Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 17 July 2018
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
Volume 12 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00286

Strong Relation Between an EEG Functional Connectivity Measure and Postmenstrual Age: A New Potential Tool for Measuring Neonatal Brain Maturation

Laura Anna van de Pol1* Charlotte van ’t Westende1 Inge Zonnenberg2 Esther Koedam3† Ineke van Rossum3 Willem de Haan3 Marjan Steenweg1 Elisabeth Catharina van Straaten4 Cornelis Jan Stam4
  • 1Department of Child Neurology, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Neonatology, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 3Department of Neurology, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands

Fetal and neonatal brain connectivity development is highly complex. Studies have shown that functional networks change dramatically during development. The purpose of the current study was to determine how the mean phase lag index (mPLI), a measure of functional connectivity (FC), assessed with electroencephalography (EEG), changes with postmenstrual age (PMA) during the early stages of brain development after birth. Neonates (N = 131) with PMA 27.6–45.3 weeks who underwent an EEG for a medical reason were retrospectively studied. For each recording, global FC was assessed by obtaining a whole-head average of all local PLI values (pairwise between sensor space EEG signals). Global FC results were consequently correlated with PMA values in seven frequency bands. Local results were obtained for the frequency band with the strongest global association. There was a strong negative correlation between mPLI and PMA in most frequency bands. The strongest association was found in the delta frequency band (R = −0.616, p < 0.001) which was therefore topographically explored; the strongest correlations were between pairs of electrodes with at least one electrode covering the central sulcus. Even in this heterogeneous group of neonates, global FC strongly reflects PMA. The decrease in PLI may reflect the process of segregation of specific brain regions with increasing PMA. This was mainly found in the central brain regions, in parallel with myelination of these areas during early development. In the future, there may be a role for PLI in detecting atypical FC maturation. Moreover, PLI could be used to develop biomarkers for brain maturation and expose segregation processes in the neonatal brain.

Introduction

The development of the fetal and neonatal brain is highly complex. The brain follows an elaborate developmental trajectory, involving the formation of 10 billion cortical neurons, which migrate from their origin in the ventricular and the later-formed sub-ventricular zones to their final position and start to grow their own synaptic connections (van den Heuvel et al., 2015; Fernández et al., 2016). For example, at a postmenstrual age (PMA) of 20 weeks, a major structural change begins; the cortex expands greatly in surface area and becomes wrinkled and folded (Keeling, 2003; Bendersky et al., 2006; Rajagopalan et al., 2011). In addition, myelination is already seen in the white matter by 28 weeks, and between 34 and 46 weeks it becomes more prominently visible in the cortex bordering the central sulcus, extending in the posterior limb of the internal capsule (Sie et al., 1997; Counsell et al., 2002). These highly plastic changes make it a challenge to study the active neonatal brain, and to distinguish normal from abnormal development patterns.

A frequently used tool to study neonatal brain function is electroencephalography (EEG). EEG non-invasively records the electrical activity of the brain from the scalp. The signals that are recorded with the electrodes reflect synchronous neuronal activity of the cortex, in particular excitatory and inhibitory post-synaptic potentials of pyramidal cells in layer three and five related to synaptic currents. An advantage of this technique in neonates is that it can be applied at the bedside in the earliest postnatal phases at the neonatal intensive care unit. Visual assessment of the developing background pattern with increasing postmenstrual age is used in clinical practice. However, little is known about the development of functional brain networks in these early stages.

In the past decade, EEG has been increasingly used for “functional connectivity (FC)” analysis (Stam and van Straaten, 2012). FC can be defined as the temporal correlation between spatially distant neurophysiological events, and is a measure of functional communication between distant brain regions. EEG is a suitable tool for analyzing FC of the brain, because of the high temporal resolution between signals recorded from different brain areas.

Efficient functional communication between neuronal regions due to a gradual optimal organization of structural connections develops mostly after birth. Distant neural populations need to be connected by associational and callosal fibers, and the myelination of these fibers is only completed after the second decade of life. Previous studies have provided evidence that functional networks change more dramatically during development than structural (physical) networks. Especially during the first years of life, many studies point toward an increase in segregation (the neuronal processing carried out by separate groups of regions) and integration (the efficiency of global communication) of functional networks in the brain (see Cao et al., 2016 for a review). However, most of these studies are conducted in subjects with PMAs above 46 weeks, and little is known about how functional brain networks develop in the period between 24 and 46 weeks.

