown, 2000; Feige et al., 2000) and imagined movements (De Lange et al., 2008), and there is even evidence that the signal is not entirely feed-forward (Fisher et al., 2002; Baker et al., 2006; Witham et al., 2011). Such findings raise important questions as to the functional role of beta-band drive to muscles.

Numerous studies have investigated low force (<5 N) precision pinch paradigms to characterize cortico-spinal interactions through the use of corticomuscular coherence (CMC) (Muir and Lemon, 1983; Lemon and Mantel, 1989; Lemon et al., 1995, 1998; Baker et al., 2003). Findings have revealed that beta-band CMC is modulated by digit displacement (Riddle and Baker, 2006), object compliance (Kilner et al., 2000), and similar studies suggest a dependence upon the history and time course of muscle contraction (Chakarov et al., 2009; Omlor et al., 2011; Nazarpour et al., 2012). In nearly all cases, findings of decreased beta-band CMC can be interpreted as reflecting either (1) a departure from steady-state control of a particular muscle, or (2) the cortical “unbinding” of muscles when individuated, rather than synergistic, activation is called for. Given that beta-band cortical activity has been suggested as a “binding” signal for many years (Gray, 1994; Santello, 2014), and that such binding would naturally favor synergistic rather than individuated control of the fingers (Boonstra et al., 2009b; Danna-Dos Santos et al., 2010; Kattla and Lowery, 2010; Aumann and Prut, 2015), our overall hypothesis was that beta-band corticomotor drive should be reduced or eliminated when the degree of individuated muscle control is increased.

To address these issues, we studied beta-band corticomuscular and inter-muscular coherence (CMC and IMC, respectively) while participants applied low magnitude precision pinch forces to one of two different objects. The first object was a solid wooden dowel. Production of a constant pinch force against a solid object represents a relatively simple task for the nervous system. The second object was a custom-designed spring which buckles when compressed unless prevented from doing so through precise dynamic adjustment of thumb and index fingertip forces. The spring task described herein has been modified from Valero-Cuevas et al. (2003) and described in Dayanidhi et al. (2013b). This task requires the dynamic regulation of thumb and index fingertip force vectors in 3-D to stabilize the spring, which can be modeled as undergoing an instability similar to a subcritical pitchfork bifurcation (Venkadesan et al., 2007). The physical movement of the fingers remains negligible because large movements tend to increase the likelihood of buckling.

If beta-band coherence depends on the generation of a relatively stable pinch force, then coherence should change relatively little across these two tasks. If beta-band coherence is inherently an intermuscular binding signal, then we would expect to see little coherence during compression of the unstable spring, either in terms of CMC or IMC. Our findings support this hypothesis and suggest that the dynamic, mechanical relationships among muscles are likely critical factors shaping the frequency content of corticomotor drive.

Methods

Participants

We recruited 15 healthy participants (30.3 ± 4.6 years, 6 females) who were self-reported as right-handed. There were no known prior or current neurological conditions in any of the participants, nor did they report any previous hand injuries or surgeries. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Southern California (USC) with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Office for the Protection of Research Subjects at USC.

Task 1: Establishing Maximal Unstable Spring Compression Force

The Strength-Dexterity test provides a quantitative measure of hand dexterity by requiring dynamic regulation of endpoint force direction and magnitude to stabilize a slender and compliant spring prone to bucking (Valero-Cuevas et al., 2003). The instability in the spring increases with compression force, and thus the maximal compression force reached is indicative of the greatest instability the neuromuscular system can control. The force required to bring the spring to solid length was approximately 3.7–3.8 N, however the maximal compression force healthy adults can reach is less than 3.0 N (Dayanidhi, 2012). We specifically chose a spring with a low strength requirement (<15% maximal precision pinch force) to focus primarily on cortical drive involved with dexterity demand, rather than strength. The resting length of the spring measured 4.2 cm in length, weighed approximately two grams and had a spring constant of 0.86 N/cm. Custom designed 3-D printed acrylonitrile butadiene styrene (ABS) plastic end caps were glued to both ends of the spring to create flat surfaces on which to attach a force transducer. Additional ABS end caps were attached on top of the sensor for two purposes: (1) to provide a place for subjects to grasp the object and (2) to serve as a thermal barrier to prevent body heat from adding a bias to the temperature-sensitive force transducer. With the addition of the end caps and sensor, the effective resting length of the spring was 5.7 cm (Figure 1A).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Visuomotor force tracking paradigm for a representative subject. (A) A small compliant spring is compressed between the index finger and thumb. Fingertip forces and bipolar surface EMG from the intrinsic hand muscles FDI and APB (not shown) were recorded. (B) Force tracking task showing three random presentations during spring compression at forces of 1.0 and 2.1 N. Each force level (high or low) was presented to the subject for 30 s with an inter-stimulus interval of 5 s. The blue trace represents the recorded compression force. Red dotted lines indicate the ±15% tolerance limits relative to target force. Data that are within the tolerance limits for a minimum of 5 s in duration (shaded regions) were used in the coherence analysis. In the fourth hold, the compression force briefly fell out of the tolerance range. (C) The visuomotor task is repeated with a stable wooden dowel. (D) Force tracking profile with the dowel at the same force levels and tolerance limits as with the spring. Gray areas represent data within tolerance and dowel-low and dowel-high refer to dowel-low and dowel-high, respectively.

In this task, subjects rested their right arm on a table and used their index finger and thumb to compress the spring with their hand resting on the table. During the task, we asked participants to ensure that their 3rd–5th fingers did not assist in the task by tucking them into their palm. They were given four attempts (90 s each) to try to compress the spring as much as possible (Valero-Cuevas et al., 2003; Dayanidhi et al., 2013a,b; Lawrence et al., 2014). The average maximal compression force reached prior to spring buckling was taken as a normalized measure of their dexterous performance. We rounded this maximal value to the nearest tenth of a Newton and defined this as the subject-specific Fmax. We then calculated 40 and 80% of the subject-specific Fmax for use in the second phase of the study.

