Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Comput. Neurosci., 02 October 2019
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2019.00070

Tonality Tunes the Statistical Characteristics in Music: Computational Approaches on Statistical Learning

  • Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany

Statistical learning is a learning mechanism based on transition probability in sequences such as music and language. Recent computational and neurophysiological studies suggest that the statistical learning contributes to production, action, and musical creativity as well as prediction and perception. The present study investigated how statistical structure interacts with tonalities in music based on various-order statistical models. To verify this in all 24 major and minor keys, the transition probabilities of the sequences containing the highest pitches in Bach's Well-Tempered Clavier, which is a collection of two series (No. 1 and No. 2) of preludes and fugues in all of the 24 major and minor keys, were calculated based on nth-order Markov models. The transition probabilities of each sequence were compared among tonalities (major and minor), two series (No. 1 and No. 2), and music types (prelude and fugue). The differences in statistical characteristics between major and minor keys were detected in lower- but not higher-order models. The results also showed that statistical knowledge in music might be modulated by tonalities and composition periods. Furthermore, the principal component analysis detected the shared components of related keys, suggesting that the tonalities modulate statistical characteristics in music. The present study may suggest that there are at least two types of statistical knowledge in music that are interdependent on and independent of tonality, respectively.

Introduction

Prediction and Production in the Statistical Learning

The brain is innately equipped with statistical learning (SL) machineries that model external phenomena as a dynamical system that encode the probability distributions. The SL is thought as an implicit process in which the brain automatically calculate transitional-probability (TP) distribution of sequential information such as music and language (Saffran et al., 1996; Cleeremans et al., 1998). Furthermore, based on the internalized statistical model, it can predict a future state and optimize action for achieving a given goal (Monroy et al., 2017a,c) to resolve the uncertainty of information (Friston, 2010). The SL has also be thought to contribute to the encoding of the complexity in the information (Hasson, 2017), and to acquisition of musical and linguistic knowledge including tonality (Daikoku et al., 2016) and syntax (Daikoku et al., 2017a). For example, an increasing volume of literature also demonstrates that SL and the knowledge associate with human's action (Zubicaray et al., 2013; Monroy et al., 2017a,b, 2018) and decision-making (Schwartenbeck et al., 2013; Friston et al., 2014, 2015; Pezzulo et al., 2015). For example, motor cortex activity contributes to SL of action words (Zubicaray et al., 2013). Furthermore, cerebellum and cerebral cortex partially share same network responsible for the interalized statistical model. That is, statistical knowledge formed in cerebral cortex may be sent to the cerebellum that is thought to play important roles in prediction of sequences (Lesage et al., 2012; Moberget et al., 2014), motor skill learning (Ito, 2008), habit learning (Friston et al., 2016), generalization or abstraction based on transitional probabilities (Shimizu et al., 2017), efficient performance in a learned context (Balsters et al., 2014). These findings may suggest that the internalized statistical model affects production of music (i.e., composition) (Daikoku, 2019a), the creativity (Wiggins, 2018), and individuality of artistic expression (Daikoku, 2018b) as well as the prediction and perception (Daikoku, 2019c). It is, however, unknown how the acquired statistical knowledge influences the production of music.

Statistical Learning Machinery in Musician

According to recent studies, musicians are better statistical learners than non-musicians (Francois and Schön, 2011; François et al., 2012; Hansen and Pearce, 2014; Przysinda et al., 2017; Elmer and Lutz, 2018). Furthermore, it is suggested that, through long-term musical training, musicians optimize the brain's probabilistic model of SL, and that the musically-optimized SL model allow the brain to precisely and efficiently predict tones during SL of another musical and auditory sequences (Francois and Schön, 2011; Kim et al., 2011; Hansen and Pearce, 2014; Przysinda et al., 2017). Recent computational studies also suggested that, from early to late periods in the composer's lifetime, the transitional probabilities of familiar phrase in each piece of music were gradually decreased (Daikoku, 2018d, 2019a). These findings were prominent in higher-, rather than lower-order SL models. These studies suggest that the higher-, rather than lower-, order statistical knowledge (Daikoku, 2018a) may be susceptible to long-term experience that modulates brain's SL model (Hansen and Pearce, 2014). Furthermore, computational studies on improvisation music suggested that lower-order SL models represented general characteristics shared among musicians, whereas higher-order SL models detected specific characteristics unique to each musician (Daikoku, 2018b). In this context, it can be hypothesized that statistical models in music, which may reflect the composer's statistical knowledge, interact with the music-specific structures of tonality. To our knowledge, however, few studies have examined how TP in music interacts with the tonalities. To understand the characteristics of music from interdisciplinary aspects that include informatics, musicology, and psychology, it is important to verify the interaction between tonality and statistical structure in music, especially regarding strategies of musical composition.

