Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 30 July 2018
Sec. Speech and Language
Volume 12 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00305

Learning a Foreign Language: A Review on Recent Findings About Its Effect on the Enhancement of Cognitive Functions Among Healthy Older Individuals

  • Department of Applied Linguistics, Faculty of Informatics and Management, University of Hradec Králové, Hradec Králové, Czechia

Currently, there is an increasing number of older population groups, especially in developed countries. This demographic trend, however, may cause serious problems, such as an increase in aging diseases, one of which is dementia whose main symptom consists in the decline of cognitive functioning. Although there has been ongoing pharmacological research on this neurological disorder, it has not brought satisfying results as far as its treatment is concerned. Therefore, governments all over the world are trying to develop alternative, non-pharmacological strategies/activities, which could help to prevent this cognitive decline while this aging population is still healthy in order to reduce future economic and social burden. One of the non-pharmacological approaches, which may enhance cognitive abilities and protect against the decline in healthy older population, seems to be the learning of a foreign language. The purpose of this mini-review article is to discuss recent findings about the effect of foreign language learning on the enhancement of cognitive functions among healthy older individuals. The findings, divided into three research areas, show that the learning of a foreign language may generate a lot of benefits for older individuals, such as enhancement of cognitive functioning, their self-esteem, increased opportunities of socializing, or reduction of costs. However, as Ware et al. (2017) indicate, any intervention program on foreign language learning should be well thought of and tailored to the needs of older people in order to be effective and avoid accompanying factors, such as older people’s anxiety or low self-confidence. Nevertheless, more empirical studies should be done in this area.

Introduction

The population is aging. For example, in Europe, older people aged 65+ years form 18% of the whole population. It is expected that by 2050, the older population will outnumber the young population in many developed countries (Statista, 2017). This demographic trend, however, may cause serious problems, such as an increase in aging diseases, one of which is dementia whose main symptom consists in the decline of cognitive functioning. This is connected with the brain atrophy, particularly in the temporal cortex, the region that is related to declarative memory (see Buckner, 2004), which is encoded by the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex, loss of synaptic connections (Maston, 2010), and the occurrence of neuropathological symptoms associated with dementia (see Antoniou and Wright, 2017). Although there has been ongoing pharmacological research on this neurological disorder, it has not brought satisfying results as far as its treatment is concerned (Karakaya et al., 2013).

Therefore, governments all over the world are trying to develop alternative, non-pharmacological strategies/activities, which could help to prevent this cognitive decline while this aging population is still healthy in order to reduce future economic and social burden (Maresova et al., 2016). These alternative, non-pharmacological intervention therapies can be divided into several groups, which have a positive impact on the enhancement of cognitive functions: physical activities, cognitive training, healthy diet (see Klimova and Kuca, 2015), as well as social enhancement interventions (see Ballesteros et al., 2015), including the use of modern information and communication technologies (Peter et al., 2013; Ballesteros et al., 2014). One of the cognitive training activities, which may enhance cognitive abilities and protect against the decline in healthy older population, seems to be the learning of a foreign language (see Antoniou et al., 2013; Kroll and Dussias, 2017). As Connor (2016) points out, learning a foreign language can promote thinking skills, increase mental agility and delay the aging of the brain. However, as Kurdziel et al. (2017) explain, the retrieval of new words among older people is harder since their fluid intelligence (i.e., the ability to reason and solve things), as well as the working, short-term, memory (i.e., management of immediately available information) are getting affected in the course of aging. On the contrary, their crystallized intelligence (i.e., the ability to use experience, knowledge and skills) remain intact in the aging process (see Kavé et al., 2008). Kurdziel et al. (2017) also state that the decline in language ability among older people is slower than the decline in global memory. In addition, older individuals possess a superior raw vocabulary even if compared with well-educated adults of young generation. In addition, foreign language learning does not have any side effect (Bak, 2016) and can help reduce country’s economic burden (Bialystok et al., 2016). Simply, it does not do any harm (see Strauss, 2015). Abutalebi and Clahsen (2015) present that knowledge about language processing in older individuals and about the potential factors that prevent cognitive decline is currently very much desirable since it may contribute to preparing for the demographic changes which our society faces.

