Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 07 March 2023
Sec. Interacting Minds and Brains
Volume 17 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1050164

Theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors: The application of complexity science and neuroscience to explain the complexities of marital behaviors

  • 1Department of Psychology, Farhangian University, Tehran, Iran
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Isfahan, Isfahan, Iran

The extant theories on the quality and stability of marital relationships have some difficulties in explaining some of the complexities of marital behaviors. The present article is an initial attempt to explain the complexities of marital behaviors based on the science of complexity and neuroscience. This article proposes a new theoretical framework relying on this simple argument that marital behaviors, as one of the most complex human behaviors, are the product of one’s brain’s complex adaptive system (CAS). Hence, to understand the complexities of marital behaviors, a movement toward familiarity with the brain’s CAS involved in marital behaviors needs to be started. The article presents the theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors (BCM) and outlines its assumptions, concepts, and propositions. Then, BCM is compared with the extant theories on happy and stable marriage, and finally, it was concluded by discussing the testability and the potential application of the theory. This article might inspire interdisciplinary studies of marital relationships, complex systems, and neuroscience and may have considerable practical implications.

“The next century [21st] will be the century of complexity” – Stephen Hawking

1. Introduction

The existing theories and models on the quality and stability of marital relationships have made unique and worthwhile contributions to the/an understanding of marriage. However, they have some difficulties in explaining some of the complexities of the marital relationship. For example, behavioral models – which focus on the quality of the couple’s interactions and communication skills – have ignored the underlying sources of spouses’ interaction. In addition, behavioral theories do not explain how couples with insufficient skills can improve on their own (Karney and Bradbury, 1995). Also, behavioral theories do not explain why some couples with negative interaction patterns continue their marriage despite long-term conflicts (Hawkins and Booth, 2005).

Similarly, social exchange theory of marriage (Levinger, 1965, 1976), explains why marriages are stable or unstable, considering the rewards, costs, alternatives, and barriers to leaving a relationship. However, how a marriage that was initially stable becomes unstable over time or vice versa is not in its scope (Karney and Bradbury, 1995). Furthermore, social exchange theory does not consider that two different people in identical situations may behave differently in low reward conditions. Clark and Mills (1979) emphasized that marriage is communal, not an exchange relationship. They believe that social exchange theory deduces human behaviors and communications as a purely rational process raised from economic theories. According to attachment theories of marriage (Hazan and Shaver, 1987, 1994), the quality and stability of adult romantic relationships depend on the attachment styles of partners developed in their infancy, in addition to the fulfillment of their basic needs for care, comfort, and sexual gratification. However, attachment theories do not explain why, under certain circumstances, people with insecure attachment styles can have stable relationships or why couples with the same attachment styles and the same levels of satisfaction with their needs may have different marriage destinations (Karney and Bradbury, 1995).

According to crisis theory (Hill’s 1949; McCubbin and Patterson, 1982) the level of the couple’s resources and their definitions of stressful events moderate the effects of these events and crises on the quality and stability of a relationship. However, as Karney and Bradbury (1995) pointed out, “rarely have crisis theorists addressed the specific coping responses that lead to either adaptation or maladaptation (Karney and Bradbury, 1995, p. 7).” Similarly, the vulnerability–stress–adaptation (VSA) model believes that adaptive processes and personal characteristics can interact with stressful events and enduring vulnerabilities and affect the couple’s level of adaptation to marital difficulties (Karney and Bradbury, 1995). However, the VSA model mentions “adaptive processes” but does not provide details about the content and sources of these processes. Additionally, the VSA model does not explain how these processes translate into positive behaviors that lead to a happy and stable marriage.

Furthermore, existing theories are facing some problems in the explanation of some complex phenomena such as non-linearity in dynamic changes in marital relationship, sudden changes in the marital relationship and its transformation into an entirely different relationship (phase transition), significant changes due to the small events (butterfly effect), the importance of starting values (positively or negatively) in marital interactions, the existence of attractors in marital interactions, difficulty in returning the relationship to its previous state after the occurrence of critical events such as a betrayal (hysteresis) (Gottman, 2014), and the possibility of self-repairing processes in marriages without external intervention (self-organization and adaptation) (Karney and Bradbury, 1995).

Marital relationship is very complex, and the brain has to take many variables into account when making decisions about the adaptive behaviors in a marital relationship. The present article raises the idea that untying the knot of complexities of the marital relationship is possible only when one accepts this simple argument that marital behavior, as one of the most complex human behaviors, is the product of their brain’s complex function. Therefore, to understand the complexities of marital behaviors, there is no choice but to become familiar with the brain’s complex system (CS), its subsystems, and how they interact to consider all of the variables and reach a final decision on marital behaviors; the fact that the existing theories have not taken it into account. Thus, this article aims to elucidate this idea by integrating evidence in the literature of marital relationships studies, CSs, neuroscience, and the author’ empirical work, which results in the “theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors” (BCM).

In the following sections, CSs’ features will be briefly discussed, followed by a discussion on the initial attempt of the author to discover the brain’s complex adaptive system (CAS) involved in marital behaviors. The BCM is then introduced, and its assumptions, concepts, and propositions are outlined in detail. BCM is compared with the extant theories, and ultimately, the conclusion is made by discussing the testability and the potential applications of the theory.

2. Complex systems, their key concepts, and features

A CS is a system that consists of numerous agents that interact with one another and potentially with their environment. These interactions lead to the emergence of a global property or collective behavior that is not observed by understanding each agent. Examples of CSs are the human brain, social and economic organizations, Earth’s global climate, and ant colonies.

Complex adaptive systems are particular kinds of CSs that are able to learn from experience, change, and evolve. See Figure 1 for more details on the features of CASs. Although readers are familiar with some of these features in family systems theory, these features are reviewed and completed in order to understand the structure and function of the brain’s CAS involved in marital behaviors:

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Characteristics of complex adaptive systems (CAS) and how they work. A CAS is a system that consists of many adaptive agents which interact with each other and their external environment. The interaction of components leads to collective behaviors which are different/greater than the sum of the parts (emergence). CAS evolves over time.

2.1. Numerosity

To generate CSs, more than a handful of individual agents have to be engaged in many interactions (Ladyman et al., 2013; Ladyman and Wiesner, 2020).

2.2. Emergence

The CS has different properties from those of the sum of its elements. In the ant colonies, the behavior of each ant is relatively simple compared to what the system does as a whole, such as building bridges and nests and raising aphid “livestock.” No single ant will know to undertake such collective tasks on its own. Just as a neuron does not know what commitment is and how passionate love is. Such aggregate behaviors that appear unexpectedly are called “emergent behaviors.” Generally, it is these “emergent behaviors” which are aimed to be understood and changed. To achieve such a goal, the system’s components and how they interact to create these collective behaviors must be understood (Turner and Baker, 2019; Ladyman and Wiesner, 2020).

