Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 04 October 2019
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00328

The Effect of Cerebellar Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Motor Learning: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials

  • Health and Rehabilitation Research Institute, Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand

Background: Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation (ctDCS) appears to modulate motor performance in both adaptation and motor skill tasks; however, whether the gains are long-lasting is unclear.

Objectives: This systematic review aims to evaluate the effect of ctDCS with respect to different time scales of motor learning.

Methods: Ten electronic databases (CINAHL, MEDLINE, SPORT Discus, Scopus, Web of Science, Cochrane via OVID, Evidence-Based Reviews (EBM) via OVID, AMED: Allied and Complementary Medicine, PsycINFO, and PEDro) were systematically searched. Studies evaluating the effect of ctDCS compared to sham ctDCS on motor learning in healthy individuals were selected and reviewed. Two authors independently reviewed the quality of the included studies using the revised Cochrane's risk-of-bias tool. The results were extracted with respect to the time scale in which changes in motor performance were evaluated.

Results: Seventeen randomized controlled trials met the eligibility criteria of which 65% of the studies had a “high” risk-of-bias, and 35% had “some concerns.” These studies included data from 629 healthy participants. Of the studies that evaluated the effect of anodal ctDCS during and immediately after the stimulation, four found enhanced, three found impaired, and ten found no effect on gains in motor performance. Of the studies that evaluated the effect of anodal ctDCS after a break of 24 h or more, seven found enhanced, two found impaired, and one found no effect on gains in motor performance. Of the studies that evaluated the effect of cathodal ctDCS across a range of time scales, five found impaired, one found enhanced, and five found no effect on gains in motor performance.

Conclusions: In healthy individuals, anodal ctDCS appears to improve short to longer-term motor skill learning, whereas it appears to have no effect on gains in motor performance during and immediate after the stimulation. ctDCS may have potential to improve motor performance beyond the training period. The challenge of the motor task and its characteristics, and the stimulation parameters are likely to influence the effect of ctDCS on motor learning.

Introduction

Motor learning is the set of processes associated with practice or experience, which lead to a relatively permanent change in skilled motor performance (Schmidt and Lee, 2011). This is fundamental for acquiring new motor skills, responding to dynamic environmental conditions and for re-learning lost motor skills after injury (Kitago and Krakauer, 2013). Repeated training or practice is required to acquire complex motor skills and achieve peak performance. Therefore, strategies which maximize performance and enhance the acquisition of motor skills have received considerable attention in motor learning and rehabilitation literature (Winstein et al., 2014).

Recently, the modulation of cortical and sub-cortical excitability through external means such as non-invasive brain stimulation has received increasing attention as a means to enhance performance during training (Banissy and Muggleton, 2013; Okano et al., 2015; Edwards et al., 2017). One such application is transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). tDCS involves the delivery of continuous, weak electric currents to the brain to alter the resting membrane potentials of neurons to influence excitability (Bolognini et al., 2009). There is growing consumer interest in the ability of tDCS to modulate brain activity. Halo Sport (2019) and Caputron (2019) are two examples of commercially available tDCS devices being marketed to sporting populations. The manufacturers make reference to research evidence which illustrates the efficacy of tDCS to enhance motor performance (Waters-Metenier et al., 2014; Ciechanski et al., 2017), including in sporting populations (Huang et al., 2019). Much of the tDCS research has focused on the primary motor cortex and pre-motor areas (Ammann et al., 2016); however, researchers are increasingly considering the cerebellum as a target (Block H. J. and Celnik, 2012; Ferrucci et al., 2016, 2019; Grimaldi et al., 2016). The cerebellum contributes to the control of both motor and non-motor behaviors, including learning, posture and balance, coordination, cognition, emotion, and language (Timmann and Daum, 2007; Manto et al., 2012; Perciavalle et al., 2013; Koziol et al., 2014; Mariën et al., 2014; Caligiore et al., 2017; Lang et al., 2017). The cerebellum has a particular role in error-based learning (Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Diedrichsen et al., 2005; Tseng et al., 2007). In error-based learning, sensory prediction errors; the difference between predicted sensory consequences of a movement command, and the resultant sensory feedback, are used to adjust the subsequent motor output (Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Wolpert and Flanagan, 2001; Izawa and Shadmehr, 2011). Furthermore, evidence from neurophysiological, neuroimaging and behavioral studies in animals and humans suggest that cerebellar activation varies with the type of motor task performed and the stage of motor learning (Doyon and Benali, 2005; Dayan and Cohen, 2011; Lohse et al., 2014). Given the importance of the cerebellum in error-based motor learning (Ito, 2000; De Zeeuw and Ten Brinke, 2015) and re-learning of motor skills after central nervous system injury (Small et al., 2002; Ward et al., 2003; Sokolov et al., 2017), transcranial direct current stimulation over the cerebellum (ctDCS) has been advocated as an alternative tDCS stimulation site to promote motor learning (Grimaldi et al., 2014; Celnik, 2015; Oldrati and Schutter, 2018).

In a laboratory setting, motor learning is often evaluated using two paradigms: motor adaptation or skill learning. Motor adaptation consists of a perturbation applied during the performance of a well-learnt motor skill, for example, perturbing limb trajectories during reaching. The learner adapts to the error induced by the perturbation rapidly over minutes to hours (adaptation). When the perturbation is removed, the adaptation is retained for a period of time (after-effects) and gradually wanes over time (de-adaptation) (Martin et al., 1996). However, with repeated exposure to the perturbation, learning is observed through rapid reductions in errors (Martin et al., 1996) and faster rates of adaptation on subsequent exposures (Kojima et al., 2004). In motor skill learning paradigms, learning is evaluated through exposure to a novel motor task. Motor learning is observed through the reduction of errors and performance improvement beyond baseline levels (Reis et al., 2009).

Motor learning occurs over distinct phases. There is the early (fast) learning in which improvements in performance are seen rapidly within a single training session (Doyon and Benali, 2005). In the later slow stage, further performance gains are seen across several sessions of practice (Dayan and Cohen, 2011). Progression from fast to slow learning depends on appropriate rest periods and subsequent sleep (Diekelmann et al., 2009), where gains in performance can be observed without the additional practice of the task (Dayan and Cohen, 2011). Changes in performance are initially transient in nature, but with extended practice, the performance of skilled behavior becomes less attention-demanding and skilled performance is possible even after long breaks (Doyon and Benali, 2005). For the purposes of this paper, the time scales of learning are represented as (1) long-term changes in performance measured after a break of 24 h or more; (2) short-term change in performance after a break of <24 h; (3) change in performance measured immediately after training; and (4) change in performance during training.