Although EEG is a very valuable clinical technique, it does have several methodological pitfalls. A common methodological problem with FC analysis is that time series recorded from different electrodes can reflect activity from common sources, yielding statistical interdependencies that are not representing FC of the brain. This is referred to by the problem of “volume conduction” (Guevara et al., 2005). The nonlinear FC measure “phase lag index” (PLI) diminishes the problem of volume conduction (Stam et al., 2007). The PLI is a measure for the asymmetry of the distribution of phase differences between two EEG signals; it reflects the consistency with which one signal is phase leading or lagging another signal. PLI discards phase differences that center around 0 mod π, reducing the effects of common sources by the principle that a non-zero phase lag cannot be explained by volume conduction. A number of studies have demonstrated that PLI can be successfully used as a measure for detecting differences between functional network organizations in a wide range of ages (Boersma et al., 2013; Engels et al., 2015; Smit et al., 2016; Yu et al., 2016; Sunwoo et al., 2017; van Lutterveld et al., 2017).

Recently, it has been shown that the PLI is sensitive to differences in brains of premature and full-term neonates at a PMA between 39 and 40 weeks in delta and beta frequency bands (González et al., 2011). In addition, networks built on PLI measures were strongly associated with gestational age (GA) in full-term healthy neonates with PMAs between 36 and 41 weeks (Tóth et al., 2017). Although these studies point towards a role for PLI as a measure of brain condition in neonates, little is known about PLI changes in the period before 36 weeks PMA, when the first brain networks start to develop, and changes in brain network organization are likely to occur on very short time scales.

The objective of this study is to investigate how the PLI develops over time during the very early stages of brain development in a diverse group of preterm and full-term born neonates who underwent an EEG in clinical practice. The relationship between PMA and PLI is retrospectively studied in a group of neonates with a PMA < 46 weeks during recording. We hypothesize that PLI reflects the strong plasticity of the development of the neonatal brain during the first weeks of life after birth.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

The group consisted of 131 subjects with PMA between 27–46 weeks, in whom an EEG had been recorded for a medical reason. Table 1 shows the patients characteristics. The most prevalent medical condition was hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy due to perinatal asphyxia, and was defined as one or more of the following criteria: Apgar score at 5 min after birth ≤ 5; post-partum resuscitation; pH < 7.0 until 1 h post-partum; base excess < −16 mmol/ml until 1 h post-partum, lactate > 10.0 mmol/l until 1 h post partum and encephalopathy defined as a Thompson score ≥ 7 or a continuous function monitoring pattern showing flat-trace, burst-suppression or continuous low voltage.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patient characteristics.

The research has been approved by the medical ethical committee of the VU University Medical Center (sub study of project: “Brain network analysis in preterm infants using electro-encephalography—a pilot study” Medical Ethical Committee protocol number: 2015.460 and NL-number NL54151.029.15). The medical ethical committee of the VUmc waived the necessity of an official consent form, since the procedures were part of a standard clinical practice.

EEG Recording and Post-processing

EEG recordings from neonates with PMA 24–46 weeks, made between March 2012 and March 2017, were retrieved from the database of the Department of Clinical Neurophysiology of the VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, using BrainRT (OSG digital equipment BrainRT; OSG b.v., Rumst, Belgium). In addition, to explore longitudinal PLI changes, subjects who had more than five EEG recordings over time before a PMA of 46 weeks, were selected from the database.

EEGs were recorded at the Department of Clinical Neurophysiology of the VU University Medical Center. 21 Ag/AgCl electrodes (Fp2, Fp1, F8, F7, F4, F3, A2, A1, T4, T3, C4, C3, T6, T5, P4, P3, O2, O1, Fz, Cz, Pz) were attached to the scalp at the positions of the International 10–20 system. Impedance was kept below 5 KOhm. Initial filters settings were a time constant of 0.6 s, resulting in a lower frequency cut-off of 12π*time constant = 0.265 Hz.

Signals were low-pass filtered at 100 Hz. The sample frequency was 500 Hz and EEG was digitized with 20 bit resolution. The average reference included all electrodes.

The EEG recordings were converted into ASCII-format, with the channels arranged according to the BrainWave software (version 0.9.152.4.1, available from http://home.kpn.nl/stam7883/brainwave.html) setting “Average” (see Supplementary Table S1). For subjects with more than one EEG recorded with PMA < 46 weeks, the first recording was used, and no further selection of EEGs was performed.

EEGs were downsampled to 250 Hz in BrainWave and the final epoch length consisted of 4096 samples (16.384 s). All channels and the first 50 epochs of each EEG recording were included for FC analysis and band-pass filtered into seven frequency bands: broad band: 0.1–45 Hz; delta: 0.5–4 Hz; theta: 4–8 Hz; lower alpha: 8–10 Hz; upper alpha: 10–13 Hz; beta: 13–30 Hz; gamma: 30–45 Hz.

