Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 10 July 2018
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 12 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00272

Extrahippocampal Contributions to Age-Related Changes in Spatial Navigation Ability

  • School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA, United States

Age-related decline in spatial navigation is well-known and the extant literature emphasizes the important contributions of a hippocampus-dependent spatial navigation system in mediating this decline. However, navigation is a multifaceted cognitive domain and some aspects of age-related navigational decline may be mediated by extrahippocampal brain regions and/or systems. The current review presents an overview of some key cognitive domains that contribute to the age-related changes in spatial navigation ability, and elucidates such domains in the context of an increased engagement of navigationally relevant extrahippocampal brain regions with advancing age. Specifically, this review focuses on age-related declines in three main areas: (i) allocentric strategy use and switching between egocentric and allocentric strategies, (ii) associative learning of landmarks/locations and heading directions, and (iii) executive functioning and attention. Thus far, there is accumulating neuroimaging evidence supporting the functional relevance of the striatum for egocentric/response strategy use in older adults, and of the prefrontal cortex for mediating executive functions that contribute to successful navigational performance. Notably, the functional role of the prefrontal cortex was particularly emphasized via the proposed relevance of the fronto-locus coeruleus noradrenergic system for strategy switching and of the fronto-hippocampal circuit for landmark-direction associative learning. In view of these putative prefrontal contributions to navigation-related functions, we recommend future spatial navigation studies to adopt a systems-oriented approach that investigates age-related alterations in the interaction between the prefrontal cortex, the hippocampus, and extrahippocampal regions, as well as an individual differences approach that clarifies the differential engagement of prefrontal executive processes among older adults.

Introduction

Spatial navigation ability is crucial for everyday living, allowing us to be cognizant of our position and orientation in our environment, as well as helping us to maintain a sense of direction when navigating to and from various locations (Wolbers and Hegarty, 2010; Chersi and Burgess, 2015). Even though spatial navigation seems effortless at the behavioral level, it is a multimodal activity that draws upon a multitude of cognitive and neural resources (Moffat, 2009, 2016; Wolbers and Hegarty, 2010; Wolbers, 2015; Zhong and Kozhevnikov, 2016; Lester et al., 2017). Numerous behavioral studies of spatial navigation in the cognitive aging literature have identified age-related declines or deficits in navigation strategies (Moffat and Resnick, 2002; Bohbot et al., 2012; Harris et al., 2012; Wiener et al., 2013; Harris and Wolbers, 2014; Colombo et al., 2017; Zhong et al., 2017), associative learning/memory (Head and Isom, 2010; Liu et al., 2011; Wiener et al., 2012, 2013; Zhong and Moffat, 2016; Allison and Head, 2017; O’Malley et al., 2018), and working memory (Mahmood et al., 2009; Taillade et al., 2013a,b, 2016; Ariel and Moffat, 2018). Complementary neuroimaging studies that investigated age-related declines in spatial navigation performance and memory have largely linked them to age-related reduction in the volume or activation of the hippocampus (e.g., Driscoll et al., 2003, 2005; Astur et al., 2006; Moffat et al., 2006, 2007; Antonova et al., 2009; Yuan et al., 2014; Daugherty et al., 2015, 2016), a region that has long been proposed as the neural basis of a “cognitive map” (O’Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978).

Notwithstanding the pertinence of the hippocampus for spatial navigation, the complexities of navigation-related cognition and behavior cannot be traced to one region alone, and the functional relevance of extrahippocampal brain regions should not be minimized (Doeller et al., 2008; Chersi and Burgess, 2015). Navigationally relevant extrahippocampal regions encompass the striatum, the prefrontal cortex, the parietal cortex and the retrosplenial cortex (RSC; see Moffat et al., 2006, 2007; Moffat, 2009, 2016; Wolbers and Hegarty, 2010; Wolbers, 2015; Lester et al., 2017). Moreover, navigation depends on the contributions of cognitive domains/brain systems that are not specifically navigation-related (e.g., working memory, attention, motor control, etc.) and age differences in these cognitive domains may contribute to and manifest as deficits in navigation tasks. With advancing age, spatial learning and navigational performance start to become more dependent on extrahippocampal regions instead of the hippocampus per se (Moffat et al., 2006, 2007; Lester et al., 2017). With a focus on extrahippocampal regions and processes, the current review presents an overview of some key cognitive domains of spatial navigation that are adversely affected by normal aging and relates them to changes in the functional integrity of both hippocampal and extrahippocampal regions. To facilitate organization, we surveyed the literature on aging and navigation strategies and associative learning before turning to a discussion of other processes that are generally executive in nature and largely dependent on the prefrontal cortex. We acknowledge the inherently complex nature of spatial navigation and do not intend to suggest that these cognitive processes and the sections herein are mutually exclusive. Table 1 shows a summary of the main cognitive aging studies covered by this review and the cognitive domains and brain systems investigated or discussed.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Main cognitive aging studies that investigated and/or discussed extrahippocampal cognitive domains in spatial navigation.

