Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Comput. Neurosci., 20 September 2019
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2019.00063

Variant and Invariant Spatiotemporal Structures in Kinematic Coordination to Regulate Speed During Walking and Running

  • 1Department of Mechanical and Systems Engineering, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Doshisha University, Kyoto, Japan
  • 2Department of Aeronautics and Astronautics, Graduate School of Engineering, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
  • 3Department of Mechanical Engineering and Intelligent Systems, Graduate School of Informatics and Engineering, The University of Electro-Communications, Tokyo, Japan

Humans walk, run, and change their speed in accordance with circumstances. These gaits are rhythmic motions generated by multi-articulated movements, which have specific spatiotemporal patterns. The kinematic characteristics depend on the gait and speed. In this study, we focused on the kinematic coordination of locomotor behavior to clarify the underlying mechanism for the effect of speed on the spatiotemporal kinematic patterns for each gait. In particular, we used seven elevation angles for the whole-body motion and separated the measured data into different phases depending on the foot-contact condition, that is, single-stance phase, double-stance phase, and flight phase, which have different physical constraints during locomotion. We extracted the spatiotemporal kinematic coordination patterns with singular value decomposition and investigated the effect of speed on the coordination patterns. Our results showed that most of the whole-body motion could be explained by only two sets of temporal and spatial coordination patterns in each phase. Furthermore, the temporal coordination patterns were invariant for different speeds, while the spatial coordination patterns varied. These findings will improve our understanding of human adaptation mechanisms to tune locomotor behavior for changing speed.

1. Introduction

Humans walk, run, and change their speed at will depending on their circumstances. These gaits are rhythmic motions generated by multi-articulated movements that have specific spatiotemporal patterns. The kinematic characteristics of locomotor behavior vary to produce different gaits and speeds. For example, the stance leg during walking is almost straight, with slight knee flexion, and it rotates around the foot-contact point like an inverted pendulum (Lee and Farley, 1998). In contrast, the stance leg during running behaves like a spring, with knee bending (Cavagna et al., 1976). Many kinematic parameters, such as stride length and gait cycle, also change at different gaits and speeds (Nilsson et al., 1985). Such kinematic variations are locomotor outcomes of the complicated musculoskeletal system controlled by the central nervous system.

Despite large differences in locomotor behavior, there are common kinematic characteristics, which were highlighted by extracting low-dimensional structures from measured kinematics data. For example, when three elevation angles of the thigh, shank, and foot of one leg in the sagittal plane were plotted for one gait cycle, the trajectory lay close to a plane, which has been referred to as the planar law (Borghese et al., 1996; Ivanenko et al., 2007). This low-dimensional structure explains the intersegmental coordination during locomotion. In addition, the orientation of the plane constraining the trajectory changes with changes in gait and speed, suggesting that humans adapt to the speed change by tuning the intersegmental coordination (Bianchi et al., 1998; Ivanenko et al., 2007, 2008).

Human locomotion, including walking and running, is generated by moving the whole body using the legs. The legs have different roles depending on the foot-contact condition. In particular, the stance leg supports the body weight and produces propulsive and decelerative forces against the ground. In contrast, the swing leg swings the foot forward in the air and determines the stride length. Our previous work (Funato et al., 2010) investigated how spatiotemporal patterns of walking kinematics vary according to the speed by focusing on the kinematic coordination depending on the foot-contact condition. Specifically, we used seven elevation angles for the trunk and thighs, shanks, and feet of both legs, and extracted the kinematic coordination patterns using singular value decomposition for the single-stance (SS) and double-stance (DS) phases independently. As a result, a large portion of the seven angles was reproduced by the average posture and only two sets of spatial (intersegmental) and temporal coordination patterns for both phases. Furthermore, the temporal coordination patterns exhibited almost no change, while the average posture and spatial coordination patterns changed with speed.

In this study, we extended the previous analysis to running. While walking has a DS phase, running has a flight (FL) phase, in which both feet are in the air. We investigated the seven elevation angles for running for the SS and FL phases separately and examined how the kinematic coordination patterns changed with speed. We analyzed measured data for both walking and running and compared the speed effect on the spatiotemporal kinematic coordination patterns between the gaits.

2. Methods

2.1. Experiments

The study subjects were eight healthy men [age: 22–24 years, weight: 64.7 ± 6.6 kg (mean ± standard deviation), height: 1.75 ± 0.07 m]. They walked at 3, 4, and 5 km/h or ran at 9, 13, and 17 km/h on a treadmill (ITR3017, Bertec Corp.). A motion capture system (MAC3D Digital RealTime System, NAC Image Technology, Inc.) was used to measure the motion. Reflective markers were attached to the subjects' skin over several body landmarks on both the left and right sides: head, upper limit of the acromion, greater trochanter, lateral condyle of the knee, lateral malleolus, second metatarsal head, and heel. The sampling frequency was 500 Hz. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Doshisha University. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after the procedures had been fully explained.