For later data exploration, EEG quality was globally assessed by one of the authors (CvtW) based on epoch numbers; 1, 10, 20, 30, 40 and 50. During the visual assessment, EEG recordings were not filtered in one of the frequency bands. Unwanted non-brain signals such as electric fields generated by muscles or nearby equipment were indicated as significant artifacts when more than 30 percent of the epoch was affected by it. Quality of EEGs was scored as “high” when none or only one artifact was visually detected and “low” when two or more artifacts were visually detected. For clinical practice, EEGs were assessed by experienced clinical neurophysiologists and visually evaluated as “normal” or “abnormal.” EEGs were not categorized based on other criteria, such as sleep-states or pattern of EEG.

EEG Data Analysis: The Phase Lag Index (PLI)

The FC measure PLI was computed between all EEG channel pairs. The PLI reflects how consistent the phase of one signal, recorded in one channel, is leading or lagging the phase of a signal recorded in another channel, and ranges between 0 (no consistent phase leading or lagging) and 1 (complete phase leading or lagging; see Supplementary Figure S1; Stam et al., 2007). The PLI is calculated using the following equation:

PLI=| sign[ sin(Δφ(tk)) ] |(1)

where Δφ(tk) is the phase difference, calculated for time point tk with k = 1 … N, dependent on the sampling frequency. Sign refers to the signum function, which translates a positive value for the phase difference into 1, a negative value for the phase difference into −1 and no phase difference into 0. 〈 〉 refers to the mean value and | | denotes absolute values. FC analysis was performed with BrainWave software (version 0.9.152.4.1, available from http://home.kpn.nl/stam7883/brainwave.html; see Supplementary Figure S1).

PLI values were further analyzed in two ways: (1) the mean PLI of all pairs of channels was calculated for each epoch, followed by calculation of the mean over the 50 epochs per subject (the resulting value is referred to as “mPLI” in this article), and (2) the PLI values for each separate pair of channels were averaged over the 50 epochs per subject, resulting in a matrix of 21 vs. 21 pair-wise PLI (pwPLI) values per subject. The global analyses (including all channels) were done separately for the seven frequency bands and the local analyses (including two channels) were performed in the frequency band with the strongest association between PMA and mPLI.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS)® version 19.0 for Windows. The level of significance was set at p < 0.005 for the global analyses and p < 1 × 10−4 for the local analyses to adjust for multiple comparisons. The relationship between PMA and mPLI was assessed by calculating the Pearson’s correlation coefficient for the frequency bands without outliers and by calculating Spearman’s ρ for the frequency bands with one or more outliers. Outliers were defined as those with z-scores > 3.29. In addition, a linear regression model, using the Enter method (Field, 2014), was constructed for the frequency band with the strongest association between mPLI and PMA. Furthermore, the influence of GA and birth weight was explored by calculating multiple regression models in the frequency band with the strongest correlation between mPLI and PMA. The influences of gender and asphyxia were assessed by means of a Mann-Whitney U test.

To evaluate the effect of EEG quality, Pearson’s correlation was calculated for the relationship between PMA and mPLI after selection of only high quality EEGs. Furthermore, to investigate the relationship between mPLI and PMA for EEG recordings that were clinically interpreted as normal, Pearson’s correlation was calculated.

To investigate the contribution to the total (broad band) power spectrum of the frequency band with the strongest association, power spectrum analysis was conducted using the Fast Fourier Transform option in Brainwave. The mean relative contribution was calculated over the 50 epochs per subject for the frequency band with the strongest association.

To extract information about the topography of the changes in mPLI with PMA, Pearson’s correlations were used, since topographical exploration was investigated in the frequency band with the strong linear correlation. Pearson’s correlations were calculated separately between all pwPLIs and PMA for the frequency band with the strongest association between mPLI and PMA. Scatterplots were checked for linearity and normally distributed data across the regression line.

Longitudinal changes in mPLI in relation to PMA within subjects were assessed by calculating Spearman’s ρ.

Results

Subjects

The final group consisted of 131 subjects with a median GA of 39.0 weeks (range: 24.00–42.43 weeks) and a median PMA at the moment of EEG recording of 40.14 weeks (range: 27.57–45.29 weeks). Table 1 shows a detailed description of the patient characteristics.

