Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 15 July 2020
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00266

Occupational Physical Stress Is Negatively Associated With Hippocampal Volume and Memory in Older Adults

Agnieszka Z. Burzynska1* Daniel C. Ganster2 Jason Fanning3 Elizabeth A. Salerno4 Neha P. Gothe5 Michelle W. Voss6 Edward McAuley5,7 Arthur F. Kramer7,8,9
  • 1Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Molecular, Cellular and Integrative Neurosciences Graduate Interdisciplinary Studies Program, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, United States
  • 2Department of Management, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, United States
  • 3Department of Health & Exercise Sciences, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC, United States
  • 4Cancer Prevention Fellowship Program, Division of Cancer Epidemiology & Genetics, National Cancer Institute, Bethesda, MD, United States
  • 5Department of Kinesiology and Community Health, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States
  • 6Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, United States
  • 7Beckman Institute for Advanced Science & Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States
  • 8Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 9Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States

Our jobs can provide intellectually and socially enriched environments but also be the source of major psychological and physical stressors. As the average full-time worker spends >8 h at work per weekday and remains in the workforce for about 40 years, occupational experiences must be important factors in cognitive and brain aging. Therefore, we studied whether occupational complexity and stress are associated with hippocampal volume and cognitive ability in 99 cognitively normal older adults. We estimated occupational complexity, physical stress, and psychological stress using the Work Design Questionnaire (Morgeson and Humphrey, 2006), Quantitative Workload Inventory and Interpersonal Conflict at Work Scale (Spector and Jex, 1998). We found that physical stress, comprising physical demands and work conditions, was associated with smaller hippocampal volume and poorer memory performance. These associations were independent of age, gender, brain size, socioeconomic factors (education, income, and job title), duration of the job, employment status, leisure physical activity and general stress. This suggests that physical demands at work and leisure physical activity may have largely independent and opposite effects on brain and cognitive health. Our findings highlight the importance of considering midlife occupational experiences, such as work physical stress, in understanding individual trajectories of cognitive and brain aging.

Introduction

An average full time worker in the United States spends 8.56 h at work per weekday (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2016) and remains in the workforce for about 40 years (Komp-Leukkunen, 2019). Thus, occupational experiences likely play an important role in cognitive and brain aging. On the one hand, a job can provide an intellectually and socially enriched environment, supporting cognitive function. Indeed, midlife occupational complexity with people and data has been associated with better verbal skills, memory, and speed of processing after retirement (Finkel et al., 2009; Smart et al., 2014; Andel et al., 2015, 2016). On the other hand, work is the second main source of stress among employees (American Psychological Association, 2015). Both psychological and physical stress (e.g., physical hazards) at work have been linked with poorer cognitive ability in midlife (McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995; Sandström et al., 2005; Scott et al., 2015) and after retirement (Gow et al., 2014; Andel et al., 2015; Sindi et al., 2017; Dong et al., 2018).

To better understand the neural underpinnings of the above associations, we recently proposed the “Brain Aging: Occupational Stimulation and Stress” (BOSS) model (Burzynska et al., 2019). The BOSS model acknowledges the possibly opposing influences of occupational complexity and stress on cognitive aging. Specifically, it posits that occupational enrichment may protect against age-related cognitive decline, either by supporting cognitive reserve [i.e., providing mind’s resistance to biological brain aging (Stern et al., 2018)] or by supporting brain maintenance [i.e., minimizing age-related neural losses (Nyberg, 2017)]. Conversely, occupational stress may deplete the brain reserve and accelerate age-related changes through a variety of neural and systemic processes (Burzynska et al., 2019).

The hippocampus is the ideal first target for studying the BOSS model. First, the hippocampus undergoes a reduction in volume in both healthy aging (Scahill et al., 2003; Kennedy and Raz, 2005) and dementia (Barnes et al., 2009), which is associated with declines in memory and general cognition (Van Petten et al., 2004; Den Heijer et al., 2010; Gorbach et al., 2017). More importantly, the hippocampus is the brain structure where occupational stimulation and stress may converge. For example, research on both humans and animals shows that enriched environments promote hippocampal neurogenesis, neuroplasticity, and neurotrophic support (Bettio et al., 2017). Alternatively, psychological and physical stress inhibits hippocampal neurogenesis, neurotrophic support, and can induce neurotoxicity via upregulation of glucocorticoid cascade, inflammation, or oxidative stress (Nagata et al., 2009; Choi et al., 2014; Bettio et al., 2017; Burzynska et al., 2019).