Navigation Strategies and Strategy Switching

A major theme in the discussion of spatial navigation pertains to navigation strategies, which commonly take the form of mental imagery techniques that require the primary engagement of an egocentric (i.e., first-person) or allocentric (i.e., third-person) frame of reference (see, e.g., Zhong and Kozhevnikov, 2016; He and McNamara, 2018; McCunn and Gifford, 2018; see also Colombo et al., 2017; for a review). While an egocentric navigation strategy requires the moving agent to visualize scenes and gauge self-to-object relationships from a body-centered viewpoint, an allocentric navigation strategy requires the agent to visualize scenes and map out object-to-object relationships from a disembodied or environment-centered viewpoint (Zhong and Kozhevnikov, 2016; Colombo et al., 2017; see also Klatzky, 1998). Previous neuroimaging work by Iaria and colleagues (based on samples of young adults) showed double dissociation between allocentric and egocentric/response strategies, with activation in the hippocampus corresponding with the former (Iaria et al., 2003, 2007), and activation in the caudate nucleus/striatum corresponding with the latter (Iaria et al., 2003). An alternative egocentric spatial strategy, requiring the tracking and spatial updating of movements away from a point of origin during path integration (Loomis et al., 1999; Mahmood et al., 2009; Wiener et al., 2011; Zhong and Kozhevnikov, 2016), depends on the stability of grid cell firing patterns in the entorhinal cortex of both humans (Stangl et al., 2018) and mice (Gil et al., 2018). Following the thematic emphasis on extrahippocampal functions, this review shall not discuss age differences in path integration that are contingent on entorhinal or hippocampal functions (see Mahmood et al., 2009; Wiener et al., 2011; Harris and Wolbers, 2012; Stangl et al., 2018), but on age differences in non-spatial egocentric/response strategy use.

In the cognitive aging literature, the proportion of older adults preferring an egocentric/response strategy for wayfinding has been shown to be higher than the proportion of younger adults preferring the same strategy (Bohbot et al., 2012; Rodgers et al., 2012; Wiener et al., 2013). For instance, in a virtual Y-maze that can be solved by either an egocentric/response (i.e., traversing the same route learned during training irrespective of the absolute location of the goal) or allocentric (i.e., judging the absolute location of the goal area relative to the origin regardless of the learned route) strategy, Rodgers et al. (2012) found that the great majority of older adults (83%) preferred the egocentric/response strategy whereas younger adults’ preferences were relatively evenly divided between egocentric (46%) and allocentric (54%) strategies. Similar, but less striking statistics were also reported by Bohbot et al. (2012) in a study involving a virtual radial-arm maze task. In this task, egocentric/response strategy use involved counting the number of arms from the origin to find the goal objects at the end of different radial arms, whereas allocentric strategy use involved the use of surrounding cues to judge the locations of the goal objects. Post-test surveys showed that most older adults (60.7%) reported preferring an egocentric/response strategy while younger adults’ preferences were more evenly divided between egocentric (53.7%) and allocentric (46.3%) strategies.

Older adults’ lower preference for the allocentric strategy can be traced to fMRI studies comparing both young and older subjects in navigating virtual environments; these studies generally showed that older adults have either reduced (Meulenbroek et al., 2004; Moffat et al., 2006; Konishi et al., 2013) or absent activation (Antonova et al., 2009) in the hippocampal formation. Lower allocentric strategy use among the older adults could also be attributed to smaller hippocampal volumes in older than younger adults, which in turn may undermine older adults’ accuracy in spatial learning and navigational performance (Driscoll et al., 2003; Moffat et al., 2007; Konishi and Bohbot, 2013; Yuan et al., 2014; Daugherty et al., 2015). By contrast, when searching for previously encoded goal locations in virtual environments, increased activation in the striatum (mainly the caudate nucleus) was found among older egocentric/response strategy users (Konishi et al., 2013), and when older adults were focused on processing local landmark information as compared to processing allocentric boundary information (Schuck et al., 2015). This elevated striatal activity reflected a change in information processing in the hippocampal-striatal circuitry with advancing age—with spatial learning and performance in older adults being more associated with the striatum, as well as with the prefrontal cortex (Moffat et al., 2006, 2007), than with the hippocampus (Moffat et al., 2007; Konishi et al., 2013; Schuck et al., 2015). Consequently, increased engagement of the striatum, complemented by decreased engagement of the hippocampus, may offer a credible source of support for explaining older adults’ “egocentric bias” in strategy use.