Relationship Between mPLI and PMA

There was a strong negative correlation between PMA and mPLI in all frequency bands; correlations for all frequency bands are summarized in Table 2. The strongest association was found in the delta frequency band, with R = −0.616 and p < 0.001.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of correlations between PMA during recording and PLI.

A linear regression model was constructed for the delta frequency band. PMA explains a significant proportion of the variance in mPLI in the delta frequency band (F(1,129) = 47.570, p < 0.001), with R = −0.616, R2 = 0.380. The regression coefficients for the intercept value and the slope value are significantly different from zero, with B0 = 0.344, t(129) = 13.436, p < 0.001 and B1 = −0.005, t(129) = −6.897, p < 0.001, respectively. The scatterplot is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Relationship between PMA and mPLI in the delta frequency band. The linear relationship between PMA and PLI is shown for the regression model, with B0 = 0.344 and B1 = −0.005. Dots correspond to individual subjects (N = 131). PMA = postmenstrual age, mPLI = mean phase lag index.

Multiple regression analysis was used to test if GA significantly predicted variance in mPLI in addition to PMA in the delta frequency band. The results of the regression indicated that GA did not significantly predict more of the variance in mPLI (R2change = 0.011, F(1,128) = 2.257, p = 0.135).

Adding birth weight to the model with PMA and to the model with PMA and GA did not significantly predict more of the variance in mPLI (R2change = 0.026, F(1,122) = 5.241, p = 0.024, and R2change = 0.014, F(1,122) = 2.933, p = 0.089, respectively).

When comparing males vs. females, and asphyxia patients vs. patients without asphyxia, no differences in mPLI values in the delta frequency band were found (U = 2280.5, z = 0.724, p = 0.724 and U = 3169.0, z = 0.096, p = 0.924, respectively).

When selecting only high-quality EEGs, the group consisted of 63 subjects with a median PMA of 39.71 weeks (range: 32.0–44.0 weeks). There was a strong correlation between mPLI and PMA, with R = −0.571, p < 0.001 in the delta frequency band. The scatterplot is shown in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatterplot of PMA and mPLI in the delta frequency band, in a subgroup of subjects with EEGs assessed as high quality. Dots correspond to individual subjects (N = 63). PMA = postmenstrual age, mPLI = mean phase lag index.

For EEGs clinically interpreted as normal (N = 30, mean PMA: 39.4 weeks, range PMA: 27.0–46.0 weeks), a strong relationship between mPLI and PMA with R = −0.757, p < 0.001 was found. The scatterplot is shown in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Scatterplot of PMA and mPLI in the delta frequency band in a subgroup with EEG clinically interpreted as normal. Dots correspond to individual subjects (N = 30). PMA = postmenstrual age, mPLI = mean phase lag index.

A power spectrum analysis was conducted to assess the percentage of delta in the power spectrum. One-hundred and twenty-four subjects (95%) had more than 60 percent of delta power in their EEG recording. Supplementary Figure S2 shows the power spectra of the individuals and the average power spectrum of the group.

The Topography of the Changes in PLI With PMA

By calculating the Pearson’s correlation between the pwPLI values and PMA, information on the topography of the changes in FC was extracted for the delta frequency band. Supplementary Table S2 shows all the Pearson’s correlations between PMA and pwPLI values. Figure 4 shows the 10 highest negative correlations on the scalp, with colors ranging from yellow (the 10th highest negative correlation) to red (the highest negative correlation). The highest negative correlations (with PMA) were the functional connectivity between electrodes T4 and F3, T4 and C3 and Cz and C3 (R = −0.609, p < 0.001, R = −0.604, p < 0.001 and R = −0.601, p < 0.001, respectively).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Top 10 correlations per couple of channels between pwPLI and PMA for the delta frequency band (N = 131). PMA = postmenstrual age, pwPLI = pair wise phase lag index.

Longitudinal Assessment of mPLI

In two subjects more than five EEGs were recorded. The first subject, with a diagnosis of a severe hemi-lissencephaly and intractable epilepsy, had eight EEG recordings, made between 41.1 and 52.3 weeks PMA. A trend towards a positive correlation between mPLI and PMA in the delta band was found, however this was not significant (ρ = 0.667, p = 0.071).