Task 2: Visuomotor Force Tracking

Subjects were seated comfortably in front of a computer providing visual feedback of their precision pinch compression force. They compressed either the same slender spring or a wooden dowel (length = 5.2 cm, diameter = 0.12 cm, Figure 1C) to visually track a series of randomly presented step targets set to 40 and 80% of their Fmax. For three trials for a given object, target force levels were presented at 30 s intervals with 5 s of rest in between (Figures 1B,D). The trial lasted until five repetitions of each force level were presented. During the resting periods, the subjects would hold the object with just enough force to prevent dropping it. Breaks were given in between trials when necessary to prevent fatigue effects. Subjects then repeated the procedure with the other object for the same number of trials and force levels. This two-by-two factorial design yielded four force conditions: spring-low, spring-high, dowel-low, and dowel-high.

Recordings

Force

Normal compression forces were measured by affixing a uni-axial load cell (ELFF-B4-10L, Measurement Specialties, Hampton, VA) with double-sided tape to the index finger side of either the compliant spring or the wooden dowel. The circular load cell measured 0.41 cm in height, 1.27 cm in diameter and aligned perfectly with the diameter of the objects. Signals from the sensor were differentially amplified with a custom designed circuit operating in the 0–5 N range. Data were captured using a USB Data Acquisition (DAQ) system (National Instruments, Austin, TX) sampling at a rate of 2,048 Hz. Prior to data collection, the sensor voltage was converted to Newtons by removing the DC offset and calibrating the load-cell using a four-point linear regression with fixed weights. The offset and gain of the load cell were corrected periodically to ensure accurate force recordings.

Electromyography (EMG)

Bipolar surface EMG were collected using a Delsys Bagnoli system (Delsys, Natick, MA) from the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) and abductor pollicis brevis (APB). Data were filtered between 20 and 450 Hz, amplified by 1000, and then sampled at a 2048 Hz. The reference electrode for the recordings was placed on the olecranon of the right arm. Recording locations were identified by palpating the muscle during force production in the direction of mechanical action for each muscle.

Electroencephalography (EEG)

Sixty four channels of EEG were recorded at a sampling rate of 2048 Hz (ANT Neuro, Enschede, The Netherlands). The fixed recording sites were arranged according to the international 10–20 system for scalp electrode placement. We ensured repeatable recordings of cortical areas across subject by taking skull measurements and placing electrode Cz at the cross section of the midway point between the nasion and inion and the midway point between the left and right tragus of the ear. Electrode impedances were kept below 10 k with respect to the reference electrode CPz.

Following digitization, EEG signals were bandpass filtered between 10 and 500 Hz and both EEG and EMG signals were notch filtered at 60 Hz and its harmonics up to 500 Hz using a 4th order Butterworth filter implemented in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA) and FieldTrip, a software package for EEG and EMG analysis (Delorme and Makeig, 2004; Oostenveld et al., 2011). Subsequently, EMG were rectified to extract group activity of motor units (Halliday et al., 1995; Mima and Hallett, 1999). Data formats collected from two separate systems were synchronized by configuring the NI-DAQ to send a trigger pulse to the EEG and EMG systems via a split BNC cable. Custom scripts were created to read in trigger events and synchronize all data.

Trial Selection

The start of a trial was defined as the time when the on-screen target transitioned from a resting value to either 40 or 80% of Fmax, and its end was defined as the time when the target value returned back to rest. Trial windows were 30 s in duration with a 5 s inter-stimulus interval. Sample traces of the matching paradigm are shown in in Figure 1B for the spring (blue trace) and in Figure 1D for the dowel (orange trace); each showing three randomized presentations of the low and high force levels (note that the force levels are the same across objects). Force data during these steady hold phases were visually examined to determine if the task was performed correctly. Our requirement was that the hold phase must be within a ±15% tolerance of the target force value and be held within this range for at least 5 s. The force profiles meeting these criteria are shown as the gray shaded areas in Figures 1B,D. Force profiles not meeting these criteria were excluded from analysis. The accuracy of the force matching task was analyzed by taking the root mean square error of the steady hold force compared to the target level for each of the four conditions.

Epochs of synchronized EEG and EMG from each condition that satisfied the steady-state criteria were normalized and pooled across conditions and subjects (Amjad et al., 1997). Normalizing each epoch of data gives equal weight to each section and effectively eliminates the possibility of coherence bias which favors sections with high EMG amplitude (Amjad et al., 1997; James et al., 2008; Schoffelen et al., 2011).

Coherence Analysis

Synchronous oscillations between cortical activity and EMG indicate functional connectivity which can be assessed through coherence analysis (Nunez et al., 1997; Mima and Hallett, 1999). Briefly, coherence measures the temporal correlation between two signals through the strength of the consistency of their phase lag as a function of frequency. The result is a coherence spectrum bounded between 0 and 1 for each frequency of interest. A value of 1 indicates a perfect temporal correlation, while 0 indicates no correlation.

For a given time series, x(t), let the auto spectrum be represented as

Pxx(f)=1Li=1LXi(f)·Xi(f)

where Xi(f) represents the Fourier transform of the signal at segment i of L, and * indicates the complex conjugate. A similar spectrum exists for the signal y(t), represented as Pyy(f). The cross spectrum between the signals x(t) and y(t) is represented as

Pxy(f)=1Li=1LXi(f)·Yi(f).

Coherence is calculated by normalizing the square of the cross-spectral density between signals x(t) and y(t) by the product of their individual auto spectral densities (Baker et al., 1997; Nunez et al., 1997) as represented by

Cxy(f)=|Pxy(f)|2Pxx(f)·Pyy(f)

Corticomuscular coherence (CMC) refers to the specific case where cortical and muscular signals represent x(t) and y(t). CMC was computed for each EEG-EMG electrode pair as well as FDI-APB coherence using FieldTrip, an open-source toolbox in MATLAB for the analysis of EEG and MEG data (Oostenveld et al., 2011). We used discrete prolate spheroidal sequences (DPSS) or Slepian tapers (Slepian, 1978) for the calculation of the auto and cross spectra. The multi-taper method provides several measures of the spectral estimation by multiplying the data series by a series of orthogonal tapers prior to calculating the Fourier transform (Pesaran, 2008). Three tapers were used in our analysis, providing a spectral bandwidth of ±5 Hz (Maris et al., 2007; Schoffelen et al., 2011).