2.3. Non-linearity

Non-linearity is characteristic of systems in which a change in the size of the input will not make a proportionate change in the output (Ladyman et al., 2013).

2.4. Edge of chaos

The edge of chaos is a transition space between order and disorder. Think about a pile of sand. If one grain of sand is dropped on top of this pile every second, the pile gradually grows into the shape of a cone. When the pile is at the point that adding even one more grain of sand may dislocate some groups of sand grains, an avalanche is caused. If the motion of the dislodged group is sufficient, a cascade failure in some neighboring groups (subsystems) is generated. CSs naturally evolve toward this state and show a fine balance of sensitivity to perturbations and robust interactions (“self-organized criticality”). Although CS has stable states (attractors) at which it can maintain its stability even if perturbed, it may have unstable states at which a small perturbation can disrupt the system due to subtle interdependencies among its elements. CSs can be sensitive to the slightest disturbance showing the “butterfly effect,” which means that small alternations in the initial conditions may lead to very different results. Sometimes, minimal changes in the environment may entirely change the behavior of the system, known as tipping points, phase transitions, or bifurcations (Lewin, 1999).

2.5. Self-organization

There are no external or central organizers in CS. Instead, the “control” of the system is distributed among its agents and integrated via their interactions. In ant colonies, the queen does not order ants what to do. Instead, depending only on its local environment and the genetically encoded rules for its responsibility, each ant reacts (Heylighen, 2001).

2.6. Hierarchical organization

Complex systems are composed of subsystems (modules) within subsystems within subsystems, which facilitate high levels of functional specificity and the capacity of the system to persist under increasingly difficult conditions. Micro, meso, and macroeconomics in a country are examples of hierarchical organization (Ladyman et al., 2013).

2.7. Circular causality

Circular causality includes a continuous and concurrent bottom-up and top-down realization of emergence through self-organization. In this process, macro-level patterns are formed through the micro-level interactions (local-to-global determination), and macro-level patterns set boundaries for micro-level interactions (global-to-local determination) (Witherington, 2011).

2.8. Feedback

There is always negative (damping) and positive (amplifying) feedback in CS. In addition to function, feedback can change the structure of the CS (Ladyman et al., 2013; Turner and Baker, 2019).

2.9. Robustness

Complex systems are in a state between order and disorder and between segregation and integration. The CSs are stable under perturbations because their organization is distributed and not centrally produced, and their modules are independent while being integrated. For example, eliminating some of the members of a flock of birds does not destroy it (Ladyman et al., 2013).

2.10. Hysteresis

In CSs and many natural phenomena, the forward and reverse phase transition pathways are different. For instance, there are distinct temperatures for freezing water and melting ice. In other words, CS may have a memory, and the system is affected by it. Recovery after a critical transition can be more difficult than a simple return to the conditions of pre-phase transition. The history of a CS may thus be important (Fisher and Pruitt, 2019; Ladyman and Wiesner, 2020).

2.11. Adaptation

Complex system may adapt and evolve (CAS). These systems can often recover and adapt their prior functionality when their components are damaged or removed, and sometimes they can even improve through loss. This may be achieved through: the capacity to endure across disturbances (robustness), recover to the initial state following a considerable disturbance (resilience), or the capability to alter the system to survive and maintain its function (adaptation). One of the mechanisms that simultaneously leads to robustness, resilience, and adaptation is modularity because each module can both function and change its function without harmfully disturbing the rest of the system (Holland, 2006).

2.12. Co-evolution

As the environment that embeds the system changes, systems change to make sure they fit in, and again, the environment changes due to system alterations. CAS are dynamic systems that can learn and evolve from those changes, help affect their environment, predict likely changes, and prepare appropriately.

The brain is one of the most common examples of CAS in the complexity science literature that seems to have all of the CAS features. Accordingly, this article intends to use the science of CSs, neuroscience, and the science of marital relations to start a movement toward understanding the functions of the brain’s CAS in directing marital behaviors (Turner and Baker, 2019).

3. Brain’s CAS and marital behaviors

A CS consists of items (nodes) and pairwise relations (edges, ties, or links) among the items. The collections of these nodes with their ties are called graphs, networks, or modules (see Figure 2). Decades of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) experiments have identified functional modules as subsystems in the human brain (including different areas of the brain as nodes (that coactivate during certain types of tasks. Brain functions are the result of the operation of locally segregated modules with specialized functions that integrate as a whole to create perceptions, cognitions, and actions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic representation of the brain’s CAS networks. Brain’s CAS is a complex system consisting of a set of brain areas (nodes; colored points) and a set of pairwise connections between them (edges, ties, or links; lines). A specific set of highly correlated nodes and their ties are called networks, modules, or subsystems. The functions of the brain’s CAS involved in marital behaviors are the result of the local integration of brain areas into segregated modules for specific marital constructs (e.g., positive illusion) and the global integration of modules for directing ultimate marital behavior.

As an initial attempt, the fundamental question the researcher is looking to answer is how the brain’s CS is related to marital behaviors. If it can be acknowledged that the brain influences behavior through its mental states and constructs, the source of marital behaviors will be the constructs such as love, empathy, and commitment. Numerous pieces of evidence in the marital relationship literature confirm that these constructs are causally related to positive marital interactions (e.g., Gottman, 2014). On the other hand, studies in social cognitive neuroscience recently show that the psychological constructs such as empathy are the product of emergence that results from the interaction of specific social brain subsystems (Driver et al., 2012).

Therefore, considering that marital relationship makes demands and goals that are so unique and vital, it seems logical for us to have probably evolved specialized neural networks/subsystems (neurologic level), in which mental constructs (psychological level) such as love, positive illusion, and commitment are the product of emergences that arise from the interaction of these subsystems, and ultimately lead to behaviors such as warmth, finding excuses for partner’s transgressions, or reconciliation (behavioral level). To investigate such a possibility, first, essential psychological constructs that can be considered as the reason for positive marital behaviors must be extracted. Citing the literature on marital relationship studies gives us a long list of such constructs. How can a comprehensive and concise list of these constructs and psychological states be obtained?

