Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 November 2020
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.568399

Neural Basis of Increased Cognitive Control of Impulsivity During the Mid-Luteal Phase Relative to the Late Follicular Phase of the Menstrual Cycle

Jin-Ying Zhuang1,2 Jia-Xi Wang2* Qin Lei2 Weidong Zhang2 Mingxia Fan3
  • 1Faculty of Education, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 2School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 3Department of Physics, Shanghai Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China

Hormonal changes across the menstrual cycle have been shown to influence reward-related motivation and impulsive behaviors. Here, with the aim of examining the neural mechanisms underlying cognitive control of impulsivity, we compared event-related monetary delay discounting task behavior and concurrent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) revealed brain activity as well as resting state (rs)-fMRI activity, between women in the mid-luteal phase (LP) and women in the late follicular phase (FP). The behavioral data were analyzed and related to neural activation data. In the delay discounting task, women in the late FP were more responsive to short-term rewards (i.e., showed a greater discount rate) than women in the mid-LP, while also showing greater activity in the dorsal striatum (DS). Discount rate (transformed k) correlated with functional connectivity between the DS and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), consistent with previous findings indicating that DS-dlPFC circuitry may regulate impulsivity. Our rs-fMRI data further showed that the right dlPFC was significantly more active in the mid-LP than in late FP, and this effect was sensitive to absolute and relative estradiol levels during the mid-LP. DS-dlPFC functional connectivity magnitude correlated negatively with psychometric impulsivity scores during the late FP, consistent with our behavioral data and further indicating that relative estradiol levels may play an important role in augmenting cognitive control. These findings provide new insight into the treatment of conditions characterized by hyper-impulsivity, such as obsessive compulsive disorder, Parkinson disease, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In conclusion, our results suggest that cyclical gonadal hormones affect cognitive control of impulsive behavior in a periodic manner, possibility via DS-dlPFC circuitry.

Introduction

It has been posited that observed influences of menstrual cycle phase in women on reward-related impulsivity reflect alterations in brain dopamine function and thus downstream effects of those alterations on dopamine efferent targets in the basal ganglia and frontal cortex (Xiao and Becker, 1994; Jackson et al., 2006). These circuits are critically involved in temporal and reward processing (McClure et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2012); they are organized by gonadal steroid hormones during early development and are modulated by these hormones in adults (Xiao and Becker, 1994; Jackson et al., 2006). When estradiol levels are highest during the late follicular phase (FP), women have been shown to be more responsive to rewarding substances, such as cocaine (Terner and De Wit, 2006), and reward-related activation in the mesolimbic system has been shown to be enhanced in the late FP, compared with that in the mid-luteal phase (LP) when progesterone levels peak (Dreher et al., 2007). Self-reported liking of smoked cocaine is greater during the late FP than the mid-LP (Sofuoglu et al., 1999; Evans and Foltin, 2006). Animal studies have also provided evidence of estrogenic modulation of reward-related impulsivity. For example, female rats exhibit their maximal cocaine self-administration levels shortly after estradiol peaks, and exogenous administration of estradiol enhances the acquisition of cocaine self-administration in ovariectomized female rats (Lynch et al., 2001; Jackson et al., 2006). These findings suggest that the proclivity of female mammals to wait for a higher reward is reduced in the late FP relative to the mid-LP.

In contrast, progesterone may play a role in reducing impulsive behavior by favoring more cognitive control. Progesterone is a female gonadal hormone that is often included in hormonal contraceptives and drugs used to maintain pregnancies (Jones et al., 2005). Exogenously delivered progesterone can alleviate stimulant abuse in animals (Anker et al., 2009; Zlebnik et al., 2014) and may support stimulant use cessation in humans (Evans and Foltin, 2006, 2010; Quinones-Jenab and Jenab, 2010; Fox et al., 2013; Carroll and Lynch, 2016; Swalve et al., 2016). Furthermore, progesterone, or its metabolite allopregnanolone, reduce stress and impulsive behavior as measured by the Stroop test in humans (Milivojevic et al., 2016). Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that increases in progesterone levels from the late FP to the mid-LP result in changes in activity in several prefrontal cortex regions (Dreher et al., 2007; Van Wingen et al., 2008; Ossewaarde et al., 2011; Marečková et al., 2012). Thus, the relatively high progesterone and low estradiol levels during the mid-LP may affect the functioning of brain regions involved in cognitive control in a manner that results in reduced impulsivity.

The main estrogen receptor is highly expressed in the amygdala (Wharton et al., 2012), which sends glutamatergic efferents to the striatum, especially the dorsal striatum (DS), a crucial brain area in the reward system related to impulsivity (Yager et al., 2015). Cummings et al.’s results (2014) suggested that estradiol enhances dopamine release in the DS, but not ventral striatum (VS), in female rats. A recent study showed that women’s responses to drug cues in the DS, specifically in the putamen, were modulated by menstrual phase (Franklin et al., 2019).

A large body of evidence indicates that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) plays an important role in cognitive control (Sheline et al., 2010; Cieslik et al., 2013). Moreover, the quality of communication between the DS and dlPFC may modulate cognitive control of impulsivity. A neural circuit linking the right caudate nucleus head and putamen with the dlPFC has been identified as a cognitive loop (Rotge et al., 2008). Commonly, patients with altered frontal-striatal function—such as in obsessive compulsive disorder (Heuvel et al., 2005), Parkinson disease (Williamsgray et al., 2007), and Huntington’s chorea (Watkins et al., 2000)—exhibit executive functioning deficits. Reduced resting state (rs)-functional connectivity between the right caudate and dlPFC has been associated with cognitive control deficits in internet gaming disorder (Yuan et al., 2016). Remarkably, in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study examining rs-functional connectivity in women smokers, Wetherill et al. (2016) found that, compared to data obtained during the LP, when in the FP of their menstrual cycles women had lower rs-functional connectivity between cognitive control areas (dorsal/subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and medial orbitofrontal cortex) and a reward-related region (i.e., the VS) as well as less cognitive control over their smoking behavior.

Humans have a tendency for delay discounting, a phenomenon wherein the value of a delayed rewards are over-discounted relative to that of a reward that can be obtained immediately or sooner (Frederick et al., 2002; Green and Myerson, 2004). Delay discounting has been conceptualized as an index of impulsivity (Staubitz, Lloyd, and Reed). Theoretically, the more a person discounts the value due to its delay, the more impulsive their choice is considered. Patients with conditions that affect brain dopamine systems, namely Parkinson disease and obsessive compulsive disorder, have been shown to exhibit more pronounced delay discounting than healthy and neurotypical people (Guttman and Seeman, 1985; Volkow et al., 1993; Hesse et al., 2005). This intensified preference for selecting of smaller, sooner rewards over larger, later rewards in these populations has been termed a Now bias (Smith et al., 2014).

The aim of the present study was to investigate the neural mechanisms underlying the modulatory effects of menstrual cycle phase on cognitive control of impulsivity with an event-related monetary delay discounting behavioral task and a rs-fMRI study. We chose to compare behavioral and imaging results between the late FP, when there are high estradiol levels with low progesterone levels, and the mid-LP, when both estradiol and progesterone levels are relatively high (Figure 1). We hypothesized that, compared to women in the mid-LP, women in the late FP would show a greater Now bias in the delay discounting task concomitant with higher activity in the DS. Furthermore, we hypothesized that a brain regions related to cognitive control, including the dlPFC, would be more active during the mid-LP, leading to the behavioral acceptance of later, larger rewards. Finally, we hypothesized that DS-dlPFC functional connectivity may play an important role in regulating impulsivity. To test our hypotheses, choices in the delay discounting task (McClure et al., 2004), performed during fMRI scanning, were compared between women in the late FP and women in the mid-LP (task fMRI study). Because fMRI signal fluctuations at rest contain information about functional network architecture (Fox et al., 2005; Fox and Raichle, 2007; Biswal, 2012), we also compared rs-functional connectivity between these two groups (rs-fMRI study) and correlated the connectivity data to hormone level data.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Transformed discount rate (k) by menstrual phase. The mean transformed k value was significantly greater during the late FP than during the mid-LP (error bars indicate the standard error of the mean).