Currently, the evidence for the effects of occupational stimulation and stress on the hippocampus is scarce. Greater midlife managerial experience has been associated with greater hippocampal volume and lesser atrophy over a period of 2.5 years in older adults (Suo et al., 2012, 2017), and taxi drivers had greater hippocampi than bus drivers, where hippocampal volume correlated with their navigating experience (Maguire et al., 2006). This suggests that certain types of occupational complexity may support brain health maintenance. Conversely, another study demonstrated that greater work complexity with data, people, and things, when controlling for cognitive function, was associated with smaller hippocampi in middle-aged adults at risk of Alzheimer’s Disease (Boots et al., 2015) this lends support to the cognitive reserve theory. Finally, others found no association between early-life occupational complexity [estimated from the ONET occupational codes; (Kaup et al., 2018)] or exposure to novelty [i.e., work task changes (Oltmanns et al., 2017)] and hippocampal volume in middle age. To our knowledge, the one existing study linking occupational stress to brain structure found that burnout in middle-aged workers was related to decreased volumes of several brain regions, but not the hippocampus (Blix et al., 2013).

In sum, the associations between occupational complexity, stress, and hippocampal health in older age need to be further investigated to understand individual differences in cognitive and brain aging. Therefore, the current study related occupational characteristics to hippocampal volume and cognitive function in healthy aging. The unique aspect of our study was the direct and subjective (as opposed to derived from occupational codes) assessment of stimulating and stressful work characteristics using three validated questionnaires. We hypothesized that older adults who experienced more cognitive complexity at work would have larger hippocampi and better cognition, and those who reported more occupational stress (either physical or psychological) would have smaller hippocampi and poorer cognition, after controlling for age, gender, and brain size. Importantly, our key question was whether occupational experiences are related to hippocampal volume and cognition beyond the known proxies of cognitive reserve related to socioeconomic status (e.g., education, occupational attainment, and income), as well as general stress and leisure physical activity.

Methods

Participants

The current study was conducted using the MRI, cognitive, and other baseline data from a 6-month randomized controlled exercise trial (clinical study identifier NCT01472744). Healthy, low active older adults were recruited in Champaign County in Illinois. Of the 1,119 participants recruited, 247 (n = 169 women, n = 78 men) met inclusion criteria of the clinical trial, agreed to enroll in the study, and underwent a series of demographic, health, neuroimaging, cognitive, and cardiorespiratory data collection at baseline. For more details on this clinical trial, its primary outcomes and neuroimaging data, refer to our earlier work (Burzynska et al., 2017; Ehlers et al., 2017; Fanning et al., 2017; Baniqued et al., 2018; Voss et al., 2019). Eligible participants met the following criteria to be enrolled in the clinical trial: (1) were between the ages of 60 and 80 years old; (2) were free from psychiatric and neurological illness and had no history of stroke, transient ischemic attack, or head trauma; (3) scored ≥23 on the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) and >21 on a Telephone Interview of Cognitive Status (TICS-M) questionnaire; (4) scored < 10 on the geriatric depression scale (GDS-15); (5) scored ≥75% right-handedness on the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire; (6) demonstrated normal or corrected-to-normal vision of at least 20/40 and no color blindness; (7) were screened for safe participation in an MRI environment (e.g., no metallic implants that could interfere with the magnetic field or cause injury and no claustrophobia); and (8) reported to have participated in no more than two moderate bouts of exercise per week within the past 6-months. The baseline data were collected in four waves in years 2011–2014.

In 2017 we sent questionnaires assessing occupational characteristics to participants who indicated interest in follow-up assessments; 100 participants returned the completed work questionnaire and participants with MMSE ≤ 23 were excluded from final analyses due to possible mild cognitive impairment (Marioni et al., 2011). Seventy of 99 participants were women, 76 identified as Caucasian White, six as Black, one as Asian, and 16 decided not to answer; 83 identified as Non-Hispanic 16 decided not to respond; 96 had MRI data (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

To estimate cognitive reserve socioeconomic factors, we used years of education, income and job title collected at the clinical trial baseline. The participants were asked to choose the following household income categories: (1) <$5,000, (2) $5,001–10,000, (3) $10,001–$15,000, (4) 15,001–20,000, (5) 20,001–25,000, (6) 25,001–30,000, (7) 30,001–40,000, (8) >40,000. Sixty-five subjects responded >40,000, six 30,000–40,000, two 25,001–30,000, three 20,001–25,000, three 15,001–20,000, and four 10,000–15,000; 16 chose not to respond. The number of participants who identified with the following job titles were: (1) officials and managers (n = 26), (2) professionals (n = 29), (3) technicians (n = 3), (4) sales (n = 6), (5) office and clerical (n = 14), (6) craft workers (skilled, n = 3), (7) operatives (semiskilled, n = 10), (8) laborers (unskilled, n = 1), (9) service workers (n = 6), (10) homemakers (n = 1). In addition, we asked the participants about their current employment status: 13 worked full time, 10 part time, 26 were retired but employed, and were retired and not employed.

