Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 22 March 2022
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.841874

Brain Response to a Knee Proprioception Task Among Persons With Anterior Cruciate Ligament Reconstruction and Controls

Andrew Strong1* Helena Grip2 Carl-Johan Boraxbekk2,3,4,5 Jonas Selling1 Charlotte K. Häger1
  • 1Department of Community Medicine and Rehabilitation, Physiotherapy, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
  • 2Department of Radiation Sciences, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
  • 3Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance (DRCMR), Centre for Functional and Diagnostic Imaging and Research, Copenhagen University Hospital Hvidovre, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 4Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging (UFBI), Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
  • 5Institute of Sports Medicine Copenhagen (ISMC), Copenhagen University Hospital Bispebjerg, Copenhagen, Denmark

Knee proprioception deficits and neuroplasticity have been indicated following injury to the anterior cruciate ligament (ACL). Evidence is, however, scarce regarding brain response to knee proprioception tasks and the impact of ACL injury. This study aimed to identify brain regions associated with the proprioceptive sense of joint position at the knee and whether the related brain response of individuals with ACL reconstruction differed from that of asymptomatic controls. Twenty-one persons with unilateral ACL reconstruction (mean 23 months post-surgery) of either the right (n = 10) or left (n = 11) knee, as well as 19 controls (CTRL) matched for sex, age, height, weight and current activity level, performed a knee joint position sense (JPS) test during simultaneous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Integrated motion capture provided real-time knee kinematics to activate test instructions, as well as accurate knee angles for JPS outcomes. Recruited brain regions during knee angle reproduction included somatosensory cortices, prefrontal cortex and insula. Neither brain response nor JPS errors differed between groups, but across groups significant correlations revealed that greater errors were associated with greater ipsilateral response in the anterior cingulate (r = 0.476, P = 0.009), supramarginal gyrus (r = 0.395, P = 0.034) and insula (r = 0.474, P = 0.008). This is the first study to capture brain response using fMRI in relation to quantifiable knee JPS. Activated brain regions have previously been associated with sensorimotor processes, body schema and interoception. Our innovative paradigm can help to guide future research investigating brain response to lower limb proprioception.

Introduction

Rupture of the anterior cruciate ligament (ACL) is a common knee injury among athletic populations (Majewski et al., 2006), with a reported 30% rate of secondary ACL injury up to 15 years post-reconstruction (Leys et al., 2012) and a four times higher risk for knee osteoarthritis (Poulsen et al., 2019). Evidence further indicates that individuals with ACL reconstruction (ACLR) have lesser bilateral corticospinal excitability than those without injury, which may have a detrimental effect on muscle recovery (Rush et al., 2021). The initial trauma and potential surgical reconstruction causes loss of neural elements such as Golgi tendon organ-like receptors (Cabuk et al., 2022). These receptors contribute with afferent information to the central nervous system (CNS) regarding proprioceptive sensations such as movement and position (Dhillon et al., 2012). Of the proprioceptive senses, joint position sense (JPS) is one of the most commonly tested, typically involving the passive or active reproduction of joint angles with the confounding sense of vision occluded (Hillier et al., 2015). Outcomes are based on the difference in degrees between the target and reproduced angles, thus reflecting the kinematic errors. Identifying deficient knee JPS is believed important as it may contribute to errors in coordination which can subsequently expose the joint to positions deemed to increase the risk for injury (Needle et al., 2017). Meta-analyses have found significantly greater knee JPS errors for ACL-injured knees compared to both the contralateral non-injured knees of the same individuals (Relph et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2017; Strong et al., 2021a) and to those of asymptomatic persons (Relph et al., 2014; Strong et al., 2021a). The clinical significance of these findings is, however, unclear given the small absolute differences of < 1° knee flexion angle. Existing JPS tests have also been criticized for lacking reliability and validity (Hillier et al., 2015). Therefore, despite the belief that neurosensory information and knee proprioception may be impaired following ACL injury (Nyland et al., 2017), simply comparing knee JPS errors may be insufficient in detecting intricate alterations to the CNS (Baumeister et al., 2008). Thus, although knee JPS tests appear not to reveal clinically significant levels of deficient proprioception following ACL injury, reorganization of the CNS has nonetheless been hypothesized (Kapreli and Athanasopoulos, 2006). It is specifically believed that effects on the somatosensory and motor cortices may lead to proprioception-related deficits in preparatory and reactive motor abilities, consequently contributing to the increased risk for secondary injury (Needle et al., 2017).

Brain response associated with lower limb proprioceptive acuity is unclear. Callaghan et al. (2012) implemented a single-joint knee JPS task during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) among asymptomatic controls and found greater blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response in the supplementary motor area, ventral tegmental area, primary sensory cortex, cerebellum and precentral gyrus compared to a similar movement without angle reproduction. However, no outcomes of the JPS test were recorded, the sample size was small (n = 8) and the authors recommended a multi-joint movement to better represent normal functional tasks. In the same study, patellar taping, hypothesized to increase proprioceptive input, decreased response in the anterior cingulate and cerebellum. Based on a similar hypothesis, Thijs et al. (2010) investigated the influence of a knee brace and knee sleeve on brain response during lower limb multi-joint movements among asymptomatic individuals. Compared with no garment, greater response was found in the frontal lobe and paracentral lobule for the knee brace, as well as for the parietal lobe and superior parietal lobule for the knee sleeve. The combined findings of these studies indicate that changes to afferent information at the knee alters brain response during lower limb movements.