Task fMRI Study

Methods

Participants

A cohort of 24 healthy, right-handed, female undergraduates (mean age [± standard deviation (SD)], 22.79 ± 1.44 years; range 20–25) were enrolled. The participants were recruited from a larger, common subject pool with certain inclusion/exclusion criteria, only individuals who were heterosexual, reported having a 28- to 30-day menstrual cycle, and did not take any form of hormones in the previous 3 months were included. Because the criteria for these other studies included heterosexuality as an inclusion criteria, the participants in the current study were exclusively heterosexual. However, it is important to note that this criteria was incidental (and otherwise irrelevant) for the research questions addressed in the current study. The participants were asked to come to the laboratory on two separate occasions (late FP and mid-LP) to complete an intertemporal choice task. The tasks were the same the two testing times, but we did not state this explicitly to the participants.

We used the backward counting method to predict each participant’s next menstrual onset, late FP (14–16 days prior to the predicted menstrual onset), and mid-LP (6–8 days prior to the predicted menstrual onset). This method has been successfully used to predict other effects of theoretical interest (Durante et al., 2011; Zhuang and Wang, 2014). If a woman’s next predicted menstrual onset was 8–14 days away, she was scheduled to complete the mid-LP testing first (N = 10); otherwise, she was scheduled to complete the late FP testing first (N = 14).

All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. No participants reported a history of a psychiatric disorder or current use of a psychoactive medication. The protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the local University and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants, and they were compensated 50 RMB per hour.

Behavioral Task and Procedure

Prior to scanning, the subjects were presented with a 9-rung ladder scale. They were instructed to place themselves on the ladder that ranged from 1 (lowest rung) to 9 (highest rung) based on where they stand compared to others in terms of family economic level and social status level, respectively (Adler et al., 1994).

The intertemporal binary choice paradigm (see Supplementary Material) was performed as described previously (McClure et al., 2004). In each pair of choices, the sooner option always had a lower reward value than the delayed option. The two options were separated by either 2 weeks or 1 month, with wait times ranging from the day of the experiment to 6 weeks later. Thus, the sooner option was sometimes available immediately and sometimes available after a delay (see Figure 2). The reward amount ranged from 31.25 to 250.00 RMB (USD equivalent, $5–40; conversion rate 1:6.25). In each experiment, the participant made 48 binary hypothetical choices, and the order of the choices was randomized within and across the participants.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Examples of sooner reward and delayed reward options. During fMRI scanning, in each trial, the participants viewed a choice screen (above), made a self-paced choice response, and subsequently viewed a choice outcome screen (below) for 2 s. The reward options were in Simplified Chinese and read “www.frontiersin.org” (now), 2 www.frontiersin.org (2 weeks later), 4www.frontiersin.org (4 weeks later), 6www.frontiersin.org (6 weeks later).

The behavioral experiment was conducted entirely inside an fMRI scanner. The participants were instructed to make a series of choices between a smaller, sooner reward (r1 available at delay t1) and a later, larger reward (r2 available at delay t2; where r1 < r2 and t1 < t2). Each participant was instructed to indicate her preference as soon as the choice was displayed by pressing one of two buttons that corresponded with the location of the preferred option with her right hand. The smaller, sooner options were always presented on the left. Decisions were self-paced with a maximum allowed reaction time of 15 s. After each choice, a feedback screen was presented for 2 s to indicate the choice outcome (McClure et al., 2004). All responses were submitted well before the 15-s time limit. Each subject completed two 24-trial runs. Each trial was followed by a jitter interval (1500 ms, 50% of trials; 4000 ms, 25%; 6000 ms, 16.7%; and 12,000 ms, 8.3%). Prior to being presented with the choices, the participants were administered two control questions to familiarize themselves with the nature of the task. The entire experiment took ∼ 8 min. The stimuli were presented via the in vivo Esys system for fMRI (Gainesville, FL, United States).

Imaging Data Acquisition

Scanning was conducted on a 3-T Siemens scanner at the fMRI laboratory of East China Normal University in Shanghai. Functional images were acquired with a gradient echo-planar imaging sequence, 2000-ms repetition time (TR), 30-ms echo time (TE), 220-mm field of view (FOV), 3 mm × 3 mm × 3.5 mm voxel size and 32 slices. 112 images per scan were acquired and a total was 224 across the two scans. The first five TRs acquired were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration. Prior to the fMRI scanning, a high-resolution structural image was acquired with a T1-weighted, multiplanar reconstruction sequence (TR = 1900 ms, TE = 3.42 ms, FOV = 256 mm, 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm voxel size, and 192 slices).

Behavioral Data Analysis

We employed a calculation method similar to that used by Kim et al. (2012), in which the behavioral data were fitted assuming a hyperbolic discount function. The discounted value function is given by

V ( r , t ) = r 1 + k t

where r is the reward amount available at delay t, and V is the subjective value of the offer. The discount rate (k) for each subject was estimated by assuming a logistic decision function and maximizing the log-likelihood of the observed choices. Best-fit model parameters were determined in Matlab1 using a simplex search algorithm with 100 random initial parameter values. Subsequently, the k values were transformed logarithmically to permit parametric statistical analyses. For each participants, we calculated an immediate choice ratio (ICR; the number of sooner choices divided by the total number of choices). Because arcsine-square root transformed ICRs correlate well with the hyperbolic discounting rate variable k (Boettiger et al., 2009; Xu et al., 2009), we subjected the ICRs to arcsine-square root transformation.

Logarithmically transformed k values were compared between the late FP and mid-LP conditions with paired samples t-tests. We also conducted a correlation analysis between the transformed k and ICR values. The paired samples t-test was used to test for an order effect on the transformed k across the two menstrual phase conditions.

Imaging Data Analysis

The fMRI data were analyzed in Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 (SPM8, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, United Kingdom). We performed a slice-timing correction and, subsequently, aligned the data to correct for head movement. Images were smoothed with an 8-mm full-width at the half-maximum Gaussian kernel and then normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template and re-sampled at 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm resolution.

General linear modeling (GLM) was conducted in SPM8. For the first level GLM analysis, we used an event-related design to estimate neural responses to events of interest. Potentially confounding variables, such as trial-by-trial head movements and choice outcomes (i.e., motor responses), were included in the GLM as regressors of no interest. As suggested by the as soon as possible model (Kable and Glimcher, 2010), we expected participants to overweight the value of the soonest available reward regardless of whether it was offered immediately or after a delay. We pooled all smaller, sooner rewards (ignoring whether they were available immediately or at a delayed time) chosen by the participants in a pair together to form the sooner reward choice condition. The remaining larger, later rewards chosen by the participants were pooled together as the delayed choice condition. Each condition [sooner reward choice in the late FP (SFP), sooner reward choice in the mid-LP (SLP), delayed reward choice in the late FP (DFP), and delayed reward choice in the mid-LP (DLP)] were modeled as reaction times from the decision onset. In the first-level analysis, simple main effects were computed for each participant for each of the above mentioned conditions by applying a ‘1 0’ contrast, where 1 represents one of these conditions, and 0 represents all other possibilities.

For the second (group) level analysis, we conducted random effect modeling (flexible factorial design) to analyze the first-level individual contrast images. The main effect of menstrual phase was calculated by comparing late FP trials versus mid-LP trials. The main effect of delay discounting was obtained by comparing sooner-reward choice trials versus delayed-reward choice trials. The interaction between menstrual phase and delay discounting was calculated to extract brain regions that showed higher or lower sensitivity to the sooner reward outcome over the delayed reward outcome among women in the late FP versus women in the mid-LP.

All data were initially thresholded at a value of p < 0.001 (uncorrected), and the results were reported at a cluster statistical threshold of a family-wise error (FWE)-corrected p < 0.05. Activations were localized with the anatomy toolbox in SPM8 (Eickhoff et al., 2005) using the MRIcron automated anatomical labeling template (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002).

Region of interest (ROI) analyses

Region of interest analyses were conducted to further refine our hypothesis. ROIs were selected based on our hypotheses and previous studies (Cummings et al., 2014; Franklin et al., 2019). Based on prior relevant work (Pine et al., 2010), the following Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates for bilateral ROIs were defined: putamen, MNI coordinates 30 −3 −12 and −24 12 −9; caudate, MNI coordinates 21 24 −3 and −9 12 9; and dlPFC, MNI coordinates 42 18 27 and −45 33 18. We extracted an average beta value for each ROI in each condition (sooner reward choice and delayed reward choice in each menstrual phase) for each participant by selecting a 6-mm sphere around the coordinates using the MarsBaR ROI toolbox 0.44 in SPM8 (Brett et al., 2002). All beta values were submitted to a 2 (delay discounting: sooner vs. delayed reward choice) × 2 (menstrual phase: FP vs. LP) × 6 (the aforementioned ROIs) mixed measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). We also completed a 2 (delay discounting: sooner vs. delayed reward choice) × 2 (menstrual phase: FP vs. LP) mixed measures ANOVA for each ROI. Mean beta values are reported with SDs.