Computational Modeling

The computational model and simulation have been used to understand SL systems (e.g., Pearce and Wiggins, 2012; Rohrmeier and Rebuschat, 2012; Daikoku, 2018a, 2019b; Wiggins, 2018). Particularly, the prediction and production of SL is partially supported by chunking hypothesis that learning is based on extracting, storing, and combining small chunks. For example, information-theoretical models including Markovian processes have been applied to neurophysiological studies of SL in human brain as well as computational simulation (Pearce et al., 2010; Pearce and Wiggins, 2012; Daikoku et al., 2014, 2015, 2017b, 2018; Yumoto and Daikoku, 2016, 2018; Daikoku and Yumoto, 2017, 2019; Daikoku, 2018c). These neurophysiological experiments showed consistent evidence: neural activities for stimuli with high information content (i.e., low probability) are larger than those with low information content (i.e., high probability). This neural phenomenon is in agreement with a Bayesian hypothesis in theoretical neurobiology that the brain encodes probabilities (beliefs) about the causes of sensory data, and that these beliefs are updated in response to new sensory evidence based on Bayesian inference (Kersten et al., 2004; Knill and Pouget, 2004; Doya et al., 2007; Friston, 2010; O'Reilly et al., 2012; Parr and Friston, 2018; Parr et al., 2018). That is, information-theoretical computational models including Markovian processes can capture a variety of neurophysiological phenomena on prediction, chunk formation, action, and production in the framework of SL theory.

The Aim of the Present Study

This study aimed to examine how the statistical structure interacts with tonality. To verify the statistical relationships in all the keys of Western classical music (Figure 1), the TPs of the sequences containing the highest pitches in Bach's Well-Tempered Clavier, BWV 846–893, which is a collection of two series (No. 1 and No. 2) of preludes and fugues in all of the 24 major and minor keys (Figure 1), were calculated using six different orders of Markov or n-gram models (i.e., first- to sixth-order Markov chains). Johann Sebastian Bach (1685–1750) was a composer during the Baroque period, who contributed to the development of musical tonality and the Western classical music theory (Rohrmeier and Cross, 2008). His music is often used to verify the probabilities of musical sequences (Rohrmeier and Cross, 2008; Kim et al., 2011). Particularly, to understand the relationships between tonality and statistical structure in music, the Well-Tempered Clavier may be one of the best mediums because it is a collection of music containing all 24 of the major and minor keys by a single composer in Western classical music. Thus, the statistics in each piece of music with a key in the Well-Tempered Clavier could be, in part, regarded as an approximation of the statistics of the entire range of Western classical music in each key. Thus, to extract statistical knowledge dependent on keys and tonalities, the present study verified the statistical structure in each key and tonality. The TPs of each sequence were compared among tonalities (major and minor), two series (No. 1 and No. 2), and music types (prelude and fugue). It was hypothesized that the statistical structure in music interacts with the tonality in music. If so, these findings suggest that music-specific knowledge of tonality modulates statistical knowledge in music.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Circle of fifths showing all 24 major and minor keys in Western classical music. A related key is one sharing many common tones with an original key, as opposed to a distant key. In music, such a key shares all, or all except one, pitches with a key with which it is being compared, and it is adjacent to it on the circle of fifths and its relative majors or minors. In a related key, a subdominant key has one more flat around the circle of fifths, and a relative key has the same key signature.

Methods

The Well-Tempered Clavier, BWV 846–893, which is a collection of two series (No. 1 and No. 2) of Preludes and Fugues in all 24 major and minor keys that was composed for solo keyboard by Johann Sebastian Bach, was used in the present study. Electronic scoring data of highest pitch were extracted from the Extensible Markup Language (XML) files. The highest pitches were chosen based on the following definitions (Figure 2): the grace notes were excluded, the pitches with slurs can be counted as one, and the highest pitches that can be played at a given point in time. According to SL theory, the brain automatically computes nth-order TPs of sequence. The transitional probability is a conditional probability of an event B given that the latest event A has occurred, written as P(B|A). The first- to six-order TPs of an event in SL were calculated from conditional probability (P) of an event en+1, given the preceding n events, based on the first- to six-order Markov models (n = 1–6):

P(en+1|en) = P(en+1en)P(en)