Injury to the ACL is believed to cause adaptations to the CNS (Needle et al., 2017). A recent scoping review of the topic by Neto et al. (2019) indeed found evidence for greater brain response compared to controls in mainly cortical areas associated with sensory and motor processes. One electroencephalography (EEG) study by Baumeister et al. (2008) incorporated a test of knee JPS and found greater frontal Theta-power for individuals with ACLR compared to controls. This response was believed to generate from the anterior cingulate cortex due to attentional demands and task complexity. Correlations for both groups additionally showed a reduction of JPS errors over time together with decreased processing in the parietal somatosensory cortex. However, EEG is limited in providing exact locations of the electrical sources from scalp recordings, while fMRI provides a higher spatial resolution (Ritter and Villringer, 2006). The only studies which used fMRI were those by Kapreli et al. (2009) and Grooms et al. (2017) who used similar task designs of single-joint knee flexion and extension. Both studies found less cerebellum activation and greater activation of secondary somatosensory areas for their ACL groups compared to their respective control groups, but also some inconsistent results for other regions. Differences between the ACL populations, such as treatment strategy, i.e., with or without reconstruction, and contrasting activity levels may have contributed to the divergent results. Grooms et al. (2017) suggested that multi-joint movements would improve the clinical applicability of future investigations. A more recent study thus included repetitive multi-joint heel slide movements and found that individuals with ACLR had greater response in areas associated with visual-spatial cognition and orientation compared with asymptomatic matched controls (Criss et al., 2020). It has, however, been recommended to improve the clinical applicability of such findings by increasing the motor control demands of such tasks by including, e.g., a proprioceptive goal-oriented element such as position matching (Grooms et al., 2017; Criss et al., 2020).

To summarize, brain response to proprioceptive tasks of the lower extremities remains unclear. Injury to the ACL may cause deficits to knee proprioception and related adaptations of the CNS. We have previously developed a supine knee JPS test which can be adapted for use in an fMRI setting (Strong et al., 2021b). Importantly, our standardized knee JPS test is unique for an fMRI setting in retaining otherwise common methods, whereby repetitions of knee angle memorization and subsequent reproduction are performed. Additionally, an integrated motion capture system not only provides test instructions to the participant based on their real-time kinematics, but is also used to identify knee movement phases for extraction of fMRI data during only relevant time periods. The kinematics further make possible the accurate reporting of angular errors to assess proprioceptive acuity and its potential association with response in specific brain regions. We therefore aimed to investigate the possibility of characterizing brain response using fMRI during simultaneous performance of a novel quantifiable knee JPS test among asymptomatic controls and individuals with unilateral ACLR. The specific research questions were: (1) Does our knee JPS test evoke a different brain response compared to a similar knee flexion movement without an angle reproduction task? (2) Is brain response different in persons with ACLR compared to asymptomatic persons during our knee JPS test? (3) Does brain response correlate to knee JPS test errors captured by kinematics? We hypothesized that our knee JPS test would recruit somatosensory and motor cortices more than during simple knee flexion and that individuals with ACLR would show greater response in such regions compared to asymptomatic persons. We further hypothesized that knee JPS errors would correlate with BOLD response in associated brain regions.

Materials and Methods

Participant Selection

For this cross-sectional study, participants were recruited from April 2017 to May 2019 using convenience sampling via orthopedic clinics, sports clubs, advertisements at the local University, social media and via word of mouth. Screening ensured the following eligibility criteria were met: aged 17–35 years, magnetic resonance imaging compliance, current Tegner activity score (Tegner and Lysholm, 1985) of at least 4, ability to understand either Swedish or English language, no known previous or ongoing injuries or diseases (other than ACLR and possible concomitant meniscus injury in the previous 5 years) that could affect the CNS or leg movements. Specific criteria for participants of the ACLR group required unilateral hamstring autograft reconstructive surgery performed 6 months to 5 years prior to testing, limited to only one ACL injury and subsequent surgery. All ACLR participants had to be cleared for full return to activity by their physical therapist. Asymptomatic controls were to be right-side dominant (leg preferred to kick a ball) and matched to ACLR participants with regard to sex, age, height, mass and current Tegner activity score. Our study is the first to incorporate this JPS paradigm during fMRI and thus data was not available to perform power calculations to estimate the required sample sizes. Considering previous similar research, one fMRI study comparing a different knee JPS task to a similar movement without a JPS task included eight healthy males (Callaghan et al., 2012). Previous fMRI studies comparing individuals with ACL injury to controls have included groups of either 15 (Grooms et al., 2017; Criss et al., 2020) or 17/18 (Kapreli et al., 2009), whereas a previous EEG study incorporating a knee JPS test included groups of 10/12 (Baumeister et al., 2008), respectively. Considering that our task design was calculated to result in fewer brain volumes than the aforementioned studies, we estimated that a pooled group of 40 participants would be required to investigate the brain regions recruited by our JPS test and 20 per group to examine potential differences in brain response between the ACLR and asymptomatic groups. The project was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Umeå, Sweden (Dnr. 2015/67-31) and was performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations stated in the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided their written informed consent prior to participation.

Procedures

Data collection occurred between June 2017 and May 2019. All participants completed the Marx Activity Scale (Marx et al., 2001) and the Tegner Activity Scale (Tegner and Lysholm, 1985). The following questionnaires were also completed by the ACLR participants: 2000 International Knee Documentation Committee Subjective Knee Form (IKDC; Irrgang et al., 2001); Lysholm Scale (Lysholm and Gillquist, 1982); and the Swedish version of the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia (TSK; Lundberg et al., 2004). Participants then performed a supine knee JPS test in the U-motion laboratory at Umeå University, Sweden to familiarize themselves with the task. Approximately 1 h later they performed the knee JPS test in an MRI scanner at the Umeå center for Functional Brain Imaging, University Hospital of Umeå, Sweden.