The second subject, with a diagnosis of cortical dysplasia causing epilepsy, had six EEG recordings, made between 33.1 and 48.9 weeks PMA. There was a significant negative correlation (ρ = −0.943, p = 0.005). A linear regression model was constructed; PMA explains a significant proportion of the variance in mPLI in the delta frequency band (F(1,4) = 35.896, p = 0.004), with R = −0.949, R2 = 0.900. The regression coefficients for the intercept value and the slope value are significantly different from zero, with B0 = 0.345, t(4) = 11.773, p < 0.001 and B1 = −0.004, t(4) = −5.991, p = 0.004, respectively. The scatterplot is given in Figure 5, including a reference line from the linear regression model of the total group in the delta frequency band.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Relationship between PMA and mPLI in the delta frequency band in one subject at six different time points. The equation line of the resulting linear regression model is shown in black. The equation line of the regression model of the relation between PLI and PMA of the total group, based on the cross-sectional data, is shown in red as a reference. PMA = postmenstrual age, mPLI = mean phase lag index.

Discussion

To date, there are few studies that have used FC measures to investigate how the neonatal brain develops. The purpose of the current study was to determine how the mPLI changes with age during the very early stages of brain development after birth. Our main finding was the strong decrease in mPLI with maturation from PMA 27–46 weeks in most frequency bands, even in this diverse group of subjects. The strongest correlation between mPLI and PMA was found in the delta frequency band, which also had the highest power in 95% of the subjects. Other functional connectivity studies in neonates have found significant results in the delta frequency band as well (González et al., 2011; Tokariev et al., 2016; Tóth et al., 2017), in line with our findings that the delta frequency band is (one of) the most interesting frequency bands to study in the neonatal brain.

Several graphoelements that partly consist of delta activity appear and disappear in the EEG during the period from PMA 27–46 weeks. For example, central delta brushes are usually seen from PMA 28–38 weeks, while anterior delta runs are usually seen from PMA 31–44 weeks (Tsuchida et al., 2013; Whitehead et al., 2017). This could influence functional connectivity outcomes that are based on the PLI measure. In addition, it is thought that various temporal frequencies in the EEG, including delta frequencies, reflect different neural sources (Pedley, 1997; Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004). However, it remains unclear what neuronal mechanisms underlie the significant contribution of delta activity to the neonatal EEG, as well as in graphoelements as in functional connectivity.

The decrease of the FC measure mPLI with age may reflect an increase of segregation in the neonatal brain, with less centralized connections. This was also found in an EEG based network study by Meijer et al. (2014) demonstrating a decrease in coherence between the hemispheres with maturation in neonates. They speculated that this reflected the increasing complexity of the neuronal network with maturation. In line with this finding our study showed that six out of the ten strongest correlations between PLI and PMA was between interhemispherical pairs of electrodes indicating differentiation between the hemispheres. Another study, in which correlations between frequency-specific amplitudes were calculated, found frontal modules (subgroups of cortical regions) in full-term neonates (GA: 37.6–43.5 weeks), whereas these were not present in preterm neonates (GA: 28.3–33.8 weeks), again supporting the idea of segregation processes occurring in early stages (Omidvarnia et al., 2014).

Further topographical exploration of the negative correlation between mPLI and PMA in the delta frequency band revealed that the strongest correlations were between pairs of electrodes that were both on the central sulcus, or between pairs of electrodes of which one was on the central sulcus and one was on a frontal region. We speculate that this pattern might reflect the current concept of myelination of the neonatal brain, with brain maturation starting in the central area and proceeding towards the parieto-occipital cortex and corticospinal tracts of the pre- and postcentral gyri as the regions with the strongest increase in myelination during this period (Sie et al., 1997; Ruoss et al., 2001; Counsell et al., 2002), as a substrate for the increasing segregation of brain functions.

In contrast to our findings, Koolen et al. (2016) report an increase in synchronization of early cortical activity across the neocortex in premature infants with PMAs between 30 and 44 weeks. They used the activation synchrony index (ASI) which statistically quantifies the temporal coincidence of bursts in the cortical activity. A possible explanation for this discrepancy is that results derived from the ASI cannot be compared to results derived from the PLI, since the ASI focuses on bursts only, and the PLI on the overall EEG. Eiselt et al. (2001) have shown that burst periods provide higher coherence values than interburst periods. In addition, the lowest values of coherence were observed just before the burst started and during the burst, maximal coherence was reached at different times in different frequency bands. This suggests that segmentation of continuous EEG would affect the outcome of functional connectivity, since bursts could be cut into half. However, since the cuts are arbitrary and 50 epochs are used to calculate the mPLI in this study, this influence is minimized.

Several questions remain unanswered at present. To develop a full picture of the integration and segregation processes in the neonatal brain, additional studies are needed that take into account different FC measures, such as the Newman’s modularity and measures derived from minimum spanning trees for example (Newman, 2006; Stam et al., 2014).