Functional connectivity analysis

To test our neural circuitry hypothesis, we conducted beta series correlation (BSC) analyses of the functional connectivity between the DS (putamen and caudate) and dlPFC within each condition (SFP, SLP, DFP, and DLP). First, based on the above-defined ROIs, each trial was modeled as a separate event of interest (Rissman et al., 2004) and the beta series associated with each trial type within each ROI were extracted and sorted by study condition. Pair-wise BSC analyses were performed for the DS and the dlPFC. After calculating the correlation between activities in each ROI pair individually for each subject and for each condition across the time series, the correlation values obtained were subjected to Fisher transformation prior to being subjected to ANOVAs designed to detect which correlations varied across delay discounting choice and menstrual phase.

Correlations between DS-dlPFC functional connectivity and discount rate

We conducted correlation analyses between discount rate (transformed k) and functional connectivity (as indexed via BSCs) within each condition to investigate the effect of DS-dlPFC circuitry on impulsivity.

Results

Demographics

A total of 8 participants were excluded from further analysis, including 3 for excessive head motion (>2-mm displacement in the x, y, or z dimension, or >2° angular shift) during scanning, 2 because tracking of their next menstruation revealed that we did not have an accurate menstrual date, and 3 whose behavioral data did not conform to the model from the log-likelihood of observed choices. The 16 remaining participants (mean age 22.44 ± 1.31 years; range, 20–24) included 8 participants who were initially scanned during their late FP, and 8 who were initially scanned during their mid-LP. The mean scores of the 16 participants in the final analyses had mean family economic and social status level scores of 5.81 ± 0.40 and 5.44 ± 0.51, respectively. There was very high agreement among the participants on subjective socioeconomic status [intra-class correlation (1,15) = 0.788], which suggests that behavioral effects observed in the experiment cannot be attributed to a confounding effect of subjective value variations.

Behavior

A paired samples t-test did not show an effect of testing order (i.e., which phase women were in during first test) on transformed k values [t2,14 = −0.97, p = 0.35, d = −0.52]. Meanwhile, a paired samples t-test showed a significant difference in transformed k values between the late FP and mid-LP (t1,15 = −2.14, p = 0.049 < 0.05, d = 0.43), with a significantly greater mean discount rate being observed in the late FP (−1.32 ± 0.49) than in the mid-LP (−1.51 ± 0.46) (Figure 1). Transformed k and ICR values were highly correlated within each menstrual phase (FP: r = 0.94, p < 0.001; LP: r = 0.96, p < 0.001) (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means and correlations of k and ICR values within menstrual phase datasets.

Whole Brain Analysis

We observed main effects of menstrual phase on activity in several visual areas, including greater activation during the mid-LP than during the late FP in the bilateral lingual gyrus, bilateral calcarine gyrus, left middle occipital gyrus, and left inferior occipital gyrus. No other meaningful brain areas were found in the opposite contrast. With respect to delay discounting, brain regions that were preferentially activated by the prospect of a sooner reward choice over a delayed reward choice (sooner > delayed) included the right putamen, the right thalamus, and the right supplementary motor area. In the reverse contrast (delayed > sooner), greater activation was identified in the left postcentral gyrus and left superior parietal lobule. Analysis of interactions between menstrual phase and delay discounting revealed more active regions in the left putamen, bilateral caudate, bilateral visual areas [Brodmann area (BA)17 and 18], left hippocampus, and left insula in the (late FP – mid-LP) - (sooner – delayed) comparison; there was no regions that were significantly more active in the reverse comparison (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain regions whose activity is altered in relation to menstrual phase or delay discounting.

ROI Analyses

A 2 (delay discounting) × 2 (menstrual phase) × 6 (ROI) mixed measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of ROI (F5,90 = 9.49, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.35) and a significant interaction of delay discounting and menstrual phase (F1,90 = 8.97, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.09) (Figure 3; non-significant effect data are reported in the Supplementary Material). Regarding the ROI effect, post hoc analysis indicated that the beta value for the left dlPFC was significantly greater than the values obtained for all other ROIs (ps ≤ 0.005), while the beta value for the right dlPFC was significantly greater than the values obtained for the right putamen (p = 0.003) and right caudate (p = 0.010). Simple effects analysis of the delay discounting × menstrual phase interaction showed that when choosing the sooner rewards, beta values differed significantly between the late FP and mid-LP (F1,90 = 6.61, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.07), with a greater mean beta value being observed in the late FP (0.98 ± 0.17) than in the mid-LP (0.58 ± 0.17). Specifically, for the left putamen, we found a significant main effect of menstrual phase (F1,15 = 5.1, p = 0.040, ηp2 = 0.25), wherein women in the late FP (0.99 ± 1.22) had higher mean beta value than in the mid-LP (0.30 ± 0.77). There was a significant delay discounting × menstrual phase interaction for the right putamen (F1,15 = 5.02, p = 0.040, ηp2 = 0.25). Simple effect analysis showed that the mean beta value was significantly higher when women chose sooner rewards in the late FP (0.02 ± 1.12) than when they did so in the mid-LP (−0.76 ± 0.72; F1,15 = 7.96, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.35). No other effects were significant (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean beta values for ROIs by condition. (A) The ROIs of the left putamen [–24, 12, –9], right putamen [30, –3, –12], left caudate [–9, 12, 9], right caudate [21, 24, –3], left dlPFC [–45, 33, 18], and right dlPFC [42, 18, 27]. (B) Activation of the left putamen was significantly higher during the late FP than during the mid-LP. Moreover, when choosing sooner rewards, the mean beta values of the right putamen and the right caudate were much greater during the late FP than during the mid-LP. A significant interaction was observed for the right dlPFC; simple effects analysis showed a tendency for right dlPFC activation to be higher in the DLP condition than in the DFP, SLP, and SFP conditions. The effect is significant at the 0.05 level.

Similarly, we found a significant delay discounting × menstrual phase interaction for the right caudate (F1,15 = 6.86, p = 0.020, ηp2 = 0.31). Simple effect analysis showed that the mean beta value was significantly higher when women chose the sooner rewards in the late FP (0.27 ± 0.77) than when they did so in the mid-LP (−0.19 ± 0.80; F1,15 = 8.65, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.37). We did not find any significant effects for the left caudate (Figure 3).

For the right dlPFC, there was a significant delay discounting × menstrual phase interaction (F1,15 = 5.90, p = 0.030, ηp2 = 0.28), but no significant effects were observed in the simple effects analysis. However, we did observe a tendency showing that the mean activation level in the DLP condition (1.67 ± 2.86) was higher than in the other conditions (SLP, 1.02 ± 2.28; SFP, 1.41 ± 1.91; DFP, 1.38 ± 2.34). No significant main effects (delay discounting, F1,15 = 1.32, p = 0.270, ηp2 = 0.81; menstrual phase F1,15 = 0.53, p = 0.480, ηp2 = 0.34) and no significant delay discounting × menstrual phase interaction (F1,15 = 0.33, p = 0.570, ηp2 = 0.02) were observed for the left dlPFC (Figure 3).

The results above indicated that the right DS was more active during the late FP than during the mid-LP when sooner rewards were chosen (i.e., SFP > SLP for right DS). In contrast, the dlPFC showed a tendency to be more active during the mid-LP than during the late FP when delayed rewards were chosen (i.e., DLP > DFP for the dlPFC).

Functional Connectivity Across Menstrual Phases

An ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of menstrual phase on the functional connectivity between the left putamen and left dlPFC (F1,15 = 7.40, p = 0.020, ηp2 = 0.33). The magnitude of this connectivity was significantly stronger in the mid-LP (0.22 ± 0.32) than in the late FP (0.00 ± 0.42). There were no other significant ROI functional connectivity effects.