Knee Joint Position Sense Test Protocol

A knee JPS test was specifically designed for fMRI compatibility by using a supine position, slow active movements and additional rest blocks. It was considered important that the test reflect those typically applied in the literature whereby target angle memorization is performed immediately prior to each attempt to reproduce the angle (Han et al., 2016). It was further important to assess brain response only during blocks for which proprioceptive signal processing was considered to be most pertinent for perception and movement control. This is in contrast to a previous study in which brain imaging analyses were performed without memorization prior to each reproduction attempt and during movement to a start position for which proprioception was less relevant and represented half of the brain images assessed (Callaghan et al., 2012). Standardized written test instructions were provided to each participant and any questions were answered. A three-camera motion capture system (Oqus MRI Qualisys AB, Gothenburg, Sweden, 120 Hz) provided real-time kinematic data for the lower limbs. Passive retro-reflective spherical markers were affixed with participants in standing on the skin overlying the greater trochanter and lateral epicondyle of each femur using skin-friendly double-sided adhesive tape. The greater trochanter markers were placed on sticks 56 mm in length (wand markers) to improve their visibility. Participants then lay supine with their feet in foot holders of a custom-made low-friction knee flexion/extension board (see Figure 1). Elastic bands with hook-and-loop fasteners secured each foot and lower shank to the foot holders to ensure a 90° ankle joint angle. A strap over the torso and cushions in the head coil limited head movement. Wand markers with 46 mm sticks were affixed on the lateral part of each foot holder in line with the lateral malleolus in the sagittal plane. Kinematic data of the knee were based on the three respective markers for each leg in the sagittal plane. To provide an asymmetrical marker set and thus more stable real-time marker tracking, two markers were additionally affixed on the middle distal edge of the right footplate and one on the left footplate of the sliding board. Participants were positioned to allow a maximum knee flexion angle of approximately 100° when reaching an in-built physical stop at the proximal end of the board. For all movements, a constant knee angular velocity of 10°/s was attempted. This was practiced during familiarization using real-time graphical feedback for three trials per leg. If knee angular velocity fluctuated by more than 5°/s for consecutive trials of the MRI protocol, participants were verbally reminded to move slower or faster accordingly. Automated instructions were provided throughout the tests based on knee angle and angular velocity calculated from the real-time kinematic data as further described henceforth.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. An illustration of the experimental setup. The individual is performing with their right leg (A) one repetition of the JPS test with a target angle of 65°, and (B) one repetition of the Flex condition. Example instructions shown at the top were displayed on a screen at the rear of the scanner visible to the participant via a tilted mirror attached to the head coil. BOLD response was measured during the highlighted “JPS” (from Start position to reproduction angle) and “Flex” (from Start position to 65° knee flexion) blocks, respectively, as well as for a “Rest” block (Start position for 15 s). TA, target angle.

While supine in the Start position (legs fully extended), participants were instructed to flex a specified leg (randomized order) until a stop sign appeared (randomly activated at 35° or 60° knee flexion, but participants told angles were random). Two seconds after stopping, participants were instructed to maintain the position and memorize the knee (target) angle. Eight seconds later, they were instructed to return to the Start position and then 5 s after returning were instructed to reproduce the same angle. Four seconds after stopping, participants were instructed to return to the Start position. This was performed eight times per angle on each leg, resulting in a total of 16 repetitions for the JPS test on each leg. Additionally, knee flexion to the physical stop at ∼100° was performed on eight separate occasions per leg, with the timeframe from 0–65° extracted for brain imaging analysis for a Flex condition. The JPS and Flex conditions were pseudorandomized with a maximum two consecutive repetitions of the same condition and a minimum 7 s between trials. A Rest condition (Start position for 15 s) was also included five times at evenly-spaced intervals throughout testing. Thus, the task had a block design, individualized based on kinematic data. We utilized three different experimental conditions: (1) JPS condition – active knee flexion during angle reproduction (start/end at onset/cessation of flexion, respectively), (2) Flex condition – active knee flexion without angle reproduction (start at onset of flexion and end when reaching 65° knee angle), and (3) Rest condition – Start position (start/end at cessation of extension and onset of flexion, respectively). The protocol lasted approximately 40 min, resulting in 1240 whole-brain sets.

The current fMRI-adapted knee JPS test was also assessed for test-retest reliability in our movement laboratory among a separate group of 15 (9 males) asymptomatic persons (mean ± SD: age 25.0 ± 3.1 years, height 1.78 ± 0.09 m, mass 74.4 ± 11.2 kg) who performed the test on two occasions 7 days apart. Reliability was estimated with Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICC) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) based on a mean rating (k = 10), two-way mixed effects model with absolute agreement and Standard Error of Measurement (SEM), calculated as the mean square error term from the ANOVA, separately for the non-dominant [ICC 3,10 = 0.64 (CI 0.02–0.87), SEM = 0.67°] and dominant leg [ICC 3,10 = 0.78 (CI 0.34–0.93), SEM = 0.86°].

Image Acquisition

A 3T General Electric MR scanner with a 32-channel head coil was used to acquire the MR images. A T1 structural image was first acquired to create a study-specific template using the following parameters: 180 slices; 1 mm thickness; repetition time 8.2 ms; echo time 3.2 ms; flip angle 12°; field of view 25 × 25 cm. The functional gradient-echo-planar imaging sequence was collected with the following scanning parameters: repetition time = 2000 ms, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 80°, field of view = 25 × 25 cm. Thirty-seven transaxial slices with a thickness of 3.4 mm (0.5 mm gap) were acquired in an interleaved order. Ten initial dummy scans were collected and discarded prior to analysis. Test instructions were presented on a computer screen, which were seen via a tilted mirror attached to the head coil. The computer parallel port was used to detect the trigger output signal from the MR scanner to synchronize kinematic data with fMRI data in later analyses.

Data Processing and Analysis

Motion capture data were exported to Visual3D software (v.5.02.19, C-Motion Inc., Germantown, MD, United States) and filtered with a 6 Hz fourth-order low-pass zero-lag Butterworth filter. Automated scripts set events based on knee angles and knee angular velocities in the sagittal plane. Target and reproduction angles were extracted 2 s after cessation of flexion during the respective phases. All events were checked visually by the lead researcher and adjusted if deemed incorrect. No data were removed from analyses. Data were exported to IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 25 (IBM Corp., Armonk, N.Y., United States) in which all statistical analyses for knee JPS outcome measures, participant characteristics and patient-reported outcomes were performed.

SPM12 software (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, United Kingdom) run under MATLAB R 2016 b (MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States) was used for automated batching, pre-processing and data analysis. SPM was used for visualization of statistical maps and MarsBaR 0.44 (Brett et al., 2002) was used to calculate the percentage of BOLD signal change. Data were pre-processed in the following way: slice timing correction (interleaved order, first image set to reference slice), movement correction by unwarping and realigning all subsequent scans to the first image, co-registration of the mean functional image set and the structural T1 image set, segmentation of the co-registered structural image, normalization to a sample-specific template based on white and gray matter segments from the segmented, co-registered, structural image [using DARTEL (Ashburner, 2007)] and affine alignment to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space and smoothing with an 8-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. The final voxel size was 2 × 2 × 2 mm.