Another approach to understanding why older adults find it easier to implement egocentric/response strategies was performed by Harris and colleagues (Harris et al., 2012; Harris and Wolbers, 2014). In their studies, the researchers applied a virtual plus maze that allowed flexibility in configuring goal locations and the paths of travel. Egocentric/response strategy use involved making repetitive turning responses regardless of the whereabouts of the goal, whereas allocentric strategy use involved judging the absolute location of the goal regardless of the turning responses. Older adults attained comparable performance as the young when implementing the egocentric/response strategy but became less accurate when implementing the allocentric strategy (Harris et al., 2012). Crucially, the decline in search accuracy, as well as in the number of trials learning and implementing a specific strategy, were most prominent after the older adults were instructed to switch to allocentric strategy use from previous egocentric/response strategy use (Harris et al., 2012). This egocentric-to-allocentric strategy switching deficit further applied to finding shortcuts when navigating a larger virtual town (Harris et al., 2014). Unlike the young adults who overwhelmingly reported the consistent use of shortcuts or switching to shortcuts at some time during testing, none of the older adults did so. Instead, three-quarters of the older adults reported using shortcuts inconsistently and the remaining quarter reported the consistent use of an egocentric route strategy. A re-administration of the virtual plus maze with a smaller number of trials showed that older adults’ search accuracy declined significantly (relative to younger adults) when they were attempting allocentric-to-egocentric strategy switches, suggesting an impairment in making allocentric-to-egocentric spatial transformations could also apply to older adults.

These observed deficits in strategy switching were suggested to represent an age-related degradation of the axonal circuitry in the prefrontal cortex (Pfefferbaum et al., 2005; as cited in Harris and Wolbers, 2014), and/or an age-related dysregulation of noradrenaline production and function along the anatomical pathways linking the locus coeruleus to the prefrontal cortex (Harris et al., 2012; Harris and Wolbers, 2014). Moreover, it was speculated that an age-related reduction of the functional connectivity between the prefrontal-noradrenergic network and the hippocampus may specifically account for the egocentric-to-allocentric strategy switching deficit (Harris et al., 2012; Harris and Wolbers, 2014). As for the allocentric-to-egocentric strategy switching deficit, the researchers did not associate it with the potential degradation in prefrontal-hippocampal connectivity but instead associated it with dysfunction within the noradrenergic system linking the prefrontal cortex and the locus coeruleus (Harris and Wolbers, 2014). The locus coeruleus is strongly implicated in strategy switching in view of its tonic and phasic outputs of noradrenaline to the prefrontal cortex (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005). Specifically, it has been proposed that tonic locus coeruleus-noradrenergic activity facilitates disengagement from a current behavior and the selection of alternative behavior (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005), whereas phasic locus coeruleus-noradrenergic activity promotes the adoption of behavioral alternative(s) and the organization of functional neural networks for tackling specific tasks (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Bouret and Sara, 2008).

Worthy of extra consideration was that older adults’ difficulties with bidirectional switching between egocentric and allocentric strategies might be attributed to lower activation in the RSC (i.e., when encoding spatial information regarding the relative locations of landmarks, see Moffat et al., 2006, Table 1). This relates well to proposals of the RSC as being involved in translating between egocentric and allocentric reference frames (Maguire, 2001; Byrne et al., 2007; Vann et al., 2009; Mitchell et al., 2018), and in mediating between different spatial representations and modes of processing (Mitchell et al., 2018). Recent studies supported these proposals by showing that the RSC is crucial for: (a) judging egocentric views of target objects from rotated viewpoints that require the retrieval of allocentric spatial information (Sulpizio et al., 2016), and (b) the implementation of allocentric reference frames for integrating egocentric heading directions across separate locales (Shine et al., 2016).

Furthermore, using multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA), Marchette et al. (2014) showed that RSC activity patterns were similar when participants faced similar directions or imagined similar locations across four virtual museums that are geometrically similar but separated perpendicular to each other in global space. Reaction times were also faster for homologous/matching directions and locations than for non-matching directions and locations both within and across museums. Interestingly, these findings offer an alternative view of the RSC as being involved in anchoring internal spatial representations to local topographical features. However, Marchette et al. (2014) did not assess the extent to which their participants integrated local spatial representations into a global schematic map of the environment, and thus there remains the possibility (as pointed out by the authors) that the so-called “local” spatial representations of some participants could be environment-centered in nature, encompassing the spatial relationships between all landmarks imaginable. Interestingly, Mitchell et al. (2018) did not regard these findings as refuting the proposed role of the RSC in mediating between spatial reference frames and suggested that there may be RSC cells that are separately responsible for local/egocentric and global/allocentric orientation.

Associative Learning

In addition to the challenges faced by older adults in implementing allocentric navigation strategies and switching between strategies, they also experience difficulties with the association of the correct heading directions (to goal) with landmarks or locations at which directional changes are required (Head and Isom, 2010; Liu et al., 2011; Wiener et al., 2012, 2013; Zhong and Moffat, 2016; Allison and Head, 2017; O’Malley et al., 2018). Specifically, older adults have been found to be less competent than younger adults at selecting correct headings or turns at intersections (Wiener et al., 2012, 2013; Zhong and Moffat, 2016), especially when approaching intersections from directions that are reversals of the directions encountered during route learning (Wiener et al., 2012, 2013). Notably, Wiener et al. (2013) showed that older adults’ difficulties with finding the correct heading direction when traveling the route in reverse were associated with older adults’ implementation of an associative cue strategy (i.e., “Turn left/right at”), and proposed that this strategy engaged the dorsolateral striatum.