The purpose of this mini-review article is to discuss recent findings about the effect of foreign language learning on the enhancement of cognitive functions among healthy older individuals.

Methods

The methodology of this mini-review article is based on Moher et al. (2009). Studies were selected on the basis of the following keyword collocations: healthy aging and foreign language learning; healthy older individuals and foreign language learning, healthy older individuals and bilingualism, found in the world’s acknowledged databases: Web of Science, PubMed, Scopus and ScienceDirect. The search was not limited by time since the studies on the research topic were scarce. Altogether 43 studies, including both review and original articles, were detected, most of them were identified in ScienceDirect and Web of Science, followed by PubMed and Scopus. The analysis was done by identifying the key words and checking duplication of available sources in the databases mentioned above. Afterwards, the studies were assessed for their relevancy, i.e., verification on the basis of abstracts whether the selected study corresponds to the set goal. After the exclusion of such studies, 26 studies remained for the full-text analysis. Out of 26 studies, 12 were empirical or randomized control studies, which are in detail described in Table 1. The review studies (e.g., Antoniou et al., 2013; Lee and Tzeng, 2016; Kurdziel et al., 2017), the studies dealing with the younger adults (e.g., Schlegel et al., 2012; Bellander et al., 2016) and the studies with patients suffering from dementia, respectively Alzheimer’s disease (e.g., Woumans et al., 2015; Bialystok et al., 2016) were used for comparison reasons. Moreover, the author also explored websites connected with the research topic, e.g., SeniorsMatter (2017).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. An overview of the detected empirical studies on the effect of foreign language learning on the enhancement of cognitive functions among healthy older individuals.

Findings and Their Discussion

As it has been stated in the “Methods” section, there is a lack of studies on the learning of a foreign language and its effect on the enhancement of cognitive functioning in older people, apart from those on bilingualism (see Klimova et al., 2017a). Overall, the identified studies can be divided into three main areas: studies concerning the brain plasticity in the old age and foreign language learning; studies focused on foreign language learning among healthy older individuals; and studies aimed at bilingualism and healthy aging, including the electrophysiological studies. All of them also discuss the cognitive aspects.

Plasticity of the Brain in the Old Age and Foreign Language Learning

The brain remains with considerable plasticity even in the old age. Although there is some neural deterioration that rises with age, the brain has the capacity to increase neural activity and develop neural scaffolding to regulate cognitive function (Park and Reuter-Lorenz, 2009; Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014). For example, Cheng et al. (2015) maintain that both short-term and long-term period of foreign language learning may lead to the changes in the structure of the brain, which consequently may contribute to the promotion of the cognitive reserve, i.e., the resilience to neuropathological damage of the brain (Stern, 2013). This has been also confirmed by Lee and Tzeng (2016), who claim that foreign language learning results in effective structural as well as functional connectivity in the brain due to neural plasticity. They indicate that the effective connectivity due to foreign language learning enhances the capacity for language processing and general executive control by reorganizing neural circuitries. Furthermore, research shows that foreign language learning has a positive impact on both white and gray matter structures (see Bellander et al., 2016). For instance, Schlegel et al. (2012) in their randomized controlled study with 11 English speakers (average age of 20 years) who took a 9-month intensive course in written and spoken Modern Standard Chinese and 16 controls who did not study a language reported that the plasticity of the white matter played a significant role in adult language learning. Although their adult learners showed progressive changes in white matter tracts, associated with traditional left hemisphere language areas and their right hemisphere analogs, the most important changes appeared in frontal lobe tracts crossing the genu of the corpus callosum-a region, which is not generally involved in current neural models of language processing. Tyler et al. (2010) in their study on preserved syntactic processing across the life span, argue that this is caused by the shift from a primarily left hemisphere frontotemporal system to a bilateral functional language network. In addition, Connor (2016) described a study of retired people doing an intensive language course of 5 h a day on the Isle of Skye to learn Gaelic (see Bak et al., 2016). After finishing the course, the scientists discovered these people were more mentally agile than those doing a course on something else. As Antoniou et al. (2013) indicate, foreign language training may engage a larger brain network than other forms of cognitive training that have been investigated (e.g., math and crossword puzzles), and it is likely to require long distance neural connections. However, not all the findings on the plasticity o the brain and aging process are positive. For instance, the controlled study by Ramos et al. (2017) maintains that the switching ability (i.e., the ability to shift attention between one task and another) was not enhanced by learning a foreign language, in this case Basque language, among elderly Spanish people.