2.2. Analysis

We used the measured data for 40 walking steps and 75 running steps for each subject and each speed. From the measured time-series data, we calculated the angles for seven segments (trunk and right and left feet, shanks, and thighs) defined on the sagittal plane: θ(t)=[θfootR(t)θshankR(t)θthighR(t)θtrunk(t)θthighL(t)θshankL(t)θfootL(t)]T7. These angles were defined as elevation angles (Figure 1A) based on the assumption that elevation angles behave more stereotypically than relative angles (Borghese et al., 1996; Ivanenko et al., 2007).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Definition of elevation angles (positive for anticlockwise direction; A). Time series of elevation angles for one gait cycle composed of double-stance phases (DS1, DS2) and single-stance phases (SS1, SS2) for walking (B) and single-stance phases (SS1, SS2) and flight phases (FL1, FL2) for running (C). These data are averages at 3 km/h for walking and 9 km/h for running by subject IG. FC-R, FC-L, FO-R, and FO-L indicate right foot contact, left foot contact, right foot off, and left foot off, respectively.

We separated the measured data into DS and SS phases for walking and FL and SS phases for running. These phases appear twice in each gait cycle, as shown in Figure 1: DS1 (starting with right foot contact), SS1 (supported on right leg), DS2 (starting with left foot contact), and SS2 (supported on left leg) for walking and SS1 (supported on right leg), FL1 (starting with right foot off), SS2 (supported on left leg), and FL2 (starting with left foot off) for running. Because SS1 and SS2, DS1 and DS2, and FL1 and FL2 are identical except for the difference in right or left, we used only DS1 and SS1 for walking and SS1 and FL1 for running. The number of data points in each phase was standardized at 100 (t = t1, …, t100).

We used Θ=[θ(t1)θ(t100)]7×100 for each phase by arranging the time-series data of the elevation angles θ(t) from t1 to t100. From singular value decomposition, we obtained

Θ=Θ0+i=17zi(λivi)T    (1)

where Θ07×100 was constructed by repeating the temporal average of θ(t), θ07, for 100 samplings; λi ∈ ℝ, zi7, and vi100 (i = 1, …, 7) are the singular value and the left and right singular vectors of Θ − Θ0, respectively; zi and λivi explain the intersegmental and temporal coordination patterns, respectively; θ0 is the average posture and can be decomposed into the amplitude |θ0| and normalized vector θ^0 (= θ0/|θ0|); and θ^0 explains the intersegmental pattern of the average posture.

To investigate the speed effect on the kinematic coordination, we used statistical methods to determine similarity for the extracted coordination patterns by singular value decomposition. To find any significant differences, we applied a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with factors speed and subject to the temporal coordination pattern λivi, intersegmental patterns zi and θ^0, and normal vector of the constraint planes spanned by z1 and z2, and applied a 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with factors speed and subject to the magnitude |θ0| of the average posture, where λivi was converted to a vector with 25 elements. Because zi and θ^0 are significant, we applied a 2-way ANOVA to each segment further, where the significance levels are based on Bonferroni correction. In addition, to compare the speed effect on the intersegmental pattern zi and average posture θ0 between the DS and SS phases for walking and between the SS and FL phases for running, we used a paired t-test to the cosine similarity of the normal vectors of the constraint planes, to the cosine similarity of θ^0, and to the difference in |θ0| between 3 and 5 km/h for walking and between 9 and 17 km/h for running. Furthermore, to determine significant differences in the SS phase between walking and running, we applied a MANOVA with factors gait and subject to the temporal coordination patterns λivi, intersegmental coordination patterns zi, and average posture intersegmental pattern θ^0 and applied a 2-way ANOVA with factors gait and subject to the average posture amplitude |θ0|. Because zi and θ^0 are significant, we also applied a 2-way ANOVA to each segment further, where the significance levels are based on Bonferroni correction. In each MANOVA and ANOVA, we confirmed that the interactions are not significant.

3. Results

3.1. Kinematic Coordination Patterns During Walking and Running

The kinematic coordination at 3 km/h for walking and 9 km/h for running for eight subjects was analyzed using singular value decomposition. Table 1 shows the singular value λi and the cumulative proportion Λi (=j=1iλj2/j=17λj2). Although the SS phase for running had a slightly smaller cumulative proportion than the other phases, most of the cumulative proportion exceeded 99% by the second coordination pattern. This indicates that the whole-body movement in each phase can be represented by only two sets of the intersegmental coordination patterns z1, z2 and temporal coordination patterns λ1v1, λ2v2 (Figure 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Singular value λi and cumulative proportion Λi for eight subjects for each phase at 3 km/h for walking and 9 km/h for running.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Intersegmental coordination pattern zi and temporal coordination pattern λivi obtained by singular value decomposition for walking (A) and running (B). These data show average and standard deviation (error bar for zi and gray region for λivi, too small to be visible) at 3 km/h for walking and 9 km/h for running by subject IG. Tr, Th, Sh, and Fo indicate trunk, thigh, shank, and foot, respectively.