There is ample evidence indicating that ctDCS can modulate cerebellar activity at a neurophysiological level (Galea et al., 2009), less is known about its effect on behavioral outcomes (Block H. and Celnik, 2012). To date, the evidence for the efficacy of ctDCS has been limited to its ability to modulate motor performance (Oldrati and Schutter, 2018). A recent meta-analysis reported the effectiveness of anodal and cathodal ctDCS in modulating motor performance in healthy individuals in both motor adaptation and motor skills tasks (Oldrati and Schutter, 2018), however, a systematic understanding of how ctDCS contributes to different timescales of motor learning is still lacking (Grimaldi et al., 2014; van Dun et al., 2016). Therefore, the present systematic review aims to elucidate the effects of ctDCS on motor learning across different time scales in healthy individuals to determine if the documented gains in performance persist for a substantial period after training. This understanding will be useful in ascertaining the prospects of using ctDCS as a neuro-modulatory tool to augment motor learning in both elite performance in healthy individuals and following brain lesions in clinical populations.

Methods

Study Design

A systematic search and review of the literature were undertaken based on an a priori plan.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Studies were included if they met all the following criteria: involved healthy individuals above the age of 18 years, delivered real or sham tDCS over the cerebellum, random assignment to groups, measured behavioral outcomes of change in motor performance, and appeared in peer-reviewed English-language journals. Studies that compared different stimulation areas in the brain were included if data from cerebellar stimulation could be extracted and viewed separately.

Studies were excluded if they were reviews, books, theses, conference papers, commentaries, letters; if the sample consisted of animals; if the motor skill learning task did not involve the use of upper and lower limb; or if ctDCS was applied in combination with another intervention.

Information Sources

A search (July 2019) of the following databases was undertaken: CINAHL, MEDLINE, SPORT Discus, Scopus, Web of Science, Cochrane via OVID, Evidence-Based Reviews (EBM) via OVID, AMED: Allied and Complementary Medicine, PsycINFO, and PEDro. No limit was placed on the publication date. The search strategy (Supplementary File 1) included following key search terms: acquisition, motor performance, motor control, learning, adapt*, ctDCS, cerebellar stimulation, tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation, non-invasive brain stimulation, noninvasive brain stimulation, direct current stimulation, cerebell*. The reference list of included studies, recent systematic reviews, and meta-analyses were also searched.

Study Selection

Following duplicate removal, the first author (N.K.) reviewed the titles and abstracts of all remaining studies. If a decision to include an article could not be made based on the title and abstract review, the full text was reviewed. A second reviewer (N.S.) was consulted if eligibility was unclear and a consensus reached.

Data Extraction

Data was extracted using a form developed from the Cochrane data extraction and assessment template (Higgins and Green, 2011). Extracted information included the study characteristics, ctDCS stimulation parameters, motor learning task description, outcome measures, and key findings.

Assessment of Study Quality

The quality of the included studies was critically appraised using the revised Cochrane's risk-of-bias tool for randomized trials (RoB 2) (Sterne et al., 2019). Two reviewers (N.K. and N.S.) independently rated the studies with any disagreements being discussed until consensus was reached. The revised Cochrane's risk-of-bias tool evaluates the methodological quality of the studies in relation to trial design, conduct, and reporting. Based on the answers to a series of signaling questions within five domains (randomization process, deviations from the intended interventions, missing outcome data, measurement of the outcome, and selection of the reported results), the studies were considered to have “low” or “high” risk-of-bias or “some concerns.” For randomized crossover trials signaling questions on carryover effect were additionally assessed. The overall risk-of-bias judgment for each study was categorized according to the revised Cochrane's risk-of-bias guidelines (Sterne et al., 2019).

Results

Search Results

The electronic search retrieved 633 studies, which was reduced to 281 following duplicate removal. Title and abstract review excluded 237 studies which did not meet the eligibility criteria. On full-text review, a further 31 studies were excluded for reasons outlined in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart showing the study selection process and results.

Seventeen RCTs met the criteria for inclusion in this systematic review. No additional studies met the inclusion criteria upon searching the reference list of the included studies. The included studies constituted a total of 629 participants with a mean age between 18 and 69 years. Only two studies had participants above the age of 40 years (Panouillères et al., 2015; Samaei et al., 2017). Random allocation of participants was in either a parallel (n = 14) (Jayaram et al., 2012; Dutta et al., 2014; Panouillères et al., 2015; Ehsani et al., 2016; Panico et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Poortvliet et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Jackson et al., 2019; Jongkees et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019) or crossover design (n = 3) (Shah et al., 2013; Fernandez et al., 2017; Foerster et al., 2017), with 349 participants receiving real ctDCS. Refer to Table 1 (study characteristics).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies.

One of the seventeen studies, six had “some concerns” (Shah et al., 2013; Ehsani et al., 2016; Fernandez et al., 2017; Samaei et al., 2017; Poortvliet et al., 2018; Jackson et al., 2019), and eleven had “high” risk-of-bias (Jayaram et al., 2012; Dutta et al., 2014; Panouillères et al., 2015; Panico et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Foerster et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Jongkees et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019). Studies having “some concerns” were due to failure to explicitly report on the randomization process and trial registration or pre-specified statistical analysis plan. Studies having a “high” risk-of-bias was due to differences in baseline characteristics between the intervention groups suggesting issues with the randomization process, lack of information on blinding of the outcome assessor, the bias in the selection of reported results, and insufficient time for washout of carry-over effects. Refer to Figure 2, Supplementary File 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Overall risk-of-bias judgments for each domain.

ctDCS Intervention

The type of ctDCS stimulation varied across the studies. Eight studies applied anodal ctDCS (Dutta et al., 2014; Panouillères et al., 2015; Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Poortvliet et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Jackson et al., 2019), two cathodal ctDCS (Panico et al., 2016; Fernandez et al., 2017), and the remaining seven applied both anodal and cathodal stimulation (Jayaram et al., 2012; Shah et al., 2013; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Foerster et al., 2017; Jongkees et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019).

All studies investigated the effects of a single session of ctDCS. In the majority of studies (n = 9) stimulation was delivered during the training of a motor task (Jayaram et al., 2012; Shah et al., 2013; Dutta et al., 2014; Ehsani et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Summers et al., 2018; Jongkees et al., 2019). In three studies stimulation was delivered prior to the training of the task (Fernandez et al., 2017; Foerster et al., 2017; Poortvliet et al., 2018) and in the remaining five studies ctDCS was delivered just prior to in conjunction with task training (Panouillères et al., 2015; Panico et al., 2016; Liew et al., 2018; Jackson et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019). The stimulation duration ranged between 8 and 30 min.