MRI Acquisition

Structural images were acquired on a 3 T Siemens Trio Tim system (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a T1-weighted 0.9 mm3 MPRAGE sequence (TR = 1,900 ms, TE = 2.32 ms, TI: 900 ms, FA = 9°; matrix = 256 × 256; FOV = 230 mm; 192 slices; GRAPPA acceleration factor 2).

MRI Data Processing

Automated brain tissue segmentation and reconstruction of cortical models was performed on T1-weighted images using the Freesurfer software, version 5.31. Individual T1-weighted images underwent non-brain tissue removal, Talairach transformation, and creation of representations of the gray/white matter boundaries (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Fischl et al., 1999). AZB screened all reconstructions to evaluate the success and plausibility of the automatically processed results, as recommended by the software developers. Volumes of estimated total intracranial volume (eTIV), left and right hippocampus were extracted per individual; total volume of hippocampus was calculated as the sum of bilateral volumes (Table 1).

Subjective Occupational Experiences

Participants were instructed to answer the questionnaires with regard to their most recent job that they had been performing (full time or part-time) for 2 years or longer. We focused on the most recent job to minimize the effects of age-related declines in memory (Park and Festini, 2017). Subjective occupational experiences were assessed with the 77-item Work Design Questionnaire (WDQ) that results in 21 occupational factors (Morgeson and Humphrey, 2006), the 5-item Interpersonal Conflict at Work Scale (Spector and Jex, 1998), and the 4-item Quantitative Workload Inventory (Spector and Jex, 1998). Both have been validated and tested in different contexts (Bayona et al., 2015; Wright et al., 2017). Among the total of 23 factors across the three questionnaires, we identified six factors that refer to cognitive job complexity (task variety, job complexity, information processing, problem solving, skill variety, and specialization), two related to psychological stress (workload and interpersonal conflict), and two representing physical stress (physical demands and work conditions). To reduce data dimensionality, we used a principal component analysis (PCA) and a varimax rotation. It resulted in three constructs with eigenvalues > 1 that represented “job complexity,” “psychological stress,” and “physical stress” (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of factor analysis with PCA on select occupational characteristics.

Cognitive Function

To measure the latent constructs of reasoning, perceptual speed, episodic memory, and vocabulary knowledge we administered a well-validated Virginia Cognitive Aging Project cognitive battery (Salthouse and Ferrer-Caja, 2003; Salthouse, 2004, 2010) consisting of 16 computer-based and pen-and-pencil tasks. To obtain components representing the four cognitive constructs and to confirm the validity of task structure, we performed a PCA with varimax rotation, with missing values replaced by the sample mean (Burzynska et al., 2015). Data were within 3SD and normally distributed.

General Stress and Leisure-Time Physical Activity

As an attempt to tease apart occupational physical demands from leisure physical activity, we used a well-validated 10-item Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE) (Washburn et al., 1993). We summed the weighted PASE scores related to leisure physical activities walking outside, light, moderate and strenuous sport/recreational activities, muscle strength/endurance exercises, light and heavy housework, home repairs, lawn work or yard care, outdoor gardening, and care for another person, omitting the score related to work or volunteering (Washburn et al., 1999).

To tease apart occupational from general psychological stress, we used the 10-item Perceived Stress Scale [PSS-10; (Cohen and Williamson, 1988)]. PSS-10 evaluates the degree to which individuals believe their life has been unpredictable, uncontrollable, and overloaded during the previous month on a 5-point Likert scale, with total score ranging from 0 to 40 (higher scores indicating higher levels of stress).

Statistical Analyses

We used a linear hierarchical regression model (SPSS v. 26), with total hippocampal volume or the four cognitive constructs as dependent variables and the three occupational measures and other covariates as independent variables. R2 change statistics were used to determine whether occupational characteristics explained a significant amount of variance in the dependent variable, after controlling for the covariates. The covariates were age, gender, eTIV (for the model with hippocampal volume), and socioeconomic factors: years of completed formal education, household income, and job title (dummy coded, with homemaker as reference), “years between MRI/cog and occupational data,” “years in the occupation,” and current employment status. The same analyses were performed with adding either the general stress or leisure physical activity variables as covariates. The assumptions of linearity, normality of distributed errors, and uncorrelated errors were tested and fulfilled by the regression models.

Results

Occupational Characteristics, Hippocampal Volume, and Cognition

Only physical stress predicted hippocampal volume in the fully adjusted model [R2 = 0.419, R2change = 0.074, F(3,74) = 3.1, p = 0.031; Table 3].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Occupational correlates of hippocampal volume.

Next, we explored the associations between occupational characteristics and the four cognitive constructs. Only physical stress predicted a significant amount of variance in memory [R2 = 0.400, R2change = 0.082, F(3,78) = 3.5, p = 0.018; Table 4].