Statistical Analyses

The ACLR group was subdivided into those with right-side (R-ACLR) and left-side (L-ACLR) reconstruction. Sex distribution between groups was analyzed with a Chi-square test. Age and current activity level (Tegner current and Marx activity) between all groups, as well as pre-injury activity level of the ACLR groups to current activity level of CTRL, were analyzed with Kruskal-Wallis tests and Dunn-Bonferroni post hoc tests for significant results. The questionnaires TSK, IKDC and Lysholm Scale were compared between ACLR groups with Mann-Whitney U tests. A One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) compared body height and mass between all groups, as well as months since reconstruction between ACLR groups.

Between-group comparisons for kinematic data and brain response were made between the injured leg of ACLR and the matched leg of CTRL. Thus, the R-ACLR group was compared to controls only when the right leg was active for each group and the L-ACLR group were compared to controls only when the left leg was active for each group. Knee JPS error was defined in degrees as the absolute difference [absolute error (AE)] between the target and reproduction angles of the knee for each repetition of the JPS test. Mean AE was calculated for each leg by pooling the 40° and 65° angle conditions for each participant. Outliers in the data set were assessed for eligibility at group level, but none were removed due to a lack of evidence to the contrary. Shapiro-Wilk tests of normality and analysis of distribution graphs confirmed non-normally distributed group-level data. Knee JPS data were thus log transformed and were subsequently considered normally distributed according to further Shapiro-Wilk tests. Independent samples t-tests were used to compare the log transformed knee JPS AE between groups. Significance levels were set a priori (α = 0.05).

Brain imaging data for each angle condition were pooled so that each participant contributed with data from 16 JPS trials per leg. The first-order (single-subject) analyses were set up by including the experimental conditions as regressors of interest in the general linear model, convolved with the hemodynamic response function. Six realignment parameters (head rotations and translations) were included as covariates of no interest to account for movement artifacts. The following contrasts were set up for each participant: (1) (JPS > Rest) and (2) (Flex > Rest). Second-order (group) analyses were based on flexible factorial models (Gläscher and Gitelman, 2008) of the (JPS > Rest) and (Flex > Rest) contrasts from each participant, including participant, group (two levels, R-ACLR or L-ACLR and CTRL), condition (two levels, JPS and Flex) and the interaction (group × condition) as factors. Separate analyses of the conditions used in the flexible factorial design (JPS > Rest) and (Flex > Rest) were also performed to investigate activation patterns in comparison to Rest. These analyses are included as supplementary material. The main effect from condition [(JPS > Rest) > (Flex > Rest)] and the interaction (group × condition) was analyzed [family-wise error (FWE) rate corrected, 0.05; voxel limit 15]. Brain regions were defined and labeled according to the MNI coordinates, which relate to the peak activity within the cluster, using automated anatomic labeling in SPM (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Brain regions that showed significant activation for any of these analyses were further analyzed by calculating the percentage of BOLD signal change during the JPS condition (i.e., the original beta values) in the significant region, compared to the overall mean brain activity of the session. The percentage of BOLD change values were exported to SPSS where Spearman’s rho was used to analyze correlations of participant mean values between percentage of BOLD change and JPS errors.

Results

Of 77 persons with ACL injury and 61 potential controls who expressed an interest in participating, 55 and 14, respectively, were considered ineligible due to either another existing injury, too low physical activity level, left leg dominance for controls, older age, or a combination of those factors. Thus, 22 individuals with ACLR and 47 asymptomatic controls were considered eligible for the study. To ensure matching of characteristics between groups, controls were invited to participate only after a matching ACLR participant had completed testing. One ACLR participant did not complete the fMRI procedure due to claustrophobic feelings in the MRI scanner. Despite completing testing, one asymptomatic control was not included in the analyses due to the presence of a benign arachnoid cyst, unknown prior to participation, which would have confounded brain imaging analyses. Thus, 10 R-ACLR, 11 L-ACLR and 19 CTRL completed testing and were included in the analyses (see Figure 2 for a flow diagram of the recruitment process and Table 1 for group characteristics). Due to a technical issue, one L-ACLR participant completed a shortened protocol of 12, instead of 16, repetitions per leg. Also, due to slow performance of the test, one participant from each group performed one less repetition on both legs. Current activity level of CTRL was significantly lower than pre-injury level of R-ACLR (P = 0.010). No groups differed significantly with regard to sex, age, height, weight or the remaining patient-report outcome measures. Months since reconstruction did not differ significantly between the ACLR groups. For the knee JPS test, no statistically significant differences in errors were seen between either the left reconstructed/non-dominant leg of L-ACLR [median (Mdn) 5.10° (Q1 4.04°, Q3 6.82°)] and CTRL [Mdn 4.17° (Q1 2.89°, Q3 4.95°)], respectively, or between the right reconstructed/dominant leg of R-ACLR [Mdn 4.89° (Q1 3.54°, Q3 6.85°)] and CTRL [4.55° (Q1 3.64°, Q3 6.56°)], respectively.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flow diagram illustrating the recruitment process.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics of the study groups.