Foreign Language Learning Among Healthy Older Individuals

In the most recent study on foreign language learning and its effect on cognitive functioning, Ware et al. (2017) developed a technology-based English training program for older French adults. The program was based on the assumptions provided by Antoniou et al. (2013). These assumptions involved various factors, such as that computer-based language training can be administered anywhere and at any time to suit learner’s needs, the content can be adjusted and items can be repeated. In addition, learners can socialize. The average age of the participants was 75 years. The course lasted for 4 months and consisted of 16 2-h sessions. The researchers used standardized tests for measuring cognitive functions (Montreal Cognitive Assessment), as well as University of California Loneliness Assessment for measuring subjective feelings of loneliness and social isolation, both of which did not significantly change after finishing the course. Nevertheless, the researchers found out that their program was feasible for this age group and the participants enjoyed it. Similarly, research performed by Bak et al. (2016) on a short 1-week Scottish Gaelic course on attentional functions among 67 adults aged between 18 years and 78 years reveals that even a short period of intensive language learning can modulate attentional functions and that all age groups can benefit from this effect. The results showed that at the beginning there was no difference between the groups. However, at the end of the course, a considerable improvement in attention switching was detected in the language group (p < 0.001) but not the control group (p = 0.127), independent of the age of subjects. In addition, they also suggested that these short-term effects could be maintained through continuous practice, but the minimum study period should be 5 h a week.

Research also indicates that the age in second language acquisition is not such a significant factor, but the length of exposure to the target language is important (Bialystok, 1997). In fact, on the one hand, it might take older people longer and more practice to learn a foreign language in the old age because of difficulty distinguishing new sounds and retrieve novel words, but on the other hand, they are more relaxed and motivated to learn (see SeniorsMatter, 2017). As it has been already pointed out, the main problem for older people is to retrieve new words (see Kurdziel et al., 2017). However, they are able to retain these new words easily if they are provided in the context. Kurdziel et al. (2017) also revealed that newly learned words were stored in hippocampus during encoding and then integrated into lexicon in the course of sleeping. Nevertheless, the quality of sleeping is often negatively affected in the old age and therefore older people are not able to retain as many words as their younger counterparts whose sleeping period is higher and unbroken.

Diaz-Orueta et al. (2012) report that the main stimulation for older people to learn a foreign language is a challenge, socialization, fun, providing learning opportunities and escape from daily routine. Moreover, the older individuals might also have experience of learning a foreign language, which can help them in acquiring a new language (see Singleton and Lengyel, 1995).

Kurdziel et al. (2017) expand by suggesting that learning throughout aging should be a must because older people who keep mentally and physically active are less likely to be cognitively impaired and depressed. In fact, depression seems to be one of the most serious comorbidities in the aging process (Popa-Wagner et al., 2014; Sandu et al., 2015). Furthermore, foreign language learning increases self-confidence, enables older people travel and communicate with their peers in foreign countries.