The extracted intersegmental coordination patterns z1, z2 are a subspace of the seven-dimensional space of θ(t). The whole-body movement lies close to the subspace in each phase and the subspace was switched between the DS and SS phases for walking and the SS and FL phases for running, depending on the foot-contact condition. To clarify this structure, we applied singular value decomposition to the data for one-half of a gait cycle by combining the DS and SS phases (DS-SS) for walking and the SS and FL phases (SS-FL) for running. Table 2 shows the singular value λi and the cumulative proportion Λi. The cumulative proportion for three elements exceeded 99% in both gaits for every subject This indicates that the whole-body movement for the half-gait cycle is included in the subspace spanned by three intersegmental coordination patterns, which we call ẑ1, ẑ2, and ẑ3. Figure 3 shows the whole-body movement and subspaces for each phase in the three-dimensional subspaces spanned by ẑ1, ẑ2, and ẑ3, illustrated in the same way as in Funato et al. (2010). The coordination patterns z1, z2 span a plane for each phase in the three-dimensional subspace. The start point of the DS phase and the end point of the SS phase appear at different positions (Figure 3A). However, when the left-right symmetry of the leg movements is assumed, these two points can be regarded as identical and the whole-body movement is represented by a closed-loop trajectory on these planes. The same is true for the SS and FL phases (Figure 3B).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Singular value λi and cumulative proportion Λi of eight subjects for half-gait cycle at 3 km/h for walking and 9 km/h for running.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Whole-body movement and constraint planes of each phase in three-dimensional subspace for walking (A) and running (B). Axes are given by three intersegmental coordination patterns ẑ1, ẑ2, and ẑ3 calculated from data of half-gait cycles DS-SS for walking and SS-FL for running. The planes are spanned by intersegmental coordination patterns z1 and z2 of each phase. These data were obtained from average at 3 km/h for walking and 9 km/h for running by subject IG. Edge points of the whole-body movement trajectory can be regarded as identical under left-right symmetry of leg movements, and the trajectory has a closed loop on these planes.

3.2. Speed Effect on Kinematic Coordination

To clarify how the kinematic coordination depends on speed, we investigated the intersegmental coordination patterns z1, z2; temporal coordination patterns λ1v1, λ2v2; and average posture θ0 for different speeds in each phase.

Figure 4 shows the temporal coordination patterns λ1v1, λ2v2 averaged across subjects for each phase at 3, 4, and 5 km/h for walking and 9, 13, and 17 km/h for running. These patterns had high similarity in each phase. To find any differences, we applied a MANOVA (Pillai's trace) with the factors of subject and speed to the discretized temporal coordination patterns, as shown in Table 3. This result shows no significant effect of speed on the temporal patterns in each phase.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Temporal coordination pattern λivi for each phase at 3, 4, and 5 km/h for walking (A) and 9, 13, and 17 km/h for running (B). These are averaged data across subjects.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. P-values of MANOVA for temporal coordination pattern λivi, MANOVA for intersegmental coordination pattern zi, and 2-way ANOVA for elements of zi.

Figure 5 shows the intersegmental coordination patterns z1, z2 averaged across subjects for each phase at 3, 4, and 5 km/h for walking and 9, 13, and 17 km/h for running. These patterns also had high similarity in each phase. We applied a MANOVA (Pillai's trace) with the factors subject and speed, as shown in Table 3. Despite high similarity in appearance, this result shows that the intersegmental patterns exhibited a statistically significant effect of speed at the 1% level in each phase. Because the MANOVA was significant, we applied a 2-way ANOVA with the factors subject and speed to each segment, as shown in Table 3. The result shows that almost all speed effects are significant at 1% level in each phase and each segment. These suggest that the orientation of the constraint planes in Figure 3 changes for the speed, as obtained in our previous work for walking (Funato et al., 2010). To clarify the speed effect on the plane orientation, we calculated the normal vector of z1 and z2 in each phase in the three-dimensional subspaces spanned by ẑ1, ẑ2, and ẑ3 at 3 km/h for walking and 9 km/h for running in the same way in Funato et al. (2010). We applied a MANOVA (Pillai's trace) with the factors subject and speed to the normal vectors and the results showed p < 0.01 for all phases. In addition, we further investigated the difference of the changes in the plane orientation between the DS and SS phases for walking and between the SS and FL phases for running. Specifically, we applied a paired t-test to the cosine similarity of the normal vectors between 3 and 5 km/h for walking and between 9 and 17 km/h for running to determine whether the SS and DS phases have different cosine similarities for walking and whether the SS and FL phases have different cosine similarities for running, as shown in Table 4. Statistically significant differences were found at the 1% level for both walking and running. These results indicate that the constraint plane orientation changes for the speed change and that different phases have different changes in the plane orientation.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Intersegmental coordination pattern zi for each phase at 3, 4, and 5 km/h for walking (A) and 9, 13, and 17 km/h for running (B). These patterns were obtained from average and standard deviation across subjects. Tr, Th, Sh, and Fo indicate trunk, thigh, shank, and foot, respectively.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Cosine similarity of normal vectors of constraint planes and p-values of paired t-test.