In tasks involving the upper limb, the stimulation was predominantly applied to the lateral cerebellum (n = 11) with respect to the training limb, ipsilaterally (n = 10) (Shah et al., 2013; Panouillères et al., 2015; Ehsani et al., 2016; Panico et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Jackson et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019), or contralaterally (n = 1) (Dutta et al., 2014). Two studies applied the stimulation to the bilateral cerebellar hemispheres (Summers et al., 2018; Jongkees et al., 2019). Four studies investigated the effect of ctDCS on a bilateral task by placing the target electrode centrally (Poortvliet et al., 2018) or with respect to the dominant limb (Jayaram et al., 2012; Fernandez et al., 2017; Foerster et al., 2017). The return electrode was placed on the forehead (Dutta et al., 2014; Poortvliet et al., 2018), buccinator muscle (Jayaram et al., 2012; Shah et al., 2013; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Fernandez et al., 2017; Summers et al., 2018), or upper limb (Panouillères et al., 2015; Ehsani et al., 2016; Panico et al., 2016; Foerster et al., 2017; Samaei et al., 2017).

ctDCS was delivered at a current density of 0.13 mA/cm2 (n = 1) (Shah et al., 2013), 0.08 mA/cm2 (n = 10) (Jayaram et al., 2012; Ehsani et al., 2016; Panico et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Foerster et al., 2017; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Jackson et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019), 0.06 mA/cm2 (n = 2) (Panouillères et al., 2015; Fernandez et al., 2017), or 0.03 mA/cm2 (n = 4) (Dutta et al., 2014; Poortvliet et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Jongkees et al., 2019). Full details of the stimulation parameters are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Stimulation parameters.

Motor Learning Tasks

Ten studies evaluated a motor adaptation task, and seven studies evaluated a motor skill task. The motor adaptation tasks included perturbation during visuomotor (Panouillères et al., 2015; Panico et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Liew et al., 2018; Mamlins et al., 2019), locomotor (Jayaram et al., 2012; Fernandez et al., 2017), reaching (Taubert et al., 2016), or postural control (Foerster et al., 2017; Poortvliet et al., 2018) tasks. Skill learning paradigms used serial reaction time task (Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jongkees et al., 2019), tracking (Shah et al., 2013; Dutta et al., 2014; Summers et al., 2018), or a throwing task (Jackson et al., 2019).

Outcomes

Motor performance outcomes were measured based on error (n = 16) (Jayaram et al., 2012; Shah et al., 2013; Dutta et al., 2014; Panouillères et al., 2015; Ehsani et al., 2016; Panico et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Foerster et al., 2017; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Poortvliet et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Jackson et al., 2019; Jongkees et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019), response latency (n = 1) (Dutta et al., 2014), response time (n = 2) (Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017), reaction time (n = 2) (Liew et al., 2018; Jongkees et al., 2019), or movement variability (n = 2) (Fernandez et al., 2017; Poortvliet et al., 2018). Studies measured outcomes over a range of time scales including; after a break of 24 h or more post intervention (n = 5) (Ehsani et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2019; Jongkees et al., 2019), after a break of <24 h post intervention (n = 5) (Shah et al., 2013; Panouillères et al., 2015; Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2019), immediately after the intervention (n = 7) (Jayaram et al., 2012; Fernandez et al., 2017; Foerster et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Poortvliet et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Mamlins et al., 2019), or during the intervention (n = 12) (Jayaram et al., 2012; Dutta et al., 2014; Panouillères et al., 2015; Ehsani et al., 2016; Panico et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Jongkees et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019).

Long-Term Motor Learning–Motor Performance After a Break of 24 h or More

Of the five studies which evaluated the effect of ctDCS after a break of 24 h or more, three reported enhanced (Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2019), while two reported impaired (Taubert et al., 2016; Jongkees et al., 2019) gains in motor performance with anodal ctDCS. Compared to sham ctDCS, anodal ctDCS enhanced the gains in the performance of a motor skill tasks evaluated after a break of 24 (Jackson et al., 2019) and 48 h (Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017). This was reflected by a greater reduction in the number of errors and/or faster response time in those aged <40 years (Ehsani et al., 2016; Jackson et al., 2019) and a greater reduction in response time, but not the number of errors, in individuals over 40 years (Samaei et al., 2017). Of the two studies that reported impaired gains in motor performance, one found impaired reaction time, but not the number of errors in a motor skill task after 24 h (Jongkees et al., 2019), and the other reported impaired early adaptation in a motor adaptation task when evaluated after 24 h (Taubert et al., 2016). Two studies evaluated the effect of cathodal ctDCS and found no difference in motor performance 24 h after the intervention (Taubert et al., 2016; Jongkees et al., 2019). These studies applied anodal and cathodal ctDCS centered over the inion (Jongkees et al., 2019) or ipsilateral to the training limb during task training or prior to and in conjunction with task training (Jackson et al., 2019). The stimulation was delivered at a current density of 0.03 mA/cm2 (Jongkees et al., 2019) or 0.08 mA/cm2 for 20–25 min (Ehsani et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2019; Jongkees et al., 2019).

Short-Term Motor Learning–Motor Performance After a Break of <24 h

Of the studies that evaluated the effect of anodal ctDCS after a break of <24 h, four found enhanced (Shah et al., 2013; Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2019) and one found no effect (Panouillères et al., 2015) on gains in motor performance compared to sham ctDCS. Anodal ctDCS enhanced the performance of a motor skill task by reducing the number of errors but not response time in healthy young individuals (Ehsani et al., 2016) and reduced the response time but not the number of errors in healthy older individuals tested after a break of 35 min (Samaei et al., 2017). Anodal ctDCS also improved performance of motor skill task 5 (Jackson et al., 2019), 10, 30, and 60 min after intervention. All four studies stimulated the lateral cerebellum ipsilateral to the training limb for 15 (Shah et al., 2013), 20 (Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017), or 25 (Jackson et al., 2019) min at a current density of 0.13 mA/cm2 (Shah et al., 2013) or 0.08 mA/cm2 (Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2019). Whereas anodal ctDCS did not affect the number of errors in a motor adaptation task performed after a gap of 50 min when the stimulation was delivered ipsilateral to the training limb at a current density of 0.06 mA/cm2 for 17 min (Panouillères et al., 2015).