According to action identification theory (Vallacher and Wegner, 1987), the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991), various hierarchical cognitive models (e.g., see Homer and Kahle, 1988; Dietz et al., 2005), and neurological evidence (e.g., see McCall et al., 2012; Spielberg et al., 2013), the representation of all of the people’s goal-directed actions (including marital behaviors) are organized in hierarchically in terms of abstraction that is causally related, wherein going to higher levels of the neuro-behavioral system is accompanied by reducing in the number of constructs and increasing in their abstraction and influence on behaviors. Figure 3 shows a summary of research in this area.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. “Why level” cognitive hierarchy. Summarizing previous studies on the causal hierarchy of cognitive constructs that leads to human behaviors, the above model is proposed. Psychological constructs that lead to one’s goal-directed behaviors (including marital behaviors) are organized within a hierarchy of abstractions that are causally related, wherein moving to higher levels is accompanied by a decrease in the number of constructs and an increase in their abstraction and impact on behaviors. Based on different theoretical approaches, some minor displacements in this hierarchy is not unexpected.

On the basis of action identification theory moving to a higher level of this neuro-behavioral system is equivalent to the “why” of human actions (motivational or “hot” domain: e.g., meaning, attitudes, motivations, and goals) and moving to lower levels is accompanied by the “how” of their actions (cold domain). At the “why level,” the critical determinant is how the individual views the goal object, while at lower levels, strategies and tactics to implement the higher-level demands are selected and implemented. In this system, lower levels are subservient to higher levels. Therefore, “forgiving your spouse for a small mistake” may be represented at higher levels of the system as “I want to protect my children’s mental health (purpose in life), or because I know that their mistake was not intentional (positive illusion)” and at the lowest level as “making a repair attempt by an apology or stop the negative cycle of interactions.”

In order to achieve a comprehensive and concise list of “why level” factors in marital behaviors, a field study has been conducted that had a mixed-method design – part of that was exploratory factor analysis – on 1,670 married individuals (see Supplementary material for details). This research was conducted as part of a series of our 12-year studies and research with the aim of extracting cognitive structures that cause positive marital behaviors from the highest level of the cognitive hierarchy.

According to the majority of approaches in marital relationship studies, marital satisfaction and stability are the two main elements that determine marital trajectories. According to Lewis and Spanier (1979), marriages are divided into four types: satisfied and stable, satisfied but unstable, dissatisfied but stable, and dissatisfied and unstable. In line with many analyses of marital relationship research by leading scholars, it can be concluded that symptoms of dissatisfied couples include more negative behaviors, fewer positive behaviors, and a negative reciprocity pattern (e.g., Driver et al., 2012).

Accordingly, 1,065 married individuals were interviewed to discover the existing themes in their responses to study questions: “(a) what is the source of your positive behaviors during your interactions, conflicts, and difficult moments of the marital relationship? Why do you continue constructive behaviors toward your spouse in such situations? (e.g., humor, empathy, responsiveness, forgiveness, and warmth), (b) When your spouse behaves negatively and destructively, what makes you not reciprocate negatively? (Two questions related to ‘why level’ factors of relationship quality) (c) Despite the conflicts and critical moments in marital life, what factors prevent you from moving toward separation and divorce? (A question related to ‘why level’ factors of relationship stability).” Thematic analysis (Braun and Clarke, 2006) was used to analyze the data obtained from written interviews, and main themes were identified from interviews. Based on the extracted themes, this section, by random selection of 605 married individuals, develops a valid (including Exploratory Factor Analysis) and reliable instrument to provide the possibility of measuring “why level” factors in similar settings (testability) (see Supplementary material for details).

The factors extracted from this study are listed below in Table 1. The findings of this study have strong support in the background of marriage studies (e.g., see Bachand and Caron, 2001), and numerous previous studies show that constructs such as love (Berscheid, 2010), marital satisfaction (Lavner et al., 2016), commitment (Menzies-Toman and Lydon, 2005), positive illusion (Murray et al., 1996), sexual satisfaction (Yeh et al., 2006), religion and spirituality (Day and Acock, 2013), forgiveness (Braithwaite et al., 2011), and other factors extracted from this study directly or indirectly (as motivations, positive attitudes, goals, etc.) play a vital role in the quality and stability of the marital relationship (e.g., see Bachand and Caron, 2001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The proposed brain’s subsystems/networks involved in marital behaviors.

Previous research shows that these relatively numerous factors impact the quality of marital behaviors. Also, previous studies confirm the interaction of these factors (for example, the interaction of marital satisfaction and forgiveness, see Allemand et al., 2007). Hence, it seems that these factors form a CS. Since these psychological constructs are the product of the brain, which itself is a CS, it seemed reasonable to assume that the networks of the brain, as corresponding neural transcripts, underlie these psychological constructs. Therefore, inspired by these factors and a comprehensive review of the neuroscience literature related to these constructs, the possible neural networks associated with each of these factors were listed (see Table 1). The first column on the left side of Table 1 shows that each neural network is inspired by which of the “why level” factors (a–o) of marital behaviors.

For example, inspired by one of these factors, the tendency to forgive, the brain forgiveness network, consisting of several subsystems whose part of their functions is associated with the tendency to forgive, has been introduced (see Li et al., 2017). Overall, the suggested brain networks in Table 1 include neural networks such as love, marital satisfaction, and positive illusion that are clearly related to marital behaviors and have a strong background in the marriage literature. Other suggested networks include networks such as goal-directed behavior networks and decision-making networks that were paid less attention to in the marriage studies, but some evidence suggests that they are related to marital behaviors (e.g., Ortigue et al., 2010; Heijne et al., 2018), and the results of the field section of the current study highlighted the need of paying attention to these networks. For example, making the proper staying/leaving decisions in a specific marital relationship requires the utilizing of different brain decision-making systems (Heijne et al., 2018) to consider different aspects of this critical decision (e.g., economic and moral aspects).

Regarding commitment, the researcher has tried to present the proposed networks in two ways: first, the neuroscience findings on the neural basis underlying monogamous pair bonding and studies on the relationship between commitment and executive control. Secondly, given that constructs such as commitment are very complex and multidimensional, and there are a variety of theories about them, researcher has tried to consider all the existing theories and then suggested possible neural networks based on the types of commitment. Finally, although there is some information about the neural networks related to some psychological constructs such as friendship (Acevedo et al., 2012a) and psychological needs satisfaction (Reeve and Lee, 2019), it seems that suggestions on the topology of the networks related to these constructs need more studies in the future.

Although most of the neural networks introduced in this article can be considered for relationships other than marital relationships, the current focus is on the marital relationship according to the statistical sample (spouses) and the target research literature.

It is assumed that the proposed networks (subsystems) in Table 1 interact with each other to form a CAS which guides marital behaviors. How this CAS relates to marital behaviors and the quality and stability of the marital relationship will be discussed in the next section of the article under the headings of the assumptions and propositions of BCM.

4. Theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors

This theory is a preliminary attempt to understand the CS of brain networks associated with marital behaviors. BCM is based on four underlying assumptions and five propositions derived from complexity theories, neuroscience, and the science of marital relationships. The following sections present and discuss these assumptions and propositions.

4.1. Assumptions

4.1.1. Like other human behaviors, the main origin of marital behaviors is the brain

Although contextual factors play an important role in marital behaviors as triggers or catalysts – and their role is considered in Proposition 5 – it is the brain that determines the final and adaptive behavior considering these contextual factors and depending on the resources it has (its unique structure and function in each individual), as two different people in the same context can behave differently.

4.1.2. The human brain influences marital behaviors and, ultimately, the quality and stability of a relationship through its psychological constructs such as love, marital satisfaction, commitment, empathy, etc.

In addition to the findings of current field research on “why level” factors of marital behaviors, the history of marital relationship science are full of evidence that shows that these abstract constructs may be considered the source of positive marital behaviors and the quality and stability of the marital relationship. For example, previous studies suggest that cognitions and behaviors that prevent the decline of relationship quality can possibly be motivated by fundamental constructs such as commitment (Menzies-Toman and Lydon, 2005) and marital satisfaction (Lavner et al., 2016). Similarly, marital therapists and ex-spouses often remark on the “death of love” as the most important reason for divorces or separations (Gigy and Kelly, 1993; Whisman et al., 1997).

4.1.3. The mentioned psychological constructs are created as a result of brain functions and their input (e.g., interactions with the spouse and learning)

All disciplines related to neuroscience and psychology concur that the mental states that lead to one’s behaviors are created by the specific functions of the brain. In addition, all of these disciplines acknowledge that the function of the brain is influenced by external inputs in order to provide an adaptive response to the environment.

4.1.4. The brain follows the rules of complex adaptive systems and biological rules of the brain

As mentioned earlier, the brain is perhaps the best example of a CAS in the literature on complexity science (e.g., see Ladyman et al., 2013). The second part of this assumption can be taken for granted.

Before introducing BCM’s propositions, it is important to get acquainted with its main concepts.

4.2. Concepts

4.2.1. Brain’s CAS involved in marital behaviors

The set of all subsystems of the brain that, as a CAS, directs marital behaviors.

4.2.2. Subsystem

The network that is composed of different areas of the brain and forms a functional module to participate in the creation of one of the marital constructs (e.g., the positive illusion subsystem; see Table 1).

4.2.3. Marital constructs

Abstract psychological constructs such as love, marital satisfaction, commitment, and positive illusion that can trigger and affect marital behaviors.

4.2.4. Marital behaviors

Behaviors that play a role in the quality and stability of the marital relationship include positive marital behaviors, such as empathetic behaviors, and negative marital behaviors, such as negative reciprocation.

4.2.5. The quality of the marital relationship

The quality of the marital relationship means the level of marital satisfaction of the couple or the happiness of the marriage.

4.2.6. The stability of the marital relationship

The stability of the marital relationship is equivalent to the degree of stableness and durability of marriage.

4.2.7. Quality of information and inputs

The quality of interactions with the spouse, learning, new skills, and information about the context of the marital relationship (e.g., socio-economic conditions, stressors, the existence of alternatives, and barriers) that enters the brain.

4.2.8. Structural architecture

Structural architecture refers to the quality of the structure of the brain’s networks involved in marital behaviors and their unique composition in one person.

4.2.9. Functional architecture

Functional architecture is the quality of functional interaction of the brain’s networks involved in marital behaviors (e.g., the quality of interaction between marital satisfaction and forgiveness networks) and their unique composition in one person.

4.2.10. Quality of recruitment of networks

The quality of using a brain’s networks efficiently in favor of the marital relationship.

4.3. Propositions

4.3.1. The marital constructs such as love, positive illusion, marital satisfaction, etc., are the product of emergences that result from the function of a network of specific areas of the brain (subsystems) and/or their interaction with other brain networks (proposition of modularity/segregation and emergence)

Today, primarily due to the success of cognitive neuroscience models, researchers know that people’s intellectual abilities emerge from the operation of neural systems that are instantiated by specific brain networks (see Driver et al., 2012). In the context of marital behaviors, is there any evidence for the phenomenon of emergence in the brain networks? Numerous studies confirm the role of positive illusion as one of the important constructs in triggering positive marital behaviors (Murray et al., 1996). Does the brain have a specific network or subsystem that emerges positive illusion? Song et al. (2018), reviewing the literature on relevant brain networks and areas involved in positive illusion, demonstrated how positive illusion might emerge from the interaction of certain parts of the brain (Song et al., 2018; see Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Positive illusion (seeing one’s partner generously) emergence in the brain and its subsystem. The caudate nucleus highlights the positive features of a romantic partner compared to negative features or other social comparisons. Decreased dACC activation suppresses the perception of negative characteristics of the partner. In the meantime, the dMPFC and vLPFC lessen the prominence of tempting rivals. Then the vACC intensifies the distinction between the favorite social features of intimate people and other people. Finally, when information is transmitted to the OFC, the weight of a partner’s positive and negative characteristics is re-assessed, reinforcing biased, subjective values (e.g., positive illusion) about the romantic partner—reprinted with permission (Song et al., 2018).

In this functional network, the caudate nucleus activation gives salience to the positive attributes of a romantic partner over the negative ones or other social comparisons. The dACC (see the footnote of Table 1 for abbreviations) has been linked to conflict monitoring, error detection, and social exclusion. Thus, decreased dACC activation suppresses the perception of a partner’s negative attributes. Increased vACC activation could boost the discrimination of favorable social qualities between beloved and non-intimate individuals. Simultaneously, the dMPFC and vLPFC could lower the superiority of charming rivals. Lastly, when information is transferred to the OFC, the evaluation of a partner’s positive and negative information is redistributed, cementing biased, subjective values (e.g., positive illusion) (see Figure 4). It appears that different areas of the brain that are active in this network have a specific job, just as distinct groups of workers in an ant colony, but their collective end product is completely different from the sum of their individual works. Notably, the above-mentioned regions of the brain do not function independently but interact with each other (synchronicity).

Similar patterns can be found in recent neuroscience findings for some other psychological constructs related to marital behaviors, such as forgiveness (Fourie et al., 2020), empathy (Zaki and Ochsner, 2012; Driver et al., 2012), romantic love, attachment, moral judgment (Driver et al., 2012), resilience (Yao and Hsieh, 2019), motivation (Spielberg et al., 2013), decision making (Basten et al., 2010), and reputation-based decision-making (Izuma, 2012).