Selection of EEG Electrodes

Although CMC can be calculated between each muscle and every scalp electrode, we limited our selection to EEG electrodes that showed high FDI-EEG coherence during the dowel-low task. Similar low-force isometric precision task have been previously used in literature for CMC analysis (Baker et al., 1997; Kilner et al., 2000; Fisher et al., 2002; Riddle and Baker, 2006; Chen et al., 2013). Raw coherence values were first normalized by conversion to standard Z-scores using the following formula

Z=arctanh(C)1/2N

Where N is the total number of tapers used in the calculation of coherence (C) (Baker et al., 2003; Laine et al., 2013, 2014). The electrode locations selected for further analysis were those at which the average Z-score exceeded a Bonferroni-corrected 99% confidence level (Z = 3.6). The correction accounts for the total number of EEG channels.

Linear Mixed-Effects Model

A linear mixed-effect model provides a method of describing a relationship for a measurable quantity as a function of the sum of weighted independent variables (Winter, 2013). We investigated the effects of task condition on beta coherence using a linear mixed-effect model with the following format:

CMCβ=β0+β1·Condition+β2·(1|Participant)+ϵ

where CMCβ is the average beta-range coherence of an epoch, Condition (high and low force for both the spring and dowel) is the fixed-effect term, Participant is the random-effect term, βn terms are the coefficients for the independent variables, and ϵ is the error. The random-effects term was inserted to account for subject variability since several measurements were taken for each condition. Models were generated to estimate CMCβ for the FDI and APB well as beta intermuscular FDI-APB coherence.

Results

Baseline Corticomuscular Coherence

To confirm the spatial sensitivity and validity of our analysis procedures, we calculated FDI-EEG coherence for the dowel-low condition. Figure 2A shows a head map of the average Z-transformed corticomuscular coherence between the FDI and all EEG channels. The locations of the four electrodes that exceeded our significance threshold (described previously) are shown in white over the left primary motor cortex and labeled C3, C1, CP3, and CP1. The spatial localization and magnitude shown over the left sensorimotor cortex coincides with previous literature findings during low force production (Witte et al., 2007; Chakarov et al., 2009; Piitulainen et al., 2013).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results for the dowel-low task. (A) Grand average Z-transformed FDI-EEG coherence head map for the dowel-low task with all electrode locations marked. The four electrodes used for statistical analyses were C1, C3, CP1, and CP3 (labeled) with respective Z-transformed coherence values of 4.81, 5.26, 4.66, and 4.54. The cluster of electrodes appears over the left sensorimotor cortex. (B) Average coherence spectra of the four electrodes shown in (A) for the spring-high (blue trace) and dowel-high (orange trace) tasks. The beta frequency band (15–30 Hz) is shown as the gray shaded area. Peak coherence of the average for the spring-low condition was 4.59 at 21.05 Hz and for the dowel-high condition, 6.84 at 20.1 Hz.

Cortical drive to the FDI during the dowel-high and spring-high conditions was directly compared. Figure 2B shows the average Z-transformed coherence spectra (5–100 Hz) of the four significant electrodes for the dowel-high (orange trace) and spring-high (blue trace) conditions. The peak coherence for both conditions appeared in the beta frequency range (gray shaded area in Figure 2B). The peak Z-transformed coherence during the dowel-high condition was 6.84 at 20.1 Hz. Despite matched force levels, however, the peak coherence during the spring-high condition decreased to 4.59 at 21.1 Hz.

Linear Mixed-Effect Model

Figure 3 shows the results of the linear mixed-effects model which tests the effects of condition on beta FDI-EEG (Figure 3A) and APB-EEG (Figure 3B) beta-range coherence. Using an F-test, we compared the difference in effect of spring-low and dowel-low model coefficients on beta CMC. We found no significant difference in effect of low force coefficients on either FDI-EEG or APB-EEG beta coherence. It is important to note that, in these low force conditions, both objects remain in the stable domain as the spring has not been compressed enough to exhibit instability.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results of the linear mixed-effects model for the four conditions: dowel-low (DL), spring-low (SL), dowel-high (DH), and spring-high (SH). The model was constructed to predict mean beta range coherence using condition as the fixed-effect with corrections for the random effects of participant. In each bar graph, the mean beta range CMC is shown on the vertical axis and condition is on the horizontal axis. Bars representing one standard error are shown for each condition and significance above the matched force level column pairs represent the statistical difference in the linear mixed-effect coefficients as determined using an F-test. (A) Linear mixed-effect found no differences in FDI-EEG beta coherence for the low forces, but found a significant difference between the higher force compression conditions. (B) Linear mixed-effect model results for APB-EEG coherence show the same statistical results for low and high conditions as in FDI-EEG.

A similar comparison of the difference in the linear mixed-effect coefficients for the spring-high and dowel-high conditions revealed a significance difference in effect on both FDI-EEG (p = 6.8459e-08) and APB-EEG (p = 1.6889e-05) beta-range coherence. Overall, CMC was reduced in the unstable task (spring-high condition) compared to the stable task (dowel-high condition) with matched force levels. During the high force compression conditions, the dowel remains in the stable domain, but the spring has been compressed to the point of instability.