One study evaluated the effect of cathodal ctDCS on motor performance after a break of <24 h and reported improvement in ankle tracking accuracy tested after 10, 30, and 60 min (Shah et al., 2013).

Immediate Motor Learning–Motor Performance Immediately After the Intervention

Of the studies that evaluated the effect of anodal ctDCS immediately after the intervention, one study reported enhanced (Poortvliet et al., 2018), and five found no effect on gains in motor performance as compared to a sham ctDCS group (Jayaram et al., 2012; Foerster et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Mamlins et al., 2019). Anodal ctDCS at a current density of 0.03 mA/cm2 for 20 min improved the performance by reducing the postural variability and increasing steadiness when the target electrode was placed centrally over the cerebellum (Poortvliet et al., 2018). While the same site of stimulation and current density delivered for 30 min had no effect on finger tracking accuracy (Summers et al., 2018). Anodal ctDCS delivered ipsilateral to the dominant limb at a current density of 0.08 mA/cm2 for around 15 min had no effect on static and dynamic balance (Foerster et al., 2017), visuomotor adaptation (Liew et al., 2018; Mamlins et al., 2019), forcefield adaptation (Mamlins et al., 2019), or locomotor adaptation (Jayaram et al., 2012).

Application of cathodal ctDCS had no effect (Mamlins et al., 2019) or impaired (Fernandez et al., 2017; Foerster et al., 2017) gains in motor performance evaluated immediately after stimulation. As compared to sham ctDCS, cathodal ctDCS increased variability in a walking adaptation task (Fernandez et al., 2017) and impaired static but not dynamic balance in adaptation task (Foerster et al., 2017). These effects were seen when ctDCS was delivered ipsilateral to the dominant limb prior to motor task training at a current density of 0.06 mA/cm2 (Fernandez et al., 2017) or 0.08 mA/cm2 (Foerster et al., 2017) for 20 (Fernandez et al., 2017) or 9 (Foerster et al., 2017) min.

Simultaneous Motor Learning–Motor Performance During the Intervention

Application of ctDCS had a varied impact on motor performance during task training. Anodal ctDCS enhanced (n = 3) (Jayaram et al., 2012; Ehsani et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016), impaired (n = 3) (Dutta et al., 2014; Taubert et al., 2016; Jongkees et al., 2019), or had no effect on gains in motor performance during task training (n = 5) (Panouillères et al., 2015; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Mamlins et al., 2019). Compared to sham ctDCS, anodal ctDCS enhanced motor performance by improving the rate of adaptation (Jayaram et al., 2012; Yavari et al., 2016) and reduced the number of errors but not response time in a serial reaction time task (Ehsani et al., 2016). These effects were primarily observed when anodal ctDCS was delivered ipsilateral to the dominant limb (Jayaram et al., 2012) or training limb (Ehsani et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016) for 15 min (Jayaram et al., 2012; Yavari et al., 2016) or more (Ehsani et al., 2016) at a current density of 0.08 mA/cm2. Anodal ctDCS impaired gains in motor performance during a perturbed reaching task (Taubert et al., 2016), visual pursuit task (Dutta et al., 2014), and serial reaction time task (Jongkees et al., 2019). In the serial reaction task, the impaired gains in motor performance occurred in reaction time but not in the number of errors. In the perturbed reaching task, ctDCS was delivered ipsilateral to the training limb for 20 min at a current density of 0.08 mA/cm2 (Taubert et al., 2016). Whereas, impaired gains in performance of the serial reaction time task or visual pursuit task were seen when the current was delivered centrally (Jongkees et al., 2019) or on the lateral cerebellum contralateral to the training limb (Dutta et al., 2014) for up to 20 min at a current density of 0.03 mA/cm2 (Dutta et al., 2014; Jongkees et al., 2019). Anodal ctDCS had no effect on response time in skill task (Samaei et al., 2017) and the number of errors in adaptation (Panouillères et al., 2015; Liew et al., 2018; Mamlins et al., 2019) or skill task (Summers et al., 2018) when the current density was 0.08, 0.06, and 0.03 mA/cm2, respectively. The target electrode was placed either centrally over the cerebellum (Summers et al., 2018) or on the lateral cerebellum ipsilateral to the training limb (Panouillères et al., 2015; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Mamlins et al., 2019) which delivered the stimulation for up to 30 min.

Of the five studies that evaluated the effect of cathodal ctDCS during task training, three reported impaired (Jayaram et al., 2012; Panico et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016) and two reported no effects (Jongkees et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019) on gains in motor performance. As compared to sham ctDCS, cathodal ctDCS resulted in impaired adaptation (Jayaram et al., 2012; Panico et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016) and impaired rate of de-adaptation (Panico et al., 2016). These effects were seen when cathodal ctDCS was delivered ipsilateral to training limb (Jayaram et al., 2012; Panico et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016) for 15 min (Jayaram et al., 2012; Yavari et al., 2016) or more (Panico et al., 2016) at a current density of 0.08 mA/cm2. Two studies found no effect of cathodal ctDCS on skill or adaptation task (Jongkees et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019). These studies applied cathodal ctDCS centrally (Jongkees et al., 2019) or ipsilateral to the training limb (Mamlins et al., 2019) during task training alone (Jongkees et al., 2019) or prior to and in conjunction with task training (Mamlins et al., 2019) for up to 20 min at a current density of 0.03 mA/cm2 (Jongkees et al., 2019) or 0.08 mA/cm2 (Mamlins et al., 2019).

Discussion

This review aimed to determine the effects of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning. For the first time, this study provides a systematic review of RCTs to quantify the effects of ctDCS based on the time scale of motor learning. There is a modest body of research, with 17 studies including 629 participants. The body of evidence is subject to considerable risk-of-bias. The main findings of this systematic review are that anodal ctDCS appears to be effective at enhancing motor skill learning in the short (<24 h) and longer-term (≥24 h). Whereas, it appears to have no effect on motor learning immediately after or during stimulation. This review suggests that the type of motor task, the tDCS stimulation parameters and the interaction between task and stimulation parameters are likely to influence the efficacy of ctDCS.