4.3.2. The mentioned psychological constructs in circular causality processes are the sources and products of brain downstream processes and inputs/outputs such as marital behaviors

In the history of marital relationship studies, we are left with a seeming inconsistency: love is both blind (top-down causation) and deep-rooted in the real world (bottom-up causation). Previous observation reasonably suggests that in intimate relationships, reality and illusion go together in the creation of a happy and stable relationship. However, the mechanism of this process has remained an unsolved conundrum.

A similar question arises about the origin of empathy in interpersonal relationships. A brief description of the circular process of empathy formation at high levels of brain hierarchy and the subsequent realization of empathy at the behavioral level can be enlightening in this regard. Neurological studies indicate that the activation of an observer’s empathetic response is usually triggered by external cues, yet it has long been argued that contextual assessment (such as the relationship value), cognitive processes, and top-down control are key components of human empathy (empathy bias). This meta-cognitive level is regularly updated by bottom-up information and, conversely, controls the lower levels via top-down feedback, which makes the individual less dependent on external clues (Singer and Lamm, 2009).

In such circular causation, lower brain-mind functions are integrated (see emergence processes in proposition 1), considering higher-order mind-brain constraints (e.g., empathy bias, the blindness of love, biased selection, amplification, or inhibition of inputs), which then provide top-down regulatory control over downstream processes in brain and behaviors such as empathetic response (see Panksepp and Panksepp, 2013, for the details of processes for empathy).

In addition to empathy, there is ample neurological and psychological evidence of circular causation for other constructs such as love, marital satisfaction, resilience, positive illusions, and commitment (e.g., see Lavner et al., 2016; Ioannidis et al., 2020).

4.3.3. The brain’s subsystems form a complex adaptive system for guiding marital behaviors (proposition of collaboration or integration)

At the psychological level, there is significant evidence for the existence of a complex relationship between psychological constructs related to marital behaviors and their interaction with each other. For example, Chung (2014) showed that insecure attachments negatively affect marital satisfaction and forgiveness through a lack of empathy. Also, in the field of marital interventions and couple therapy, the failure of marital interventions due to the interference of higher-order cognitive constructs such as marital satisfaction and spouses’ affect has astonished researchers (see Gottman and Tabares (2018).

Signs of the presence of a complex neural system related to marital behaviors and interaction between its subsystems can be seen in experiments conducted to investigate the effect of holding a spouse’s hand compared to a stranger or no hand at all when spouses are threatened with an electric shock. Coan et al. (2006) found that holding the spouse’s hands in a threatening situation results in a pervasive decrease in the activation of threat-related neural networks, while this attenuation is more limited in the case of strangers. More interestingly, with the increase in the quality of the marital relationship, the calming effect of holding the spouse’s hand increased on neural threat responses (superior frontal gyrus, right anterior insula, and hypothalamus). Similarly, Coan et al. (2017) found that under the threat of shock, higher perceived social support is associated with lower neural activity in an extended networks related to regulatory self-control, salience, and vigilance, but only when holding a partner’s hand and not friends. Similar findings have been reported in psychological (Horn et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2021; Jakubiak, 2022) and neuroscience (Younger et al., 2010; Morriss et al., 2019) studies.

As an additional example, it can be useful to consider the connections of marital satisfaction networks with other neural networks and some of the possible effects of these interactions on marital behaviors. In the context of the romantic relationship and at the behavioral level, both trait forgiveness and relationship satisfaction are related to forgiveness behavior. When all three variables are considered at the same time, the interaction of lower levels of marital satisfaction of the hurt partners with their trait forgiveness may reverse the benefits of trait forgiveness; high trait forgiveness in satisfying relationships leads to the possible episodic forgiveness, but in dissatisfying relationships, it results in the least possible episodic forgiveness (Allemand et al., 2007).

To clarify such observations, considering the connections between the marital satisfaction network and the forgiveness network can be informative. Acevedo et al. (2012b), found that marital satisfaction is significantly correlated with activation in several brain areas, including the AI (associated with empathy); the VTA (reflecting reward and motivation); the OFC (related to the evaluation of rewards); the IFG (related to the mirror system), the BNST (related to stress control); and the PFC (related to affective regulation). Among the mentioned areas, PFC and OFC, as the two “connector hubs” (nodes that belong to several intersecting networks and link multiple modules) of the brain’s functional network (van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2013), overlap with the forgiveness network. Several other of these areas overlap with empathy, emotion regulation, and mentalizing networks that indirectly play a vital role in the forgiveness process. This integration of information may lead to an adaptive decision about forgiving the transgressor partner depending on the context and temperature of the relationship. Acevedo et al. (2012b) believe that these findings exemplify how perceived relationship satisfaction “evokes brain systems that influence choices and guide behaviors that may serve to promote relationship well-being and stability (p. 26).”

The marital satisfaction network overlaps with the vast majority of marital networks (see Table 1). Such a broad impact on the functional networks of the brain may explain phenomena such as sentiment override (Weiss, 1980), which means people, interpret their spouses’ behaviors as consistent with their current level of marital satisfaction rather than the partner’s actual behaviors. Similar to what came about marital satisfaction, there is ample evidence that the activity of brain regions supporting empathy is not fixed but may be modulated by contextual appraisal or personal motivations such as liking (see Weisz and Zaki, 2018, for review). Acknowledging the overlap of the empathy network and the other brain networks proposed in Table 1 can explain such findings. In this regard, social neuroscience findings indicate that there is a complex relationship between empathy and morality, and neural networks of empathy and morality meet each other in the vmPFC as a critical hub (Decety and Cowell, 2014).

4.3.4. The quality of marital interactions and the quality and stability of the marital relationship are influenced by the quality of the structural and functional architecture and the recruitment of the brain’s CAS involved in marital behaviors

The field is just at the beginning of the road of the neuroscience of marital behaviors, and part of a few existing findings in this area are usually correlational, and causal inferences need further research. However, there still are valuable findings that show the existence of individual differences in the mentioned domains and their possible impact on behaviors related to the marital relationship. For instance, Ma et al. (2022a) demonstrated that husbands’ differences in large-scale neural networks when they process their wives’ marital interactions may predict their variability in marital relationship quality 13 months later. Xu et al. (2012) found that the quality and stability of marriage until the 40th month of the relationship is predicted by lower activation of medial OFC, right accumbens, and right subcallosal cingulate during the early stages of love (Xu et al., 2012). Ueda et al. (2018) showed that VLPFC activity, which is implicated in executive control, can predict the stability of monogamous relationships in long-term relationships.