From the perspective of psychology, the formula can be interpreted as positing that the brain predicts a subsequent event en+1 based on the preceding events en in a sequence (for more details, see Daikoku, 2018c). In other words, learners expect the events with higher TPs based on the latest n states (i.e., nth-order), whereas they are likely to be surprised by events with lower TPs. Then, all of the pitch transitions were numbered so that the first pitch was 0 in each sequential pattern, and an increase or decrease in a semitone was 1 and −1 based on the first pitch, respectively (Figure 2). This reveals interval patterns but not pitch pattern, and eliminates the effects of the change of key on sequential patterns. This procedure was employed because the interpretation of the change of key depends on musicians, and it is difficult to define it in an objective manner. Thus, the results in this study may represent statistics based on relative, rather than absolute pitches. To verify the difference in statistical structures between prelude and fugue, the sequential patterns that appear in all pieces of music that were divided between prelude and fugue were only used in the present study (1st: 4). In the second- to sixth-order Markov chains, sequential patterns that appear in all music could not be detected. The empirical logit transformation was applied to normalize the TPs. The empirical logit transform allows data distribution to be normalized, and is used for a tolerence such that infinity is not returned when the argument is zero (0%) or one (100%). Thus, it is applicable when the TP values, which often show 0% and 100%, are analyzed. Then, we conducted repeated-measure analysis of variances (ANOVAs) based on a factor type (prelude vs. fugue), a factor tonality (major vs. minor), a factor number (No. 1 vs. No. 2), and a factor sequence (4 sequences) for the 1st-order Markov model. Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc tests were conducted for further analysis (Statistical significance levels: p < 0.05). It has been suggested that the TP distribution represents statistical characteristics in music (Daikoku, 2018b). Thus, using the nth-order TP distributions, the musical characteristic in each tonality was verified by correlation analysis. Furthermore, based on the result of correlation analysis, the TPs, in which there are a number of correlations of at least 0.3 (30), were analyzed by principal component analysis (PCA). The criteria of eigenvalue were set over 1. The first three components (i.e., the first to third highest cumulative contribution ratios) were adopted in the present study. The present study focus on the values of “loadings.” The loading has generally been understood as the weights for each original variable when calculating the principal component. The representative phrases of sequential patterns with mean highest and lowest probabilities were decoded as musical scores (Figure 2). The criterion of the eigenvalue was set over 1 (Statistical significance levels: p < 0.05).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Representative phrases of sequential patterns with mean highest (left and red) and lowest (right and blue) probabilities in the six different hierarchical models of TPs for the Well-Tempered Clavier, BWV 846–893, which is a collection of two series (No. 1 and No. 2) of Preludes and Fugues in all 24 major and minor keys that was composed for solo keyboard by Johann Sebastian Bach.

Results

ANOVA

Higher-order of model represents exponentially larger numbers of sequential patterns: over forty in the first-order models, 600 in the second-order models, 3,500 in the third-order models, 9,000 in the fourth-order models, 15,000 in the fifth-order models, 20,000 in the sixth-order models. The results were shown in Figure 3. The main tonality effect showed that TPs of sequence that appear in all music in major key were lower than those in minor key [F(1, 11) = 9.83, p = 0.009, partial η2 = 0.47; Figure 3A]. The main type effect showed that TPs of sequence that appear in all music in preludes were lower than those in fugues [F(1, 11) = 140.74, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.93; Figure 3B]. The main sequence effect were significant [F(2.16, 23.76) = 26.54, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.71; Figure 3C]. The TPs of [0, −2] was significantly higher compared with those of [0, −1], [0, 1], and [0, −3] (all: p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, −1] was higher compared with those of [0, 1] (p = 0.005) and [0, −3] (p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, 1] was higher compared with those of [0, −3] (p < 0.001). The tonality-number interactions were significant [F(1, 11) = 7.57, p = 0.019, partialη2 = 0.41; Figure 3D]. In No. 1 of a collection of two series, the TPs in major key were significantly lower than those in minor key (p = 0.001). In minor key, the TPs in No. 1 were higher compared with those in No. 2 (p = 0.044). The tonality-sequence interactions were significant [F(1.68, 18.46) = 5.35, p = 0.019, partial η2 = 0.33; Figure 3E]. In sequences of [0, −1], the TPs in major key were significantly lower than those in minor key (p = 0.001). In sequences of [0, 1], the TPs in major key were significantly lower than those in minor key (p = 0.019). In major key, the TPs of [0, −2] was higher compared with those of [0, −1], [0, 1], and [0, −3] (all: p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, −1] was higher compared with those of [0, −3] (p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, 1] was higher compared with those of [0, −3] (p = 0.002). In minor key, the TPs of [0, −2] was higher compared with those of [0,−1] (p = 0.001), [0, 1] (p < 0.001), and [0, −3] (p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, −1] was higher compared with those of [0, 1] (p = 0.006) and [0, −3] (p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, 1] was higher compared with those of [0, −3] (p < 0.001).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The results of ANOVA in analysis 2. The main effects of (A) tonality, (B) type, and (C) sequence. The interactions of (D) tonality-number, (E) tonality-sequence, and (F) type-sequence.