When compared to sham ctDCS, anodal ctDCS appears to be effective at improving short and longer-term motor learning in healthy individuals when applied primarily during motor skill learning (Shah et al., 2013; Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2019) but not motor adaptation paradigms (Panouillères et al., 2015; Taubert et al., 2016). Task characteristics and their interaction with the time scale of learning may explain this. Motor skill training paradigms use novel or complex motor skills, which may take weeks or months to master (Schmidt and Lee, 2011). In contrast, motor adaptation tasks involve modifying a well-learnt skill in response to error feedback. Often participants adapt to induced errors within minutes to hours in motor adaptation tasks (Bastian, 2008). It is possible that motor adaptation paradigms are subject to a ceiling effect in healthy individuals. Repeated exposure to the same adaptation task may not provide sufficient stimulus to induce learning (Bastian, 2008; Criscimagna-Hemminger et al., 2010). In addition, an interference task was undertaken between the intervention and testing sessions of one of the motor adaptation tasks, making interpretation of their results challenging (Taubert et al., 2016).

The reported gains in the performance of a motor skill task in response to anodal ctDCS may also depend on the measure of motor performance used and the age of the participants. In studies investigating healthy young individuals undertaking a unimanual serial reaction time task, ctDCS enhances accuracy but not response time after a break of <24 h and enhanced accuracy and response time after a break of 24 h or more (Ehsani et al., 2016). A previous non-randomized experimental study has also reported that ctDCS may have a greater effect on accuracy than response time within and after 24 h (Cantarero et al., 2015). In contrast, in a study investigating healthy older individuals undertaking the same task, a greater reduction in response time but not the number of errors was observed in response to ctDCS irrespective of the time scale of measurement (Samaei et al., 2017). These findings suggest that ctDCS may differentially influence short and longer-term motor learning of different parameters of movement performance. However, it is unclear whether the difference between older and younger individuals reflects differences in the mechanism of action of ctDCS or that older individuals have slower response time but not greater inaccuracy in these types of task (Voelcker-Rehage, 2008).

In studies which investigated the effects of ctDCS using serial reaction time tasks, conflicting results were observed. Improved response times were seen in a unimanual task (Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017), whereas impaired reaction time was seen in a bimanual task (Jongkees et al., 2019). The performance measure used to reflect motor learning in the two tasks may evaluate different aspects of motor performance. Reaction time reflects the time between stimulus appearance and movement initiation. Whereas, response time is comprised of both reaction time and movement time (Pascual-Leone et al., 1995). However, it is notable that the studies also differed in the stimulation parameters used, where a current density of 0.03 mA/cm2 centered over bilateral cerebellar hemisphere impaired gains, while a current density of 0.08 mA/cm2 targeting the lateral cerebellum ipsilateral to the training limb enhanced gains in motor performance. The challenge of unpacking these conflicting results illustrates the importance of taking a systematic approach to investigating ctDCS; where the influence of motor task, performance metric, and stimulation parameters should be considered.

Anodal ctDCS appears to have no effect on gains in motor performance measured during and immediately after the intervention, where most of the studies demonstrated no effect (Jayaram et al., 2012; Panouillères et al., 2015; Foerster et al., 2017; Samaei et al., 2017; Liew et al., 2018; Summers et al., 2018; Mamlins et al., 2019) and some enhanced (Jayaram et al., 2012; Ehsani et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Poortvliet et al., 2018) or impaired (Dutta et al., 2014; Taubert et al., 2016; Jongkees et al., 2019) gains in motor performance. These results were observed irrespective of the type of task being studied (adaptation or skill) as has been noted in previous narrative reviews (Ferrucci et al., 2015; van Dun et al., 2016). It is therefore unclear whether ctDCS has any effect on motor learning during or immediately after task training. Motor learning research highlights the paradoxical relationship between learning and performance. That is, motor learning, as defined as a permanent change in motor performance, can occur without immediate changes in motor performance. In fact, immediate changes in motor performance in response to an intervention are often not sustained after a break (Soderstrom and Bjork, 2015). This suggests that changes in motor performance during and immediately after anodal ctDCS are less relevant in determining the effectiveness of anodal ctDCS than changes observed after 24 h or more.

This systematic review highlights that the site of anodal ctDCS stimulation and current density are the critical stimulation parameters which appear to impact the effect produced, irrespective of time scale. Greater gains in motor performance were seen with the target electrode placed centrally on the cerebellum in a bilateral postural control task (Poortvliet et al., 2018) and ipsilateral to the training limb in unilateral tasks (Shah et al., 2013; Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017). In addition, motor performance is enhanced during a bilateral task involving greater perturbation to one of the limbs with the placement of target electrode ipsilateral to that limb (Jayaram et al., 2012). This suggests that the parameters of the motor task may be an important consideration in determining an appropriate site for stimulation. Therefore, researchers should explicitly consider where in the cerebellum motor control and learning is occurring for a given task and select electrode configuration with this in mind (Hulst et al., 2017), acknowledging that current density and specificity is dependent on electrode size and position (Ferrucci et al., 2013). Positive effects were more likely to be observed when anodal ctDCS was delivered with a current density of 0.08 mA/cm2 or more. This current density is greater than that recommended for cerebral ctDCS (Nitsche et al., 2003); however, modeling studies illustrate the need for higher current density to stimulate the cerebellum to overcome large shunting of current at the base of the skull (Rampersad et al., 2014). Other stimulation parameters such as stimulation duration and timing of stimulation delivery (at rest or during task training) had an equivocal effect. The total duration of stimulation was not hugely variable and ranged from 15 to 20 min. Contrary to previous literature (Monte-Silva et al., 2010), no relationship between stimulation duration and time scale of effect was observed. Further research is required to unpack the effect of stimulation duration on the permanence of ctDCS effects across time scales.

When compared to sham ctDCS, cathodal ctDCS has an equivocal effect on short and longer-term motor learning in healthy individuals. However, most of the studies found impaired gains in motor performance of adaptation tasks during and immediately after cathodal ctDCS (Jayaram et al., 2012; Panico et al., 2016; Yavari et al., 2016; Fernandez et al., 2017; Foerster et al., 2017) with few reporting no effect on gains in motor performance (Jongkees et al., 2019; Mamlins et al., 2019). Overall, there is insufficient evidence to infer the effect of cathodal ctDCS on motor learning.

Although most of the included studies employed randomized, blinded, sham-controlled designs, their methodological quality was globally considered to have “high” risk-of-bias. Potential sources of bias included failure to report the method of randomization used, allocation concealment and failure to explicitly state who was blinded: the participant, the person administering the intervention, and/or the outcome assessor. The majority of studies did not report trial registration details or a pre-specified statistical analysis plan. Further, some studies had baseline differences between intervention groups that suggested a problem with the randomization process. Whilst these judgments of research quality may not reflect what the researchers actually did during the protocol but rather a lack of explicit documentation; it is essential that adherence to, and reporting of, these standards of practice become commonplace in this body of literature. The potential for bias may contribute to the reporting of contradictory results and suggests that the interpretation of the research findings to date must be approached with some caution (Steiner et al., 2016; Hulst et al., 2017; Jalali et al., 2017).