To investigate the speed effect on the average posture, we examined the amplitude |θ0| and intersegmental pattern θ^0. Figure 6 shows the amplitudes |θ0|, patterns θ^0, and stick pictures averaged across subjects for each phase at 3, 4, and 5 km/h for walking and 9, 13, and 17 km/h for running. Although the amplitudes clearly increased as the speed increased, these also showed high similarity in each phase. We applied a 2-way ANOVA with the factors subject and speed for |θ0| and a MANOVA (Pillai's trace) with the factors subject and speed for θ^0, as shown in Table 5. Despite high similarity in appearance, statistically significant differences were found at the 1% level in both |θ0| and θ^0 for all phases. Because the MANOVA was significant for θ^0, we applied a 2-way ANOVA with the factors subject and speed to each segment, as shown in Table 5. The result shows that almost all speed effects are significant at 1% level in each phase and each segment. To further clarify the speed effect on the average posture, we compared the changes in |θ0| and θ^0 between the DS and SS phases for walking and between the SS and FL phases for running. For |θ0|, we applied a paired t-test to the difference in |θ0| between 3 and 5 km/h for walking and between 9 and 17 km/h for running to determine whether the DS and SS phases have different changes for walking and whether the SS and FL phases have different changes for running, as shown in Table 6. Statistically significant differences were found at the 1% level for both walking and running. For θ^0, we applied a paired t-test to the cosine similarity of θ^0 at 3 and 5 km/h for walking and 9 and 17 km/h for running to determine whether the DS and SS phases have different cosine similarities for walking and the SS and FL phases have different cosine similarities for running, as shown in Table 6. Statistically significant differences were found at the 1% level for both walking and running. These results indicate that both the amplitude and intersegmental pattern of the average posture change for the speed changes and that different phases have different changes in the average posture characteristics.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Amplitude |θ0| of average posture for each phase at 3, 4, and 5 km/h for walking (A) and 9, 13, and 17 km/h for running (B). Intersegmental pattern θ^0 of average posture for each phase at 3, 4, and 5 km/h for walking (C) and 9, 13, and 17 km/h for running (D). Tr, Th, Sh, and Fo indicate trunk, thigh, shank, and foot, respectively. Stick picture of average posture for each phase at 3, 4, and 5 km/h for walking (E) and 9, 13, and 17 km/h for running (F). These data were obtained from average and standard deviation across subjects.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. P-values of 2-way ANOVA for average posture amplitude |θ0|, MANOVA for average posture intersegmental pattern θ^0, and 2-way ANOVA for elements of θ^0.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Difference of average posture amplitude |θ0|, cosine similarity of average posture intersegmental pattern θ^0, and p-values of paired t-test.

3.3. Comparison Between Walking and Running

Because both walking and running have the SS phase, we compared the kinematic coordination between 3 km/h for walking and 9 km/h for running. We applied a MANOVA (Pillai's trace) for the temporal coordination patterns λ1v1, λ2v2; intersegmental coordination patterns z1, z2; and average posture intersegmental pattern θ^0 and applied a paired t-test for the average posture amplitude |θ0|, as shown in Table 7. Although the temporal coordination patterns had no apparent difference between the gaits, the others had statistically significant differences at the 1% level. Because the MANOVA was significant for θ^0, we applied a 2-way ANOVA with the factors subject and gait to each segment, as shown in Table 7. The result shows that almost all gait effects are significant at 1% level in each phase and each segment.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. P-values of MANOVA for temporal coordination pattern λivi, intersegmental coordination pattern zi, and average posture intersegmental pattern θ^0 and 2-way ANOVA for elements of zi, θ^0, and average posture amplitude |θ0| to determine significant differences in the SS phase between walking and running.

4. Discussion

In this study, we analyzed the whole-body movement using measurements taken during walking and running. In particular, the time series of seven elevation angles were decomposed by singular value decomposition after being separated into the DS and SS phases for walking and SS and FL phases for running (Figure 1). The whole-body movement was revealed to be composed of the average posture and only two sets of principal intersegmental and temporal coordination patterns irrespective of the phase and gait (Table 1). We investigated the relationship between the coordination patterns and gait speed to clarify the underlying mechanism for adapting the whole-body movement to the speed change at each phase.