Brain Response During the Knee Joint Position Sense Test

The JPS condition evoked significantly greater BOLD response (P = 0.05, FWE corrected; voxel limit 15) in seven brain regions for each leg compared to the Flex condition without a JPS task. These included prefrontal regions, the precentral gyrus, cingulate gyri and insula (see Figures 3, 4 and Table 2 for all significant regions with voxel extent, exact statistics, and MNI coordinates).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Brain regions with significant main effect condition in (JPS > Rest) > (Flex > Rest), P = 0.05, family-wise corrected, voxel limit 15. Slices –5:2:38 mm (MNI) in inferior-superior direction are shown. Group mean brains (Dartel) are used for the illustration for (A) left-side analyses – L-ACLR moving their injured left leg and CTRL moving their left non-dominant leg, and (B) right-side analyses – R-ACLR moving their right injured leg and CTRL moving their right non-dominant leg.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. BOLD signal change (%) during angle reproduction of the JPS test for the brain regions with a significant main effect from condition [(JPS > Rest) > (Flex > Rest)]. Ant., Anterior; Contra., contralateral; CTRL, asymptomatic control group; Flex, flex condition; Ipsi., ipsilateral; JPS, joint position sense condition; L-ACLR, left side anterior cruciate ligament-reconstructed group; R-ACL, right-side anterior cruciate ligament-reconstructed group; Temp., Temporal.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain regions with significantly greater bold response during JPS than Flex [(JPS > Rest)] > (Flex > Rest)].

Between-Group Comparisons of Brain Response During the Knee Joint Position Sense Test

No significant between-group differences were found on the corrected level (FWE 0.05).

Correlations Between Brain Response and Knee Joint Position Sense Errors

Correlation analyses were performed for the seven regions per test side that were found to have significantly greater BOLD response during the JPS condition compared to the Flex condition (see Table 2 for a list of the regions). When performing the test with the right leg (R-ACL and CTRL, n = 29), significant positive correlations were found between JPS errors and BOLD signal percentage change (i.e., greater JPS AE correlated with greater BOLD response) in the ipsilateral anterior cingulate (r = 0.476, P = 0.009; Figure 5A) as well as the ipsilateral supramarginal gyrus (r = 0.395, P = 0.034; Figure 5B). Close to significance was also the ipsilateral middle frontal gyrus (r = 0.364, P = 0.052). For the left leg (L-ACLR and CTRL, n = 30), a significant positive correlation was found for the ipsilateral insula (r = 0.474, P = 0.008; Figure 5C).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Scatter plots illustrating the significant correlations between mean knee joint position sense absolute errors and simultaneous percentage change in BOLD response for the right test side (A,B) and the left test side (C).

Discussion

We aimed to characterize brain response during a knee JPS test among asymptomatic controls and individuals with ACL reconstruction. We further investigated whether brain response would differ between groups and whether brain response would correlate with knee JPS errors. Our hypothesis that our knee JPS test would evoke greater response in somatosensory and motor cortices compared to simple knee flexion was confirmed by observations of greater response during angle reproduction in, for example, the precentral gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, insula and cingulate gyri. The lack of significant differences between individuals with ACL reconstruction compared to asymptomatic controls was, however, contradictory to our hypothesis. Greater knee JPS errors correlated with BOLD response in the insula, anterior cingulate and supramarginal gyrus, thus confirming our hypothesis of correlations between brain response and knee JPS errors.

Brain Response During a Knee Proprioception Test

Our knee JPS test evoked response in the ipsilateral precentral gyrus for the right test side and cingulate gyri for both test sides. Response in these regions has also been observed among asymptomatic individuals during active knee flexion tasks of JPS (Callaghan et al., 2012) and force matching (Grooms et al., 2021). Response in the right middle frontal gyrus was seen for both test sides and has been previously associated with switching between external and internal focus of attention (Japee et al., 2015), relevant for our JPS task where the focus of attention changes from external instructions on a screen to internal sensations related to proprioception. Also common for both test sides was recruitment of the ipsilateral insula, previously associated with sensorimotor processes such as active and passive stepping motions (Jaeger et al., 2014). This finding also aligns with the body image and body schema concepts of body representations (Head and Holmes, 1911), in which current understandings attribute the insula with conscious perceptual representation of the body and memory (Dijkerman and De Haan, 2007). The insula and anterior cingulate are further thought to play a key role in interoception, a term originally introduced by Sherrington (1906) to describe visceral sensations, but now often used as a broader term encompassing the subjective experience of the body state (Ceunen et al., 2016) and even proprioception (Hillier et al., 2015). In fact damage to the insula due to stroke has been associated with poor position sense of the upper limbs (Findlater et al., 2016). The same study also found similar associations for the inferior frontal and superior temporal gyri, two areas that were activated during our JPS test in the current study. Thus, our findings add to previous evidence of distributed neural networks involved when processing proprioceptive tasks and expand these to those associated with the lower limbs. Interestingly, 11 of the 14 regions with significantly greater BOLD response during the JPS condition compared with the Flex condition were of the right hemisphere (Table 2). A general dominance of the right hemisphere for proprioception in the lower limbs is supported by previous findings for a foot position matching task among healthy individuals who were right hand and foot dominant, suggesting a role for position sense of the right parietal and frontal cortex (Iandolo et al., 2018). In fact, research into body representations further seems to implicate the right hemisphere with kinesthetic sensations (Naito et al., 2016). If future research confirms these results, the traditional network of brain regions believed to be associated with proprioception of the lower limbs should thus be extended and include an emphasis on the right hemisphere. To improve our understanding further, extended mapping of associated neural networks is required.

Brain Response of Individuals With Anterior Cruciate Ligament Reconstruction Compared to Matched Controls

There were no significant between-group differences for brain response nor for knee JPS errors. Previous research investigating knee JPS and somatosensory evoked potentials of individuals 18 months after surgical reconstruction of the ACL also found a lack of difference compared with controls as evidence of sensory neurone regeneration (Ochi et al., 1999). The original version of the current supine knee JPS test also did not detect any significant differences in errors for a separate ACLR group approximately 2 years after surgical reconstruction compared with matched athletes (Strong et al., 2021b). In that study, less-active controls instead showed significantly greater JPS errors compared to the ACLR group, suggesting that activity level is a more important factor in this context. It is therefore possible that deficits in proprioception were not present among the individuals of our ACLR group, who were active and participated on average 23 months after surgical reconstruction. Additionally, a recent meta-analysis found that only knee JPS tests with passive rather than active movements differentiate between ACL-injured knees and those of asymptomatic controls (Strong et al., 2021a). The active movements of the current test, which also incorporated the hip, may further have contributed to the lack of between-group difference seen here. The target angles of 40° and 65° knee flexion used in our JPS test both lie close to the mid-range of motion for the joint. These angles may not have been optimal for elucidating differences between groups where joint receptors are the focus of investigation, given that they are believed to play a more predominant role toward the limits of joint rotation (Proske and Chen, 2021). It is also possible that small group sizes (due to separating ACLR into right and left leg analyses), as well as the contrast to such a similar movement, may have reduced the sensitivity of our analysis.