Correlations Between DS-dlPFC Functional Connectivity and Discount Rate Across the Menstrual Phases

In the SFP condition, functional connectivity between the right caudate and the bilateral dlPFC correlated inversely with discount rate (right caudate-right dlPFC: r = −0.64, p = 0.010; right caudate-left dlPFC: r = −0.64, p = 0.010) (Figure 4). Conversely, in the SLP condition, functional connectivity between the right dlPFC and the right putamen correlated directly with discount rate (r = 0.50, p = 0.050).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlations between DS-dlPFC functional connectivity and discount rate across menstrual phases. (A) ROIs: right putamen [30, –3, –12], right caudate [21, 24, –3], right dlPFC [42, 18, 27] and left dlPFC [–45, 33, 18]. (B) Discount rate correlated positively with right putamen-right dlPFC functional connectivity in the SLP condition. Discount rate correlated negatively with right caudate-bilateral dlPFC functional connectivity in the SFP condition.

Discussion

Using the delay discounting task, which assesses intertemporal choice similar to the task used by McClure et al. (2004), we demonstrated that delay discounting behavior in women was affected by menstrual phase, such that the discount rate was significantly greater in the late FP than in the mid-LP, and this augmented discount rate was associated with enhanced activity in the DS. Specifically, greater activation was observed in the left putamen during the late FP than during the mid-LP. Moreover, women showed greater activation in the right DS (putamen and caudate) when choosing sooner rewards in the late FP than when do so in the mid-LP (i.e., SFP > SLP), indicating that the right DS is more responsive to immediate rewards during the late FP than during the mid-LP. These results are consistent with the prior findings showing that a heightened drug cue-responsivity in women is associated with enhanced activity in the DS, especially in the putamen (Cummings et al., 2014; Franklin et al., 2019). Indeed, behavioral impulsivity has been linked to dopamine levels in the putamen. For example, in adult men, higher trait impulsivity as measured by Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) scores correlated negatively with dopamine transporter availability in the putamen (Costa et al., 2013). Additionally, a lower dopamine synthesis capacity in the putamen, indexed by 6-[18 F]fluoro-L-m-tyrosine signal, was shown to be predictive of an elevated Now bias and a reduced willingness to accept low-interest rate delayed rewards (Smith et al., 2016).

The VS/nucleus accumbens have been strongly implicated in the seeking the sooner rewards in intertemporal choice paradigms (McClure et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2012), with nucleus accumbens activation in particular being related to reward magnitude sensitivity (Ballard and Knutson, 2009). Our not finding an effect of menstrual phase on these areas suggests that VS reward sensitivity may not be modulated by menstrual phase. Indeed, intertemporal choice in humans has been shown to vary with region-specific dopamine processing, with regionally distinct associations with sensitivity to delay (the putamen) and reward magnitude (the VS) (Ballard and Knutson, 2009; Smith et al., 2016). Studies have linked dopamine availability in the putamen with time perception, a cognitive process thought to contribute to discounting of delayed rewards (Wittmann and Paulus, 2008; Takahashi, 2011; Smith et al., 2016). For example, Parkinson disease patients, who have deficits in putamen dopamine signaling, show selective impairments in time duration comparison (Dormal et al., 2012). A pharmaco-fMRI study related the relationship between dopamine depletion and time perception specifically to activity in the putamen (Coull et al., 2012). Our finding the putamen was more active in the SFP condition than in the SLP condition suggests that naturally cycled ovarian steroid hormones may modulate an aspect of time perception in intertemporal choice, perhaps through steroid hormone modulation of dopamine levels. However, proving such a modulatory effect would require dopamine level data. Future research should compare dopamine levels across menstrual phases to help elucidate the influence of steroid hormones on intertemporal choices.

Although we observed a significant menstrual phase × delay discounting interaction influence on right dlPFC activity, simple effect analysis revealed only a trend toward higher activation in the DLP condition relative the other conditions. Post hoc analysis of ROIs indicated that beta values were significantly higher for the dlPFC than for the DS. Because the striatum and prefrontal cortex are intermodulated via frontostriatal networks (Yuan et al., 2016), impulsivity variance across menstrual phases may be determined, at least in part, by relative activity levels between the DS and dlPFC. Although the activity in the dlPFC was stable across menstrual phases, heightened DS activity during sooner reward selection in women late FP may lead to relatively less potent top-down cognitive control. Conversely, lesser DS activity during the mid-LP may enable more potent cognitive control. Thus, our results suggest that the activities of the DS and dlPFC relative to each other may determine the level of cognitive control over impulsivity, such that there is stronger cognitive control during the mid-LP than during the late FP.

The present results also support the notion that DS-dlPFC circuitry is involved in the regulation of impulsivity. Specifically, when participants chose sooner rewards during the late FP, right caudate-dlPFC functional connectivity correlated negatively with discount rate, indicating a linear relationship between the reduced right caudate-dlPFC connectivity and increased impulsivity (indexed by discount rate) during the late FP, consistent with rs-functional connectivity findings in participants with internet gaming disorder (Yuan et al., 2016) and cigarette-dependent women (Wetherill et al., 2016). Conversely, in the SLP condition, right putamen-right dlPFC functional connectivity correlated directly with discount rate. The hierarchical reinforcement learning theory (Holroyd and Yeung, 2012) has suggested the dlPFC and motor structures in the DS (mainly the putamen) execute options chosen by other brain regions. If so, the aforementioned correlation may reflect an effect of execution function.

Anatomically, the DS includes the dorsal regions of the putamen and caudate nucleus (Porter et al., 2014). Although these two subregions both receive nigrostriatal dopaminergic projections (Fallon and Moore, 1978) and are involved in motivated behavior via the prefrontal cortex, functionally, the caudate is important for reward-related cognition whereas the putamen is more involved in motor behaviors (Baskerville and Douglas, 2010). Thus, the opposing directionality found in our results may reflect different functions of the DS-dlPFC circuitry.

Our results suggest that DS-dlPFC functional connectivity may modulate impulsivity in intertemporal choices, with opposing directionality and differential involvement of brain regions depending upon menstrual phase.

This study had notable limitations. First, although we hypothesized that, brain regions related to cognitive control would be more active during the mid LP, we did not find a significant main effect of menstrual phase on the activation of brain regions related to cognitive control. This negative finding could be related to inherent characteristics of cognitive involvement in the task (McClure et al., 2004), which could confound the effect of menstrual phase. Second, menstrual phase was not confirmed by any biological tests such as hormonal assays, potentially reducing phase designation accuracy. Previously studies have found that estimates based on the backward counting method, as used here, are correct 80–90% of the time (Carroll, 2018). Indeed, several participants were excluded after follow-up revealed inaccurate estimates. Third, the delay discounting task is hypothetical. Although previous studies have shown no significant differences behaviorally or neurologically between hypothetical and real reward outcomes (Johnson and Bickel, 2002; Madden et al., 2004; Lagorio and Madden, 2005; Bickel et al., 2009; Kim et al., 2012), it is possible that an extraordinarily impulsive person might much more easily forego a tantalizing immediate reward for a delayed larger reward in a hypothetical situation than in a real situation (Reynolds et al., 2006; Rosati et al., 2007; Jimura et al., 2009; Smits et al., 2013; Carroll, 2018). Fourth, the sample size of the present study was small, which weakens the strength of our results. Finally, that the smaller, sooner options were always presented on the left during scanning would influence our results on hemispheric asymmetry.

Use of the bilateral dlPFC as a seed can be a valuable tool for exploring the function of cognitive control networks in rs-fMRI studies (Cieslik et al., 2013; Hwang et al., 2015; Tao et al., 2017). Thus, to further probe whether there is increased cognitive control functioning during the mid-LP, relative to the late FP, and the involvement of DS-dlPFC circuitry in impulsivity regulation, we conducted a rs-fMRI experiment in the following accompanying study with a inclusion of hormonal assay confirmed menstrual phases.

Hormone and rs-fMRI Study

Methods

Participants

A cohort of 53 healthy, right-handed, female undergraduate participants (mean age ± SD, 22.77 ± 2.35 years; range 19–28) were recruited from a larger, common subject pool with certain inclusion criteria, namely: heterosexual sexual orientation; 28- to 30-day menstrual cycle; and no use of any form of hormones in the previous 3 months. The reason that the participants in the current study were exclusively heterosexual was the same as in the task fMRI study above. Therefore, the heterosexuality as an inclusion criteria was incidental (and otherwise irrelevant) for the research questions addressed in the current study. The study cohort included 28 women in their late FP (mean age, 22.54 ± 2.18 years) and 25 women in their mid-LP (mean age, 23.04 ± 2.54 years). Each woman was subjected to rs-fMRI. Late FP and mid-LP were considered to be 14–16 days and 6–8 days prior to the next predicted menstrual onset, respectively, determined as in Study 1. The testing order was randomized across participants and phases.