Recently Ma et al. (2022b) compared resting-state functional connectivity (FC) of couples with their FC during social information processing (watching relationship-specific and general emotional stimuli) and examined the relationship between their relationship quality and reconfiguration efficiency (these two FC similarities) 13 months later. Results indicated that the more easily wives can shift from a resting state to a relationship-specific information processing state (higher reconfiguration efficiency), the higher relationship quality they experienced 13 months later. The previous research shows that the pattern of brain function of people who have been married for an average of 21 years while viewing the facial images of their spouse is different than when they view the image of a close long-term friend, a highly familiar acquaintance, or a low-familiar person. The former involves recruiting areas of the brain related to reward and attachment. Also, the level of functioning of these parts of the brain was correlated with the love scores, its type and frequency of sexual activity (Acevedo et al., 2012a).

In addition to the neurological differences directly associated with the quality and stability of the marital relationship, neurological differences related to psychological constructs that may be the source of marital behaviors have also been observed. For example, structural imaging studies discovered that affective empathic abilities correlate negatively with gray matter volume (GMV) in the precuneus, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and anterior cingulate (Banissy et al., 2012).

Fincham et al. (2007) believe that the differences in the quality and stability of marriages are due to the existence of constructs such as forgiveness, commitment, sacrifice, and sanctification, which trigger transformational and homeostatic processes in the relationships, and therefore reduce negative retaliation responding across repetitive cycles of couples’ interactions (Fincham et al., 2007). It is likely that the individual differences in these internal resources are equivalent to the differences in the quality of structure, function, and recruitment of neural networks associated with these psychological constructs. For example, Li et al. (2017) showed that there are associations between higher tendency to forgive (TTF) scores and larger GMV in the areas of dlPFC and smaller GMV in the areas of the IFG and right insular cortex. In addition, higher TTF scores are associated with the smaller white matter volume in the left IFG.

Theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors proposes that, in addition to structure and function, the quality of recruitment of brain networks also affects the quality of marital interactions and their consequences. For example, neurological studies show that the quality of theory-of-mind (ToM) network recruitment can determine the outcomes of empathy and mentalizing exercises. Partners might consider a wide range of actions representative of ToM (e.g., nodding), while there is neurological evidence that suggests only in certain cases do such behaviors are indicative of actual involvement of brain regions that are related to the ToM (e.g., nodding and considering the point of view of partner, compared with nodding while discretely checking phone notifications). Dodell-Feder et al. (2016) found that only high neural selectivity in the left temporoparietal junction (LTPJ) and precuneus (the neural network supporting ToM) for beliefs versus physical qualities of the partner is significantly related to the wellbeing of the partner, up to two days after a conflict. Remarkably no association was found between perceived understanding and subsequent partner’s wellbeing in the case of low activation of LTPJ and precuneus for belief information.

Correspondingly, an fMRI study on forgiveness showed that costly apologies (as a signal of conciliatory intention) significantly activate the ToM network (i.e., precuneus, bilateral TPJ, and mPFC), unlike non-costly apologies (i.e., only saying “sorry”), and non-apologies. Importantly, Ohtsubo et al. (2018) reported that no significant difference in brain responses to non-apology controls and non-costly apologies was observed. Also, for forgiveness in which people experience emotional concern for their transgressors, activation in the inferior parietal lobule is selective, a process that facilitates empathy with the offender (see Ricciardi et al., 2013, for review).

4.3.5. The quality of information and inputs received and processed by the brain’s CAS involved in marital behaviors affects the quality and stability of the marital relationship

According to BCM, inputs, including the information about the quality of a partner and couple’s interactions (equivalent to what behavioral theories emphasize), the value of a relationship, alternatives, and barriers (social exchange theory), the fulfillment of needs quality (attachment theory) or the stressors (crisis theory and VSA model) is monitored, stored and updated in the brain networks and ultimately affects the spouses’ interactions and the quality and stability of their relationship.

In this regard, neurological studies indicate that the partner and relationship value is upregulated in response to the partner’s pro-relationship behaviors, and the mOFC – which as a hub, contributes to various systems – is involved in this recalibration process (Ohtsubo et al., 2020). Furthermore, the partner’s positive reciprocity robustly engages the ventral striatum and OFC (Phan et al., 2010). This reciprocity signal in these areas emerges only in reaction to partners who have consistently returned the investment and do not exist for partners who do not have a reputation for reciprocity. A previous study has found that having a greater amount of positive experience in a relationship before a trust breach helps with the recovery of trust—even after trust breaches—by activating the automatic social cognition neural system (or X-system: habitualized, more automatic, and less reflective system) (Schilke et al., 2013). Similarly, higher perceived support from the partner activates specific areas of the brain, which simultaneously trigger down-regulation of physiological response to stress (Eisenberger, 2013), and according to the VSA model and crisis theory, any resource that can act as a buffer against stress can improve the quality and stability of a marital relationship (Karney and Bradbury, 1995).

In addition, if concurrent marital satisfaction is considered a significant representative of the quality of marital interactions (Gottman and Krokoff, 1989), different brain systems associated with empathy, emotion regulation, decision-making, reward and motivation, and stress control can be evoked by marital satisfaction (Acevedo et al., 2012b) which ultimately can affect the quality of marital behaviors, conflict management, and relationship wellbeing and stability. This is consistent with the VSA model of marriage, which believes that the quality and stability of a relationship are directly influenced by relationship satisfaction, which interacts with the coping processes or problem-solving skills of the partners (Karney and Bradbury, 1995).

Finally, due to the subjectivity of perceptions and top-down control of the nervous system, it is believed that (in selecting, amplifying, modifying, or inhibiting inputs), the effectiveness of the inputs (including couple interactions) depends on the quality of the architecture of the individual’s neurocognitive organization.

5. BCM and existing theories

At the same time that BCM is consistent with existing theories, it has the potential to address part of the complexities of marital behaviors beyond the scope of extant theories. For example, unlike behavioral models, BCM considers the underlying neurocognitive sources of marital interactions (e.g., positive illusion, marital satisfaction, and resilience). BCM can also explain the phenomenon of marital self-repair as one of the CAS’s characteristics (self-organization and adaptation). Fincham et al. (2007) suggest that such self-repair processes can be realized via “positive, meaning-related constructs” such as forgiveness, sacrifice, commitment, and sanctification (Fincham et al., 2007). In line with this assumption, BCM can explain this process by introducing a complex neurocognitive system part of its subsystems is forgiveness, commitment, religion, and spirituality. Also, BCM has the potential to explain why some couples with negative interaction patterns continue their relationship despite long-term conflicts. In situations where the subsystems of marital satisfaction and love are inactive because of successive conflicts or the low rate of positive interactions, the presence of other subsystems, such as hope, resilience, religiosity, etc., can stabilize the marriage (robustness, resilience, and adaptation of CAS).

Theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors also has this strength of behavioral theory that describes marriage as a dynamic phenomenon and believes that marital quality and stability are justified by each satisfying interaction, making further satisfying interactions more likely. As mentioned earlier, parts of the brain networks involved in marital behaviors regularly update and recalibrate the value of the partner and relationship based on information received every day from the partner’s behaviors. In this respect, BCM and behavioral theory provide explanations for the dynamic change in the marriage that are not considered in the perspective of social exchange theory. From the perspective of social exchange theory, factors such as religion, commitment, and children are considered barriers to marital dissolution just at the moment of deciding to maintain or dissolve the marital relationship. Whereas in the BCM, these factors, in addition to stability, can be sources of satisfaction and quality of the relationship and motivating factors to create positive behaviors in every moment of the marital relationship and therefore have a dynamic role.

Furthermore, BCM, in contrast with social exchange theory, has the potential to explain how different people may behave differently in low reward conditions as a result of psychological characteristics and constructs such as resilience. Lawler and Thye (1999) believe that “the actors of exchange theories are normally viewed as individualistic, instrumental, and unemotional, whereas those of emotion theories are socially oriented, expressive, and emotionally deep and complex. The former is driven by reason, the latter by passion (Lawler and Thye, 1999, p. 33).” In contrast, BCM considers the human as a multidimensional being who simultaneously has a passion (e.g., love) and reason (e.g., economic decision-making), and the integrated information from the interaction of all the neural networks forms the final emerged marital behaviors and decisions. In this regard, fMRI studies performed to examine activated brain regions when deciding to maintain or dissolve a relationship indicate that the remaining decision goes above and beyond the objective and economic value of staying (Heijne et al., 2018).

Theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors also seems to have the capacity to address the gap in crisis theory and the VSA model regarding the details of adaptation resources, adaptive processes, and personal characteristics, which act as buffers against stress. It seems that the quality of structure, function, and recruitment of neural networks introduced in the BCM, the psychological construct emerging from these networks, and their potential cognitive and behavioral implications are theoretically related to adaptive processes. In addition, according to Hobfoll (1989), everything that people value can be a resource for adjusting to stressful life events and increasing compatibility with critical situations. Therefore according to the previous studies (e.g., see Eisenberger, 2013; Basińska and Sołtys, 2020), resilience, hope, perceived support, religiosity, and spirituality directly, or anything that can activate the motivational system, goal-directed behavior system, and/or decision-making systems (such as having children), directly or indirectly, have the potential to play a significant role in coping with stress. Also, BCM, by including love, sex, perceived support, and marital satisfaction subsystems, addresses the concerns of attachment theories, which emphasize the satisfaction of needs and sexual gratification as determinants of stability and quality of the relationship.

Finally, psychological capitals such as hope have no decisive role in existing theories of marital relationship, whereas hope for future satisfaction is a more important factor in determining the quality and stability of marriage than current relationship conditions (Baker et al., 2017). By emphasizing important constructs and resources such as hope, forgiveness, religion, spirituality, and resilience, BCM brings them from the margin to the center.

6. Testability of the theory: Replicability and falsifiability

Replicability and falsifiability are two of the main criteria of a scientific theory (e.g., see Earp and Trafimow, 2015). A study is replicable when a replication study of it (utilizing sufficiently similar methods under sufficiently similar circumstances) can be carried out (Peels, 2019). A scientific theory must be falsifiable in the sense that it can be logically contradicted through an empirical test utilizing existing technologies (see Earp and Trafimow, 2015, for review).

It seems that the replicability of BCM can be discussed on three levels. Level (a): the field part of the present study, which led to the extraction of the list of higher-order cognitive factors (e.g., love and commitment) effective on marital behaviors. Level (b): the attribution of these factors to the corresponding neural networks. And level (c): all of the neurological–psychological studies mentioned by other researchers that potentially support this theory and its propositions. Details of the methodology for replication at the (a) level are available in Supplementary material of the article. In addition, the inductive approach in the field part of the study was completed simultaneously by the deductive methods (see Supplementary material for details) to align with the repeated findings in the literature of marital relationship studies. Therefore findings in the field section of the study are not surprising or new. Different types of studies conducted in the history of marriage studies in a relatively similar way to the field part of this research (e.g., see Bachand and Caron (2001); and Asoodeh et al. (2010), as review studies (Billingsley et al., 2005; Karimi et al., 2019), or research which have studied each factor independently (Murray et al., 1996; Menzies-Toman and Lydon, 2005; Yeh et al., 2006; Berscheid, 2010; Braithwaite et al., 2011; Day and Acock, 2013; Lavner et al., 2016), have obtained similar and consistent findings to field section of present study indicating the validity and replicability of the results.

Regarding the replicability at level 2 (attribution of psychological constructs to neural networks), it seems that other researchers also act similarly regarding the neural networks introduced in the case of constructs such as love, marital satisfaction, and positive illusion. However, the replicability of how neural networks are introduced in the case of constructs such as commitment is debatable. In level 3, the available neuroscience evidence about some neural networks (for example, empathy and love) has reached a significant degree of consistency, but about others, it raises the possibility that future research will cause changes in the topology of the networks presented in this article.

In order to test and falsify BCM theory and its propositions, it is feasible to utilize MRI studies, fMRI studies, brain lesions studies, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of a specific area of the brain which interferes with its activity and provides a causal inference (virtual lesion), or other neurological instruments and assessments and analyzing and relating the results obtained from these methods with behavioral observations of couples or psychological assessments (such as measuring their love, empathy, tendency to forgive, marital satisfaction, etc.) using valid and reliable questionnaires. In order to falsify BCM theory, it should be shown that the mentioned neural networks (e.g., empathy network) are unrelated to marital behaviors (e.g., empathy with a spouse). It should also be shown that there is no interaction between the neural networks mentioned in BCM theory (for example, between the empathy network and the moral decision-making network – falsifiability of the theory and proposition 3). It seems that the evidence available in the field of marital relationships or similar settings not only indicates the testability of this theory but also confirms it. For example, refer to the studies mentioned in Table 1 for the possibility of investigation of the relationship between the mentioned neural networks and behavior. Also, the studies conducted to investigate the interaction between the mentioned neural networks and their effect on behavior are available in similar settings (e.g., Decety and Cowell (2014).