Bilingualism and Healthy Aging

The theory of bilingualism states that people acquiring a second language in their adulthood may prevent cognitive decline in later life by approximately 4.5 years (see Bialystok et al., 2007, 2016; Bak et al., 2014; Wilson et al., 2015; Woumans et al., 2015). In their recent study, on the basis of measures of cognitive function and brain structure, Bialystok et al. (2016) show that bilingualism can delay cognitive decline. As Bialystok et al. (2004) and Bialystok (2006) state, bilingualism contributes to compensate age-related losses in certain executive processes. Furthermore, bilingual people possess better mental flexibility because they are used to adapting to constant changes and processing information in a more effective way than the monolingual individuals. However, these results especially concern the retrospective studies on bilingualism since the prospective studies on bilingualism, such as Lawton et al. (2015), Sanders et al. (2012), Yeung et al. (2014), or Zahodne et al. (2014), have not exerted significant results in this respect (see Klimova et al., 2017a). For instance, Mukadam et al. (2017) in the most recent study revealed that retrospective studies inclined to confounding by education, or cultural differences in presentation to dementia and are thus not relevant to set causative links between risk factors and results. However, the electrophysiological studies on bilingualism indicate that bilingualism may enhance cognitive functions among healthy older individuals (i.e., Kousaie and Phillips, 2017). Moreover, as Ansaldo et al. (2015) state, healthy older bilinguals deal with interference control without recruiting a circuit that is particularly vulnerable to aging.

Table 1 below then summarizes the main findings of the studies on the effect of foreign language learning on the enhancement of cognitive functions for healthy older individuals.

The limitations of this mini-review study mainly involve a lack of relevant studies on the research topic. This fact may cause the overestimated effects of the results, which may have an adverse impact on the validity of these reviewed studies (see Melby-Lervåg and Hulme, 2016).

Conclusion

Overall, some of the findings in Table 1, as well as from other mentioned studies indicate that the learning of a foreign language may generate benefits for older individuals, such as enhancement of cognitive functioning (Bak et al., 2014, 2016; Ansaldo et al., 2015; Kousaie and Phillips, 2017) their self-esteem (Ware et al., 2017), or increased opportunities of socializing (Diaz-Orueta et al., 2012; Ballesteros et al., 2015). Bialystok et al. (2016) also emphasize that second-language learning has long-term implications for public health in terms of cost-effectiveness. In addition, as Ware et al. (2017) indicate, any intervention program on foreign language learning should be well thought of and tailored to the needs of older people in order to be effective and avoid accompanying factors, such as older people’s anxiety or low self-confidence.

In comparison with the intervention studies focusing on physical activities (see Klimova et al., 2017b), there is still smaller evidence of the effect of foreign language learning on the enhancement of cognitive functions among the healthy aging population. This is especially caused by a lack of research in this area.

Author Contributions

BK drafted, analyzed, wrote and read the whole manuscript herself.

Funding

This article is supported by the project Excellence (2018) at the Faculty of Informatics and Management of the University of Hradec Králové, Czechia.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abutalebi, J., and Clahsen, H. (2015). Bilingualism, cognition, and aging. Editorial. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 18, 1–2. doi: 10.1017/s1366728914000741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ansaldo, A. I., Ghazi-Saidi, L., and Adrover-Roig, D. (2015). Interference control in elderly bilinguals: appearances can be misleading. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 37, 455–470. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.990359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antoniou, M., Gunasekera, G., and Wong, P. C. M. (2013). Foreign language training as cognitive therapy for age-related cognitive decline: a hypothesis for future research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2689–2698. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antoniou, M., and Wright, S. M. (2017). Uncovering the mechanisms responsible for why language learning may promote healthy cognitive aging. Front. Psychol. 8:2217. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H. (2016). Language lessons to help protect against dementia. BMJ 354:i5039. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i5039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H., Long, M. R., Vega-Mendoza, M., and Sorace, A. (2016). Novelty, challenge and practice: the impact of intensive language learning on attentional functions. PLoS One 11:e0153485. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., and Deary, I. J. (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Ann. Neurol. 75, 959–963. doi: 10.1002/ana.24158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., Kraft, E., Santana, S., and Tziraki, C. (2015). Maintaining older brain functionality: a targeted review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 55, 453–477. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballesteros, S., Toril, P., Mayas, J., Reales, J. M., and Waterworth, J. A. (2014). An ICT social network in support of successful ageing. Gerontechnology 13, 37–46. doi: 10.4017/gt.2014.13.1.007.00