The type-sequence interactions were significant [F(1.85, 20.34) = 7.64, p = 0.004, partial η2 = 0.41]. In sequences of [0, −2], the TPs in prelude were significantly lower than those in fugue (p < 0.001). In sequences of [0, −1], the TPs in prelude were significantly lower than those in fugue (p = 0.012). In sequences of [0, 1], the TPs in prelude were significantly lower than those in fugue (p = 0.005). In prelude, the TPs of [0, −2] was higher compared with those of [0, −1], [0, 1], and [0, −3] (all: p < 0.001). In fugue, the TPs of [0, −2] was higher compared with those of [0, −1], [0, 1], and [0, −3] (all: p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, −1] was higher compared with those of [0, 1] (p = 0.006) and [0, −3] (p = 0.001). The TPs of [0, 1] was higher compared with those of [0, −3] (p = 0.010). In fugue, the TPs of [0, −2] was higher compared with those of [0, −1], [0, 1], and [0, −3] (all: p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, −1] was higher compared with those of [0, 1] (p = 0.047) and [0, −3] (p < 0.001). The TPs of [0, 1] was higher compared with those of [0, −3] (p < 0.001).

Correlation Analysis

All the results of the correlation analysis are shown in Supplementary Material. In the first-order TPs, all the pieces of music are strongly (0.7 ≦ |r| < 1.0, p < 0.01; Supplementary Material, red) or moderately (0.4 ≦ |r| < 0.7, p < 0.01; Supplementary Material, green) related to each other (Figure 4A). In the second-order TPs, all the pieces of music are moderately (0.4 ≦ |r| < 0.7, p < 0.01; Supplementary Material, green) or weakly (0.2 ≦ |r| < 0.4, p < 0.01; Supplementary Material, yellow) related to each other (Figure 4B). In the third- and fourth-order TPs, some of the music is weakly (0.2 ≦ |r| < 0.4, p < 0.01; Supplementary Material, yellow) related to each other (Figures 4C,D). There are more weak correlations in the third-order than in the fourth-order TPs. In the fifth- and sixth-order TPs, no strong, moderate, or weak correlations were detected (Figures 4E,F).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) The first-, (B) second-, (C) third-, (D) fourth-, (E) fifth-, and (F) sixth-order TPs in each sequential pattern. The horizontal and vertical axes represent sequential patterns and the TPs, respectively. The sequential patterns were arranged in descending order in each hierarchy.

Principal Component Analysis

Based on the results of correlation analysis, the first- and second-order TPs, in which there are a number of correlations of at least 0.3 (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007), were analyzed by principal component analysis. In the first-order TP, the decision was made to specify two principal component solutions (eigenvalue >1; Table 2A and Figure 5). The two principal components accounted for 92.4% of the total variance. All of the music loaded higher than 0.58 on component 1. The “loadings” can be understood as the weights for each original variable when calculating the principal component. Thus, the result explains the general component of the Well-Tempered Clavier. The C major and D minor in the first series (No. 1) of the Well-Tempered Clavier loaded higher than 0.45 on 2. This explains a component of related keys (i.e., the relative key of the subdominant key; Table 1) between C major and D minor. In the second-order TP, the decision was made to specify a three principal component solution (eigenvalue >1; Table 2B and Figure 5). The three principal components accounted for 83.2% of the total variance. All of the music loaded higher than 0.55 on 1,. This explains the general component of the Well-Tempered Clavier. On the other hand, compared to the other music, the C major and D minor in No. 1 of the Well-Tempered Clavier loaded <0.57 on component 1. The C minor in No. 1 and E♭ major in No. 2 of the Well-Tempered Clavier loaded at 0.41 or higher on component 2. This explains shared components of a related key (i.e., relative keys). The only D minor in No. 1 of the Well-Tempered Clavier loaded heavily (0.52) on component 3.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Principal component analysis scatter plots. The dots represent each piece of music in the Well-Tempered Clavier, which is a collection of two series (No. 1 and No. 2) in all 24 major and minor keys that was composed for solo keyboard by Johann Sebastian Bach. The dots in each circle represent pieces of music with the component of each related key: between D minor and C major, Eb major and C minor, and C major and D minor.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Related key in all 24 major and minor keys.