All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. No participants reported a history of a psychiatric disorder or current use of psychoactive medications. The protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the local university, and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants, and they were compensated 50 RMB per hour.

Hormone Assays

A saliva sample was obtained from each participant immediately before scanning. To control for circadian influences on hormone levels, all experimental sessions were performed between 12:00 pm and 8:00 pm. Each participant drooled ∼2 mL of saliva passively into a collection tube, and each saliva sample was preserved in a refrigerator (−20°C). All samples were processed for estradiol and progesterone levels with DRG International ELISA kits and the ELISA results were measured with a Thermo Devices Multiskan MK3 by ThermoFisher Scientific Shanghai Company. One-way ANOVAs were conducted on each hormone separately to verify cycle phases with sample collection time as the covariate.

BIS-11

After finishing the saliva sample collection, participants completed the BIS-11 (Patton et al., 1995). The BIS-11 is a self-report questionnaire containing 30 items divided into three subscales: (i) attentional impulsiveness (e.g., “I am a careful thinker”); (ii) motor impulsiveness (e.g., “I do things without thinking”); and (iii) non-planning impulsiveness (e.g., “I am more interested in the present than the future”). Each item was scored from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree), with higher scores indicating higher levels of impulsivity.

Image Acquisition

Previously, rs-functional connectivity has been used to analyze neural circuitry dynamics in normal populations (Choi et al., 2012) and in pathological states (Fox and Greicius, 2010). The rs-fMRI scanning was conducted in a 3-T Siemens scanner at our institution’s fMRI facility. Functional images were acquired with a gradient echo-planar imaging sequence, 2000-ms TR, 30-ms TE, 384-mm FOV, 3 mm × 3 mm × 3.5 mm voxel size, and 33 slices. The images associated with the first ten repetitions were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration. Participants were instructed to relax with their eyes open during scanning, which lasted about 8 min. Prior to fMRI scanning, a high-resolution structural image was acquired with a T1-weighted, multiplanar reconstruction sequence (TR = 2530 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, FOV = 256 mm, 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm voxel size, and 192 slices).

Preprocessing

Preprocessing was performed in advanced DPARSF module V3.2 software. After correcting all volume slices for varying signal acquisition times, the images in each participant’s series were realigned. Individual structural images were then co-registered to the mean functional image. The transformed structural images were segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (Ashburner et al., 2005). Friston’s 24-parameter model was utilized to regress out head motion signal artifacts from the realigned data. White matter and cerebrospinal fluid signals were regressed out to reduce respiratory and cardiac effects. Spatial smoothing (4-mm full-width at half-maximum kernel) were applied to the functional images.

Independent Component Analysis (ICA)

GIFT (Group ICA of fMRI Toolbox) (Calhoun et al., 2001) was used to conduct group-level ICA. In the pre-processing step, datasets from each individual were mean corrected by subtracting the image mean per time point. Thereafter, for each participant, dataset dimensionality was reduced with principal component analysis using the default setting. Then, the data were group concatenated and subjected to two further principal component analysis data reduction steps. Next, the infomax algorithm was used to estimate 53 independent components from the reduced data. Finally, individual spatial maps were back-constructed from group-level component estimates with group ICA. The time courses and values of each map were scaled to represent percent signal change. No temporal filtering was applied to the data in GIFT.

Networks of interest were identified from the 53 components by spatial sorting. The executive control network (ECN) was identified by spatial sorting and statistical comparison to the resting state network 2 (RSN2) described by Mantini et al. (2007), which is strongly associated with goal-directed stimulus-response selection. RSN2 is comprised of areas within the bilateral intraparietal sulcus and at the intersections of the precentral and superior frontal sulcus, ventral precentral cortex, and middle frontal gyrus regions (Mantini et al., 2007). An RSN2 mask was constructed in WFU_PickAtlas 3.0 (Berry et al., 2015). Correlations between ECN components and RSN2 components were calculated and reported with the standard of r ≥ 0.30 (Weis et al., 2017).

An independent sample t-test was carried out on the group level to estimate the effect of menstrual phase (late FP vs. mid-LP) on the beta weights of each identified component of the ECN in SPM8. Initially, the data were thresholded at p < 0.001 (uncorrected); the results are reported at a cluster statistical threshold of p < 0.05 (FWE-corrected). Activations were localized based on the MRIcron automated anatomical labeling template (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002).

Correlations Between Hormone Levels and Activity in Brain Regions

After preprocessing, the amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations of the BOLD signal (ALFF), which is thought to be related to regional spontaneous neural activity, was used to identify differences in regional resting cerebral function between menstrual phases (Cordes et al., 2001). After bandpass filtering (0.01–0.08 Hz), white matter and cerebrospinal fluid signals were removed. Following linear detrending, voxel-wise time series were transformed to the frequency domain by fast Fourier transformation to obtain power spectra. The ALFF measure at each voxel represents the square root of the power across a low-frequency range. The ALFF of each voxel was z-transformed for each subject to standardize the data to allow inter-subject comparisons. Multiple regressions of estradiol, progesterone, and relative estradiol [calculated as (estradiol - progesterone)/progesterone], and relative progesterone [calculated as (progesterone - estradiol)/estradiol] levels on z-transformed ALFF data were conducted for each menstrual phase to verify hormone-correlated brain activity.

Correlations Between Behavioral Impulsivity and DS-dlPFC Functional Connectivity

The relationship between the impulsivity and DS-dlPFC functional connectivity was investigated by correlation analyses. We defined ROIs (bilateral putamen/caudate and dlPFC) and analyzed the functional connectivity between these ROIs from each participant’s preprocessed data using the coordinates and methods described for the task fMRI study above. Regression analyses were conducted on DS-dlPFC functional connectivity data relative to each BIS-11 subscale score for each menstrual phase.

Results

Demographics

Two participants were excluded for excessive movement during scanning (≥2-mm maximum displacement in the x, y, or z dimension; or ≥2° angular motion). Two participants were excluded due to incorrect menstrual date estimation discovered upon tracking the next menstrual cycle. Of the 49 remaining participants (mean age, 22.86 ± 2.29 years; range, 19–28), 25 were scanned during their late FP (mean age, 22.52 years ± 2.38), and 24 were scanned during their mid-LP (mean age, 23.21 ± 2.19). Age did not differ significantly between the two menstrual phase groups (t2,47 = 1.05, p = 0.30, d = 0.31).

Hormone Assays

Hormone concentrations for each menstrual phase group are reported in Table 3. A one-way ANOVA with collection time as a covariate confirmed significantly higher progesterone levels in the mid LP group than in the late FP group (F1,46 = 4.16, p = 0.047, ηp2 = 0.08). Meanwhile, estradiol levels were similar between these two groups (t2,49 = 0.17, p = 0.68, ηp2 = 0.004).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean (± SD) estradiol and progesterone levels.

ECN

An independent sample t-test revealed a significant difference between the late FP and the mid-LP groups for component 50 activity. Higher activity was observed in the right dlPFC (superior frontal gyrus, BA 8, p = 0.050, FWE-corrected, k = 20) during the mid-LP than during the late FP (Table 4 and Figure 5). No other significant differenced were found between the menstrual phases for the activities of any other ECN components.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Brain regions whose activity is altered in relation to menstrual phase in the ECN.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Differences in the ECN across menstrual phases. The right dlPFC was significantly more activated during the mid-LP than during the late FP.

Correlations Between Hormone Levels and Brain Region Activity in Whole-Brain Analysis

During the late FP, no significant correlations were found between absolute levels of estradiol or progesterone and the observed ALFF in brain regions. There was a significant positive correlation between the relative progesterone level and the ALFF in areas within the right hippocampus, thalamus, precuneus, and left angular gyrus. During the mid-LP, there was a significant positive correlation between absolute estradiol level and ALFF of brain areas within the bilateral dlPFC and superior medial prefrontal cortex. Relative estradiol level correlated with the ALFF of brain regions in the right dlPFC and left postcentral gyrus as well. Relative progesterone level correlated with the ALFF of brain areas within the right superior temporal and middle temporal cortices (Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlations between hormone levels and brain activity.