Several studies have shown that EEG can be used as a sensitive and objective evaluation of the severity of brain injury related to asphyxia (Mariani et al., 2008; Nash et al., 2011; Briatore et al., 2013). Therefore, it was an unanticipated finding that asphyxia had no significant effect on functional connectivity outcome in our study. A possible explanation for this might be that the changes in EEG patterns of neonates with asphyxia are related to other aspects of the EEG, such as background amplitude, and that functional connectivity in the delta frequency band stays unaffected. Furthermore, it is difficult to draw general conclusions from the results of the longitudinal analyses, since only two subjects were investigated.

Studying FC in neonates with the use of EEG recordings is technically challenging, due to highly plastic changes in the brain during this period. Researchers use a variety of selection criteria for analyzing FC, including selections based on sleep-states and EEG quality (Grieve et al., 2008; González et al., 2011; Omidvarnia et al., 2014; Koolen et al., 2016; Tokariev et al., 2016; Tóth et al., 2017). Background patterns change dramatically with PMA in the neonatal period from discontinuous to more continuous patterns. EEG phenomena recorded during the early periods of discontinuity are thought to result from input generated spontaneously at the sensory periphery rather than by cortico-cortical connections, which makes it difficult to understand what changes in FC measures in the neonatal period actually indicate (Colonnese and Khazipov, 2012). In our explorative study no selection based on sleep states was made. Nevertheless, a strong correlation between mPLI and PMA was found. It remains unclear to what extent the age related changes in background pattern or sleep stages have contributed to the observed decrease in mPLI with age.

The influence of common sources on the ability of the PLI to detect real changes in synchronization is studied in different montages by Stam et al. (2007). They showed that PLI values were hardly influenced by different montages during pre-seizure epochs, and intermediately influenced by average referencing during seizure epochs. The PLI was less influenced by different montages (average, source, mastoids, Cz, bipolar) than the phase coherence and the imaginary part of coherence (Stam et al., 2007). Peraza et al. (2012) investigated the effects of volume conduction on PLI networks. They concluded that PLI networks based on independent sources in a volume conduction environment were different from random networks, providing evidence that volume conduction affects PLI inference in general. However, they investigated the performance of PLI networks and not PLI as a functional connectivity measure (Peraza et al., 2012).

In this study, we investigated the influence of artifacts by comparing outcomes between the total group without any kind of selection and the group with only high quality EEGs, which did not influence our results. This observation could be used to argue against arbitrary selection of EEG epochs when using the PLI, as long as a sufficiently large number of epochs is included in the analysis. However, there still is abundant room for further progress in determining the best ways to analyze FC in neonatal EEG recordings.

In addition to the strengths of our study (the large number of neonates included in this study and the very young PMA at the time of EEG recording) there are some drawbacks of this study. This retrospective study was conducted in a heterogeneous group of neonates, who had an indication for an EEG, with a wide range of clinical characteristics and administered medication, possibly influencing the EEG. On the other hand it is interesting that even despite this heterogeneity such a strong negative correlation between mPLI and PMA could be demonstrated. Interestingly, the strength of the negative correlation between mPLI and PMA increased when we repeated the analyses in a subgroup with EEGs that were clinically interpreted as normal, indicating that this relation might even be stronger when assessed in a healthy subgroup. Additionally, other possible influences, such as changes in head circumference, on the FC analysis were not completely unraveled in this study. A prospective EEG study including healthy and preterm neonates with a relatively uncomplicated clinical course is currently carried out to replicate this data.

This study contributes to the fundamental understanding of FC dynamics in the neonatal brain. We conclude that the PLI could serve as an objective measure for cerebral maturation that exposes segregation processes of neural circuits, resulting in better understanding of how the brain matures. Because of the high sensitivity of mPLI for PMA, PLI could possibly be used for detecting atypical FC maturation of the brain, on group or even subject-specific level. Moreover, the PLI may play a role in developing markers for brain maturation for future treatment strategies. Subjects at risk could be followed in time which may contribute to earlier detection of pathology and adequate long term prognosis of psychomotor development.