Limitations, Implications and Future Research

The included studies had considerable variability in both measurement and data processing methods. Some studies measured the time course of change in error throughout the task training (Panouillères et al., 2015), some in specific epochs (early or late epochs) (Panico et al., 2016; Taubert et al., 2016), some fitted an exponential curve (Jayaram et al., 2012; Yavari et al., 2016), while other measured change scores (Shah et al., 2013; Ehsani et al., 2016; Samaei et al., 2017; Jackson et al., 2019). Furthermore, the method for calculating changes in motor performance was inconsistent across studies. For instance, the error was calculated as mean error (Jayaram et al., 2012), mean absolute error (Dutta et al., 2014), or normalized accuracy index using root mean square error (Shah et al., 2013) while others failed to describe how the error was calculated (Ehsani et al., 2016). The method by which error is calculated affects its accuracy; for example, a simple mean of errors may not reflect individual variability while a mean absolute error encompasses bias due to individual variability (Schmidt and Lee, 2011). This makes comparing results across studies challenging.

Despite these limitations, the review adds to our understanding of the potential of ctDCS to impact motor learning, with particular reference to the time scale of learning. It highlights the importance of task characteristics, movement parameter outcome measurement techniques, participant age, and stimulation parameters when interpreting the research body and designing future studies. Further research, which explores the time scales of >24 h are required. There are also many unanswered questions regarding the cumulative effects of ctDCS over multiple sessions and the long-term retention of performance after a delay of weeks and months. More studies evaluating the effect of ctDCS on motor adaptation tasks over longer time scales are needed to elucidate its effect on adaptive learning.

Conclusions

In conclusion, anodal ctDCS appears to be effective at improving short and long-term motor skill learning. However, these results are predicated upon just four modest-quality studies. While these findings illustrate the potential of targeting the cerebellum with tDCS to enhance learning in healthy and clinical populations, researchers need to take a methodologically robust and systematic approach to future research. Factors including the challenge of the motor task and its characteristics, the ctDCS stimulation parameters, method of measuring motor performance, and participant age are likely to influence whether ctDCS will enhance or have no effect on motor learning.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Author Contributions

All authors were involved in the conceptualization and designing of the study. NK was involved with the literature search and data extraction. NK and NS were involved with manuscript preparation. NS and DT were involved in supervision. All authors were involved in reviewing and editing the manuscript.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00328/full#supplementary-material

References

Ammann, C., Spampinato, D., and Márquez-Ruiz, J. (2016). Modulating motor learning through transcranial direct-current stimulation: an integrative view. Front. Psychol. 7:1981. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banissy, M. J., and Muggleton, N. G. (2013). Transcranial direct current stimulation in sports training: potential approaches. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:129. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastian, A. J. (2008). Understanding sensorimotor adaptation and learning for rehabilitation. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 21, 628–633. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328315a293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, H., and Celnik, P. (2012). Stimulating the cerebellum affects visuomotor adaptation but not intermanual transfer of learning. Cerebellum 12, 781–793. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0486-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, H. J., and Celnik, P. (2012). Can cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation become a valuable neurorehabilitation intervention? Expert Rev. Neurother. 12, 1275–1277. doi: 10.1586/ern.12.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolognini, N., Pascual-Leone, A., and Fregni, F. (2009). Using non-invasive brain stimulation to augment motor training-induced plasticity. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 6:8. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-6-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caligiore, D., Pezzulo, G., Baldassarre, G., Bostan, A. C., Strick, P. L., Doya, K., et al. (2017). Consensus paper: towards a systems-level view of cerebellar function: the interplay between cerebellum, basal ganglia, and cortex. Cerebellum 16, 203–229. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0763-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantarero, G., Spampinato, D., Reis, J., Ajagbe, L., Thompson, T., Kulkarni, K., et al. (2015). Cerebellar direct current stimulation enhances on-line motor skill acquisition through an effect on accuracy. J. Neurosci. 35, 3285–3290. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2885-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caputron (2019). What is TDCS New York City. Available online at: https://caputron.com/collections/research-and-clinical (accessed March 27, 2019).

PubMed Abstract

Celnik, P. (2015). Understanding and modulating motor learning with cerebellar stimulation. Cerebellum 14, 171–174. doi: 10.1007/s12311-014-0607-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciechanski, P., Cheng, A., Lopushinsky, S., Hecker, K., Gan, L. S., Lang, S., et al. (2017). Effects of transcranial direct-current stimulation on neurosurgical skill acquisition: a randomized controlled trial. World Neurosurg. 108, 876–84.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2017.08.123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Criscimagna-Hemminger, S. E., Bastian, A. J., and Shadmehr, R. (2010). Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 2275–2284. doi: 10.1152/jn.00822.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, E., and Cohen, L. G. (2011). Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron 72, 443–454. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Zeeuw, C. I., and Ten Brinke, M. M. (2015). Motor learning and the cerebellum. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 7:a021683. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichsen, J., Hashambhoy, Y., Rane, T., and Shadmehr, R. (2005). Neural correlates of reach errors. J Neurosci. 25, 9919–9931. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diekelmann, S., Wilhelm, I., and Born, J. (2009). The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Med. Rev. 13, 309–321. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doyon, J., and Benali, H. (2005). Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutta, A., Paulus, W., and Nitsche, M. A. (2014). Facilitating myoelectric-control with transcranial direct current stimulation: a preliminary study in healthy humans. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 11:13. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, D. J., Cortes, M., Wortman-Jutt, S., Putrino, D., Bikson, M., Thickbroom, G., et al. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation and sports performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:243. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehsani, F., Bakhtiary, A., Jaberzadeh, S., Talimkhani, A., and Hajihasani, A. (2016). Differential effects of primary motor cortex and cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning in healthy individuals: A randomized double-blind sham-controlled study. Neurosci. Res. 112, 10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2016.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez, L., Albein-Urios, N., Kirkovski, M., McGinley, J. L., Murphy, A. T., Hyde, C., et al. (2017). Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to the right cerebellar hemisphere affects motor adaptation during gait. Cerebellum 16, 168–177. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0788-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrucci, R., Bocci, T., Cortese, F., Ruggiero, F., and Priori, A. (2016). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation in neurological disease. Cerebellum Ataxias 3:16. doi: 10.1186/s40673-016-0054-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrucci, R., Bocci, T., Cortese, F., Ruggiero, F., and Priori, A. (2019). Noninvasive cerebellar stimulation as a complement tool to pharmacotherapy. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 17, 14–20. doi: 10.2174/1570159X15666171114142422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrucci, R., Brunoni, A. R., Parazzini, M., Vergari, M., Rossi, E., Fumagalli, M., et al. (2013). Modulating human procedural learning by cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation. Cerebellum 12, 485–492. doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0436-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrucci, R., Cortese, F., and Priori, A. (2015). Cerebellar tDCS: how to do it. Cerebellum 14, 27–30. doi: 10.1007/s12311-014-0599-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foerster, Á., Melo, L., Mello, M., Castro, R., Shirahige, L., Rocha, S., et al. (2017). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation (ctDCS) impairs balance control in healthy individuals. Cerebellum 16, 872–875. doi: 10.1007/s12311-017-0863-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galea, J. M., Jayaram, G., Ajagbe, L., and Celnik, P. (2009). Modulation of cerebellar excitability by polarity-specific noninvasive direct current stimulation. J. Neurosci. 29, 9115–9122. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2184-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimaldi, G., Argyropoulos, G., Boehringer, A., Celnik, P., Edwards, M., Ferrucci, R., et al. (2014). Non-invasive cerebellar stimulation-a consensus paper. Cerebellum 13, 121–138. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0514-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimaldi, G., Argyropoulos, G. P., Bastian, A., Cortes, M., Davis, N. J., Edwards, D. J., et al. (2016). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation (ctDCS): a novel approach to understanding cerebellar function in health and disease. Neuroscientist 22, 83–97. doi: 10.1177/1073858414559409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halo (2019). Improve Your Game San Francisco. Available online at: https://www.haloneuro.com/ (accessed March 14, 2019).