ROI Based rs-Functional Connectivity Correlations With BIS-11

A significant negative correlation was found between BIS-11 attentional impulsivity subscale scores and rs-functional connectivity between the right caudate and right dlPFC (r = −0.47, p = 0.020) during the late FP (Figure 6). No significant correlations were found during the mid-LP.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Negative correlation between attentional impulsivity and DS-dlPFC functional connectivity in the late FP. (A) Rs-functional connectivity between the right caudate [21, 24, –3] and right dlPFC [42, 18, 27]. (B) Negative correlation of attentional impulsivity with right caudate-right dlPFC rs-functional connectivity in the late FP; no such effect was observed in the mid-LP.

Discussion

Hormone assays confirmed the menstrual phases (late FP, mid-LP) predicted by the backward counting method. In the rs-fMRI study, the right dlPFC (superior frontal gyrus) was significantly more active during the mid-LP than during the late FP, consistent with our hypothesis of there being greater cognitive control functioning in the mid-LP than in the late FP.

The magnitude of right caudate-right dlPFC rs-functional connectivity correlated negatively with BIS-11 attentional impulsivity subscale scores during the late FP, consistent with the above reported findings in our delay discounting task-based experiment in study 1. No positive correlations between rs-functional connectivity and impulsivity were observed.

Our finding of positive correlations of dlPFC activity with absolute or relative estradiol levels during the mid-LP suggests that augmented cognitive control function during the mid-LP may be accounted for by the relative levels of estradiol to progesterone, wherein both hormones are elevated. Previous work has shown that a change in the Now bias in intertemporal choices correlates inversely with changes in estradiol levels, decreasing from the low estradiol period of the menstrual phase (estradiol nadir is during menstruation) to the late FP (estradiol levels increase gradually in the early FP then increase rapidly until peaking in the late FP) (Smith et al., 2014). The present results further suggest that it may be the relative level of estradiol that modulates cognitive control of impulsivity during the mid-LP. Consistent with this possibility, the induction of several adaptive behaviors associated with gonadal hormones in intact animals have been shown to require concurrent changes in circulating levels of estradiol and progesterone (Tennent et al., 1980). It is possible that the lack of significant correlations between estradiol levels and activities of brain regions during the late FP in this study were due to there being a quite narrow range in hormone level variabilities (Smith et al., 2014).

On the other hand, our correlation analysis indicated that brain regions related to social cognition, especially in relation to theory of mind (Gallagher and Frith, 2003), are particularly sensitive to the relative levels of progesterone during the mid-LP. Higher progesterone levels in humans have been reported to be associated with greater motivation for affiliation (Schultheiss et al., 2004); whereas, cooperative tendencies have been shown to correlate negatively with estimated estradiol levels (Anderl et al., 2015). From an evolutionary perspective, it is noteworthy that the mid-LP is the phase when women’s progesterone levels are highest and women’s reproductive systems are preparing for possible pregnancy (Maner and Miller, 2014). Thus, the activation of brain regions related to social cognition and theory of mind may provide advantages for pregnant women.

General Discussion

In the presently reported task-based fMRI and rs-fMRI studies, we demonstrated that circulating gonadal steroid hormones in women affected cognitive control of impulsivity, such that women had greater cognitive control on impulsivity during the mid-LP than during the late FP. Neurophysiologically, the DS and dlPFC are the main brain regions involved in cognitive control of impulsivity via the DS-dlPFC neural circuit. Specifically, right dlPFC activity was significantly stronger relative to the activity of DS during the mid-LP than during the late FP, and right dlPFC activity was sensitive to relative estradiol levels during the mid-LP. This cognitive advantage during the mid-LP may have evolutionary roots. As mentioned above, from an evolutionary perspective, the immediate goal of women in their mid-LP would be protection of their potential baby. The present finding of relative progesterone levels correlating positively with brain regions involved in social cognition provides direct evidence for this proposed goal. Thus, according to hierarchical reinforcement learning theory (Holroyd and Yeung, 2012), women keeping a higher level of cognitive control function would be adaptive for facilitating the realization of these goals by fulfilling a variety of women’s specific social goals during their mid-LP.

On the other hand, the DS was significantly more responsive to reward stimuli in the late FP, the phase associated with weak cognitive control, than it was during the mid LP. This change in neural responsiveness could also serve the evolutionary goal of mating. Evidence has shown that menstrual phase influences women’s cognitive control in relation to male faces but not in relation to female faces in a manner that appears to reflect their potential fertility during the late FP (Roberts et al., 2009). Indeed, a number of studies have shown increased impulsive behavior in relation to mating motivation in the late FP relative to the mid-LP. For example, women have been shown to exhibit enhanced impulsivity favoring the selection of sexier clothing and accessories near ovulation (Durante et al., 2008, 2011; Zhuang and Wang, 2014), especially under the priming condition of mating motivation (Zhuang and Wang, 2014). Also, women have been found to be more mobile and socially active in contexts related to mating motivation (Fessler and Navarrete, 2003; Miller et al., 2007).

Limitations, Implications, and Future Researches

The current findings indicate that ovarian hormones impact impulsive behaviors and that relative estradiol and progesterone levels may modulate the relationship between cognitive control and impulsivity. These results and their underlying neural basis have important implications for understanding the neural mechanisms that mediate impulsive control in a variety of contexts, including drug abuse (Cummings et al., 2014), postpartum psychosis, which is associated with drastic hormonal changes and a loss of inhibition (Ahokas et al., 2000), and borderline personality disorder, which has been associated with a variance in aggressive and impulsive behaviors over different menstrual phases (Dougherty et al., 1999). Our findings may provide novel directions for the treatment of these disorders and others, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, Parkinson disease, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. For example, manipulating the hormonal milieu may be helpful for alleviating these disorders and diseases. Treatment during the mid-LP may be particularly effective and important for attenuating hyperimpulsivity.

The present findings should be considered in the context of two notable limitations. Firstly, the sample size in the task-based fMRI study was relatively small, which may have weakened the power of and level of significance in our results. Secondly, the hormone and rs-fMRI study did not include direct assessments of dopamine levels across the menstrual phases, which limits our ability to make conclusions regarding a direct relationship between dopaminergic pathway activity and the observed difference in impulsive behavior between menstrual phases. Determination of dopamine levels at different points within the menstrual cycle in future research would help to provide a more detailed understanding of the influence of female gonadal hormones on impulsive behaviors.

In conclusion, natural menstrual cycle phase affected cognitive control of impulsivity. Women were more apt to postpone receiving rewards during the mid-LP than during the late FP. Our fMRI findings support the possibility that menstrual phase-associated behavioral changes may be consequent to hormonally induced alterations in the dlPFC and DS and the communication between these brain regions.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Committee on Human Research Protection, East China Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

J-YZ conceived and designed the experiments. J-XW, QL, and MF performed the experiments. J-XW analyzed the data. J-YZ wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Peak Discipline Construction Project of Education at East China Normal University, the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (41300-20101-222128) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (71971084).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2020.568399/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.mathworks.com/

References

Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R. L., et al. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: the challenge of the gradient. Am. Psychol. 49, 15–24. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.49.1.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahokas, A., Aito, M., and Rimón, R. (2000). Positive treatment effect of estradiol in postpartum psychosis: a pilot study. J. Clin. Psychiatry 61, 166–169. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v61n0303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderl, C., Hahn, T., Notebaert, K., Klotz, C., Rutter, B., and Windmann, S. (2015). Cooperative preferences fluctuate across the menstrual cycle. Judgm. nt Decis. Mak. 10, 400–406.