Author Contributions

LAP: concept and design of the project, execution, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting and writing of the research report. CW: execution, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting and writing of the research report. IZ: concept and design of the project, revising of the research report. EK and IR: data analysis, revising of the research report. WH: interpretation of data, revising of the research report. MS and ECS: revising of the research report. CJS: concept and design of the project, interpretation of data, drafting and revising the research report. All authors approved the final article.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

FC, Functional connectivity; mPLI, mean Phase lag index; PMA, post-menstrual age; pwPLI, pair-wise PLI.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00286/full#supplementary-material

References

Bendersky, M., Musolino, P. L., Rugilo, C., Schuster, G., and Sica, R. E. (2006). Normal anatomy of the developing fetal brain. Ex vivo anatomical-magnetic resonance imaging correlation. J. Neurol. Sci. 250, 20–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2006.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boersma, M., Kemner, C., de Reus, M. A., Collin, G., Snijders, T. M., Hofman, D., et al. (2013). Disrupted functional brain networks in autistic toddlers. Brain Connect. 3, 41–49. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briatore, E., Ferrari, F., Pomero, G., Boghi, A., Gozzoli, L., Micciolo, R., et al. (2013). EEG findings in cooled asphyxiated newborns and correlation with site and severity of brain damage. Brain Dev. 35, 420–426. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsaki, G., and Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, M., Huang, H., Peng, Y., Dong, Q., and He, Y. (2016). Toward developmental connectomics of the human brain. Front. Neuroanat. 10:25. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colonnese, M., and Khazipov, R. (2012). Spontaneous activity in developing sensory circuits: implications for resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 62, 2212–2221. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Counsell, S. J., Maalouf, E. F., Fletcher, A. M., Duggan, P., Battin, M., Lewis, H. J., et al. (2002). MR imaging assessment of myelination in the very preterm brain. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 23, 872–881.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Eiselt, M., Schindler, J., Arnold, M., Witte, H., Zwiener, U., and Frenzel, J. (2001). Functional interactions within the newborn brain investigated by adaptive coherence analysis of EEG. Neurophysiol. Clin. 31, 104–113. doi: 10.1016/s0987-7053(01)00251-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engels, M. M., Stam, C. J., van der Flier, W. M., Scheltens, P., de Waal, H., and van Straaten, E. C. (2015). Declining functional connectivity and changing hub locations in Alzheimer’s disease: an EEG study. BMC Neurol. 15:145. doi: 10.1186/s12883-015-0400-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, V., Llinares-Benadero, C., and Borrell, V. (2016). Cerebral cortex expansion and folding: what have we learned? EMBO J. 35, 1021–1044. doi: 10.15252/embj.201593701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. P. (2014). Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics. 4th Edn. London: Sage.