TABLE 2A
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2A. The results of principal component analysis.

TABLE 2B
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2B. The eigenvectors for the principal components.

Discussion

Psychological Aspects of TP in Musical Sequence

Based on the information theory (Shannon, 1948) covering multi-order Markov models and the cognitive models, a tone with a higher TP may be one that a composer is more likely to choose than those with lower TPs. Thus, the TP distributions sampled from music may represent the musical characteristics based on a composer's statistical knowledge underlying prediction. The present study aimed to examine how the statistical structure interacts with tonality in music. To verify it in all 24 major and minor keys (Figure 1), the TPs of the sequences containing the highest pitches in Well-Tempered Clavier were calculated based on Markov stochastic models. It was hypothesized that the statistical structure in music interacts with tonality in music and that music-specific knowledge of tonality may modulates statistical knowledge in music.

The Relationships Between Tonality and Hierarchy of Stochastic Structure in Music

The present study adopted the sequences that appear in all pieces of music (i.e., universal sequences in the Well-Tempered Clavier). The TP differences between major and minor keys could be detected in lower-order (1st and 2nd in Figure 3A) but not in higher-order hierarchical models. This implies that these sequences may have specific semantics in each major and minor key. In the context of statistical learning, the tonality may modulate a lower- rather than a higher-order statistical knowledge of music. The TPs in the fugue were higher than those in the prelude (Figure 3B), and the difference was prominent in sequences in which the interval was not more than a whole step (i.e., ±2), such as those found in musical scales (Figure 3F). It is well-known that the prelude less strictly follows the rules of Western classical music compared to the fugue. The findings in the present study may reflect the difference in statistical knowledge related to strategies for musical composition.

As a general tendency, the TPs of universal sequences were higher in minor than in major keys (Figures 3A,E). However, the difference became weaker in the series of No. 2 compared to that in No. 1 (Figure 3D). Statistical knowledge of universal sequences might be modulated from composition periods in No. 1 to No. 2. It would be interesting if the time-course variation of statistical structures may reflect the time-course variation of statistical knowledge. It is of note, however, that this study did not directly investigate the composer's statistical knowledge of music, as only the statistics of musical scores were analyzed. There may be other possible explanations for the findings of this study. For instance, it might have been Bach's intentional plan to compose music based on the statistical structure of music. Future studies should examine the effects of statistical knowledge on music compositions and neurological responses in parallel.

In the first- and second-order TPs, all of the pieces of music are related to each other (Supplementary Material and Figure 4). In the third- and fourth-order TPs, some of the music is related to each other, regardless of tonalities. There are more correlations in the third-order than fourth-order TPs. In the fifth- and sixth-order TPs, no remarkable correlations were detected. These results suggest that there are statistical characteristics that are shared among each piece of music at least in the first- and second-order hierarchical levels of statistical structure. In other words, there may be universal implicit knowledge of music in the composer at the lower hierarchical levels, regardless of tonalities and pitch frequencies. The higher the hierarchical levels of TPs, the less the music was correlated with each other. From information theoretical viewpoint, the statistical models at lower hierarchical levels increases joint probability and mutual information, whereas statistical structures at higher hierarchical levels are less correlated, and interpreted as surprisal information (Gupta and Bahmer, 2019). The combined increase in mutual information at lower hierarchical level and surprisal information at higher hierarchical level would serve as the basis of specific knowledge about music (Gupta and Bahmer, 2019). These results also suggest that the higher the hierarchical level of statistical structure, the stronger the independence of characteristics in each piece of music. The specific characteristics in each piece of music may exist in higher hierarchical levels of statistical structure. This may imply that greater creativity is attributed at higher hierarchical level (Daikoku, 2018b). Thus, it could be assumed that the general statistical structure that is shared among many pieces of music is formed by low-hierarchical implicit knowledge, whereas the specific structure that is independent of each piece of music is formed by high-hierarchical implicit knowledge (Gupta and Bahmer, 2019).