Previous works (Borghese et al., 1996; Ivanenko et al., 2008) have shown that three elevation angles for one leg in one gait cycle can be described by a closed loop on a plane. Such planar covariation of the elevation angles held for various gaits, such as running (Hicheur et al., 2006; Ivanenko et al., 2007), curved walking (Courtine and Schieppati, 2004), backward walking (Grasso et al., 1998; Hicheur et al., 2006), walking on inclined surfaces (Noble and Prentice, 2008), walking with bent or erect posture (Grasso et al., 2000), stepping over an obstacle (Ivanenko et al., 2005b; Maclellan and McFadyen, 2010), walking with body weight unloading (Ivanenko et al., 2002), and walking on a slippery surface (Cappellini et al., 2010), which suggests an invariant characteristic in locomotion. In addition, this characteristic appeared in toddlers (Cheron et al., 2001; Ivanenko et al., 2004, 2005a; Dominici et al., 2007, 2010; Hallemans and Aerts, 2009; Cappellini et al., 2016); neonates (Dominici et al., 2011); gait disorders (Grasso et al., 2004; Laroche et al., 2007; Leurs et al., 2012; Martino et al., 2014; Cappellini et al., 2016; Ishikawa et al., 2017; Wallard et al., 2018); and also various animals (Catavitello et al., 2018), including cats (Poppele and Bosco, 2003), dogs (Catavitello et al., 2015), monkeys (Courtine et al., 2005; Ogihara et al., 2012), and birds (Ogihara et al., 2014). Investigating the coordination structures has provided useful insights for adaptation mechanisms in locomotion. To reveal a more detailed structure of kinematic coordination during human walking and running, we used seven angles of the whole body, including the trunk, and separated the measured data depending on the foot-contact condition. Because relative joint angles are not stereotypical across subjects and are more variable than elevation angles, the planarity of the joint angles is weaker for the analysis of three angles of one leg (Borghese et al., 1996; Ivanenko et al., 2007). Extracting the low-dimensional structure depends on the coordinate system (Yamasaki et al., 2013). We used the elevation angles for the analysis and our results showed that most of the cumulative proportion exceeded 99% by the second coordination pattern (Table 1), which indicates that we successfully extracted the low-dimensional structure from the seven angles. Specifically, the seven angles can be described by a closed-loop trajectory on two different constraint planes under the condition of left-right symmetry (Figure 3). The spatial nature was characterized by the location and orientation of the planes, and the temporal nature by the trajectory on the planes. The location and orientation were determined by the average posture θ0 and intersegmental coordination patterns z1, z2, and the trajectory by the temporal coordination patterns λ1v1, λ2v2.

The extracted temporal coordination patterns λ1v1, λ2v2 showed no apparent effect of the speed condition irrespective of the phase and gait (Table 3). This implies that the shape of the trajectory remained on the constraint planes. Such temporal invariance has been observed in curved walking when compared with straight-ahead walking (Courtine and Schieppati, 2004). In contrast, the extracted intersegmental coordination patterns z1, z2 showed apparent variance with the gait speed in each phase (Table 3). Previous works (Bianchi et al., 1998; Ivanenko et al., 2007, 2008) have shown that the orientation of the constraint plane for three elevation angles of one leg varies for the gait and speed. The orientation of the constraint planes of the seven elevation angles for the whole body also changes for the gait and speed. Furthermore, the DS phase for walking and FL phase for running had larger changes in the plane orientation for speed than did the SS phase [0.97 ± 0.01 (95%CI) and 1.00 ± 0.00 (95%CI) for the DS and SS phases, respectively, of walking and 0.97 ± 0.00 (95%CI) and 0.99 ± 0.00 (95%CI) for the FL and SS phases, respectively, of running] (Table 4). The average posture amplitude |θ0| and intersegmental pattern θ^0 also showed apparent speed effects in each phase (Table 5). From a comparison of these characteristics between the DS and SS phases for walking and SS and FL phases for running, the DS phase has larger changes in |θ0| and θ^0 than the SS phase for walking [0.25 ± 0.01 (95%CI) and 0.05 ± 0.01 (95%CI) for the DS and SS phases, respectively, in |θ0| and 0.98 ± 0.00 (95%CI) and 0.99 ± 0.00 (95%CI) for the DS and SS phases, respectively, in θ^0], similar to the orientation of the constraint planes (Table 6). In contrast, while the SS phase has a larger change in |θ0| than the FL phase for running [0.77 ± 0.03 (95%CI) and 0.63 ± 0.01 (95%CI) for the SS and FL phases, respectively], the FL phase has a larger change in θ^0 than the SS phase [0.988 ± 0.001 (95%CI) and 0.992 ± 0.001 (95%CI) for the FL and SS phases, respectively]. These results suggest that, to change speed, humans tune their locomotor kinematics largely in the gait-specific phases.

A comparison of the kinematic coordination patterns in the SS phase between walking and running revealed that while the temporal coordination patterns λ1v1, λ2v2 were similar, the intersegmental coordination patterns z1, z2 and the average posture amplitude |θ0| and pattern θ^0 differed (Table 7). In particular, the amplitude |θ0| had a large difference and was larger for running than for walking [e.g., 0.86 ± 0.01 (95%CI) at 3 km/h for walking and 1.70 ± 0.03 (95%CI) at 9 km/h for running], as shown in Figures 6A,B. This suggests that while walking uses an erect posture, running uses a bent posture. In addition to this difference, the foot of the stance leg and the thigh and shank of the swing leg in z1 and the thigh and shank of the stance leg in z2 had clearly different contributions between the gaits (Figure 5). Specifically, the foot had a larger contribution for running in the stance leg of z1. In the swing leg of z1, while the shank had a larger contribution than the thigh for walking, the shank and thigh had similar contributions for running. In the stance leg of z2, while the thigh had a larger contribution for walking, the shank had a larger contribution for running. While the first intersegmental coordination pattern z1 mainly contributed to the limb axis orientation, the second intersegmental coordination pattern z2 contributed to the limb axis length. The foot movement of the stance leg in running is larger than that in walking and contributes to the limb axis rotation. For the swing leg, while the thigh movement is larger than the shank movement for walking, they are comparable for running. From z2(λ2v2)T, this induced knee extension and then flexion sequentially for walking, which corresponds to the last half of the movement of the double-knee action. In contrast, for running, this induced sequential knee flexion and extension, which corresponds to the spring-like knee bending. These differences reflect different movements between the gaits.