Also noteworthy are the findings by Zhong and Moffat (2016), which showed that older adults exhibited a landmark-direction associative memory deficit at intersections, inasmuch that older adults performed poorer than younger adults at associating correct/goal-directed heading directions with varying views of landmarks at intersections but did not perform poorer than younger adults when tested on the recognition memory of landmarks alone. The authors speculated that this selective deficit in “binding” landmark and directional information was related to senescent changes in the fronto-hippocampal circuitry (Li et al., 2001, 2005), as characterized by degraded functional connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus in older adults (Grady et al., 2003). Interestingly, this proposed involvement of the fronto-hippocampal circuitry in forming landmark-direction associations parallels the relevance of the same system in making successful egocentric-to-allocentric strategy switches (Harris et al., 2012; Harris and Wolbers, 2014).

Executive Functioning and Attention

The proposed involvement of the prefrontal cortex in both navigation-related associative learning and strategy switching necessitates a closer look at its relevance for spatial navigation. When navigating towards a specific goal location, the prefrontal cortex mediates many goal-directed processes (Spiers, 2008). Spontaneously thinking about the goal location and the best route or path to reach it have been shown to be related to increased activity in the anterior prefrontal cortex (BA 10), the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (BA 9; in middle-aged London Taxi drivers, Spiers and Maguire, 2006, 2007), and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (in a college-aged sample, Carrieri et al., 2018). On the other hand, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex has been related to maintaining the intention to reach the goal in working memory and suppressing irrelevant information [as suggested by Spiers (2008), based on evidence from a patient with ventromedial prefrontal damage documented by Ciaramelli (2008)], and exerting top-down control in mediating between a hippocampus-dependent boundary processing strategy and a striatum-dependent landmark-based strategy (in a college-aged sample, Doeller et al., 2008).

When comparing young and older adults in learning the spatial layout of a virtual environment from self-directed navigation, Moffat et al. (2006) showed that older adults exhibited higher activations in the medial frontal gyrus (BA 10; see Table 1) and the anterior cingulate gyrus than younger adults, whereas younger adults exhibited higher activations than older adults in the hippocampal formation and posterior extrahippocampal areas such as the RSC, parietal cortex (i.e., inferior parietal lobule, precuneus) and angular gyrus. The authors suggested that these findings reflected a potential age-related compensatory shift in spatial memory performance toward anterior frontal systems away from medial temporal and posterior brain systems.

Notably, the authors also considered the possibility that the higher frontal activations of older adults could simply reflect age group differences in performance, since the older adults made slower virtual movements and more errors when recalling landmark locations. This alternative view relates well to other findings showing that older adults performed poorer than younger adults even on the first trial of a virtual Morris water maze (vMWM) when either group had no prior knowledge of the hidden platform’s location (Moffat and Resnick, 2002; Driscoll et al., 2005; Moffat et al., 2007). This suggests that older adults may possess impaired executive or strategic functions that are mediated by frontal areas at the outset of a navigational task (Moffat, 2009). Maintaining a high integrity of prefrontal gray and white matter in old age may help to attenuate or offset such executive declines, as greater volumes of lateral prefrontal gray matter and white matter have been shown to correlate positively with accurate vMWM search performance in in the first trial and onwards (Moffat et al., 2007). These findings corresponded well with additional findings showing robust negative associations between perseverative errors from the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and vMWM search accuracy, reinforcing the proposed role of the prefrontal cortex in mediating successful navigational/search performance (Moffat et al., 2007).

In addition, recent findings by Ariel and Moffat (2018) showed that older adults attained lower metacognitive confidence judgments than younger adults in a cognitive mapping task, and that these confidence judgments mediated the relationship between age and allocentric strategy use. As the prefrontal cortex has been widely implicated as the neural basis of metacognition (see Fleming and Dolan, 2012), these findings suggest that the differential engagement of prefrontal processes in metacognition could partially account for robust age-related differences in allocentric strategy use.

Furthermore, it is important to acknowledge the effects of motor control on age-related differences in navigational performance, particularly in virtual environments. When navigating virtual environments, older adults have been observed to move more slowly when completing learning trials than younger adults (e.g., Driscoll et al., 2005; Moffat et al., 2006; Taillade et al., 2013a). This has been dealt with by using movement speed or some motion-related variables as covariates (e.g., Moffat et al., 2001, 2006; Zhong et al., 2017). While important, this only partially addresses potential qualitative effects of motor control on older adults’ navigational performance. This is because older adults’ slower movements effectively increase the delay or retention intervals across learning trials, and sets up dual tasking scenarios (e.g., attending to stimuli on screen and manipulating the control device simultaneously) for older adults that may not necessarily apply to younger adults. More specifically, prefrontal executive resources have been shown to be vital for older adults in mediating the dual task demands of visual and motor processing (Taillade et al., 2013a), as well as in supporting gait (Szturm et al., 2017). Declines in these functions may thus predispose older adults to slower movements and poorer cognitive performance in both virtual (Taillade et al., 2013a,b, 2016; Szturm et al., 2017) and real-world (Taillade et al., 2016) environments.