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Occupational correlates of memory.

Physical Demands at Work, Leisure Physical Activity, and General Stress

The physical stress construct referred to both physical demands and work conditions. Post hoc two-tailed bivariate correlations indicated that “physical demands” correlated with hippocampal volume (r = −0.305, p = 0.003, n = 96) and memory (r = −0.247, p = 0.014, n = 99) but not with “work conditions.” Thus, we investigated whether occupational physical demands were related to hippocampal volume or memory beyond leisure physical activity or general stress.

After controlling additionally for leisure physical activity, physical stress remained associated with hippocampal volume (βphysical_stress = −0.367, t = −3.4, p = 0.001) and leisure physical activity was also positively associated with hippocampal volume (βleisure physical activity = 0.195, t = 2.01, p = 0.048). The result was similar when controlling for general stress, with only physical stress being associated with hippocampal volume (βphysical_stress = −0.317, t = −2.9, p = 0.005). General stress was not related to hippocampal volume.

After controlling for leisure physical activity, the association of physical stress at work with memory remained significant (βphysical_stress = −0.287, t = −2.6, p = 0.011). After controlling for general stress, both physical stress (βphysical_stress = −0.295, t = −2.8, p = 0.007) and psychological stress at work (βpsychological_stress = 0.203, t = 2.1, p = 0.040) were associated with memory. General stress and leisure physical activity were not related to memory.

Discussion

Our results indicate that physical stress, comprising physical demands and work conditions, was associated with smaller hippocampal volume and poorer memory performance. These associations were independent of age, gender, brain size, socioeconomic factors (education, income, and job title), duration of the job, time passed between MRI/cognitive and occupational data collection, employment status, general stress, and leisure physical activity. In addition, there was a weak positive association of leisure physical activity with hippocampal volume, and of occupational psychological stress with memory.

Occupational Physical Stress and Hippocampal Volume

The main result of this study was a negative association between physical stress at work and hippocampal volume. In general, our results give support to the brain maintenance model of cognitive aging (Nyberg, 2017), where presence of a stressor may deplete brain health and memory, and the absence of the stressor may be protective. Importantly, post hoc correlations indicated that this association was driven by the “physical demand” aspect of occupational physical stress (i.e., need for muscular strength, endurance, and physical effort), with a lesser role for working conditions (e.g., environmental hazards). However, it is important to note that a very small portion of our sample worked in occupations that are defined as physical labor; only one person identified as an unskilled laborer and six as service workers. Therefore, our findings likely refer to one’s subjective or job-specific physical demand, beyond the official job title or a general job definition.

The negative physical demand-hippocampal volume association is an intriguing result, given that aerobic fitness, aerobic exercise, as well as resistance training have been shown to increase hippocampal volume and improve cognitive performance in older adults (Erickson et al., 2011; Szabo et al., 2011; Best et al., 2015; Ten Brinke et al., 2015). However, we found that controlling for leisure physical activity did not attenuate this negative association, although leisure physical activity showed the expected positive association with hippocampal volume. This suggests that leisure physical activity and physical demands at work may have independent and opposing associations with hippocampal structure and warrant replication and further study in larger samples.

Research on both animals and humans has demonstrated the negative effects of stress on the function and structure of the hippocampus (McEwen, 2006). However, to our knowledge, our results are novel in that we showed negative associations of (a) occupational and (b) subjective physical stress with hippocampal volume. Physical restraint or forced swimming has been used to induce physical stress in mice, resulting in increased oxidative stress (Nagata et al., 2009) or reduced serotonin receptor density in the hippocampus (Choi et al., 2014). With no doubt, more research is needed to understand the effects of occupational physical stress on the structure and function of the human hippocampus. Importantly, our data indicated that the occupational physical stress-hippocampus association was independent of general stress, suggesting that this relationship may be specific to occupational experiences.

Finally, our data indicated that the association between occupational physical stress and hippocampal volume was independent of socioeconomic factors, including early-life education, the related subsequent occupational attainment (i.e., job title), and income. This is in line with earlier studies linking occupational experiences to cognitive outcomes in older age, suggesting that the recency of occupational experiences may take precedence in predicting neural and cognitive outcomes in older age (Dartigues et al., 1992a, b; Frisoni et al., 1993).

Occupational Characteristics and Cognition

We found that greater occupational physical stress was related to poorer episodic memory, independent of demographic and socioeconomic covariates. This is consistent with recent studies showing that older adults who reported high physical work strain showed declines in memory and general cognition (Gow et al., 2014; Sindi et al., 2017; Dong et al., 2018), controlling for sex, education and social class. These studies resembled ours in the subjective assessment of physical demands, physical hazards, and risk of injury at work, but assessed cognitive ability using single tasks or crude composite scores. Therefore, our study extends these earlier findings by a using a broader construct of memory.