The central pattern generator (CPG) in the spinal cord is largely responsible for adaptive motor control in locomotion (Orlovsky et al., 1999). It has been suggested that the CPG consists of hierarchical networks that include the rhythm generator (RG) and pattern formation (PF) networks (Burke et al., 2001; Lafreniere-Roula and McCrea, 2005; Rybak et al., 2006a,b). The RG network generates the locomotion rhythm in response to sensory feedback, while the PF network shapes the rhythm into spatiotemporal motor patterns through interneurons. The CPG separately controls the spatial and temporal patterns in the RG and PF networks, respectively. In this study, we investigated the adaptation mechanism that produces different speeds in human walking and running by extracting low-dimensional structures from measured kinematics data with singular value decomposition to study the kinematic spatiotemporal coordination patterns. The singular value decomposition divides the data into spatial and temporal patterns on an orthonormal basis, which is useful for elucidating the underlying mechanism for such spatiotemporal patterns. In particular, our results revealed invariant features in the temporal coordination patterns and variant features in the spatial coordination patterns, which show different control strategies for the spatial and temporal patterns in the CPG. In addition to the kinematics data, the analysis of electromyographic data, which reflects motor control strategies more directly than kinematics data, has also shown low-dimensional structures for different walking and running speeds (Cappellini et al., 2006; Hagio et al., 2015; Yokoyama et al., 2016, 2017). These results suggest coordinated motor control patterns and provided useful insights for the adaptation mechanisms in locomotion. Because human locomotion is generated through the control of a redundant musculoskeletal system, the analysis of the low-dimensional coordination structures is useful. In addition to the analysis of measured data, modeling approaches also provided useful insights for the mechanism that forms the low-dimensional structures from a mathematical viewpoint (Jo and Massaquoi, 2007; Barliya et al., 2009; Neptune et al., 2009; Aoi et al., 2010, 2019; Aoi and Funato, 2016). We would like to integrate the measured data analysis and modeling approach to further clarify the adaptation mechanism for locomotion in the future.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Doshisha University. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after the procedures had been fully explained.

Author Contributions

HO, SA, and KT developed the study design. HO analyzed the data in consultation with SA, TF, NT, and KT. HO and SA wrote the manuscript. All the authors reviewed and approved it.