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellander, M., Berggren, R., Mårtensson, J., Brehmer, Y., Wenger, E., Li, T. Q., et al. (2016). Behavioral correlates of changes in hippocampal gray matter structure during acquisition of foreign vocabulary. Neuroimage 131, 205–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E. (1997). The structure of age: in search of barriers to second language acquisition. Second Lang. Rese. 13, 116–137. doi: 10.1191/026765897677670241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E. (2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 68–79. doi: 10.1037/cjep2006008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., Abutalebi, J., Bak, T. H., Burke, D. M., and Kroll, J. (2016). Aging in two languages: implications for public health. Ageing Res. Rev. 27, 56–60. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., and Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia 45, 459–464. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., and Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychol. Aging 19, 290–303. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 44, 195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, K. W., Deng, Y. H., Li, M., and Yan, H. M. (2015). The impact of L2 learning on cognitive aging. ADMET 3, 260–273. doi: 10.5599/admet.3.3.206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connor, S. (2016). Learning second language can delay ageing of the brain, say scientists. Available online at: http://www.independent.co.uk/news/science/learning-second-language-can-delay-ageing-of-the-brain-say-scientists-a6873796.html [Accessed on December 28, 2017].

Google Scholar

Diaz-Orueta, U., Facal, D., Nap, H. H., and Ranga, M. M. (2012). What is the key for older people to show interest in playing digital learning games? Initial qualitative findings from the LEAGE project on a multicultural European sample. Games Health J. 1, 115–123. doi: 10.1089/g4h.2011.0024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karakaya, T., Fußer, F., Schröder, J., and Pantel, J. (2013). Pharmacological treatment of mild cognitive impairment as a prodromal syndrome of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 11, 102–108. doi: 10.2174/157015913804999487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., and Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychol. Aging 23, 70–78. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.70

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimova, B., and Kuca, K. (2015). Alzheimer’s disease: potential preventive, non-invasive, intervention strategies in lowering the risk of cognitive decline—a review study. J. Appl. Biomed. 13, 257–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jab.2015.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimova, B., Valis, M., and Kuca, K. (2017a). Bilingualism as a strategy to delay the onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Clin. Interv. Aging 12, 1731–1737. doi: 10.2147/CIA.s145397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimova, B., Valis, M., and Kuca, K. (2017b). Cognitive decline in normal aging and its prevention: a review on non-pharmacological lifestyle strategies. Clin. Interv. Aging 12, 903–910. doi: 10.2147/CIA.s132963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kousaie, S., and Phillips, N. A. (2017). A behavioral and electrophysiological investigation of the effect of bilingualism on aging and cognitive control. Neuropsychologia 94, 23–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroll, J. F., and Dussias, P. E. (2017). The benefits of multilingualism to the personal and professional development of residents of the US. Foreign Lang. Ann. 50, 248–259. doi: 10.1111/flan.12271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurdziel, L. B. F., Mantua, J., and Spencer, R. M. C. (2017). Novel word learning in older adults: a role for sleep? Brain Lang. 167, 106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawton, D. M., Gasquoine, P. G., and Weimer, A. A. (2015). Age of dementia diagnosis in community dwelling bilingual and monolingual Hispanic Americans. Cortex 66, 141–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, R. R. W., and Tzeng, O. J. L. (2016). Neural bilingualism: a new look at an old problem. Lang. Linguist. 17, 147–193. doi: 10.1177/1606822X15614523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maresova, P., Klimova, B., Novotny, M., and Kuca, K. (2016). Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s diseases: expected economic impact on Europe—a call for a uniform European strategy. J. Alzheimers Dis. 54, 1123–1133. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maston, L. (2010). Declarative (explicit) and procedural (implicit) memory. Available online at: http://www.human-memory.net/types_declarative.html [Accessed on March 11, 2018].