Interestingly, our findings also converge with studies that used occupational category as a proxy of physical stress at work. For instance, an epidemiological study in France found that older farmers, service employees, or blue-collar workers had higher risk of memory (Dartigues et al., 1992b) or general cognitive decline (Dartigues et al., 1992a) than those working in professional/managerial occupations, controlling for age, sex, and education. A similar association was found among older adults in Italy, after adjustment for age, education, and financial dissatisfaction (Frisoni et al., 1993). Thus, the association between physical stress at work and poorer cognitive abilities in older age, independent of socioeconomic status, seems to hold across different countries and operationalizations of physical stress.

In addition, we found a weak but positive association between occupational psychological stress (i.e., workload and intrapersonal conflict) and memory when controlling for general stress. This finding is an important addition to the mixed results linking work-related psychological stress to cognition in later life: one study reported that greater work strain predicted poorer memory performance and greater memory decline 15–21 years later (Agbenyikey et al., 2015). Another study found that midlife work-related stress predicted poorer global cognition and processing speed 25 years later, but was unrelated to episodic memory, executive functioning, and verbal fluency (Sindi et al., 2017). Similarly, adults who worked in jobs with high psychological demands or strain had greater 11-year declines in MMSE scores that those in low strain jobs, with no difference in verbal memory (Dong et al., 2018). These discrepancies may stem from different operationalizations of cognitive function, work-related stress, and study timelines. In sum, as research in both animals and humans shows that stress can facilitate memory and learning under some conditions (e.g., Joëls et al., 2006) our preliminary findings warrant further research.

Finally, we did not find positive associations between occupational complexity, hippocampal size and cognition. This resembles findings by Oltmanns et al. (2017) and Kaup et al. (2018), but not Suo et al. (2012), suggesting that different operationalizations of occupational stimulation (e.g., novelty at work, managerial experience, complexity with data or people, or subjective variety of tasks or information processing) may lead to different outcomes.

Limitations and Future Directions

The main limitation of our study was the retrospective collection of occupational data, which occurred several years after the MRI and cognitive measurements. Although controlling for this time lag, job duration, or employment status did not change the results, we do not know whether our participants referred to their current or past job, how long ago they retired from the job, and whether it was their main lifetime occupation, a later post-retirement, or a bridge job. Furthermore, although all our participants scored high on the MMSE at the time of MRI and cognitive data collection, it remains unknown whether and to what extent their responses to occupational questionnaire were affected by age-related declines in memory (Park and Festini, 2017) or the positivity effect (Mather and Carstensen, 2005). Future studies could consider combining occupational surveys with 24-h recalls (Matthews et al., 2018) or real-time assessments such as the Ecological Momentary Assessment (Shiffman et al., 2008) to gauge the nature of job tasks, their intellectual and physical challenge, objective stress, and emotional reactivity with lesser recall bias and greater ecological validity. Next, our cross-sectional approach does not allow for any causal interpretations. Thus, the observed associations are equally likely to be bidirectional (i.e., one’s memory ability or brain health could affect one’s occupational choices or opportunities) or driven by a third variable that was not captured in our study but could affect both hippocampal volume, memory, and perception of work environment, for example, quality of sleep, metabolic disorders such as obesity and diabetes, chronic pain and inflammation, and depression (Fotuhi et al., 2012; Mutso et al., 2012). However, our approach is the next best way to establish the relationships between occupational exposures, brain and cognitive health, given it is not possible to experimentally manipulate long-term occupational exposures in a randomized design. Future observational longitudinal studies may help establish time-ordered associations. Finally, our promising results should be extended to other brain regions and metrics of brain health, and possible interactions of occupational stress and stimulation on brain and cognition need to be explored.

Conclusion

Using validated questionnaires of subjective work characteristics, we found a negative association between occupational physical demands, hippocampal volume, and memory in cognitively healthy older adults. The observed associations were independent of early-life education and socioeconomic factors, which highlights the importance of considering occupational experiences in understanding individual trajectories of cognitive and brain aging. Our findings suggest that future interventions aimed at maintaining hippocampal and cognitive health may need to target both workplace and leisure activities.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Illinois institutional review board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study and the study was performed in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

AB, DG, AK, and EM conceived and designed the occupational data collection. MV, AK, and EM conceived and designed the MRI and cognitive data collection. AB, JF, ES, and NG collected and preprocessed the data. AB and DG performed the statistical analysis. AB wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging at the National Institutes of Health (R37 AG025667), funding from Abbott Nutrition through the Center for Nutrition, Learning, and Memory at the University of Illinois (PIs AK and EM), and Mather LifeWays Institute on Aging research grant (PI AB).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Anya Knecht, Susan Houseworth, Nancy Dodge, Holly Tracy, and the Lifelong Brain and Cognition and Exercise Psychology Laboratory graduate students and staff for their help in participant recruitment and data collection, and Jessy Jiao for occupational data entry.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00266/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/

References

Agbenyikey, W., Karasek, R., Cifuentes, M., Wolf, P. A., Seshadri, S., Taylor, J. A., et al. (2015). Job strain and cognitive decline: a prospective study of the framingham offspring cohort. Int. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 6, 79–94. doi: 10.15171/ijoem.2015.534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychological Association. (2015). “Stress in AmericaTM paying with our health,” in Stress in America, (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association).