EMG-EMG Coherence

We tested for changes in FDI-APB beta coherence across the higher force conditions. Figure 4A shows the FDI-APB intermuscular coherence for the spring and dowel objects. The dowel-high condition showed a peak beta range Z-transformed coherence of 6.59 at 24.6 Hz. This peak value was significantly higher than for that of the spring-high condition, which had a peak value of 2.33 at 24.4 Hz. Figure 4B depicts the results of the linear mixed-effect model showing that the effects of the dowel was significantly higher than for the spring at the high force level (p = 4.9415e-10).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. FDI-APB intermuscular coherence. (A) Grand average EMG-EMG coherence calculated between the FDI and APB for the spring-high (SH, blue trace) and dowel-high (DH, orange trace) tasks. Peak coherence was 6.59 at 24.63 Hz for the dowel-high condition and 2.33 at 24.44 Hz for the spring-high task. (B) Linear mixed-effect model results for the effect of condition on FDI-APB coherence. The effect of dowel-high on beta coherence was significantly higher than for the spring-high condition (p < 0.001).

Discussion

In this study, we have examined the functional meaning of beta-band corticomotor drive. Specifically, we tested the hypothesis that beta-band cortical drive reflects the “binding” of motor neurons into functional units for synergistic cortical control, and thus should depend upon the type/degree of muscle coordination required by a task. Our findings provide evidence that beta-band corticomotor drive is inherently a reflection of intermuscular “binding” rather than steady isometric force production.

It is known that synergistically-activated muscles share beta-band cortical drive, as measured by intermuscular EMG coherence (Kilner et al., 1999; Boonstra and Breakspear, 2012; Boonstra, 2013). It has been suggested that this phenomenon reflects the “binding” of these muscles into a functional unit (or synergy) at the cortical level (Gray, 1994; Santello, 2014). As such, we predicted that beta-band CMC should emerge more prominently when the task of the motor cortex can be reduced to scaling the activation of a functional group of motor neurons (Valero-Cuevas, 2000). The idea that cortical output should reflect the dimensionality of a task is in line with the work of Rathelot and Strick (2006, 2009) who proposed that the motor cortex in humans and some higher primates may have evolved specific pathways (an “old” and “new” M1) to allow optional and flexible utilization of muscle synergies or “motor primitives.”

In this study we have used compression of a slender spring to obligate time-varying fingertip force adjustment while minimizing physical movement and changes in net force (Valero-Cuevas et al., 2003; Dayanidhi et al., 2013a,b; Lawrence et al., 2014). This paradigm has been shown in an fMRI study to increase the engagement diverse brain networks (Mosier et al., 2011) beyond what is required for less-demanding actions. Succeeding at the unstable task (spring-high) requires a level of independent dynamic control of the muscles of the thumb and index finger to dynamically regulate fingertip force vectors to stabilize the spring (Johanson et al., 2001; Venkadesan et al., 2007), which is not the case for isometric tasks like when a wooden dowel is pinched (Valero-Cuevas, 2000). Accordingly, our data showed a general reduction of intermuscular coherence between the EMG signals recorded from the APB and FDI muscles when the spring was compressed at a level that induced instability. The reduction of beta-band (15–30 Hz) coherence between muscles can be interpreted as stemming from an “unbinding” of these two muscles at the level of the cortex. Changes in force alone would not explain our results, since little change in IMC was observed in the dowel-low vs. dowel high condition. Further, increased spring compression force is associated with increased instability, but not changes in spring compliance. Our results almost certainly reflect a neural reaction to the demands of controlling instabilities.

Similar to our findings for IMC, CMC in the beta-band was markedly reduced for the FDI and ABP muscles when the spring became unstable in the spring-high condition. If this phenomenon were simply due to increased noise in the EEG signal (e.g., due to increased cortico-cortical communication during the more difficult task) we would expect that IMC would have been preserved (since the muscles would still share beta-band cortical drive), but this was not the case. Further, a change from static to dynamic force production is known to increase high-frequency (30–50 Hz) CMC as beta-band CMC shifts to higher frequencies (Omlor et al., 2007). We did not observe this type of shift. The simplest interpretation would be that the beta-band cortical drive was fundamentally an intermuscular signal in this task, and that a shift in cortical control strategy occurred when the thumb and index finger muscles required independent control. This interpretation is also in line with evidence from a pilot study in which within-muscle motor unit coherence was reduced in the APB muscle during the same high-force spring compression task (Laine et al., 2015). A relationship between beta-band cortical drive and control strategy also agrees with its dependence on psychosensory aspects of a task (Laine et al., 2014), although typically it is the higher frequencies of cortical drive (30–50 Hz) which show the greatest sensitivity to such features (Brown et al., 1998; Omlor et al., 2007, 2011; Patino et al., 2008; Mehrkanoon et al., 2014).

Recently it has been hypothesized that beta oscillations arise from closed loops from M1 to muscle synergies back to M1 (Aumann and Prut, 2015). As such, during sustained contraction of a muscle, groups of sensorimotor neurons oscillate in synchrony to maintain the current state (Engel and Fries, 2010). Conversely during movement, de-synchrony of the local group disrupts beta rhythms. The results obtained in this study support this closed-loop hypothesis given in the fact that during the stable conditions (i.e., spring-low, dowel-low and dowel-high), synergistic muscle activations were necessary for producing the target forces. However, in the spring-high condition, it was necessary to disrupt these synergies despite the fact that a relatively constant net force was maintained.

Confounds and Alternative Explanations

Physical differences in the objects compressed are of key importance to this study. Our interpretation is that the primary factor was the decreased stability of the spring when compressed at higher forces, however, small differences finger position, movement, object compliance, etc. could influence coherence measures and must be considered.

In a previous study, Kilner et al. found positive correlations between the magnitude of beta-band CMC and object compliance during a ramp and hold precision pinch task (Kilner et al., 2000). In our study, we did not observe a difference in beta-band CMC between the dowel-low and spring-low conditions, and at high forces, we observed a reduction rather than an increase in beta-band CMC. While the study of Kilner et al. shares many similarities with the work presented here, the compliant object used in our study was free at both ends and had the propensity to buckle and slip out of the hand when compressed at applied forces >2.2 N. In contrast, two fixed levers with programmable compliance were compressed in the study of Kilner et al. Such differences make direct comparison between studies inexact, but it is clear that the higher compliance of the spring relative to the dowel should, if anything, favor beta-band coherence (Kilner et al., 2000), as should higher force relative to lower force (Witte et al., 2007). Accordingly, our observation of decreased beta-band coherence in the spring-high condition appears to be related to a change in neuromuscular control strategy in response to the reduced stability of the spring. If very small movements can influence CMC (Kilner et al., 1999), then it is possible that the underlying reason for this may relate to the fact that movement may not allow muscles to be “bound” in the same way as is possible during static isometric force production. Although the physical positioning of the fingers can also have an influence on coherence (Riddle and Baker, 2006) the final position of the fingers during spring vs. dowel compression did not differ by more than about 1 cm, and we have no reason to believe that this would have been a major factor in the present study. Moreover, prior fMRI work in this same task has shown that the presence of instability has an effect distinct from that of compliance (Mosier et al., 2011).