Correlations Between Brain Response and Knee Joint Position Sense Errors

Our results showed that greater knee JPS errors, i.e., poorer knee proprioceptive acuity, was associated with greater brain response in the ipsilateral insula for the left test side, as well as the ipsilateral anterior, paracingulate and supramarginal gyri for the right test side. These results are line with the Embodied Predictive Interoception Coding (EPIC) model proposed by Barrett and Simons (Barrett and Simmons, 2015), which describes the process of active inference in interoception. Part of this model describes the role of the mid- and posterior insula in computing and transmitting prediction errors as well as the integration of other agranular visceromotor cortices such as the cingulate cortex in this process. This model may thus be relevant for proprioceptive tasks. The importance of the insula to position sense is further supported by findings previously mentioned here for the upper limbs following stroke, whereby lesions in this region were associated with greater errors when attempting to actively move the unaffected arm to the mirror-matched position of the passively moved contralateral arm (Findlater et al., 2016). Associated response for the right supramarginal gyrus is supported by previous findings of greater response during a proprioceptive task of force matching at the knee among asymptomatic females (Grooms et al., 2021).

Limitations of the Study

A limitation of our study was the additional task of attempting a constant knee angular velocity, although this was similar for the contrast Flex condition. Further, the active movement of the whole lower limb meant that proprioceptive feedback was not isolated to the knee, but also incorporated the hip. Although this is more similar to everyday activities and thus may enhance the ecological validity of the task compared with a single-joint movement, compensations at the hip joint for potential deficits at the knee are possible. Despite the active task, head movement (range 0.14 – 0.88 mm) did not confound brain imaging analyses. Due to challenges in recruiting participants, we included ACLR individuals who had injured either knee, but a control group with only right-side dominance. Comparisons were thus made to the non-dominant and dominant legs of CTRL for the L-ACLR and R-ACLR groups, respectively. Evidence regarding whether leg dominance influences knee proprioception among healthy controls has, however, shown both positive (Azevedo et al., 2020) and negative (Cug et al., 2016; Galamb et al., 2018) findings. Defining leg dominance for an unfamiliar task is also complicated given evidence of task specificity (Van Melick et al., 2017). It is thus unclear whether leg dominance influenced our results. Further, having to split the ACL participants into two groups meant that the sample sizes for between-groups comparisons were not in line with our aims. We acknowledge this as an important limitation of the current study. Future studies with a greater number of and more homogenous participants are likely required to further elucidate potential group differences. Additionally, both sexes were represented in each of our groups. Two similar fMRI studies have, however, indicated potentially different functional brain connectivity between males (Diekfuss et al., 2020) and females (Diekfuss et al., 2019) who later suffered an ACL injury. Our eligibility criteria required a minimum physical activity level score of four according to the Tegner activity scale. Although we matched current activity level between groups, a sub-analysis using Wilcoxon Signed Ranks tests compared pre-injury and current activity levels within the ACLR groups and found that R-ACLR had significantly reduced their activity level (P = 0.011), but the level was not significantly changed among L-ACLR. A difference in activity level change from pre- to post-injury was thus a potentially confounding factor in our analyses. To explore this further, a Mann-Whitney U test to compare change in activity level between the groups found no significant between-group difference. A significant reduction in activity level following ACL injury is, however, considered a useful outcome for defining non-copers (Button et al., 2006). Future studies may thus benefit from analyses that consider coping classification of individuals with ACL injury. To improve the sensitivity of the analyses, future test designs should also consider how to increase the number of brain images during movements which require proprioceptive processing. This could, for example, be done by increasing the duration of the proprioceptive element of such tasks.

Conclusion

Our paradigm found greater BOLD response for a number of brain regions which have previously been associated with processes that can be linked to proprioception. These results thus indicate that the experimental design was successful in recruiting brain regions involved in proprioception and adds valuable information regarding central processing of such tasks. The lack of differences between groups adds to the mixed evidence of proprioceptive deficits among individuals nearly 2 years post-ACLR. The small sample sizes as a result of splitting the ACLR group into left and right-side injuries may, however, have been a contributing factor to the lack of significance. The significant correlations between knee JPS error and activation in some brain regions further indicates that demands were placed on the proprioceptive acuity of the participants. The novel integration of kinematics with fMRI thus provided added value to the paradigm by providing behavioral data as well as specific time frames for extraction of brain images to isolate such processes. Our unique paradigm demonstrates a method to expand these findings and provide further insights into brain response to proprioception at the knee and other joints as well as among different populations.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Regionala etikprövningsnämnden i Umeå. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JS created the integrated software used for data collection. AS and CH recruited participants. AS and JS performed data collection. AS and HG processed and analyzed the kinematic and brain imaging data. AS, HG, and C-JB performed statistical analyses. AS wrote the first draft of the manuscript. HG and JS wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the conception and design of the study and contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was funded by the Swedish Research Council (Grant No. 2017-00892), Region Västerbotten (Grant No. ALF VLL548501, VLL838421 and Strategic funding VLL-358901; Project No. 7002795), the Swedish Research Council for Sports Science (Grant No. Dnr CIF P2018-0104; P2019-0068), and Umeå University School of Sport Science (Grant No. Dnr IH 5.3-13-2017). King Gustaf V and Queen Victoria’s Foundation of Freemasons. C-JB was supported by The Novo Nordisk Foundation grant to Team Danmark. The funders did not have any role in the study design or outcomes.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging (UFBI) for collaboration and assistance, Ashokan Arumugam and Ph.D. student Adam Grinberg for assistance during data collection, and all of the study participants.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2022.841874/full#supplementary-material

References

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azevedo, J., Rodrigues, S., and Seixas, A. (2020). The influence of sports practice, dominance and gender on the knee joint position sense. Knee 28, 117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.knee.2020.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., and Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 419–429. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, J., Reinecke, K., and Weiss, M. (2008). Changed cortical activity after anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction in a joint position paradigm: an EEG study. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 18, 473–484. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00702.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R., and Poline, J.-B. (2002). Modeling & analysis. NeuroImage 16, 769–1198.