J.S. Bach's Music for Study on Implicit and Explicit Knowledge

Johann Sebastian Bach (1685–1750), a German composer and musician of the Baroque period, is considered to have contributed to the development of musical tonality and has been central to Western classical music theory until the present (Rohrmeier and Cross, 2008). His music is often used to investigate the probabilities of musical sequences. Furthermore, to investigate the relationships between tonality and statistical structure in music, the Well-Tempered Clavier is considered an excellent medium because it is a collection of music containing all the keys of Western classical music (i.e., 24 major and minor keys). Thus, the statistical characteristics of each piece of music with a key in the Well-Tempered Clavier could be, in part, regarded as approximations of the statistical characteristics of the entire range of Western classical music in each key. In other words, the findings in the present study may reflect the implicit knowledge in each musical key in humans who explicitly learn the music-specific knowledge based on Western classical music and who intentionally follow these frameworks when composing music. Furthermore, the present study may suggest that there are at least two types of implicit knowledge that are dependent on and independent of tonality, respectively. This study, however, did not directly demonstrate that the implicit musical knowledge is reflected in music, as only the statistics of musical scores were analyzed. Future studies should investigate, in parallel, how implicit learning in music is reflected in the neurological response and how the learned knowledge is expressed when composing music.

The representative phrases of sequential patterns with mean highest and lowest probabilities were decoded as musical scores in Figure 2, based on each hierarchical level of first- (highest: P[−2|0], lowest: P[−16|0]), second- (highest: P[−3|0, −2], lowest: P[10|0, −1]), third- (highest: P[−5|0, −2, −4], lowest: P[−6|0, −4, −8]), fourth- (highest: P[−7|0, −2, −4, −5], lowest: P[0|0, 5, 1, −2]), fifth- (highest: P[−9|0, −2, −4, −5, −7], lowest: P[−2|0, −7, −8, −7, −3]), and sixth- (highest: P[−3|0, −1, −3, −5, −6, −5], lowest: P[−3|0, 8, 6, 7, 0, −2]). The sequential patterns with the highest sequential patterns are familiar ones in Western classical music, suggesting that implicit statistical knowledge and explicit music-specific knowledge interact, in part, with each other. The principal component analysis detected the shared components of related keys (Figure 5). This suggests that tonalities modulate implicit knowledge in music. However, these findings are not detected in all the types of related keys (Supplementary Material). Future studies will be needed to clarify the relationships between statistical structure and tonalities in music. In the present study, all of the pitch transitions were numbered to understand how the pitches, but not the notes, were transitioned to from the first pitch. This was performed to eliminate the effects of the change of key on sequential patterns. Thus, the results may represent statistics based on relative pitches rather than absolute pitches. Nonetheless, the present study suggests that explicit knowledge on tonality could, in part, modulate implicit knowledge in music.

Conclusion

The present study indicated that, in the lower hierarchical levels of statistical structure (first and second orders), all the pieces of music are related to each other. However, the higher the hierarchical levels of TPs, the less the music was correlated with each other, regardless of tonality. These findings suggest that the general statistical structure that is shared among many pieces of music is formed by low-hierarchical implicit knowledge, whereas the specific structure that is independent of each piece of music is formed by high-hierarchical implicit knowledge. This may imply that greater creativity is attributed at higher hierarchical level. On the other hand, the principal component analysis detected the shared components of related keys, suggesting that tonalities modulate implicit knowledge in music. The implicit statistical knowledge and explicit music-specific knowledge could, in part, interact with each other. It is suggested that there are at least two types of implicit knowledge that are dependent on and independent of tonality, respectively. The present study sheds new light on novel methodologies that can be employed to evaluate the implicit knowledge of a composer using musical scores in interdisciplinary studies that include psychology, informatics, and musicology.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript/Supplementary Files.

Author Contributions

The methodology of the present study was considered by the authors. The author analyzed all of the data and prepared the figures, and wrote the manuscript text.

Funding

The present study was supported by Suntory Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fncom.2019.00070/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Table 1. The results of correlation analysis. The red, green, yellow cells represent strong (0.7 ≦ |r| < 1.0), moderate (0.4 ≦ |r| < 0.7), and weak (0.2 ≦ |r| < 0.4) correlations, respectively. M and m indicate major and minor keys, respectively.