Google Scholar

Andel, R., Finkel, D., and Pedersen, N. L. (2016). Effects of preretirement work complexity and postretirement leisure activity on cognitive aging. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 71, 849–856. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andel, R., Silverstein, M., and Kareholt, I. (2015). The role of midlife occupational complexity and leisure activity in late-life cognition. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 70, 314–321. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baniqued, P. L., Gallen, C. L., Voss, M. W., Burzynska, A. Z., Wong, C. N., Cooke, G. E., et al. (2018). Brain network modularity predicts exercise-related executive function gains in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:426. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, J., Bartlett, J. W., van de Pol, L. A., Loy, C. T., Scahill, R. I., Frost, C., et al. (2009). A meta-analysis of hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 30, 1711–1723. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayona, J. A., Caballer, A., and Peiró, J. M. (2015). The work design questionnaire: Spanish version and validation. Revist. Psicol. Del Trabajo Las Organ. 31, 187–200. doi: 10.1016/j.rpto.2015.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Chiu, B. K., Liang Hsu, C., Nagamatsu, L. S., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2015). Long-term effects of resistance exercise training on cognition and brain volume in older women: results from a randomized controlled trial. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 21, 745–756. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettio, L. E. B., Rajendran, L., and Gil-Mohapel, J. (2017). The effects of aging in the hippocampus and cognitive decline. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 79, 66–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blix, E., Perski, A., Berglund, H., and Savic, I. (2013). Long-term occupational stress is associated with regional reductions in brain tissue volumes. PLoS One 8:e64065. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boots, E. A., Schultz, S. A., Almeida, R. P., Oh, J. M., Koscik, R. L., Dowling, M. N., et al. (2015). Occupational complexity and cognitive reserve in a middle-Aged cohort at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 30, 634–642. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acv041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bureau of Labor Statistics (2016). Average Hours Employed People Spent Working on Days Worked by Day of Week. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau Of Labor Statistics.

Google Scholar

Burzynska, A. Z., Jiao, Y., and Ganster, D. C. (2019). Adult-life occupational exposures: enriched environment or a stressor for the aging brain? Work Aging Retir. 5, 3–23. doi: 10.1093/workar/way007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burzynska, A. Z., Jiao, Y., Knecht, A. M., Fanning, J., Awick, E. A., Chen, T., et al. (2017). White matter integrity declined over 6-months, but dance intervention improved integrity of the Fornix of older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:59. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burzynska, A. Z., Wong, C. N., Voss, M. W., Cooke, G. E., Gothe, N. P., Fanning, J., et al. (2015). Physical activity is linked to greater moment-to-moment variability in spontaneous brain activity in older adults. PLoS One 10:e0134819. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J. Y., Shin, S., Lee, M., Jeon, T. J., Seo, Y., Kim, C. H., et al. (2014). Acute physical stress induces the alteration of the serotonin 1A receptor density in the hippocampus. Synapse 68, 363–368. doi: 10.1002/syn.21748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., and Williamson, G. (1988). “Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States,” in The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. The social psychology of health, eds S. Spacapan and S. Oskamp (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc), 31–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1983.tb02325.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., and Sereno, M. I. (1993). Improved localizadon of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 162–176. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dartigues, J., Gagnon, M., Letenneur, L., Barberger-Gateau, P., Commenges, D., Evaldre, M., et al. (1992a). Principal lifetime occupation and cognitive impairment in a french elderly cohort (PAQUID). Am. J. Epidemiol. 135, 981–988. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dartigues, J., Gagnon, M., Mazaux, J. M., Barberger-Gateau, P., Commenges, D., Letenneur, L., et al. (1992b). Occupation during life and memory performance in nondemented French elderly community residents. Neurology 42, 1697–1701. doi: 10.1212/wnl.42.9.1697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Den Heijer, T., Van Der Lijn, F., Koudstaal, P. J., Hofman, A., Van Der Lugt, A., Krestin, G. P., et al. (2010). A 10-year follow-up of hippocampal volume on magnetic resonance imaging in early dementia and cognitive decline. Brain 133(Pt 4), 1163–1172. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, L., Eaton, W. W., Spira, A. P., Agnew, J., Surkan, P. J., and Mojtabai, R. (2018). Job strain and cognitive change: the Baltimore epidemiologic catchment area follow-up study. Occup. Environ. Med. 75, 856–862. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2018-105213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlers, D. K., Daugherty, A. M., Burzynska, A. Z., Fanning, J., Awick, E. A., Chaddock-Heyman, L., et al. (2017). Regional brain volumes moderate, but do not mediate, the effects of group-based exercise training on reductions in loneliness in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:110. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanning, J., Porter, G., Awick, E. A., Ehlers, D. K., Roberts, S. A., Cooke, G., et al. (2017). Replacing sedentary time with sleep, light, or moderate-to-vigorous physical activity: effects on self-regulation and executive functioning. J. Behav. Med. 40, 332–342. doi: 10.1007/s10865-016-9788-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finkel, D., Andel, R., Gatz, M., and Pedersen, N. L. (2009). The role of occupational complexity in trajectories of cognitive aging before and after retirement. Psychol. Aging 24, 563–573. doi: 10.1037/a0015511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., and Dale, A. M. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195–207. doi: 10.1006/NIMG.1998.0396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fotuhi, M., Do, D., and Jack, C. (2012). Modifiable factors that alter the size of the hippocampus with ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 8, 189–202. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.27