Google Scholar

Button, K., Van Deursen, R., and Price, P. (2006). Classification of functional recovery of anterior cruciate ligament copers, non-copers, and adapters. Br. J. Sports Med. 40, 853–859. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2006.028258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabuk, H., Kusku Cabuk, F., and Turan, K. (2022). The time from injury to surgery is an important factor affecting the mechanoreceptors at stump of torn anterior cruciate ligament. Arch. Orthop. Trauma. Surg. [Epub online ahead of print]. doi: 10.1007/s00402-021-04310-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callaghan, M. J., Mckie, S., Richardson, P., and Oldham, J. A. (2012). Effects of patellar taping on brain activity during knee joint proprioception tests using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Phys. Ther. 92, 821–830. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20110209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceunen, E., Vlaeyen, J. W. S., and Van Diest, I. (2016). On the Origin of Interoception. Front. Psychol. 7:743–743. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Criss, C. R., Onate, J. A., and Grooms, D. R. (2020). Neural activity for hip-knee control in those with anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction: a task-based functional connectivity analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 730:134985. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.134985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cug, M., Wikstrom, E. A., Golshaei, B., and Kirazci, S. (2016). The Effects of Sex, Limb Dominance, and Soccer Participation on Knee Proprioception and Dynamic Postural Control. J. Sport Rehab. 25, 31–39. doi: 10.1123/jsr.2014-0250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhillon, M. S., Bali, K., and Prabhakar, S. (2012). Differences among mechanoreceptors in healthy and injured anterior cruciate ligaments and their clinical importance. Muscles Ligaments Tendons J. 2, 38–43.

Google Scholar

Diekfuss, J. A., Grooms, D. R., Nissen, K. S., Schneider, D. K., Foss, K. D. B., Thomas, S., et al. (2020). Alterations in knee sensorimotor brain functional connectivity contributes to ACL injury in male high-school football players: a prospective neuroimaging analysis. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 24, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/j.bjpt.2019.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diekfuss, J. A., Grooms, D. R., Yuan, W., Dudley, J., Barber Foss, K. D., Thomas, S., et al. (2019). Does brain functional connectivity contribute to musculoskeletal injury? A preliminary prospective analysis of a neural biomarker of ACL injury risk. J. Sci. Med. Sport 22, 169–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkerman, H. C., and De Haan, E. H. (2007). Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action. Behav. Brain Sci. 30, 189–201. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Findlater, S. E., Desai, J. A., Semrau, J. A., Kenzie, J. M., Rorden, C., Herter, T. M., et al. (2016). Central perception of position sense involves a distributed neural network - Evidence from lesion-behavior analyses. Cortex 79, 42–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galamb, K., Szilagyi, B., Magyar, O. M., Hortobagyi, T., Nagatomi, R., Vaczi, M., et al. (2018). Effects of side-dominance on knee joint proprioceptive target-matching asymmetries. Physiol. Int. 105, 257–265. doi: 10.1556/2060.105.2018.3.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gläscher, J. P., and Gitelman, D. (2008). Contrast Weights in Flexible Factorial Design With Multiple Groups of Subjects. Unpublished tutorial. Available: http://www.jiscmail.ac.uk/cgi-bin/webadmin?A2=ind0803&L=SPM&P=R16629 (accessed July, 2020).

Google Scholar

Grooms, D. R., Criss, C. R., Simon, J. E., Haggerty, A. L., and Wohl, T. R. (2021). Neural Correlates of Knee Extension and Flexion Force Control: a Kinetically-Instrumented Neuroimaging Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:622637. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.622637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grooms, D. R., Page, S. J., Nichols-Larsen, D. S., Chaudhari, A. M., White, S. E., and Onate, J. A. (2017). Neuroplasticity associated with anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction. J. Orthop. Sports Phys. Ther. 47, 180–189. doi: 10.2519/jospt.2017.7003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, J., Waddington, G., Adams, R., Anson, J., and Liu, Y. (2016). Assessing proprioception: a critical review of methods. J. Sport Health Sci. 5, 80–90.

Google Scholar

Head, H., and Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory Disturbances from Cerebral Lesions. Brain 34, 102–254. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00123-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillier, S., Immink, M., and Thewlis, D. (2015). Assessing Proprioception: a Systematic Review of Possibilities. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 29, 933–949. doi: 10.1177/1545968315573055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iandolo, R., Bellini, A., Saiote, C., Marre, I., Bommarito, G., Oesingmann, N., et al. (2018). Neural correlates of lower limbs proprioception: an fMRI study of foot position matching. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 1929–1944. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irrgang, J. J., Anderson, A. F., Boland, A. L., Harner, C. D., Kurosaka, M., Neyret, P., et al. (2001). Development and validation of the international knee documentation committee subjective knee form. Am. J. Sports Med. 29, 600–613.