References

Balsters, J. H., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2014). Bridging the gap between functional and anatomical features of cortico-cerebellar circuits using meta-analytic connectivity modeling. Human Brain Mapping 35, 3152–3169. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A., and Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit learning: news from the front. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 406–416. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01232-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T. (2018a). Entropy, uncertainty, and the depth of implicit knowledge on musical creativity: computational study of improvisation in melody and rhythm. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 12:97. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2018.00097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T. (2018b). Musical creativity and depth of implicit knowledge: spectral and temporal individualities in improvisation. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 12, 1–27. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2018.00089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T. (2018c). Neurophysiological markers of statistical learning in music and language: hierarchy, entropy, and uncertainty. Brain Sci. 8:E114. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8060114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T. (2018d). Time-course variation of statistics embedded in music: corpus study on implicit learning and knowledge. PLoS ONE 13:e0196493. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0196493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T. (2019a). Implicit knowledge and the uncertainty on musical creativity fluctuate over a composer's lifetime. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 13:27. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2019.00027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T. (2019b). Computational models and neural bases of statistical learning in music and language. Phys. Life Rev. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2019.09.001. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T. (2019c). Implicit learning in the developing brain: an exploration of ERP indices for developmental disorders. Clin. Neurophysiol. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.09.001. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T., Takahashi, Y., Futagami, H., Tarumoto, M., and Yasuda, H. (2017b). Physical fitness modulates incidental but not intentional statistical learning of simultaneous auditory sequences during concurrent physical exercise. Neurol. Res. 30, 107–116. doi: 10.1080/01616412.2016.1273571

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T., Takahashi, Y., Tarumoto, M., and Yasuda, H. (2018). Auditory statistical learning during concurrent physical exercise and the tolerance for pitch, tempo, and rhythm changes. Motor Control 22, 233–244. doi: 10.1123/mc.2017-0006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T., Yatomi, Y., and Yumoto, M. (2014). Implicit and explicit statistical learning of tone sequences across spectral shifts. Neuropsychologia 63, 194–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T., Yatomi, Y., and Yumoto, M. (2015). Statistical learning of music- and language-like sequences and tolerance for spectral shifts. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 118, 8–19. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T., Yatomi, Y., and Yumoto, M. (2016). Pitch-class distribution modulates the statistical learning of atonal chord sequences. Brain Cogn. 108, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T., Yatomi, Y., and Yumoto, M. (2017a). Statistical learning of an auditory sequence and reorganization of acquired knowledge: a time course of word segmentation and ordering. Neuropsychologia 95, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T., and Yumoto, M. (2017). Single, but not dual, attention facilitates statistical learning of two concurrent auditory sequences. Sci. Rep. 7:10108. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-10476-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daikoku, T., and Yumoto, M. (2019). Concurrent statistical learning of ignored and attended sound sequences: an MEG study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:102. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doya, K., Ishii, S., Pouget, A., and Rao, R. P. N. (2007). Bayesian Brain: Probabilistic Approaches to Neural Coding. Oxford: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Elmer, S., and Lutz, J. (2018). Relationships between music training, speech processing, and word learning: a network perspective. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1423, 10–18. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