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frisoni, G. B., Rozzini, R., Bianchetti, A., and Trabucchi, M. (1993). Principal lifetime occupation and MMSE score in elderly persons. J. Gerontol. 48, S310–S314. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.6.S310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorbach, T., Pudas, S., Lundquist, A., Orädd, G., Josefsson, M., Salami, A., et al. (2017). Longitudinal association between hippocampus atrophy and episodic-memory decline. Neurobiol. Aging 51, 167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gow, A. J., Avlund, K., and Mortensen, E. L. (2014). Occupational characteristics and cognitive aging in the Glostrup 1914 Cohort. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 69, 228–236. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joëls, M., Pu, Z., Wiegert, O., Oitzl, M. S., and Krugers, H. J. (2006). Learning under stress: how does it work? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaup, A. R., Xia, F., Launer, L. J., Sidney, S., Nasrallah, I., Erus, G., et al. (2018). Occupational cognitive complexity in earlier adulthood is associated with brain structure and cognitive health in midlife: the CARDIA study. Neuropsychology 32, 895–905. doi: 10.1037/neu0000474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, K. M., and Raz, N. (2005). Age, sex and regional brain volumes predict perceptual-motor skill acquisition. Cortex 41, 560–569. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70196-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komp-Leukkunen, K. (2019). Capturing the diversity of working age life-courses: a European perspective on cohorts born before 1945. PLoS One 14:e0212400. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K., and Spiers, H. J. (2006). London taxi drivers and bus drivers: a structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus 16, 1091–1101. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marioni, R. E., Chatfield, M., Brayne, C., and Matthews, F. E., Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study Group (2011). The reliability of assigning individuals to cognitive states using the Mini Mental-State Examination: a population-based prospective cohort study. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 11:127. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-11-127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, C. E., Keadle, S. K., Moore, S. C., Schoeller, D. S., Carroll, R. J., Troiano, R. P., et al. (2018). Measurement of active and sedentary behavior in context of large epidemiologic studies. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 50:266. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2006). Plasticity of the hippocampus: adaptation to chronic stress and allostatic load. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 933, 265–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05830.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S., and Sapolsky, R. M. (1995). Stress and cognitive function. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5, 205–216. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(95)80028-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgeson, F. P., and Humphrey, S. E. (2006). The work design questionnaire (WDQ): developing and validating a comprehensive measure for assessing job design and the nature of work. J. Appl. Psychol. 91, 1321–1339. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.6.1321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutso, A. A., Radzicki, D., Baliki, M. N., Huang, L., Banisadr, G., Centeno, M. V., et al. (2012). Abnormalities in hippocampal functioning with persistent pain. J. Neurosci. 32, 5747–5756. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0587-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagata, K., Nakashima-Kamimura, N., Mikami, T., Ohsawa, I., and Ohta, S. (2009). Consumption of molecular hydrogen prevents the stress-induced impairments in hippocampus-dependent learning tasks during chronic physical restraint in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 501–508. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L. (2017). Neuroimaging in aging: brain maintenance. F1000Research 6:1215. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.11419.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oltmanns, J., Godde, B., Winneke, A. H., Richter, G., Niemann, C., Voelcker-Rehage, C., et al. (2017). Don’t lose your brain at work – The role of recurrent novelty at work in cognitive and brain aging. Front. Psychol. 8:117. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., and Festini, S. B. (2017). Theories of memory and aging: a look at the past and a glimpse of the future. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 72, 82–90. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2004). Localizing age-related individual differences in a hierarchical structure. Intelligence 32, 541–561. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 754–760. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., and Ferrer-Caja, E. (2003). What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables? Psychol. Aging 18, 91–110. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.1.91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandström, A., Rhodin, I. N., Lundberg, M., Olsson, T., and Nyberg, L. (2005). Impaired cognitive performance in patients with chronic burnout syndrome. Biol. Psychol. 69, 271–279. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scahill, R. I., Frost, C., Jenkins, R., Whitwell, J. L., Rossor, M. N., and Fox, N. C. (2003). A longitudinal study of brain volume changes in normal aging using serial registered magnetic resonance imaging. Arch. Neurol. 60, 989–994. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.7.989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, S. B., Graham-Engeland, J. E., Engeland, C. G., Smyth, J. M., Almeida, D. M., Katz, M. J., et al. (2015). The effects of stress on cognitive aging, physiology and emotion (ESCAPE) project. BMC Psychiatry 15:146. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0497-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., and Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 4, 1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sindi, S., Kåreholt, I., Solomon, A., Hooshmand, B., Soininen, H., and Kivipelto, M. (2017). Midlife work-related stress is associated with late-life cognition. J. Neurol. 264, 1996–2002. doi: 10.1007/s00415-017-8571-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smart, E. L., Gow, A. J., and Deary, I. J. (2014). Occupational complexity and lifetime cognitive abilities. Neurology 83, 2285–2291. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spector, P. E., and Jex, S. M. (1998). Development of four self-report measures of job stressors and strain: interpersonal conflict at work scale, organizational constraints scale, quantitative workload inventory, and physical symptoms inventory. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 3, 356–367. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.3.4.356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés-Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., et al. (2018). Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219 [Epub ahead of print]