Google Scholar

Jaeger, L., Marchal-Crespo, L., Wolf, P., Riener, R., Michels, L., and Kollias, S. (2014). Brain activation associated with active and passive lower limb stepping. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:828. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Japee, S., Holiday, K., Satyshur, M. D., Mukai, I., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2015). A role of right middle frontal gyrus in reorienting of attention: a case study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:23. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapreli, E., and Athanasopoulos, S. (2006). The anterior cruciate ligament deficiency as a model of brain plasticity. Med. Hypothes. 67, 645–650. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.01.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapreli, E., Athanasopoulos, S., Gliatis, J., Papathanasiou, M., Peeters, R., Strimpakos, N., et al. (2009). Anterior cruciate ligament deficiency causes brain plasticity: a functional MRI study. Am. J. Sports Med. 37, 2419–2426. doi: 10.1177/0363546509343201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. J., Lee, J. H., and Lee, D. H. (2017). Proprioception in Patients With Anterior Cruciate Ligament Tears: a Meta-analysis Comparing Injured and Uninjured Limbs. Am. J. Sports Med. 45, 2916–2922. doi: 10.1177/0363546516682231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leys, T., Salmon, L., Waller, A., Linklater, J., and Pinczewski, L. (2012). Clinical results and risk factors for reinjury 15 years after anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction: a prospective study of hamstring and patellar tendon grafts. Am. J. Sports Med. 40, 595–605. doi: 10.1177/0363546511430375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundberg, M. K. E., Styf, J., and Carlsson, S. G. (2004). A psychometric evaluation of the Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia — from a physiotherapeutic perspective. Physiother. Theor. Prac. 20, 121–133. doi: 10.1080/09593980490453002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lysholm, J., and Gillquist, J. (1982). Evaluation of knee ligament surgery results with special emphasis on use of a scoring scale. Am. J. Sports Med. 10, 150–154. doi: 10.1177/036354658201000306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Majewski, M., Susanne, H., and Klaus, S. (2006). Epidemiology of athletic knee injuries: a 10-year study. Knee 13, 184–188. doi: 10.1016/j.knee.2006.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marx, R. G., Stump, T. J., Jones, E. C., Wickiewicz, T. L., and Warren, R. F. (2001). Development and Evaluation of an Activity Rating Scale for Disorders of the Knee. Am. J. Sports Med. 29, 213–218. doi: 10.1177/03635465010290021601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naito, E., Morita, T., and Amemiya, K. (2016). Body representations in the human brain revealed by kinesthetic illusions and their essential contributions to motor control and corporeal awareness. Neurosci. Res. 104, 16–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2015.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Needle, A., Lepley, A., and Grooms, D. (2017). Central Nervous System Adaptation After Ligamentous Injury: a Summary of Theories. Evid. Clin. Interpret. Sports Med. 47, 1271–1288. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0666-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neto, T., Sayer, T., Theisen, D., and Mierau, A. (2019). Functional Brain Plasticity Associated with ACL Injury: a Scoping Review of Current Evidence. Neural Plast. 2019:3480512. doi: 10.1155/2019/3480512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyland, J., Gamble, C., Franklin, T., and Caborn, D. N. M. (2017). Permanent knee sensorimotor system changes following ACL injury and surgery. Knee Surg. Sports Traumatol. Arthrosc. 25, 1461–1474. doi: 10.1007/s00167-017-4432-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochi, M., Iwasa, J., Uchio, Y., Adachi, N., and Sumen, Y. (1999). The regeneration of sensory neurones in the reconstruction of the anterior cruciate ligament. J. Bone. Joint Surg. Br. 81, 902–906. doi: 10.1302/0301-620x.81b5.9202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulsen, E., Goncalves, G. H., Bricca, A., Roos, E. M., Thorlund, J. B., and Juhl, C. B. (2019). Knee osteoarthritis risk is increased 4-6 fold after knee injury - a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 1454–1463. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2018-100022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proske, U., and Chen, B. (2021). Two senses of human limb position: methods of measurement and roles in proprioception. Exp. Brain Res. 239, 3157–3174. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06207-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Relph, N., Herrington, L., and Tyson, S. (2014). The effects of ACL injury on knee proprioception: a meta-analysis. Physiotherapy 100, 187–195. doi: 10.1016/j.physio.2013.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritter, P., and Villringer, A. (2006). Simultaneous EEG–fMRI. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 823–838.

Google Scholar

Rush, J. L., Glaviano, N. R., and Norte, G. E. (2021). Assessment of Quadriceps Corticomotor and Spinal-Reflexive Excitability in Individuals with a History of Anterior Cruciate Ligament Reconstruction: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Sports Med. 51, 961–990. doi: 10.1007/s40279-020-01403-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherrington, C. S. (1906). The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. New Haven, CT, US: Yale University Press.

Google Scholar

Strong, A., Arumugam, A., Tengman, E., Röijezon, U., and Häger, C. K. (2021a). Properties of Knee Joint Position Sense Tests for Anterior Cruciate Ligament Injury: a Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Orthop. J. Sports Med. 9, 23259671211007878. doi: 10.1177/23259671211007878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strong, A., Srinivasan, D., and Häger, C. K. (2021b). Development of supine and standing knee joint position sense tests. Phys. Ther. Sport 49, 112–121. doi: 10.1016/j.ptsp.2021.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tegner, Y., and Lysholm, J. (1985). Rating systems in the evaluation of knee ligament injuries. Clin Orthop. Relat. Res. 198, 43–49.

Google Scholar

Thijs, Y., Vingerhoets, G., Pattyn, E., Rombaut, L., and Witvrouw, E. (2010). Does bracing influence brain activity during knee movement: an fMRI study. Knee Surg. Sports Traumatol. Arthrosc. 18, 1145–1149. doi: 10.1007/s00167-009-1012-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain. NeuroImage 15, 273–289.

Google Scholar

Van Melick, N., Meddeler, B. M., Hoogeboom, T. J., Nijhuis-Van Der, Sanden, M. W. G., and Van Cingel, R. E. H. (2017). How to determine leg dominance: the agreement between self-reported and observed performance in healthy adults. PLoS One 12:e0189876. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anterior cruciate ligament, anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction, knee, rehabilitation, position sense, magnetic resonance imaging, neuronal plasticity

Citation: Strong A, Grip H, Boraxbekk C-J, Selling J and Häger CK (2022) Brain Response to a Knee Proprioception Task Among Persons With Anterior Cruciate Ligament Reconstruction and Controls. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:841874. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.841874

Received: 22 December 2021; Accepted: 25 February 2022;
Published: 22 March 2022.

Edited by:

Jason Friedman, Tel Aviv University, Israel

Reviewed by:

Tim Lehmann, University of Paderborn, Germany
Zengyong Li, National Research Center for Rehabilitation Technical Aids, China

Copyright © 2022 Strong, Grip, Boraxbekk, Selling and Häger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andrew Strong, andrew.strong@umu.se

Download