It is also important to consider potential drawbacks of using EEG and surface EMG signals. For example, surface EMG will not be as sensitive as single motor unit recordings (Keenan et al., 2012) and may be influenced by signal processing techniques such as filtering or rectification (Boonstra and Breakspear, 2012; Farina et al., 2013). Even so, a systematic difference in the sensitivity of EMG signals to beta-band drive across tasks seems an unlikely explanation for our results, especially given the low levels of force required and our use of the most appropriate signal processing methods (i.e., EMG rectification) under these conditions (Farina et al., 2013).

It is relevant that compression of the spring to the point that it becomes unstable is an inherently difficult and demanding task. While there is increasing evidence that M1 activity may be involved with the perception of task goals (Shen and Alexander, 1997; Cisek et al., 2003; Scott, 2003), our suggestion is that the perception of difficulty in motor tasks is secondary to more tangible factors, such as the need for time-critical sensorimotor corrections, and individuated vs. synergistic control of muscles. In one recent study, for example, beta-band CMC was reduced when the degree of bimanual muscle coordination was increased through a visual feedback manipulation (de Vries et al., 2016). The interpretation was that beta-band CMC relates to the control of individual muscles, rather than coordination. The opposite argument could also be made, however, because in that study, errors of force may have been perceived and corrected individually for each hand in order to achieve better overall bimanual coordination. This would be in line with our coordination-based interpretation of beta-band drive to muscles. The same study also found that low-frequency IMC (~10 Hz) across hands was highest when bimanual coordination was highest. Because ~10 Hz CMC is not present in that study nor our own, low-frequency drive to individual muscles cannot be measured or compared concurrently with inter-muscular drive (i.e., IMC). This makes it difficult to attribute changes in IMC to inherently coordination-related aspects of our tasks, rather than other factors which might influence the production of low-frequency neural drive in general. That said, our Figure 4A does visibly show low-frequency IMC reduced along with the beta-band in the spring-high condition, which certainly justifies further investigation of this issue.

Overall, our study tests the notion that beta-band cortical drive essentially reflects the dimensionality of cortical commands, and our results strongly suggest that beta-band CMC should be interpreted carefully, with special attention to muscle coordination and time-critical sensorimotor demands. Our current speculation, supported by the findings of this investigation, is that beta-band CMC reflects the use of a low-dimensional mode of cortical control over groups of muscles, rather than the maintenance of a steady-state force output. While fully understanding the neurophysiology and task-dependence of cortico-motor oscillations requires further study, our results justify such future work and provide a springboard for more focused investigations into the relationship between the physical requirements of a task and neural control strategies necessary to satisfy them. Finally, such insights into the origin and modulation of cortico-motor oscillations would not only clarify fundamental mechanism for sensorimotor control, but perhaps provide well-founded tasks and analyses directly translatable to clinical measures and diagnostic tests (Norton and Gorassini, 2006; Hammond et al., 2007; Pogosyan et al., 2009; Fisher et al., 2012; Ko et al., 2016).

Author Contributions

AR, JK, and FV designed research; AR performed research; AR, CL, and JK analyzed data; AR, CL, JK, and FV wrote the paper.

Conflict of Interest Statement

FV holds US Patent No. 6,537,075 on some of the technology used in this study that is commercialized by Neuromuscular Dynamics, LLC.

The other authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This material is in part based upon work supported by NSF Grant EFRI-COPN 0836042, NIH Grant R01-052345, NIH Grant R01-050520 to FV, and Supplement R01-050520-W1 to AR.