Together with these concerns, age-related executive declines have also been suggested to adversely affect the allocation of attentional resources (Hartmeyer et al., 2017; Merriman et al., 2018). Specifically, older adults were slower than younger adults in responding to auditory probes at intersections when simultaneously deciding on the correct heading directions to take, especially when turning movements were required (Hartmeyer et al., 2017). Older adults were also distracted by crowds of moving pedestrians and demonstrated lower accuracy in finding the correct heading directions at intersections in the presence of such crowds (Merriman et al., 2018). This susceptibility to distraction in the presence of other moving agents may have been brought about by a corresponding age-related reduction in goal-tracking eye movements or saccades (Dowiasch et al., 2015).

Overall, it is important to understand the challenges older adults experience under dual task and attention-demanding scenarios because such knowledge will invariably inform future efforts at investigating how such challenges can be managed or ameliorated.

Summary and Future Directions

Taken together, this survey of spatial navigation studies in the cognitive aging literature suggests a shift away from hippocampal engagement toward increased engagement of extrahippocampal areas, notably the striatum and the prefrontal cortex, with advancing age. While extant neuroimaging studies have provided reliable findings showing that an increased engagement of the striatum, coupled with reduced engagement of the hippocampus, could account for older adults’ egocentric/response strategy use (Konishi et al., 2013; Schuck et al., 2015), the findings concerning age-related deficits in strategy switching (Harris et al., 2012; Harris and Wolbers, 2014), and landmark-direction associative learning (Zhong and Moffat, 2016) were behavioral in nature and speculative. It thus remains unknown as to whether dysfunctions in the fronto-hippocampal and the fronto-locus coeruleus circuits (as implicated in associative learning and strategy switching) would indeed be related to age-related declines in navigational ability. It is also possible that dysfunctions in these circuits would adversely affect navigational ability regardless of age and they may be essential for successful navigation. Moreover, we do not yet know whether the association of landmark and directional information is distinct from the association of other types of information (e.g., paired word associates, name-picture pairs, see Cabeza and Dennis, 2012). It thus remains possible that the brain systems related to the associations of these different pieces of information may be dissociable. Further investigations are definitely needed to clarify all these possibilities.

In addition, considering the importance of the prefrontal cortex for strategy switching, associative learning and mediating executive functions, future spatial navigation studies can take a system-oriented approach that investigate the interaction between prefrontal cortex, the hippocampus, the striatum, and/or other navigationally relevant regions (e.g., the RSC and precuneus; see, e.g., Brown et al., 2012; Sherrill et al., 2013; Chrastil et al., 2017) in both young and older adults. Most likely, this endeavor will present a broader picture of how age-related changes in prefrontal activity mediates or coordinates corresponding changes in activities in both hippocampal and extrahippocampal regions during spatial navigation.

Finally, it may also be important to clarify the significance of increased prefrontal activity in older adults during spatial navigation. Specifically, does higher (pre)frontal activation in older adults represent an adaptive (or maladaptive) compensation mechanism among older adults? (cf. Grady et al., 1994; Gutchess et al., 2005). To address this question, further investigations can adopt an individual differences approach when assessing older adults, as some recent studies have demonstrated that older adults with relatively high cognitive functions (O’Malley et al., 2018) or intact spatial learning ability (Zhong et al., 2017) can perform as well as younger adults in virtual navigation tasks. Consequently, the separate analysis of frontal activation profiles of older adults with differential levels of navigational performance or strategy preference shall provide greater insight into how older adults engage navigationally relevant executive processes.

Author Contributions

JZ and SM conceived the framework for the review. Both authors conducted literature review and drafted the manuscript. Both authors edited the manuscript and finalized it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer JMW declared a past co-authorship with one of the authors SM to the handling Editor.

References

Allison, S., and Head, D. (2017). Route repetition and route reversal: effects of age and encoding method. Psychol. Aging 32, 220–231. doi: 10.1037/pag0000170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonova, E., Parslow, D., Brammer, M., Dawson, G. R., Jackson, S. H., and Morris, R. G. (2009). Age-related neural activity during allocentric spatial memory. Memory 17, 125–143. doi: 10.1080/09658210802077348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ariel, R., and Moffat, S. D. (2018). Age-related similarities and differences in monitoring spatial cognition. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 25, 351–377. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2017.1305086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astur, R. S., St. Germain, S. A., Tolin, D., Ford, J., Russell, D., and Stevens, M. (2006). Hippocampus function predicts severity of post-traumatic stress disorder. Cyberpsychol. Beh. 9, 234–240. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohbot, V. D., McKenzie, S., Konishi, K., Fouquet, C., Kurdi, V., Schachar, R., et al. (2012). Virtual navigation strategies from childhood to senescence: evidence for changes across the life span. Front. Aging Neurosci. 4:28. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouret, S., and Sara, S. J. (2008). Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function. Trends Neurosci. 28, 574–582. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. I., Ross, R. S., Tobyne, S. M., and Stern, C. E. (2012). Cooperative interactions between hippocampal and striatal systems support flexible navigation. Neuroimage 60, 1316–1330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, P., Becker, S., and Burgess, N. (2007). Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol. Rev. 114, 340–375. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.114.2.340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., and Dennis, N. A. (2012). “Frontal lobes and aging,” in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, 2nd Edn. eds D. T. Stuss and R. T. Knight (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 628–652.