François, C., Chobert, J., Besson, M., and Schön, D. (2012). Music training for the development of speech segmentation. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2038–2043. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francois, C., and Schön, D. (2011). Musical expertise boosts implicit learning of both musical and linguistic structures. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2357–2365. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Fitzgerald, T., Rigoli, F., Schwartenbeck, P., Doherty, J. O., and Pezzulo, G. (2016). Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Active inference and learning. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 862–879. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Rigoli, F., Ognibene, D., Mathys, C., Fitzgerald, T., and Pezzulo, G. (2015). Active inference and epistemic value. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 187–224. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1020053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T., Moutoussis, M., Behrens, T., and Dolan, R. J. (2014). The anatomy of choice: dopamine and decision-making. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, S. D., and Bahmer, A. (2019). Increase in mutual information during interaction with the environment contributes to perception. Entropy 21:365. doi: 10.3390/e21040365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, N. C., and Pearce, M. T. (2014). Predictive uncertainty in auditory sequence processing. Front. Psychol. 5, 1–17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, U. (2017). The neurobiology of uncertainty: implications for statistical learning. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372:20160048. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 304–313. doi: 10.1038/nrn2332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kersten, D., Mamassian, P., and Yuille, A. (2004). Object perception as Bayesian inference. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 55, 271–304. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. G., Kim, J. S., and Chung, C. K. (2011). The effect of conditional probability of chord progression on brain response: an MEG study. PLoS ONE 6:17337. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knill, D. C., and Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends Neurosci. 27, 712–719. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesage, E., Morgan, B. E., Olson, A. C., Meyer, A. S., and Miall, R. C. (2012). Cerebellar rTMS disrupts predictive language processing. Curr. Biol. 22, R794–R795. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moberget, T., Gullesen, E. H., Andersson, S., Ivry, R. B., and Endestad, T. (2014). Generalized role for the cerebellum in encoding internal models: evidence from semantic processing. J. Neurosci. 34, 2871–2878. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2264-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroy, C., Meyer, M., Gerson, S., and Hunnius, S. (2017c). Statistical learning in social action contexts. PLoS ONE 12, 1–20. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroy, C. D., Gerson, S. A., Domínguez-Martínez, E., Kaduk, K., Hunnius, S., and Reid, V. (2017a). Sensitivity to structure in action sequences: an infant event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia 126, 92–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroy, C. D., Gerson, S. A., and Hunnius, S. (2018). Translating visual information into action predictions: statistical learning in action and nonaction contexts. Memory Cogn. 46, 600–613. doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0788-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroy, C. D., Meyer, M., Schröer, L., Gerson, S. A., and Hunnius, S. (2017b). The infant motor system predicts actions based on visual statistical learning. Neuroimage 185, 947–954. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Reilly, J. X., Jbabdi, S., and Behrens, T. E. J. (2012). How can a Bayesian approach inform neuroscience? Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 1169–1179. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08010.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parr, T., and Friston, K. J. (2018). The anatomy of inference: generative models and brain structure. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 12:90. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2018.00090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parr, T., Rees, G., and Friston, K. J. (2018). Computational neuropsychology and Bayesian inference. Front. Human Neurosci. 12, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, M. T., Ruiz, M. H., Kapasi, S., Wiggins, G. A., and Bhattacharya, J. (2010). Unsupervised statistical learning underpins computational, behavioural, and neural manifestations of musical expectation. Neuroimage 50, 302–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, M. T., and Wiggins, G. A. (2012). Auditory expectation: the information dynamics of music perception and cognition. Topics Cogn. Sci. 4, 625–652. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01214.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F., and Friston, K. (2015). Active Inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Progress Neurobiol. 134, 17–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Przysinda, E., Zeng, T., Maves, K., Arkin, C., and Loui, P. (2017). Jazz musicians reveal role of expectancy in human creativity. Brain Cogn. 119, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohrmeier, M., and Cross, I. (2008). “Statistical properties of tonal harmony in Bach's Chorales,” in Proceedings of 10th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition. Retrieved from: http://icmpc10.psych.let.hokudai.ac.jp/%5Cnhttp://www.mus.cam.ac.uk/files/2009/09/bachharmony.pdf

Google Scholar

Rohrmeier, M., and Rebuschat, P. (2012). Implicit learning and acquisition of music. Topics Cogn. Sci. 4, 525–553. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01223.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saffran, J., Aslin, R., and Newport, E. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274, 1926–1928.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T., Dolan, R. J., and Friston, K. (2013). Exploration, novelty, surprise, and free energy minimization. Front. Psychol. 4, 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical J. 27, 623–656.

Google Scholar

Shimizu, R. E., Wu, A. D., Samra, J. K., and Knowlton, B. J. (2017). The impact of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation (Tdcs) on learning fine-motor sequences. Philos. Transac. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 372:20160050. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics, 5th edn. New York, NY: Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

Wiggins, G. A. (2018). Creativity, information, and consciousness: the information dynamics of thinking. Phys. Life Rev. 1, 1–39. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2018.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yumoto, M., and Daikoku, T. (2016). “Basic function,” in Clinical Applications of Magnetoencephalography, eds S. Tobimatsu and R. Kakigi (New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media, 97–112.

Google Scholar

Yumoto, M., and Daikoku, T. (2018). Neurophysiological studies on auditory statistical learning. Jpn. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 38–43. doi: 10.11253/ninchishinkeikagaku.20.38

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zubicaray, G., Arciuli, J., and Mcmahon, K. (2013). Putting an “end” to the motor cortex representations of action words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 1957–1974. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: creativity, Markov model, n-gram, information theory, corpus, prediction, composition, implicit learning

Citation: Daikoku T (2019) Tonality Tunes the Statistical Characteristics in Music: Computational Approaches on Statistical Learning. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 13:70. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2019.00070

Received: 20 April 2019; Accepted: 19 September 2019;
Published: 02 October 2019.

Edited by:

Hamid R. Rabiee, Sharif University of Technology, Iran

Reviewed by:

Tuo Zhang, Northwestern Polytechnical University, China
Daya Shankar Gupta, Camden County College, United States

Copyright © 2019 Daikoku. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tatsuya Daikoku, daikoku@cbs.mpg.de

Download