References

Amjad, A. M., Halliday, D. M., Rosenberg, J. R., and Conway, B. A. (1997). An extended difference of coherence test for comparing and combining several independent coherence estimates: theory and application to the study of motor units and physiological tremor. J. Neurosci. Methods 73, 69–79. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(96)02214-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Androulidakis, A. G., Doyle, L. M., Gilbertson, T. P., and Brown, P. (2006). Corrective movements in response to displacements in visual feedback are more effective during periods of 13–35 Hz oscillatory synchrony in the human corticospinal system. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 3299–3304. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05201.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Androulidakis, A. G., Doyle, L. M., Yarrow, K., Litvak, V., Gilbertson, T. P., and Brown, P. (2007). Anticipatory changes in beta synchrony in the human corticospinal system and associated improvements in task performance. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 3758–3765. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05620.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aumann, T. D., and Prut, Y. (2015). Do sensorimotor β-oscillations maintain muscle synergy representations in primary motor cortex?. Trends Neurosci. 38, 77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, S. N. (2007). Oscillatory interactions between sensorimotor cortex and the periphery. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 649–655. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, S. N., Chiu, M., and Fetz, E. E. (2006). Afferent encoding of central oscillations in the monkey arm. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 3904–3910. doi: 10.1152/jn.01106.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, S. N., Pinches, E. M., and Lemon, R. N. (2003). Synchronization in monkey motor cortex during a precision grip task. II. Effect of oscillatory activity on corticospinal output. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 1941–1953. doi: 10.1152/jn.00832.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, S. N., Olivier, E., and Lemon, R. N. (1997). Coherent oscillations in monkey motor cortex and hand muscle EMG show task-dependent modulation. J. Physiol. 501, 225–241. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1997.225bo.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, T. W. (2013). The potential of corticomuscular and intermuscular coherence for research on human motor control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:855. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, T. W., and Breakspear, M. (2012). Neural mechanisms of intermuscular coherence: implications for the rectification of surface electromyography. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 796–807. doi: 10.1152/jn.00066.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, T. W., Daffertshofer, A., Roerdink, M., Flipse, I., Groenewoud, K., and Beek, P. J. (2009b). Bilateral motor unit synchronization of leg muscles during a simple dynamic balance task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 613–622. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06584.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, T. W., van Wijk, B. C., Praamstra, P., and Daffertshofer, A. (2009a). Corticomuscular and bilateral EMG coherence reflect distinct aspects of neural synchronization. Neurosci. Lett. 463, 17–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.07.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, P. (2000). Cortical drives to human muscle: the Piper and related rhythms. Prog. Neurobiol. 60, 97–108. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(99)00029-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, P., Farmer, S. F., Halliday, D. M., Marsden, J., and Rosenberg, J. R. (1999). Coherent cortical and muscle discharge in cortical myoclonus. Brain 122, 461–472. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.3.461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, P., Salenius, S., Rothwell, J. C., and Hari, R. (1998). Cortical correlate of the Piper rhythm in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 2911–2917.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chakarov, V., Naranjo, J. R., Schulte-Mönting, J., Omlor, W., Huethe, F., and Kristeva, R. (2009). Beta-range EEG-EMG coherence with isometric compensation for increasing modulated low-level forces. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 1115–1120. doi: 10.1152/jn.91095.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S., Entakli, J., Bonnard, M., Berton, E., and De Graaf, J. B. (2013). Functional corticospinal projections from human supplementary motor area revealed by corticomuscular coherence during precise grip force control. PLoS ONE 8:e60291. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisek, P., Crammond, D. J., and Kalaska, J. F. (2003). Neural activity in primary motor and dorsal premotor cortex in reaching tasks with the contralateral versus ipsilateral arm. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 922–942. doi: 10.1152/jn.00607.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, B. A., Halliday, D. M., Farmer, S. F., Shahani, U., Maas, P., Weir, A. I., et al. (1995). Synchronization between motor cortex and spinal motoneuronal pool during the performance of a maintained motor task in man. J. Physiol. 489, 917–924. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp021104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danna-Dos Santos, A., Poston, B., Jesunathadas, M., Bobich, L. R., Hamm, T. M., and Santello, M. (2010). Influence of fatigue on hand muscle coordination and EMG-EMG coherence during three-digit grasping. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 3576–3587. doi: 10.1152/jn.00583.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayanidhi, S. (2012). Behavioral, Muscular and Dynamical Changes in Low Force Dexterous Manipulation during Development and Aging. Los Angeles, CA: University of Southern California.

Google Scholar

Dayanidhi, S., Hedberg, A., Valero-Cuevas, F. J., and Forssberg, H. (2013a). Developmental improvements in dynamic control of fingertip forces last throughout childhood and into adolescence. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 1583–1592. doi: 10.1152/jn.00320.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayanidhi, S., Kutch, J. J., and Valero-Cuevas, F. J. (2013b). Decrease in muscle contraction time complements neural maturation in the development of dynamic manipulation. J. Neurosci. 33, 15050–15055. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1968-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Lange, F. P., Jensen, O., Bauer, M., and Toni, I. (2008). Interactions between posterior gamma and frontal alpha/beta oscillations during imagined actions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2:7. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.007.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vries, I. E., Daffertshofer, A., Stegeman, D. F., and Boonstra, T. W. (2016). Functional connectivity in neuromuscular system underlying bimanual coordination. J. Neurophysiol. 116, 2576–2585. doi: 10.1152/jn.00460.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donoghue, J. P., Sanes, J. N., Hatsopoulos, N. G., and Gaál, G. (1998). Neural discharge and local field potential oscillations in primate motor cortex during voluntary movements. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 159–173.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., and Fries, P. (2010). Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farina, D., Negro, F., and Jiang, N. (2013). Identification of common synaptic inputs to motor neurons from the rectified electromyogram. J. Physiol. 591, 2403–2418. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.246082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farmer, S. F., Bremner, F. D., Halliday, D. M., Rosenberg, J. R., and Stephens, J. A. (1993). The frequency content of common synaptic inputs to motoneurones studied during voluntary isometric contraction in man. J. Physiol. 470, 127–155. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farmer, S., Halliday, D., Conway, B., Stephens, J., and Rosenberg, J. (1997). A review of recent applications of cross-correlation methodologies to human motor unit recording. J. Neurosci. Methods 74, 175–187. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(97)02248-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feige, B., Aertsen, A., and Kristeva-Feige, R. (2000). Dynamic synchronization between multiple cortical motor areas and muscle activity in phasic voluntary movements. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 2622–2629.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fisher, K. M., Zaaimi, B., Williams, T. L., Baker, S. N., and Baker, M. R. (2012). Beta-band intermuscular coherence: a novel biomarker of upper motor neuron dysfunction in motor neuron disease. Brain 135, 2849–2864. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, R. J., Galea, M. P., Brown, P., and Lemon, R. N. (2002). Digital nerve anaesthesia decreases EMG-EMG coherence in a human precision grip task. Exp. Brain Res. 145, 207–214. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1113-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbertson, T., Lalo, E., Doyle, L., Di Lazzaro, V., Cioni, B., and Brown, P. (2005). Existing motor state is favored at the expense of new movement during 13-35 Hz oscillatory synchrony in the human corticospinal system. J. Neurosci. 25, 7771–7779. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1762-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. M. (1994). Synchronous oscillations in neuronal systems: mechanisms and functions. J. Comput. Neurosci. 1, 11–38. doi: 10.1007/BF00962716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliday, D. M., Rosenberg, J. R., Amjad, A. M., Breeze, P., Conway, B. A., and Farmer, S. F. (1995). A framework for the analysis of mixed time series/point process data—theory and application to the study of physiological tremor, single motor unit discharges and electromyograms. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol. 64, 237–278. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6107(96)00009-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, C., Bergman, H., and Brown, P. (2007). Pathological synchronization in Parkinson's disease: networks, models and treatments. Trends Neurosci. 30, 357–364. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, L. M., Halliday, D. M., Stephens, J. A., and Farmer, S. F. (2008). On the development of human corticospinal oscillations: age-related changes in EEG–EMG coherence and cumulant. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 3369–3379. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06277.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johanson, M. E., Valero-Cuevas, F. J., and Hentz, V. R. (2001). Activation patterns of the thumb muscles during stable and unstable pinch tasks. J. Hand. Surg. Am. 26, 698–705. doi: 10.1053/jhsu.2001.26188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kattla, S., and Lowery, M. M. (2010). Fatigue related changes in electromyographic coherence between synergistic hand muscles. Exp. Brain Res. 202, 89–99. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2110-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keenan, K. G., Massey, W. V., Walters, T. J., Collins, J. D. (2012). Sensitivity of EMG-EMG coherence to detect the common oscillatory drive to hand muscles in young and older adults. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 2866–2875. doi: 10.1152/jn.01011.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilner, J. M., Baker, S. N., Salenius, S., Jousmäki, V., Hari, R., and Lemon, R. N. (1999). Task-dependent modulation of 15–30 Hz coherence between rectified EMGs from human hand and forearm muscles. J. Physiol. 516, 559–570. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0559v.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilner, J. M., Baker, S. N., Salenius, S., Hari, R., and Lemon, R. N. (2000). Human cortical muscle coherence is directly related to specific motor parameters. J. Neurosci. 20, 8838–8845.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kilner, J. M., Fisher, R. J., and Lemon, R. N. (2004). Coupling of oscillatory activity between muscles is strikingly reduced in a deafferented subject compared with normal controls. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 790–796. doi: 10.1152/jn.01247.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ko, N. H., Laine, C. M., Fisher, B. E., and Valero-Cuevas, F. J. (2016). Dynamic fingertip force variability in individuals with Parkinson's disease. J. Hand Ther. 29, e8. doi: 10.1016/j.jht.2016.02.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kristeva, R., Patino, L., and Omlor, W. (2007). Beta-range cortical motor spectral power and corticomuscular coherence as a mechanism for effective corticospinal interaction during steady-state motor output. Neuroimage 36, 785–792. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laine, C. M., Negro, F., and Farina, D. (2013). Neural correlates of task-related changes in physiological tremor. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 170–176. doi: 10.1152/jn.00041.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laine, C. M., Reyes, A., Farina, D., and Valero-Cuevas, F. J. (2015). “Stability-dependent modulation of neural drive to the thumb during precision grip,” in Proceedings of the 25th Annual Conference of the Society for Neural Control of Movement (Charleston, SC).