Higgins, J., and Green, S (eds.). (2011). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Version 5.1.0.

Google Scholar

Huang, L., Deng, Y., Zheng, X., and Liu, Y. (2019). Transcranial direct current stimulation with halo sport enhances repeated sprint cycling and cognitive performance. Front. Physiol. 10:118. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hulst, T., John, L., Kuper, M., van der Geest, J. N., Goricke, S. L., Donchin, O., et al. (2017). Cerebellar patients do not benefit from cerebellar or M1 transcranial direct current stimulation during force-field reaching adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 732–748. doi: 10.1152/jn.00808.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, M. (2000). Mechanisms of motor learning in the cerebellum. Brain Res. 886, 237–245. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)03142-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izawa, J., and Shadmehr, R. (2011). Learning from sensory and reward prediction errors during motor adaptation. PLoS Comput. Biol. 7:e1002012. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, A. K., de Albuquerque, L. L., Pantovic, M., Fischer, K. M., Guadagnoli, M. A., Riley, Z. A., et al. (2019). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation enhances motor learning in a complex overhand throwing task. Cerebellum 18, 813–816. doi: 10.1007/s12311-019-01040-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jalali, R., Miall, R., and Galea, J. M. (2017). No consistent effect of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on visuomotor adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 655–665. doi: 10.1152/jn.00896.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jayaram, G., Tang, B., Pallegadda, R., Vasudevan, E. V., Celnik, P., and Bastian, A. (2012). Modulating locomotor adaptation with cerebellar stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 2950–2957. doi: 10.1152/jn.00645.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jongkees, B. J., Immink, M. A., Boer, O. D., Yavari, F., Nitsche, M. A., and Colzato, L. S. (2019). The effect of cerebellar tDCS on sequential motor response selection. Cerebellum 18, 738–749. doi: 10.1007/s12311-019-01029-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitago, T., and Krakauer, J. W. (2013). “Chapter 8 - Motor learning principles for neurorehabilitation,” in Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 110, eds M. P. Barnes and D. C. Good (New York, NY: Elsevier), 93–103. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-52901-5.00008-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kojima, Y., Iwamoto, Y., and Yoshida, K. (2004). Memory of learning facilitates saccadic adaptation in the monkey. J. Neurosci. 24, 7531–7539. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1741-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koziol, L. F., Budding, D., Andreasen, N., D'Arrigo, S., Bulgheroni, S., Imamizu, H., et al. (2014). Consensus paper: the cerebellum's role in movement and cognition. Cerebellum 13, 151–177. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0511-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, E. J., Apps, R., Bengtsson, F., Cerminara, N. L., De Zeeuw, C. I., Ebner, T. J., et al. (2017). The roles of the olivocerebellar pathway in motor learning and motor control. A consensus paper. Cerebellum 16, 230–252. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0787-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liew, S. L., Thompson, T., Ramirez, J., Butcher, P. A., Taylor, J. A., and Celnik, P. A. (2018). Variable neural contributions to explicit and implicit learning during visuomotor adaptation. Front. Neurosci. 12:610. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohse, K. R., Lang, C. E., and Boyd, L. A. (2014). Is more better? Using metadata to explore dose–response relationships in stroke rehabilitation. Stroke 45, 2053–2058. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.004695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mamlins, A., Hulst, T., Donchin, O., Timmann, D., and Claassen, J. (2019). No effects of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on force field and visuomotor reach adaptation in young and healthy subjects. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 2112–2125. doi: 10.1152/jn.00352.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manto, M., Bower, J. M., Conforto, A. B., Delgado-García, J. M., Da Guarda, S. N. F., Gerwig, M., et al. (2012). Consensus paper: roles of the cerebellum in motor control—the diversity of ideas on cerebellar involvement in movement. Cerebellum 11, 457–487. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0331-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mariën, P., Ackermann, H., Adamaszek, M., Barwood, C. H., Beaton, A., Desmond, J., et al. (2014). Consensus paper: language and the cerebellum: an ongoing enigma. Cerebellum 13, 386–410. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0540-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, T. A., Keating, J. G., Goodkin, H. P., Bastian, A. J., and Thach, W. T. (1996). Throwing while looking through prismsII. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze—throw calibrations. Brain 119, 1199–1211. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.4.1199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miall, R. C., and Wolpert, D. M. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural netw. 9, 1265–1279. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(96)00035-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monte-Silva, K., Kuo, M. F., Liebetanz, D., Paulus, W., and Nitsche, M. A. (2010). Shaping the optimal repetition interval for cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). J. Neurophysiol. 103, 1735–1740. doi: 10.1152/jn.00924.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., Antal, A., Tergau, F., and Paulus, W. (2003). Safety criteria for transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in humans. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 2220–2222; author reply 2–3. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00235-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okano, A. H., Fontes, E. B., Montenegro, R. A., Cyrino, E. S., Li, L. M., et al. (2015). Brain stimulation modulates the autonomic nervous system, rating of perceived exertion and performance during maximal exercise. Br. J. Sports Med. 49, 1213–1218. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldrati, V., and Schutter, D. J. (2018). Targeting the human cerebellum with transcranial direct current stimulation to modulate behavior: a meta-analysis. Cerebellum 17, 228–236. doi: 10.1007/s12311-017-0877-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panico, F., Sagliano, L., Grossi, D., and Trojano, L. (2016). Cerebellar cathodal tDCS interferes with recalibration and spatial realignment during prism adaptation procedure in healthy subjects. Brain Cogn. 105, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panouillères, M. T., Joundi, R. A., Brittain, J.-S., and Jenkinson, N. (2015). Reversing motor adaptation deficits in the ageing brain using non-invasive stimulation. J. Physiol. (Lond). 593, 3645–3655. doi: 10.1113/JP270484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Leone, A., Nguyet, D., Cohen, L. G., Brasil-Neto, J. P., Cammarota, A., and Hallett, M. (1995). Modulation of muscle responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation during the acquisition of new fine motor skills. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 1037–1045. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.3.1037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perciavalle, V., Apps, R., Bracha, V., Delgado-García, J. M., Gibson, A. R., Leggio, M., et al. (2013). Consensus paper: current views on the role of cerebellar interpositus nucleus in movement control and emotion. Cerebellum 12, 738–757. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0464-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poortvliet, P., Hsieh, B., Cresswell, A., Au, J., and Meinzer, M. (2018). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation improves adaptive postural control. Clin. Neurophysiol. 129, 33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.09.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rampersad, S. M., Janssen, A. M., Lucka, F., Aydin, Ü., Lanfer, B., Lew, S., et al. (2014). Simulating transcranial direct current stimulation with a detailed anisotropic human head model. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 22, 441–452. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2014.2308997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, J., Schambra, H. M., Cohen, L. G., Buch, E. R., Fritsch, B., Zarahn, E., et al. (2009). Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1590–1595. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805413106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samaei, A., Ehsani, F., Zoghi, M., Yosephi, M. H., and Jaberzadeh, S. (2017). Online and offline effects of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning in healthy older adults: a randomized double-blind sham-controlled study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 45, 1177–1185. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. A., and Lee, T. D. (2011). Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis, 5th Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Shah, B., Nguyen, T. T., and Madhavan, S. (2013). Polarity independent effects of cerebellar tDCS on short term ankle visuomotor learning. Brain Stimul. 6, 966–968. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Small, S., Hlustik, P., Noll, D., Genovese, C., and Solodkin, A. (2002). Cerebellar hemispheric activation ipsilateral to the paretic hand correlates with functional recovery after stroke. Brain 125, 1544–1557. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soderstrom, N. C., and Bjork, R. A. (2015). Learning versus performance: An integrative review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 176–199. doi: 10.1177/1745691615569000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokolov, A. A., Miall, R. C., and Ivry, R. B. (2017). The cerebellum: adaptive prediction for movement and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed). 21, 313–332. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, K. M., Enders, A., Thier, W., Batsikadze, G., Ludolph, N., Ilg, W., et al. (2016). Cerebellar tDCS does not improve learning in a complex whole body dynamic balance task in young healthy subjects. PLoS ONE 11:e0163598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163598