Google Scholar

Anker, J. J., Holtz, N. A., Zlebnik, N., and Carroll, M. E. (2009). Effects of allopregnanolone on the reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in male and female rats. Psychopharmacology 203, 63–72. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1371-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, J., Friston, K. J., Price, C. J., and Mechelli, A. (2005). Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: methods and applications. Curr. Med. Imaging Rev. 1, 105–113. doi: 10.2174/1573405054038726

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballard, K., and Knutson, B. (2009). Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. Neuroimage 45, 143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baskerville, T. A., and Douglas, A. J. (2010). Dopamine and oxytocin interactions underlying behaviors: potential contributions to behavioral disorders. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 16, e92–e123. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00154.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, A. S., Blakely, R. D., Sarter, M., and Lustig, C. (2015). Cholinergic capacity mediates prefrontal engagement during challenges to attention: evidence from imaging genetics. Neuroimage 108, 386–395. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickel, W. K., Pitcock, J. A., Yi, R., and Angtuaco, E. J. C. (2009). Congruence of bold response across intertemporal choice conditions: fictive and real money gains and losses. J. Neurosci. 29, 8839–8846. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5319-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B. B. (2012). Resting state fMRI: a personal history. Neuroimage 62, 938–944. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boettiger, C. A., Kelley, E. A., Mitchell, J. M., D’esposito, M., and Fields, H. L. (2009). Now or Later? An fMRI study of the effects of endogenous opioid blockade on a decision-making network. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 93, 291–299. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brett, M., Anton, J. L., Valabregue, R., and Poline, J. B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using the MarsBar toolbox. Neuroimage 16, 210–217.

Google Scholar

Calhoun, V. D., Adali, T., Pearlson, G. D., and Pekar, J. J. (2001). A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 14, 140–151. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carroll, H. A. (2018). Dispositional Factors that Predict Alcohol Consumption in Young Adult Women [D]. Seattle, WA: University of Washington.

Google Scholar

Carroll, M. E., and Lynch, W. J. (2016). How to study sex differences in addiction using animal models. Addict. Biol. 21, 1007–1029. doi: 10.1111/adb.12400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, E. Y., Yeo, B. T. T., and Buckner, R. L. (2012). The organization of the human striatum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 2242–2263. doi: 10.1152/jn.00270.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cieslik, E. C., Zilles, K., Caspers, S., Roski, C., Kellermann, T. S., Jakobs, O., et al. (2013). Is there “one” DLPFC in cognitive action control? Evidence for heterogeneity from co-activation-based parcellation. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2677–2689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordes, D., Haughton, V. M., Arfanakis, K., Carew, J. D., Turski, P. A., Moritz, C. H., et al. (2001). Frequencies contributing to functional connectivity in the cerebral cortex in “resting-state” data. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 22, 1326–1333.

Google Scholar

Costa, A., la Fougère, C., Pogarell, O., Möller, H.-J., Riedel, M., and Ettinger, U. (2013). Impulsivity is related to striatal dopamine transporter availability in healthy males. Psychiatry Res. 211, 251–256. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coull, J. T., Hwang, H. J., Leyton, M., and Dagher, A. (2012). Dopamine precursor depletion impairs timing in healthy volunteers by attenuating activity in putamen and supplementary motor area. J. Neurosci. 32, 16704–16715. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1258-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummings, J. A., Jagannathan, L., Jackson, L. R., and Becker, J. B. (2014). Sex differences in the effects of estradiol in the nucleus accumbens and striatum on the response to cocaine: neurochemistry and behavior. Drug Alcohol Depend. 135, 22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dormal, V., Grade, S., Mormont, E., and Pesenti, M. (2012). Dissociation between numerosity and duration processing in aging and early Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 50, 2365–2370. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dougherty, D. M., Bjork, J. M., Huckabee, H. C., Moeller, F. G., and Swann, A. C. (1999). Laboratory measures of aggression and impulsivity in women with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 85, 315–326. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(99)00011-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreher, J.-C., Schmidt, P. J., Kohn, P., Furman, D., Rubinow, D., and Berman, K. F. (2007). Menstrual cycle phase modulates reward-related neural function in women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 2465–2470. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605569104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Hill, S. E., Perilloux, C., and Li, N. P. (2011). Ovulation, female competition, and product choice: hormonal influences on consumer behavior. J. Consum. Res. 37, 921–934. doi: 10.1086/656575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durante, K. M., Li, N. P., and Haselton, M. G. (2008). Changes in women’s choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle: naturalistic and laboratory task-based evidence. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1451–1460. doi: 10.1177/0146167208323103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., Amunts, K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25, 1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, S. M., and Foltin, R. W. (2006). Exogenous progesterone attenuates the subjective effects of smoked cocaine in women, but not in men. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 659–674. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, S. M., and Foltin, R. W. (2010). Does the response to cocaine differ as a function of sex or hormonal status in human and non-human primates? Horm. Behav. 58, 13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallon, J. H., and Moore, R. Y. (1978). Catecholamine innervation of the basal forebrain IV. Topography of the dopamine projection to the basal forebrain and neostriatum. J. Comp. Neurol. 180, 545–579. doi: 10.1002/cne.901800310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fessler, D. M., and Navarrete, C. D. (2003). Domain-specific variation in disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 406–417. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(03)00054-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, H. C., Sofuoglu, M., Morgan, P. T., Tuit, K. L., and Sinha, R. (2013). The effects of exogenous progesterone on drug craving and stress arousal in cocaine dependence: impact of gender and cue type. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1532–1544. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., and Greicius, M. (2010). Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:19. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Essen, D. C. V., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, T. R., Jagannathan, K., Ketcherside, A., Spilka, N., and Wetherill, R. R. (2019). Menstrual cycle phase modulates responses to smoking cues in the putamen: preliminary evidence for a novel target. Drug Alcohol Depend. 198, 100–104. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.01.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., and O’donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: a critical review. J. Econ. Liter. 40, 351–401. doi: 10.1257/jel.40.2.351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, H. L., and Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 77–83. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00025-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, L., and Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychol. Bull. 130, 769–792. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guttman, M., and Seeman, P. (1985). L-DOPA reverses the elevated density of D 2 dopamine receptors in Parkinson’s diseased striatum. J. Neural Trans. 64, 93–103. doi: 10.1007/bf01245971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesse, S., Müller, U., Lincke, T., Barthel, H., Villmann, T., Angermeyer, M. C., et al. (2005). Serotonin and dopamine transporter imaging in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 140, 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heuvel, O. A. V. D., Veltman, D. J., Groenewegen, H. J., Cath, D. C., and Dyck, R. V. (2005). Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 301–309. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holroyd, C. B., and Yeung, N. (2012). Motivation of extended behaviors by anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 122–128. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, J. W., Egorova, N., Yang, X. Q., Zhang, W. Y., Chen, J., Yang, X. Y., et al. (2015). Subthreshold depression is associated with impaired resting-state functional connectivity of the cognitive control network. Transl. Psychiatry 5:e683. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, L. R., Robinson, T. E., and Becker, J. B. (2006). Sex differences and hormonal influences on acquisition of cocaine self-administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 129–138. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jimura, K., Myerson, J., Hilgard, J., Braver, T. S., and Green, L. (2009). Are people really more patient than other animals? evidence from human discounting of real liquid rewards. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 1071–1075. doi: 10.3758/pbr.16.6.1071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. W., and Bickel, W. K. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 77, 129–146. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.77-129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B., Little, A., Boothroyd, L., DeBruine, L., Feinberg, D., Smith, M., et al. (2005). Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high. Horm. Behav. 48, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kable, J. W., and Glimcher, P. W. (2010). An “as soon as possible” effect in human intertemporal decision making: behavioral evidence and neural mechanisms. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 2513–2531. doi: 10.1152/jn.00177.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, B., Sung, Y. S., and McClure, S. M. (2012). The neural basis of cultural differences in delay discounting. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 650–656. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lagorio, C. H., and Madden, G. J. (2005). Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards: III. Steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards. Behav. Process. 69, 173–187. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2005.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch, W. J., Roth, M. E., Mickelberg, J. L., and Carroll, M. E. (2001). Role of estrogen in the acquisition of intravenously self-administered cocaine in female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 68, 641–646. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00455-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madden, G. J., Raiff, B. R., Lagorio, C. H., Begotka, A. M., Mueller, A. M., Hehli, D. J., et al. (2004). Delay discounting of potentially real and hypothetical rewards: II. Betweenand within-subject comparisons. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 12, 251–261. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.12.4.251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maner, J. K., and Miller, S. L. (2014). Hormones and social monitoring: menstrual cycle shifts in progesterone underlie women’s sensitivity to social information. Evol. Hum. Behav. 35, 9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantini, D., Perrucci, M. G., Gratta, C., Del Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2007). Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 13170–13175. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700668104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marečková, K., Perrin, J. S., Nawaz Khan, I., Lawrence, C., Dickie, E., McQuiggan, D. A., et al. (2012). Hormonal contraceptives, menstrual cycle and brain response to faces. Soc. Cog. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 191–200. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306, 503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milivojevic, V., Fox, H. C., Sofuoglu, M., Covault, J., and Sinha, R. (2016). Effects of progesterone stimulated allopregnanolone on craving and stress response in cocaine dependent men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 65, 44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, G., Tybur, J. M., and Jordan, B. D. (2007). Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap dancers: economic evidence for human estrus? Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 375–381. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ossewaarde, L., Qin, S., Van Marle, H. J., van Wingen, G. A., Fernández, G., and Hermans, E. J. (2011). Stress-induced reduction in reward-related prefrontal cortex function. Neuroimage 55, 345–352. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., and Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pine, A., Shiner, T., Seymour, B., and Dolan, R. J. (2010). Dopamine, time, and impulsivity in humans. J. f Neurosci. 30, 8888–8896. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.6028-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porter, J. N., Roy, A. K., Benson, B., Carlisi, C., Collins, P. F., and Leibenluft, E. (2014). Age-related changes in the intrinsic functional connectivity of the human ventral vs. dorsal striatum from childhood to middle age. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 83–95. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinones-Jenab, V., and Jenab, S. (2010). Progesterone attenuates cocaine-induced responses. Horm. Behav. 58, 22–32. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, B., Richards, J. B., and de Wit, H. (2006). Acute-alcohol effects on the Experiential Discounting Task (EDT) and a question based measure of delay discounting. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 83, 194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rissman, J., Gazzaley, A., and D’Esposito, M. (2004). Measuring functional connectivity during distinct stages of a cognitive task. Neuroimage 23, 752–763. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, G. M. P., Newell, F., Simes-Franklin, C., and Garavan, H. (2009). Menstrual cycle phase modulates cognitive control over male but not female stimuli. Brain Res. 1224, 79–87. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosati, A. G., Stevens, J. R., Hare, B., and Hauser, M. D. (2007). The evolutionary origins of human patience: temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Curr. Biol. 17, 1663–1668. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotge, J. Y., Guehl, D., Dilharreguy, B., Cuny, E., and Aouizerate, B. (2008). Provocation of obsessive-compulsive symptoms: a quantitative voxel-based meta-analsysis of functional neuroimaging studies. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. Jpn. 33, 405–412.