Google Scholar

González, J. J., Mañas, S., De Vera, L., Méndez, L. D., López, S., Garrido, J. M., et al. (2011). Assessment of electroencephalographic functional connectivity in term and preterm neonates. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 696–702. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grieve, P. G., Isler, J. R., Izraelit, A., Peterson, B. S., Fifer, W. P., Myers, M. M., et al. (2008). EEG functional connectivity in term age extremely low birth weight infants. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 2712–2720. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guevara, R., Velazquez, J. L., Nenadovic, V., Wennberg, R., Senjanovic, G., and Dominguez, L. G. (2005). Phase synchronization measurements using electroencephalographic recordings: what can we really say about neuronal synchrony? Neuroinformatics 3, 301–314. doi: 10.1385/ni:3:4:301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keeling, J. (2003). Fetal and Neonatal Pathology. 2nd Edn. London: Springer-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Koolen, N., Dereymaeker, A., Räsänen, O., Jansen, K., Vervisch, J., Matic, V., et al. (2016). Early development of synchrony in cortical activations in the human. Neuroscience 322, 298–307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mariani, E., Scelsa, B., Pogliani, L., Introvini, P., and Lista, G. (2008). Prognostic value of electroencephalograms in asphyxiated newborns treated with hypothermia. Pediatr. Neurol. 39, 317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2008.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meijer, E. J., Hermans, K. H., Zwanenburg, A., Jennekens, W., Niemarkt, H. J., Cluitmans, P. J., et al. (2014). Functional connectivity in preterm infants derived from EEG coherence analysis. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 18, 780–789. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2014.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nash, K. B., Bonifacio, S. L., Glass, H. C., Sullivan, J. E., Barkovich, A. J., Ferriero, D. M., et al. (2011). Video-EEG monitoring in newborns with hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy treated with hypothermia. Neurology 76, 556–562. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31820af91a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, M. E. (2006). Modularity and community structure in networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 8577–8582. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601602103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Omidvarnia, A., Fransson, P., Metsäranta, M., and Vanhatalo, S. (2014). Functional bimodality in the brain networks of preterm and term human newborns. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2657–2668. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedley, T. A. T. R. (1997). Physiological Basis of the EEG. New York, NY: Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Peraza, L. R., Asghar, A. U., Green, G., and Halliday, D. M. (2012). Volume conduction effects in brain network inference from electroencephalographic recordings using phase lag index. J. Neurosci. Methods 207, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajagopalan, V., Scott, J., Habas, P. A., Kim, K., Corbett-Detig, J., Rousseau, F., et al. (2011). Local tissue growth patterns underlying normal fetal human brain gyrification quantified in utero. J. Neurosci. 31, 2878–2887. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5458-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruoss, K., Lävblad, K., Schroth, G., Moessinger, A. C., and Fusch, C. (2001). Brain development (sulci and gyri) as assessed by early postnatal MR imaging in preterm and term newborn infants. Neuropediatrics 32, 69–74. doi: 10.1055/s-2001-13871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sie, L. T., van der Knaap, M. S., Van Wezel-Meijler, G., and Valk, J. (1997). MRI assessment of myelination of motor and sensory pathways in the brain of preterm and term-born infants. Neuropediatrics 28, 97–105. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-973680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smit, D. J., de Geus, E. J., Boersma, M., Boomsma, D. I., and Stam, C. J. (2016). Life-span development of brain network integration assessed with phase lag index connectivity and minimum spanning tree graphs. Brain Connect. 6, 312–325. doi: 10.1089/brain.2015.0359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., Nolte, G., and Daffertshofer, A. (2007). Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1178–1193. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., Tewarie, P., Van Dellen, E., van Straaten, E. C., Hillebrand, A., and Van Mieghem, P. (2014). The trees and the forest: characterization of complex brain networks with minimum spanning trees. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 92, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., and van Straaten, E. C. (2012). The organization of physiological brain networks. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1067–1087. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunwoo, J. S., Lee, S., Kim, J. H., Lim, J. A., Kim, T. J., Byun, J. I., et al. (2017). Altered functional connectivity in idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder: a resting-state EEG study. Sleep 40:zsx058. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tokariev, A., Videman, M., Palva, J. M., and Vanhatalo, S. (2016). Functional brain connectivity develops rapidly around term age and changes between vigilance states in the human newborn. Cereb. Cortex 26, 4540–4550. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tóth, B., Urbán, G., Háden, G. P., Márk, M., Török, M., Stam, C. J., et al. (2017). Large-scale network organization of EEG functional connectivity in newborn infants. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4019–4033. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuchida, T. N., Wusthoff, C. J., Shellhaas, R. A., Abend, N. S., Hahn, C. D., Sullivan, J. E., et al. (2013). American clinical neurophysiology society standardized EEG terminology and categorization for the description of continuous EEG monitoring in neonates: report of the American Clinical Neurophysiology Society critical care monitoring committee. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 30, 161–173. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0b013e3182872b24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, M. P., Kersbergen, K. J., de Reus, M. A., Keunen, K., Kahn, R. S., Groenendaal, F., et al. (2015). The neonatal connectome during preterm brain development. Cereb. Cortex 25, 3000–3013. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Lutterveld, R., van Dellen, E., Pal, P., Yang, H., Stam, C. J., and Brewer, J. (2017). Meditation is associated with increased brain network integration. Neuroimage 158, 18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehead, K., Pressler, R., and Fabrizi, L. (2017). Characteristics and clinical significance of delta brushes in the EEG of premature infants. Clin. Neurophysiol. Pract. 2, 12–18. doi: 10.1016/j.cnp.2016.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, M., Gouw, A. A., Hillebrand, A., Tijms, B. M., Stam, C. J., van Straaten, E. C., et al. (2016). Different functional connectivity and network topology in behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: an EEG study. Neurobiol. Aging 42, 150–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: electroencephalography, phase lag index, functional connectivity, maturation, neonate

Citation: van de Pol LA, van ’t Westende C, Zonnenberg I, Koedam E, van Rossum I, de Haan W, Steenweg M, van Straaten EC and Stam CJ (2018) Strong Relation Between an EEG Functional Connectivity Measure and Postmenstrual Age: A New Potential Tool for Measuring Neonatal Brain Maturation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:286. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00286

Received: 20 February 2018; Accepted: 26 June 2018;
Published: 17 July 2018.

Edited by:

Felix Scholkmann, UniversitätsSpital Zürich, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Christos Frantzidis, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
Amir Omidvarnia, Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Australia

Copyright © 2018 van de Pol, van ’t Westende, Zonnenberg, Koedam, van Rossum, de Haan, Steenweg, van Straaten and Stam. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Laura Anna van de Pol, l.vandepol@vumc.nl

Present address: Esther Koedam, Stichting Epilepsie Instellingen Nederland-SEIN, Heemstede, Netherlands

Download