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterne, J. A. C., Savović, J., Page, M. J., Elbers, R. G., Blencowe, N. S., Boutron, I., et al. (2019). RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 366:l4898. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, R. L. S., Chen, M., Hatch, A., and Kimberley, T. J. (2018). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation modulates corticospinal excitability during motor training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:118. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taubert, M., Stein, T., Kreutzberg, T., Stockinger, C., Hecker, L., Focke, A., et al. (2016). Remote effects of non-invasive cerebellar stimulation on error processing in motor re-learning. Brain Stimul. 9, 692–699. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmann, D., and Daum, I. (2007). Cerebellar contributions to cognitive functions: a progress report after two decades of research. Cerebellum 6:159. doi: 10.1080/14734220701496448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, Y. W., Diedrichsen, J., Krakauer, J. W., Shadmehr, R., and Bastian, A. J. (2007). Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 54–62. doi: 10.1152/jn.00266.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dun, K., Bodranghien, F., Mariën, P., and Manto, M. U. (2016). tDCS of the cerebellum: where do we stand in 2016? technical issues and critical review of the literature. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:199. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2008). Motor-skill learning in older adults—a review of studies on age-related differences. Euro. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 5, 5–16. doi: 10.1007/s11556-008-0030-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, N., Brown, M., Thompson, A., and Frackowiak, R. (2003). Neural correlates of motor recovery after stroke: a longitudinal fMRI study. Brain 126, 2476–2496. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waters-Metenier, S., Husain, M., Wiestler, T., and Diedrichsen, J. (2014). Bihemispheric transcranial direct current stimulation enhances effector-independent representations of motor synergy and sequence learning. J. Neurosci. 34, 1037–1050. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2282-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winstein, C., Lewthwaite, R., Blanton, S. R., Wolf, L. B., and Wishart, L. (2014). Infusing motor learning research into neurorehabilitation practice: a historical perspective with case exemplar from the accelerated skill acquisition program. J. Neurol. Phys. Ther. 38, 190–200. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., and Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Curr. Biol. 11, R729–R32. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00432-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yavari, F., Mahdavi, S., Towhidkhah, F., Ahmadi-Pajouh, M. A., Ekhtiari, H., and Darainy, M. (2016). Cerebellum as a forward but not inverse model in visuomotor adaptation task: a tDCS-based and modeling study. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 997–1012. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4523-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: transcranial direct current stimulation, tDCS, cerebellum, motor learning, motor adaptation, skill learning

Citation: Kumari N, Taylor D and Signal N (2019) The Effect of Cerebellar Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Motor Learning: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:328. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00328

Received: 19 June 2019; Accepted: 06 September 2019;
Published: 04 October 2019.

Edited by:

Ferdinand Binkofski, RWTH Aachen University, Germany

Reviewed by:

Wei-Peng Teo, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
Timmann Dagmar, Essen University Hospital, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Kumari, Taylor and Signal. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nitika Kumari, nitika.kumari@aut.ac.nz

Download