Funding

This study was supported in part by JSPS KAKENHI Grant numbers JP15KT0015, JP26120006, and JP17H04914.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Aoi, S., and Funato, T. (2016). Neuromusculoskeletal models based on the muscle synergy hypothesis for the investigation of adaptive motor control in locomotion via sensory-motor coordination. Neurosci. Res. 104, 88–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2015.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aoi, S., Ogihara, N., Funato, T., Sugimoto, Y., and Tsuchiya, K. (2010). Evaluating functional roles of phase resetting in generation of adaptive human bipedal walking with a physiologically based model of the spinal pattern generator. Biol. Cybern. 102, 373–387. doi: 10.1007/s00422-010-0373-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aoi, S., Ohashi, T., Bamba, R., Fujiki, S., Tamura, D., Funato, T., et al. (2019). Neuromusculoskeletal model that walks and runs across a speed range with a few motor control parameter changes based on the muscle synergy hypothesis. Sci. Rep. 9:369. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-37460-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barliya, A., Omlor, L., Giese, M. A., and Flash, T. (2009). An analytical formulation of the law of intersegmental coordination during human locomotion. Exp. Brain Res. 193, 371–385. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1633-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, L., Angelini, D., Orani, G. P., and Lacquaniti, F. (1998). Kinematic coordination in human gait: relation to mechanical energy cost. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 2155–2170. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.4.2155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghese, N. A., Bianchi, L., and Lacquaniti, F. (1996). Kinematic determinants of human locomotion. J. Physiol. 494, 863–879. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, R. E., Degtyarenko, A. M., and Simon, E. S. (2001). Patterns of locomotor drive to motoneurons and last-order interneurons: Clues to the structure of the CPG. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 447–462. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.1.447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappellini, G., Ivanenko, Y. P., Dominici, N., Poppele, R. E., and Lacquaniti, F. (2010). Motor patterns during walking on a slippery walkway. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 746–760. doi: 10.1152/jn.00499.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappellini, G., Ivanenko, Y. P., Martino, G., MacLellan, M. J., Sacco, A., Morelli, D., et al. (2016). Immature spinal locomotor output in children with cerebral palsy. Front. Physiol. 7:478. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappellini, G., Ivanenko, Y. P., Poppele, R. E., and Lacquaniti, F. (2006). Motor patterns in human walking and running. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 3426–3437. doi: 10.1152/jn.00081.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catavitello, G., Ivanenko, Y., and Lacquaniti, F. (2018). A kinematic synergy for terrestrial locomotion shared by mammals and birds. eLife 7:e38190. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catavitello, G., Ivanenko, Y. P., and Lacquaniti, F. (2015). Planar covariation of hindlimb and forelimb elevation angles during terrestrial and aquatic locomotion of dogs. PLoS ONE 10:e0133936. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavagna, G. A., Thys, H., and Zamboni, A. (1976). The sources of external work in level walking and running. J. Physiol. 262, 639–657. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheron, G., Bouillot, E., Dan, B., Bengoetxea, A., Draye, J. P., and Lacquaniti, F. (2001). Development of a kinematic coordination pattern in toddler locomotion: planar covariation. Exp. Brain Res. 137, 455–466. doi: 10.1007/s002210000663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courtine, G., Roy, R. R., Hodgson, J., McKay, H., Raven, J., Zhong, H., et al. (2005). Kinematic and EMG determinants in quadrupedal locomotion of a non-human primate (Rhesus). J. Neurophysiol. 93, 3127–3145. doi: 10.1152/jn.01073.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courtine, G., and Schieppati, M. (2004). Tuning of a basic coordination pattern constructs straight-ahead and curved walking in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 1524–1535. doi: 10.1152/jn.00817.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dominici, N., Ivanenko, Y. P., Cappellini, G., d'Avella, A., Mondì, V., Cicchese, M., et al. (2011). Locomotor primitives in newborn babies and their development. Science 334, 997–999. doi: 10.1126/science.1210617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dominici, N., Ivanenko, Y. P., Cappellini, G., Zampagni, M. L., and Lacquaniti, F. (2010). Kinematic strategies in newly walking toddlers stepping over different support surfaces. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 1673–1684. doi: 10.1152/jn.00945.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dominici, N., Ivanenko, Y. P., and Lacquaniti, F. (2007). Control of foot trajectory in walking toddlers: adaptation to load changes. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 2790–2801. doi: 10.1152/jn.00262.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funato, T., Aoi, S., Oshima, H., and Tsuchiya, K. (2010). Variant and invariant patterns embedded in human locomotion through whole body kinematic coordination. Exp. Brain Res. 205, 497–511. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2385-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grasso, R., Bianchi, L., and Lacquaniti, F. (1998). Motor patterns for human gait: backward versus forward locomotion. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 1868–1885. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.4.1868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grasso, R., Ivanenko, Y. P., Zago, M., Molinari, M., Scivoletto, G., Castellano, V., et al. (2004). Distributed plasticity of locomotor pattern generators in spinal cord injured patients. Brain 127, 1019–1034. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grasso, R., Zago, M., and Lacquaniti, F. (2000). Interactions between posture and locomotion: Motor patterns in humans walking with bent posture versus erect posture. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 288–300. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.1.288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagio, S., Fukuda, M., and Kouzaki, M. (2015). Identification of muscle synergies associated with gait transition in humans. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:48. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallemans, A., and Aerts, P. (2009). Effects of visual deprivation on intra-limb coordination during walking in children and adults. Exp. Brain Res. 198, 95–106. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1937-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hicheur, H., Terekhov, A.V., and Berthoz, A. (2006). Intersegmental coordination during human locomotion: does planar covariation of elevation angles reflect central constraints? J. Neurophysiol. 96, 1406–1419. doi: 10.1152/jn.00289.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa, Y., An, Q., Nakagawa, J., Oka, H., Yasui, T., Tojima, M., et al. (2017). Gait analysis of patients with knee osteoarthritis by using elevation angle: confirmation of the planar law and analysis of angular difference in the approximate plane. Adv. Robot. 31, 68–79. doi: 10.1080/01691864.2016.1229217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanenko, Y. P., Cappellini, G., Dominici, N., Poppele, R. E., and Lacquaniti, F. (2005b). Coordination of locomotion with voluntary movements in humans. J. Neurosci. 25, 7238–7253. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1327-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanenko, Y. P., Cappellini, G., Dominici, N., Poppele, R. E., and Lacquaniti, F. (2007). Modular control of limb movements during human locomotion. J. Neurosci. 27, 11149–11161. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2644-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanenko, Y. P., d'Avella, A., Poppele, R. E., and Lacquaniti, F. (2008). On the origin of planar covariation of elevation angles during human locomotion. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1890–1898. doi: 10.1152/jn.01308.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanenko, Y. P., Dominici, N., Cappellini, G., Dan, B., Cheron, G., and Lacquaniti, F. (2004). Development of pendulum mechanism and kinematic coordination from the first unsupported steps in toddlers. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 3797–3810. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanenko, Y. P., Dominici, N., Cappellini, G., and Lacquaniti, F. (2005a). Kinematics in newly walking toddlers does not depend upon postural stability. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 754–763. doi: 10.1152/jn.00088.2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanenko, Y. P., Grasso, R., Macellari, V., and Lacquaniti, F. (2002). Control of foot trajectory in human locomotion: Role of ground contact forces in simulated reduced gravity. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 3070–3089. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.87.6.3070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jo, S., and Massaquoi, S. G. (2007). A model of cerebrocerebello-spinomuscular interaction in the sagittal control of human walking. Biol. Cybern. 96, 279–307. doi: 10.1007/s00422-006-0126-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafreniere-Roula, M., and McCrea, D. A. (2005). Deletions of rhythmic motoneuron activity during fictive locomotion and scratch provide clues to the organization of the mammalian central pattern generator. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1120–1132. doi: 10.1152/jn.00216.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laroche, D., Ornetti, P., Thomas, E., Ballay, Y., Maillefert, J. F., and Pozzo, T. (2007). Kinematic adaptation of locomotor pattern in rheumatoid arthritis patients with forefoot impairment. Exp. Brain Res. 176, 85–97. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0597-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C. R., and Farley, C. (1998). Determinants of the center of mass trajectory in human walking and running. J. Exp. Biol. 201, 2935–2944.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Leurs, F., Bengoetxea, A., Cebolla, A. M., De Saedeleer, C., Dan, B., and Cheron, G. (2012). Planar covariation of elevation angles in prosthetic gait. Gait Posture 35, 647–652. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maclellan, M. J., and McFadyen, B. J. (2010). Segmental control for adaptive locomotor adjustments during obstacle clearance in healthy young adults. Exp. Brain Res. 202, 307–318. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2133-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martino, G., Ivanenko, Y. P., Serrao, M., Ranavolo, A., d'Avella, A., Draicchio, F., et al. (2014). Locomotor patterns in cerebellar ataxia. J. Neurophysiol. 112, 2810–2821. doi: 10.1152/jn.00275.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neptune, R. R., Clark, D. J., and Kautz, S. A. (2009). Modular control of human walking: a simulation study. J. Biomech. 42, 1282–1287. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2009.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson, J., Thorstensson, A., and Halbertsma, J. (1985). Changes in leg movements and muscle activity with speed of locomotion and mode of progression in humans. Acta. Physiol. Scand. 123, 457–475. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1985.tb07612.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, J. W., and Prentice, S. D. (2008). Intersegmental coordination while walking up inclined surfaces: age and ramp angle effects. Exp. Brain Res. 189, 249–255. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1464-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogihara, N., Kikuchi, T., Ishiguro, Y., Makishima, H., and Nakatsukasa, M. (2012). Planar covariation of limb elevation angles during bipedal walking in the Japanese macaque. J. R. Soc. Interface 9, 2181–2190. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2012.0026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogihara, N., Oku, T., Andrada, E., Blickhan, R., Nyakatura, J. A., and Fischer, M. S. (2014). Planar covariation of limb elevation angles during bipedal locomotion in common quails (Coturnix coturnix). J. Exp. Biol. 217, 3968–3973. doi: 10.1242/jeb.109355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orlovsky, G. N., Deliagina, T. G., and Grillner, S. (1999). Neuronal Control of Locomotion: From Mollusc to Man. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Poppele, R. E., and Bosco, G. (2003). Sophisticated spinal contributions to motor control. Trends Neurosci. 26, 269–276. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00073-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rybak, I. A., Shevtsova, N. A., Lafreniere-Roula, M., and McCrea, D. A. (2006a). Modelling spinal circuitry involved in locomotor pattern generation: insights from deletions during fictive locomotion. J. Physiol. 577, 617–639. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.118703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rybak, I. A., Stecina, K., Shevtsova, N. A., and McCrea, D. A. (2006b). Modelling spinal circuitry involved in locomotor pattern generation: insights from the effects of afferent stimulation. J. Physiol. 577, 641-658. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.118711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallard, L., Boulet, S., Cornu, O., Dubuc, J. E., Mahaudens, P., Postlethwaite, D., et al. (2018). Intersegmental kinematics coordination in unilateral peripheral and central origin: Effect on gait mechanism? Gait Posture 62, 124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamasaki, T., Nakadachi, S., and Xin, X. (2013). On coordinate systems for describing the planar law during human locomotion (in Japanese). Trans. SICE 49, 1172–1178. doi: 10.9746/sicetr.49.1172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yokoyama, H., Ogawa, T., Kawashima, N., Shinya, M., and Nakazawa, K. (2016). Distinct sets of locomotor modules control the speed and modes of human locomotion. Sci. Rep. 6:36275. doi: 10.1038/srep36275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yokoyama, H., Ogawa, T., Shinya, M., Kawashima, N., and Nakazawa, K. (2017). Speed dependency in α-motoneuron activity and locomotor modules in human locomotion: indirect evidence for phylogenetically conserved spinal circuits. Proc. R. Soc. B 284:20170290. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.0290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: walk, run, kinematic coordination, spatiotemporal pattern, speed effect, singular value decomposition

Citation: Oshima H, Aoi S, Funato T, Tsujiuchi N and Tsuchiya K (2019) Variant and Invariant Spatiotemporal Structures in Kinematic Coordination to Regulate Speed During Walking and Running. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 13:63. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2019.00063

Received: 12 March 2019; Accepted: 27 August 2019;
Published: 20 September 2019.

Edited by:

Petr Lansky, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic (ASCR), Czechia

Reviewed by:

Taishin Nomura, Osaka University, Japan
Joachim Hass, Central Institute for Mental Health, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Oshima, Aoi, Funato, Tsujiuchi and Tsuchiya. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shinya Aoi, shinya_aoi@kuaero.kyoto-u.ac.jp

Download