Laine, C. M., Yavuz, S. U., and Farina, D. (2014). Task-related changes in sensorimotor integration influence the common synaptic input to motor neurones. Acta Physiol. 211, 229–239. doi: 10.1111/apha.12255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lalo, E., Gilbertson, T., Doyle, L., Di Lazzaro, V., Cioni, B., and Brown, P. (2007). Phasic increases in cortical beta activity are associated with alterations in sensory processing in the human. Exp. Brain Res. 177, 137–145. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0655-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, E. L., Fassola, I., Werner, I., Leclercq, C., and Valero-Cuevas, F. J. (2014). Quantification of dexterity as the dynamical regulation of instabilities: comparisons across gender, age, and disease. Front. Neurol. 5:53. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebedev, M. A., and Wise, S. P. (2000). Oscillations in the premotor cortex: single-unit activity from awake, behaving monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 130, 195–215. doi: 10.1007/s002210050022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemon, R. N., Baker, S. N., Davis, J. A., Kirkwood, P. A., Maier, M. A., and Yang, H. S. (1998). The importance of the cortico-motoneuronal system for control of grasp. Novartis Found Symp. 218, 202–215.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lemon, R. N., and Mantel, G. W. (1989). The influence of changes in discharge frequency of corticospinal neurones on hand muscles in the monkey. J. Physiol. 413, 351–378. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemon, R. N., Johansson, R. S., and Westling, G. (1995). Corticospinal control during reach, grasp, and precision lift in man. J. Neurosci. 15, 6145–6156.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Maris, E. J., Schoffelen, J. M., and Fries, P. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of coherence differences. J. Neurosci. Methods 163, 161–175. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehrkanoon, S., Breakspear, M., and Boonstra, T. W. (2014). The reorganization of corticomuscular coherence during a transition between sensorimotor states. Neuroimage 100, 692–702. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mima, T., and Hallett, M. (1999). Corticomuscular coherence: a review. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 16, 501. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199911000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mosier, K., Lau, C., Wang, Y., Venkadesan, M., and Valero-Cuevas, F. J. (2011). Controlling instabilities in manipulation requires specific cortical-striatal-cerebellar networks. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 1295–1305. doi: 10.1152/jn.00757.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muir, R. B., and Lemon, R. N. (1983). Corticospinal neurons with a special role in precision grip. Brain Res. 261, 312–316. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90635-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murthy, V. N., and Fetz, E. E. (1992). Coherent 25-to 35-Hz oscillations in the sensorimotor cortex of awake behaving monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89, 5670–5674. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.12.5670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murthy, V. N., and Fetz, E. E. (1996a). Synchronization of neurons during local field potential oscillations in sensorimotor cortex of awake monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 3968–3982.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Murthy, V. N., and Fetz, E. E. (1996b). Oscillatory activity in sensorimotor cortex of awake monkeys: synchronization of local field potentials and relation to behavior. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 3949–3396.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nazarpour, K., Barnard, A., and Jackson, A. (2012). Flexible cortical control of task-specific muscle synergies. J. Neurosci. 32, 1234–12360. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5481-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, J. A., and Gorassini, M. A. (2006). Changes in cortically related intermuscular coherence accompanying improvements in locomotor skills in incomplete spinal cord injury. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 2580–2589. doi: 10.1152/jn.01289.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunez, P. L., Srinivasan, R., Westdorp, A. F., Wijesinghe, R. S., Tucker, D. M., Silberstein, R. B., et al. (1997). EEG coherency: I: statistics, reference electrode, volume conduction, Laplacians, cortical imaging, and interpretation at multiple scales. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 103, 499–515. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(97)00066-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Omlor, W., Patino, L., Mendez-Balbuena, I., Schulte-Mönting, J., and Kristeva, R. (2011). Corticospinal beta-range coherence is highly dependent on the pre-stationary motor state. J. Neurosci. 31, 8037–8045. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4153-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Omlor, W., Patino, L., Hepp-Reymond, M. C., and Kristeva, R. (2007). Gamma-range corticomuscular coherence during dynamic force output. Neuroimage 34, 1191–1198. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 1, 2011. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patino, L., Omlor, W., Chakarov, V., Hepp-Reymond, M. C., and Kristeva, R. (2008). Absence of gamma-range corticomuscular coherence during dynamic force in a deafferented patient. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1906–1916. doi: 10.1152/jn.00390.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesaran, B. (2008). “Spectral analysis for neural signals,” in Short Course III, ed P. Mitra (Washington, DC: Society for Neuroscience), 1.