Google Scholar

Melby-Lervåg, M., and Hulme, C. (2016). There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: a reply to Au et al. (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 324–330. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0862-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukadam, N., Sommerland, A., and Livingston, G. (2017). The relationship of bilingualism compared to monolingualism to the risk of cognitive decline or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Alzheimers Dis. 58, 45–54. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peter, C., Kreisner, A., Schröter, M., Hyosun, K., Bieber, G., Öhberg, F., et al. (2013). AGNES: connecting people in a multimodal way. J. Multimod. User Interf. 7, 229–245. doi: 10.1007/s12193-013-0118-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popa-Wagner, A., Buga, A. M., Tica, A. A., and Albu, C. V. (2014). Perfusion deficits, inflammation and aging precipitate depressive behavior. Biogerontology 15, 439–448. doi: 10.1007/s10522-014-9516-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramos, S., Garcia, Y. F., Anton, E., Casaponsa, A., and Dunabeitia, J. A. (2017). Does learning a language in the elderly enhance switching ability? J. Neurolinguistics 43, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Park, D. C. (2014). How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 355–370. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, A. E., Hall, C. B., Katz, M. J., and Lipton, R. B. (2012). Non-native language use and risk of incident dementia in the elderly. J. Alzheimers Dis. 29, 99–108. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-111631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandu, R. E., Buga, A. M., Uzoni, A., Petcu, E. B., and Popa-Wagner, A. (2015). Neuroinflammation and comorbidities are frequently ignored factors in CNS pathology. Neural Regen. Res. 10, 1349–1355. doi: 10.4103/1673-5374.165208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlegel, A. A., Rudelson, J. J., and Tse, P. U. (2012). White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1664–1670. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

SeniorsMatter. (2017). Learning a language as a senior. Available online at: http://seniorsmatter.com/learning-a-language-as-a-senior/

Singleton, D. M., and Lengyel, Z. (1995). The Age Factor in Second Language Acquisition: A Critical Look at the Critical Period Hypothesis. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

Google Scholar

Statista. (2017). Proportion of selected age groups of world population in 2017, by region. Available online at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/265759/world-population-by-age-and-region/ [Accessed on January 2, 2018].

Stern, Y. (2013). Cognitive reserve: implications for assessment and intervention. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 65, 49–54. doi: 10.1159/000353443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, S. (2015). Does bilingualism delay dementia? CMAJ 187, E209–E210. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.109-5022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, L. K., Shafto, M. A., Randall, B., Wright, P., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., and Stamatakis, E. A. (2010). Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: the modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cereb. Cortex 20, 352–364. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ware, C., Damnee, S., Djabelkhir, L., Cristancho, V., Wu, Y. H., Benovici, J., et al. (2017). Maintaining cognitive functioning in healthy seniors with a technology-based foreign language program: a pilot feasibility study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:42. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Boyle, P. A., Yang, J., James, B. D., and Bennett, D. A. (2015). Early life instruction in foreign language and music and incidence of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 29, 292–302. doi: 10.1037/neu0000129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woumans, E., Santens, P., Sieben, A., Versijpt, J., Stevens, M., and Duyck, W. (2015). Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 18, 568–574. doi: 10.1017/s136672891400087x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, C. M., St John, P. D., Menec, V., and Tyas, S. L. (2014). Is bilingualism associated with a lower risk of dementia in community-living old adults? Cross-sectional and prospective analyses. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 28, 326–332. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zahodne, L. B., Schofield, P. W., Farrell, M. T., Stern, Y., and Manly, J. J. (2014). Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology 28, 238–246. doi: 10.1037/neu0000014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: healthy older adults, foreign language learning, cognitive functioning, efficacy, benefits, a lack of evidence

Citation: Klimova B (2018) Learning a Foreign Language: A Review on Recent Findings About Its Effect on the Enhancement of Cognitive Functions Among Healthy Older Individuals. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:305. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00305

Received: 25 January 2018; Accepted: 16 July 2018;
Published: 30 July 2018.

Edited by:

Juliana Yordanova, Institute of Neurobiology (BAS), Bulgaria

Reviewed by:

Soledad Ballesteros, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED), Spain
Michael Falkenstein, Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors (LG), Germany

Copyright © 2018 Klimova. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Blanka Klimova, blanka.klimova@uhk.cz

Download