Google Scholar

Carrieri, M., Lancia, S., Bocchi, A., Ferrari, M., Piccardi, L., and Quaresima, V. (2018). Does ventrolateral prefrontal cortex help in searching for the lost key? Evidence from an fNIRS study. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 785–797. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9734-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chersi, F., and Burgess, N. (2015). The cognitive architecture of spatial navigation: hippocampal and striatal contributions. Neuron 88, 64–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrastil, E. R., Sherrill, K. R., Aselcioglu, I., Hasselmo, M. E., and Stern, C. E. (2017). Individual differences in human path integration abilities correlate with gray matter volume in retrosplenial cortex, hippocampus, and medial prefrontal cortex. eNeuro 4:ENEURO.0346-16.2017. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0346-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciaramelli, E. (2008). The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in navigation: a case of impaired wayfinding and rehabilitation. Neuropsychologia 46, 2099–2105. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, D., Serino, S., Tuena, C., Pedroli, E., Dakanalis, A., Cipresso, P., et al. (2017). Egocentric and allocentric spatial reference frames in aging: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 605–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daugherty, A. M., Bender, A. R., Yuan, P., and Raz, N. (2016). Changes in Search Path complexity and length during learning of a virtual water maze: age differences and differential associations with hippocampal subfield volumes. Cereb. Cortex 26, 2391–2401. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daugherty, A. M., Yuan, P., Dahle, C. L., Bender, A. R., Yang, Y., and Raz, N. (2015). Path complexity in virtual water maze navigation: differential associations with age, sex, and regional brain volume. Cereb. Cortex 25, 3122–3131. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doeller, C. F., King, J. A., and Burgess, N. (2008). Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 105, 5915–5920. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801489105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowiasch, S., Marx, S., Einhäuser, W., and Bremmer, F. (2015). Effects of aging on eye movements in the real world. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:46. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Driscoll, I., Hamilton, D. A., Petropoulos, H., Yeo, R. A., Brooks, W. M., Baumgartner, R. N., et al. (2003). The aging hippocampus: cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1344–1351. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Driscoll, I., Hamilton, D. A., Yeo, R. A., Brooks, W. M., and Sutherland, R. J. (2005). Virtual navigation in humans: the impact of age, sex, and hormones on place learning. Horm. Behav. 47, 326–335. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, S. M., and Dolan, R. J. (2012). The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 1338–1349. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil, M., Ancau, M., Schlesiger, M. I., Neitz, A., Allen, K., De Marco, R. J., et al. (2018). Impaired path integration in mice with disrupted grid cell firing. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 81–91. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0039-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. L., Maisog, J. M., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Mentis, M. J., Salerno, J. A., et al. (1994). Age-related changes in cortical blood flow activation during visual processing of faces and location. J. Neurosci. 14, 1450–1462. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-03-01450.1994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. L., McIntosh, A. R., and Craik, F. I. (2003). Age-related differences in the functional connectivity of the hippocampus during memory encoding. Hippocampus 13, 572–586. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutchess, A. H., Welsh, R. C., Hedden, T., Bangert, A., Minear, M., Liu, L. L., et al. (2005). Aging and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 84–96. doi: 10.1162/0898929052880048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, M. A., Wiener, J. M., and Wolbers, T. (2012). Aging specifically impairs switching to an allocentric navigational strategy. Front. Aging Neurosci. 4:29. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, M. A., and Wolbers, T. (2012). Ageing effects on path integration and landmark navigation. Hippocampus 22, 1770–1780. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, M. A., and Wolbers, T. (2014). How age-related strategy switching deficits affect wayfinding in complex environments. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 1095–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.10.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartmeyer, S., Grzeschik, R., Wolbers, T., and Wiener, J. M. (2017). The effects of attentional engagement on route learning performance in a virtual environment: an aging study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:235. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Q., and McNamara, T. P. (2018). Spatial updating strategy affects the reference frame in path integration. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 1073–1079. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1307-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Head, D., and Isom, M. (2010). Age effects on wayfinding and route learning skills. Behav. Brain Res. 209, 49–58. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iaria, G., Chen, J. K., Guariglia, C., Ptito, A., and Petrides, M. (2007). Retrosplenial and hippocampal brain regions in human navigation: complementary functional contributions to the formation and use of cognitive maps. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 890–899. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05371.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iaria, G., Palermo, L., Committeri, G., and Barton, J. J. (2009). Age differences in the formation and use of cognitive maps. Behav. Brain Res. 196, 187–191. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iaria, G., Petrides, M., Dagher, A., Pike, B., and Bohbot, V. D. (2003). Cognitive strategies dependent on the hippocampus and caudate nucleus in human navigation: variability and change with practice. J. Neurosci. 23, 5945–5952. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-13-05945.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klatzky, R. L. (1998). “Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: definitions, distinctions, and interconnections,” in Spatial Cognition: An Interdisciplinary Approach to Representing and Processing Spatial Knowledge LNAI (Vol. 1404), eds C. Freksa, C. Habel and K. F. Wender (Berlin Heidelberg, New York, NY: Springer), 1–17.