Google Scholar

Piitulainen, H., Bourguignon, M., De Tiège, X., Hari, R., and Jousmäki, V. (2013). Coherence between magnetoencephalography and hand-action-related acceleration, force, pressure, and electromyogram. Neuroimage 72, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pogosyan, A., Gaynor, L. D., Eusebio, A., and Brown, P. (2009). Boosting cortical activity at beta-band frequencies slows movement in humans. Curr. Biol. 19, 1637–1641. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rathelot, J. A., and Strick, P. L. (2006). Muscle representation in the macaque motor cortex: an anatomical perspective. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 8257–8262. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602933103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rathelot, J. A., and Strick, P. L. (2009). Subdivisions of primary motor cortex based on cortico-motoneuronal cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 918–923. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808362106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riddle, C. N., and Baker, S. N. (2006). Digit displacement, not object compliance, underlies task dependent modulations in human corticomuscular coherence. Neuroimage 33, 618–627. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, J. R., Amjad, A. M., Breeze, P., Brillinger, D. R., and Halliday, D. M. (1989). The Fourier approach to the identification of functional coupling between neuronal spike trains. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol. 53, 1–31. doi: 10.1016/0079-6107(89)90004-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salenius, S., Portin, K., Kajola, M., Salmelin, R., and Hari, R. (1997). Cortical control of human motoneuron firing during isometric contraction. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 3401–3405.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sanes, J. N., and Donoghue, J. P. (1993). Oscillations in local field potentials of the primate motor cortex during voluntary movement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 90, 4470–4474. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.10.4470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santello, M. (2014). “Synergistic control of hand muscles through common neural input,” in The Human Hand as an Inspiration for Robot Hand Development, eds R. Balasubramanian and V. J. Santos (Cham: Springer), 23–48.

Google Scholar

Schoffelen, J. M., Poort, J., Oostenveld, R., and Fries, P. (2011). Selective movement preparation is subserved by selective increases in corticomuscular gamma-band coherence. J. Neurosci. 31, 6750–6758. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4882-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, S. H. (2003). The role of primary motor cortex in goal-directed movements: insights from neurophysiological studies on non-human primates. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 671–677. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2003.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, L., and Alexander, G. E. (1997). Neural correlates of a spatial sensory-to-motor transformation in primary motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 1171–1194.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Slepian, D. (1978). Prolate spheroidal wave functions, Fourier analysis, and uncertainty—V: the discrete case. Bell Syst. Techn. J. 57, 1371–1430. doi: 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1978.tb02104.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stancák, A. Jr., and Pfurtscheller, G. (1996). Event-related desynchronisation of central beta-rhythms during brisk and slow self-paced finger movements of dominant and nondominant hand. Cogn. Brain Res. 4, 171–183. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(96)00031-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valero-Cuevas, F. J. (2000). Predictive modulation of muscle coordination pattern magnitude scales fingertip force magnitude over the voluntary range. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 1469–1479.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Valero-Cuevas, F. J., Smaby, N., Venkadesan, M., Peterson, M., and Wright, T. (2003). The strength-dexterity test as a measure of dynamic pinch performance. J. Biomech. 36, 265–270. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(02)00340-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venkadesan, M., Guckenheimer, J., and Valero-Cuevas, F. J. (2007). Manipulating the edge of instability. J. Biomech. 40, 1653–1661. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2007.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, B. (2013). A very basic tutorial for performing linear mixed effects analyses arXiv preprint arXiv:1308.5499.

Google Scholar

Witham, C. L., Riddle, C. N., Baker, M. R., and Baker, S. N. (2011). Contributions of descending and ascending pathways to corticomuscular coherence in humans. J. Physiol. 589, 3789–3800. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.211045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witham, C. L., Wang, M., and Baker, S. N. (2010). Corticomuscular coherence between motor cortex, somatosensory areas and forearm muscles in the monkey. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:38. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witte, M., Patino, L., Andrykiewicz, A., Hepp-Reymond, M. C., and Kristeva, R. (2007). Modulation of human corticomuscular beta-range coherence with low-level static forces. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 3564–3570. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05942.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: coherence, dexterity, beta-band, neuromuscular, synergy, EMG, EEG

Citation: Reyes A, Laine CM, Kutch JJ and Valero-Cuevas FJ (2017) Beta Band Corticomuscular Drive Reflects Muscle Coordination Strategies. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 11:17. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2017.00017

Received: 29 December 2016; Accepted: 10 March 2017;
Published: 04 April 2017.

Edited by:

Sliman J. Bensmaia, University of Chicago, USA

Reviewed by:

Gordon M. G. Shepherd, Northwestern University, USA
Aaron J. Suminski, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA

Copyright © 2017 Reyes, Laine, Kutch and Valero-Cuevas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Francisco J. Valero-Cuevas, valero@usc.edu

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Download