Google Scholar

Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., and Stanton, S. J. (2004). Effects of affiliation and power motivation arousal on salivary progesterone and testosterone. Horm. Behav. 46, 592–599. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I., Joseph, L., Yan, Z., and Mintun, A. (2010). Resting-state functional MRI in depression unmasks increased connectivity between networks via the dorsal nexus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 11020–11025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000446107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, C. T., Sierra, Y., Oppler, S. H., and Boettiger, C. A. (2014). Ovarian cycle effects on immediate reward selection bias in humans: a role for estradiol. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 34, 5468–5476. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0014-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, C. T., Wallace, D. L., Dang, L. C., Aarts, E., Jagust, W. J., D’Esposito, M., et al. (2016). Modulation of impulsivity and reward sensitivity in intertemporal choice by striatal and midbrain dopamine synthesis in healthy adults. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 1146–1156. doi: 10.1152/jn.00261.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smits, R. R., Stein, J. S., Johnson, P. S., Odum, A. L., and Madden, G. J. (2013). Test-retest reliability and construct validity of the experiential discounting task. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 21, 155–163. doi: 10.1037/a0031725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sofuoglu, M., Dudish-Poulsen, S., Nelson, D., Pentel, P. R., and Hatsukami, D. K. (1999). Sex and menstrual cycle differences in the subjective effects from smoked cocaine in humans. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 7, 274–283. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.7.3.274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swalve, N., Smethells, J. R., and Carroll, M. E. (2016). Sex differences in attenuation of nicotine reinstatement after individual and combined treatments of progesterone and varenicline. Behav. Brain Res. 308, 46–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, T. (2011). A neuroeconomic theory of rational addiction and nonlinear time-perception. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 32, 221–225.

Google Scholar

Tao, J., Chen, X., Egorova, N., Liu, J., Xue, X., Wang, Q., et al. (2017). Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin practice modulates functional connectivity of the cognitive control network in older adults. Sci. Rep. 7: 41581.

Google Scholar

Tennent, B. J., Smith, E. R., and Davidson, J. M. (1980). The effects of estrogen and progesterone on female rat proceptive behavior. Horm. Behav. 14, 65–75. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(80)90016-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terner, J. M., and De Wit, H. (2006). Menstrual cycle phase and responses to drugs of abuse in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 84, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Wingen, G., Van Broekhoven, F., Verkes, R., Petersson, K. M., Bäckström, T., Buitelaar, J., et al. (2008). Progesterone selectively increases amygdala reactivity in women. Mol. Psychiatry 13, 325–333. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Wang, G. J., Hitzemann, R., Logan, J., Schlyer, D. J., et al. (1993). Decreased dopamine D2 receptor availability is associated with reduced frontal metabolism in cocaine abusers. Synapse 14, 169–177. doi: 10.1002/syn.890140210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, L. H. A., Rogers, R. D., Lawrence, A., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2000). Impaired planning but intact decision making in early Huntington’s disease: implications for specific fronto-striatal pathology. Neuropsychologia 38, 1112–1125. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00028-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weis, S., Hodgetts, S., and Hausmann, M. (2017). Sex differences and menstrual cycle effects in cognitive and sensory resting state networks. Brain Cogn. 131, 66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherill, R. R., Jagannathan, K., Hager, N., Maron, M., and Franklin, T. R. (2016). Influence of menstrual cycle phase on resting-state functional connectivity in naturally cycling, cigarette-dependent women. Biol. Sex Differ. 7:24. doi: 10.1186/s13293-016-0078-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wharton, W., Gleason, C. E., Olson, S. R., Carlsson, C. M., and Asthana, S. (2012). Neurobiological underpinnings of the estrogen - mood relationship. Curr. Psychiatry Rev. 8, 247–256. doi: 10.2174/157340012800792957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamsgray, C. H., Hampshire, A., Robbins, T. W., Owen, A. M., and Barker, R. A. (2007). Catechol O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype influences frontoparietal activity during planning in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurosci. 27, 4832–4838. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0774-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittmann, M., and Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, L., and Becker, J. B. (1994). Quantitative microdialysis determination of extracellular striatal dopamine concentration in male and female rats: effects of estrous cycle and gonadectomy. Neurosci. Lett. 180, 155–158. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(94)90510-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, L., Liang, Z.-Y., Wang, K., Li, S., and Jiang, T. (2009). Neural mechanism of intertemporal choice: from discounting future gains to future losses. Brain Res. 1261, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yager, L. M., Garcia, A. F., Wunsch, A. M., and Ferguson, S. M. (2015). The ins and outs of the striatum: role in drug addiction. Neuroscience 301, 529–541. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.06.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, K., Yu, D., Cai, C., Feng, D., Li, Y., Bi, Y., et al. (2016). Frontostriatal circuits, resting state functional connectivity and cognitive control in internet gaming disorder. Addict. Biol. 22, 813–822. doi: 10.1111/adb.12348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhuang, J.-Y., and Wang, J.-X. (2014). Women ornament themselves for intrasexual competition near ovulation, but for intersexual attraction in luteal phase. PLoS One 9:e106407. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zlebnik, N. E., Saykao, A. T., and Carroll, M. E. (2014). Effects of combined exercise and progesterone treatments on cocaine seeking in male and female rats. Psychopharmacology 231, 3787–3798. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3513-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive control, impulsivity, menstrual phase, dlPFC, dorsal striatum

Citation: Zhuang J-Y, Wang J-X, Lei Q, Zhang W and Fan M (2020) Neural Basis of Increased Cognitive Control of Impulsivity During the Mid-Luteal Phase Relative to the Late Follicular Phase of the Menstrual Cycle. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:568399. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.568399

Received: 01 June 2020; Accepted: 13 October 2020;
Published: 12 November 2020.

Edited by:

Todd S. Braver, Washington University in St. Louis, United States

Reviewed by:

Adele Diamond, University of British Columbia, Canada
Stephanie D. Preston, University of Michigan, United States

Copyright © 2020 Zhuang, Wang, Lei, Zhang and Fan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jia-Xi Wang, wangjiaxi1990@126.com

Download