Google Scholar

Konishi, K., and Bohbot, V. D. (2013). Spatial navigational strategies correlate with gray matter in the hippocampus of healthy older adults tested in a virtual maze. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:1. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konishi, K., Etchamendy, N., Roy, S., Marighetto, A., Rajah, N., and Bohbot, V. D. (2013). Decreased functional magnetic resonance imaging activity in the hippocampus in favor of the caudate nucleus in older adults tested in a virtual navigation task. Hippocampus 23, 1005–1014. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lester, A. W., Moffat, S. D., Wiener, J. M., Barnes, C. A., and Wolbers, T. (2017). The aging navigational system. Neuron 95, 1019–1035. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U., and Sikström, S. (2001). Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 479–486. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01769-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S. C., Naveh-Benjamin, M., and Lindenberger, U. (2005). Aging neuromodulation impairs associative binding: a neurocomputational account. Psychol. Sci. 16, 445–450. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01555.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, I., Levy, R. M., Barton, J. J., and Iaria, G. (2011). Age and gender differences in various topographical orientation strategies. Brain Res. 1410, 112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G., and Philbeck, J. W. (1999). “Human navigation by path integration,” in Wayfinding Behavior: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes, ed. R. G. Golledge (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press), 125–151.

Google Scholar

Mahmood, O., Adamo, D., Briceno, E., and Moffat, S. D. (2009). Age differences in visual path integration. Behav. Brain Res. 205, 88–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maguire, E. A. (2001). The retrosplenial contribution to human navigation: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. Scand. J. Psychol. 42, 225–238. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchette, S. A., Vass, L. K., Ryan, J., and Epstein, R. A. (2014). Anchoring the neural compass: coding of local spatial reference frames in human medial parietal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1598–1606. doi: 10.1038/nn.3834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCunn, L. J., and Gifford, R. (2018). Spatial navigation and place imageability in sense of place. Cities 74, 208–218. doi: 10.1016/j.cities.2017.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merriman, N. A., Ondřej, J., Rybicki, A., Roudaia, E., O’Sullivan, C., and Newell, F. N. (2018). Crowded environments reduce spatial memory in older but not younger adults. Psychol. Res. 82, 407–428. doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0819-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meulenbroek, O., Petersson, K. M., Voermans, N., Weber, B., and Fernández, G. (2004). Age differences in neural correlates of route encoding and route recognition. Neuroimage 22, 1503–1514. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, A. S., Czajkowski, R., Zhang, N., Jeffery, K., and Nelson, A. J. (2018). Retrosplenial cortex and its role in spatial cognition. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 2:239821281875709. doi: 10.1177/2398212818757098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffat, S. D. (2009). Aging and spatial navigation: what do we know and where do we go? Neuropsychol. Rev. 19, 478–489. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9120-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffat, S. D. (2016). “Spatial cognition and wayfinding,” in Encyclopedia of Geropsychology, ed. N. A. Pachana (New York, NY: Springer), 2236–2244.

Google Scholar

Moffat, S. D., Elkins, W., and Resnick, S. M. (2006). Age differences in the neural systems supporting human allocentric spatial navigation. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 965–972. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffat, S. D., Kennedy, K. M., Rodrigue, K. M., and Raz, N. (2007). Extrahippocampal contributions to age differences in human spatial navigation. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1274–1282. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffat, S. D., and Resnick, S. M. (2002). Effects of age on virtual environment place navigation and allocentric cognitive mapping. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 851–859. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.5.851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffat, S. D., Zonderman, A. B., and Resnick, S. M. (2001). Age differences in spatial memory in a virtual environment navigation task. Neurobiol. Aging 22, 787–796. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00251-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Keefe, J., and Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map: preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 34, 171–175. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Keefe, J., and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