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Suo, C., Gates, N., Fiatarone Singh, M., Saigal, N., Wilson, G. C., Meiklejohn, J., et al. (2017). Midlife managerial experience is linked to late life hippocampal morphology and function. Brain Imaging Behav. 11, 333–345. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9649-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suo, C., León, I., Brodaty, H., Trollor, J., Wen, W., Sachdev, P., et al. (2012). Supervisory experience at work is linked to low rate of hippocampal atrophy in late life. Neuroimage 63, 1542–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szabo, A. N., McAuley, E., Erickson, K. I., Voss, M., Prakash, R. S., Mailey, E. L., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness, hippocampal volume, and frequency of forgetting in older adults. Neuropsychology 25, 545–553. doi: 10.1037/a0022733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ten Brinke, L. F., Bolandzadeh, N., Nagamatsu, L. S., Hsu, C. L., Davis, J. C., Miran-Khan, K., et al. (2015). Aerobic exercise increases hippocampal volume in older women with probable mild cognitive impairment: a 6-month randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Sports Med. 49, 248–254. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-093184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Petten, C., Plante, E., Davidson, P. S. R., Kuo, T. Y., Bajuscak, L., and Glisky, E. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in older adults: relationships with temporal and prefrontal gray matter volumes and white matter hyperintensities. Neuropsychologia 42, 1313–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Sutterer, M., Weng, T. B., Burzynska, A. Z., Fanning, J., Salerno, E., et al. (2019). Nutritional supplementation boosts aerobic exercise effects on functional brain systems. J. Appl. Physiol. 126, 77–87. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00917.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Washburn, R. A., McAuley, E., Katula, J., Mihalko, S. L., and Boileau, R. A. (1999). The physical activity scale for the elderly (PASE): evidence for validity. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 52, 643–651. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(99)00049-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Washburn, R. A., Smith, K. W., Jette, A. M., and Janney, C. A. (1993). The physical activity scale for the elderly (PASE): development and evaluation. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 46, 153–162. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, R. R., Nixon, A. E., Peterson, Z. B., Thompson, S. V., Olson, R., Martin, S., et al. (2017). The workplace interpersonal conflict scale: an alternative in conflict assessment. Psi Chi J. Psychol. Res. 22, 163–180. doi: 10.24839/2325-7342.jn22.3.163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, hippocampus, occupation, stress, complexity, memory

Citation: Burzynska AZ, Ganster DC, Fanning J, Salerno EA, Gothe NP, Voss MW, McAuley E and Kramer AF (2020) Occupational Physical Stress Is Negatively Associated With Hippocampal Volume and Memory in Older Adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:266. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00266

Received: 16 April 2020; Accepted: 15 June 2020;
Published: 15 July 2020.

Edited by:

Suleyman Kaplan, Ondokuz Mayıs University, Turkey

Reviewed by:

Tony George Jacob, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, India
Kıymet Kübra Yurt, Kastamonu University, Turkey

Copyright © 2020 Burzynska, Ganster, Fanning, Salerno, Gothe, Voss, McAuley and Kramer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Agnieszka Z. Burzynska, agaburza@colostate.edu

Download