O’Malley, M., Innes, A., and Wiener, J. M. (2018). How do we get there? Effects of cognitive aging on route memory. Mem. Cognit. 46, 274–284. doi: 10.3758/s13421-017-0763-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfefferbaum, A., Adalsteinsson, E., and Sullivan, E. V. (2005). Frontal circuitry degradation marks healthy adult aging: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 26, 891–899. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodgers, M. K., Sindone, J. A. III., Moffat, S. D. (2012). Effects of age on navigation strategy. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 202.e15–222.e22. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuck, N. W., Doeller, C. F., Polk, T. A., Lindenberger, U., and Li, S. C. (2015). Human aging alters the neural computation and representation of space. Neuroimage 117, 141–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherrill, K. R., Erdem, U. M., Ross, R. S., Brown, T. I., Hasselmo, M. E., and Stern, C. E. (2013). Hippocampus and retrosplenial cortex combine path integration signals for successful navigation. J. Neurosci. 33, 19304–19313. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1825-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shine, J. P., Valdés-Herrera, J. P., Hegarty, M., and Wolbers, T. (2016). The human retrosplenial Cortex and thalamus code head direction in a global reference frame. J. Neurosci. 36, 6371–6381. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1268-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiers, H. J. (2008). Keeping the goal in mind: prefrontal contributions to spatial navigation. Neuropsychologia 46, 2106–2108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiers, H. J., and Maguire, E. A. (2006). Thoughts, behaviour, and brain dynamics during navigation in the real world. Neuroimage 31, 1826–1840. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiers, H. J., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). A navigational guidance system in the human brain. Hippocampus 17, 618–626. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stangl, M., Achtzehn, J., Huber, K., Dietrich, C., Tempelmann, C., and Wolbers, T. (2018). Compromised grid-cell-like representations in old age as a key mechanism to explain age-related navigational deficits. Curr. Biol. 28, 1108.e6–1115.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulpizio, V., Committeri, G., Lambrey, S., Berthoz, A., and Galati, G. (2016). Role of the human retrosplenial cortex/parieto-occipital sulcus in perspective priming. Neuroimage 125, 108–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szturm, T. J., Sakhalkar, V. S., Kanitkar, A., and Nankar, M. (2017). Computerized dual-task testing of gait and visuospatial cognitive functions; test-retest reliability and validity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:105. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taillade, M., N’Kaoua, B., and Sauzéon, H. (2016). Age-related differences and cognitive correlates of self-reported and direct navigation performance: the effect of real and virtual test conditions manipulation. Front. Psychol. 6:2034. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taillade, M., Sauzéon, H., Arvind Pala, P., Dejos, M., Larrue, F., Gross, C., et al. (2013a). Age-related wayfinding differences in real large-scale environments: detrimental motor control effects during spatial learning are mediated by executive decline? PLoS One 8:e67193. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taillade, M., Sauzéon, H., Dejos, M., Pala, P. A., Larrue, F., Wallet, G., et al. (2013b). Executive and memory correlates of age-related differences in wayfinding performances using a virtual reality application. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 20, 298–319. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2012.706247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vann, S. D., Aggleton, J. P., and Maguire, E. A. (2009). What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 792–802. doi: 10.1038/nrn2733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiener, J. M., Berthoz, A., and Wolbers, T. (2011). Dissociable cognitive mechanisms underlying human path integration. Exp. Brain Res. 208, 61–71. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2460-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiener, J. M., de Condappa, O., Harris, M. A., and Wolbers, T. (2013). Maladaptive bias for extrahippocampal navigation strategies in aging humans. J. Neurosci. 33, 6012–6017. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0717-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiener, J. M., Kmecova, H., and de Condappa, O. (2012). Route repetition and route retracing: effects of cognitive aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 4:7. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolbers, T. (2015). “Spatial navigation,” in International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. (Vol. 23) ed. J. D. Wright (Oxford: Elsevier), 161–171.

Wolbers, T., and Hegarty, M. (2010). What determines our navigational abilities? Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 138–146. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, P., Daugherty, A. M., and Raz, N. (2014). Turning bias in virtual spatial navigation: age-related differences and neuroanatomical correlates. Biol. Psychol. 96, 8–19. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, J. Y., and Kozhevnikov, M. (2016). Relating allocentric and egocentric survey-based representations to the self-reported use of a navigation strategy of egocentric spatial updating. J. Environ. Psychol. 46, 154–175. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, J. Y., Magnusson, K. R., Swarts, M. E., Clendinen, C. A., Reynolds, N. C., and Moffat, S. D. (2017). The application of a rodent-based Morris water maze (MWM) protocol to an investigation of age-related differences in human spatial learning. Behav. Neurosci. 131, 470–482. doi: 10.1037/bne0000219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, J. Y., and Moffat, S. D. (2016). Age-related differences in associative learning of landmarks and heading directions in a virtual navigation task. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:122. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: spatial navigation, cognitive aging, associative learning and memory, executive functioning, prefrontal cortex

Citation: Zhong JY and Moffat SD (2018) Extrahippocampal Contributions to Age-Related Changes in Spatial Navigation Ability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:272. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00272

Received: 21 April 2018; Accepted: 13 June 2018;
Published: 10 July 2018.

Edited by:

Elizabeth Chrastil, University of California, Santa Barbara, United States

Reviewed by:

Jan M. Wiener, Bournemouth University, United Kingdom
Lukas Kunz, Albert Ludwigs Universität Freiburg, Germany
Vladislava Segen, Bournemouth University, United Kingdom, in collaboration with reviewer JMW.

Copyright © 2018 Zhong and Moffat. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Scott D. Moffat, scott.moffat@psych.gatech.edu

Download