To falsify proposition 1, it should be shown that the collective behavior of the agents of a specific neural network (for example, in the love or moral decision-making system) is unrelated to their love, quality of marital interactions or spouses’ decisions, or that the function of individual elements of these networks is not different from the collective behavior of networks as a whole (definition of emergence; Holland, 2002). For example, it should be indicated that real or virtual lesions (by rTMS) in one or a number of nodes of these networks do not affect the networks’ collective behavior. Such testability is feasible and has been experienced in similar or related contexts, and the findings are in line with confirming this proposition. For instance, Knoch et al. (2006) showed that virtual lesions (by rTMS) in dlPFC influenced the participants’ decisions to accept their partner’s unfair offers. Damasio et al. (1990) have documented the case of a patient (E.V.R.) who had a happy and stable marriage and a successful professional life prior to his orbitofrontal lesion because of meningioma and its inevitable surgery. After the surgery, his social behavior changed profoundly; he divorced twice and went bankrupt in his professional life (note Table 1 and the extensive roles of the OFC as a hub). Also, the study of patients with real brain damage in different areas of the brain related to empathy shows that changes in empathy as an integrated construct differ depending on the location of the brain lesions (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009). It means that empathy, as a whole construct, is the product of the integrated function of all elements of the empathy network and is different from the function of individual elements or a limited number of them.

To falsify other propositions, it should be indicated that effective environmental inputs such as positive marital behaviors or psychological interventions have no effect on this CAS (falsifiability of propositions 2 and 5) and this possible obtained effect at the neurological level does not extend to marital behaviors at the same time (falsifiability of proposition 2). It seems that such experiments have been done, and it shows that these propositions can be testable/falsifiable, and the results obtained so far confirm these propositions. For instance, one of these studies shows that satisfying sexual activity and its frequency in marital relationships is associated with the activation of cortical areas that mediate empathy, self−other processes and complex thinking, as well as subcortical areas which support basic physiological and motivational processes (Acevedo et al., 2019). Similar findings regarding the significant and positive effect of some simple and short-term learning experiences on the empathy and function of parts of the empathy neural network (anterior insular cortex) (Hein et al., 2016) are available.

It should also be shown that the quality and stability of the marital relationship are unrelated to the structure, function and type of recruitment of this CAS and its neural networks (falsifiability of proposition 4). Recent studies suggest the possibility of such assessments. In this regard, some studies show that differences in the structure, function and type of recruitment of brain networks are associated with different outcomes in the quality and stability of marital relationships (Xu et al., 2012; Dodell-Feder et al., 2016; Ueda et al., 2018; Ma et al., 2022a,b).

7. Practical implications

Theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors can have therapeutic, educational, and research implications in the field. In other words, the utilization of the brain’s and CSs language in the design of interventions and therapies can improve achievements in this area. For instance, discovering the details of the structure and function of the subsystems of the brain’s CAS involved in marital behaviors (e.g., positive illusion: see Figure 4 and its caption) can lead to the design of relevant interventions and therapies. In addition, this approach can also trigger research on developing researchers’ knowledge about the structure of the brain’s CAS involved in marital behaviors. Also, discovering the connections and interactions between the brain’s subsystems [e.g., between marital satisfaction and forgiveness (Allemand et al., 2007) or between moral decision-making and empathy (Decety and Cowell, 2014)] can lead to considering these interactions in designing interventions and therapies. Heino et al. (2021) believe that behavior change interventions without considering the rules governing CSs “is akin to attempts to work against gravity, which pulls a ball to the bottom of a valley (p. 4).”

Furthermore, today the vast majority of the interventions and therapies in the field have mainly focused on conflict resolution and communication skills. However, longitudinal studies indicate that this approach has not led to promising results (Karney and Bradbury, 2020). Whereas, based on BCM, it seems that paying attention to constructs such as forgiveness, hope, resilience, spirituality, religiosity, and morality, is necessary while considering communication and conflict management skills to improve marital relations. In this regard, future interventions and therapies may have the potential to utilize neurobehavioral networks that already exist in people (e.g., people’s spirituality) and have not yet been tapped to benefit the relationship, or improve the structure and function of the brain’s CAS in favor of the relationship, relying on the plasticity of the social brain (Valk et al., 2017). BCM also raises the potential value of research on the practicability of using non-invasive neurological interventions such as transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) to improve marital relations, which its practicability has recently been discussed at a theoretical level (Liu et al., 2019).

8. Conclusion

This article, relying upon the science of complexity, neuroscience, a vast body of evidence in the history of marital relationship studies, and significant qualitative and quantitative findings from the study of 1,670 married individuals, presented the BCM. In the initial stages of the research, the author sought to find higher-order cognitive factors as “why level” factors of marital behaviors. The findings of the field section of the study resulted in a set of well-known cognitive constructs in the research literature on marital relationships, such as love, commitment, marital satisfaction, and other motivational factors. Analyzing the complex relationships between these factors, their complex relationships with marital behaviors, the existence of hierarchy in this cognitive-behavioral organization, and the interaction between these factors suggested the possibility of the existence of a CS consisting of these factors. Since these factors and their related processes are the product of the brain and the brain is a CS, the researcher investigated the possibility that this complex cognitive-behavioral system is an abstract version of a CS consisting of specific neural networks. Therefore the study of the neuroscience literature on the neural networks attributable to these extracted psychological factors led to the presentation of the BCM theory.

Although there is a wide background of knowledge on the relationship between the brain and behavior in psychology and neuroscience literature, BCM, for the first time, explained the relationship between the brain and a complex behavior such as marital behavior from the perspective of complexity science. Being on the rails of CSs will cause fundamental changes in the science of marital relationships because the laws governing CSs fundamentally differ from the laws of simple or even complicated systems and reductionistic perspectives.

It seems that integrating the science of complexity, neuroscience, and the science of marital relationships can lead to the emergence of novel capabilities that facilitate the evolution of marital relations science. Future findings in neuroscience and the science of marital relationships can lead to the completion and adjustment of some of the networks presented in this article. Future research will be utilizing BCM in the fields of education, prevention, and therapy. This article may inspire interdisciplinary studies of marital relationships, CSs, and neuroscience.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Acknowledgments

The author grateful to Farnaz Anari, Mohammad Erfan Nikrahan, Hosein Sarami, Saeed Jamshidi (preparing figures), Dr. Mohammad Reza Abedi, Dr. Hosein Ali Mehrabi, Dr. Maryam B. Kermani, Daniyal Nasr, Zahra Mohseni, Dr. Hosein Molavi, Dr. Fatemeh Bahrami, Dr. Farshad Bahari, Dr. Amin Nematollahi, Dr. Farzaneh Fayazpour, Dr. Afshin Montakhab, Dr. Keyvan Aghababaei Samani, Dr. Farhad Shahbazi, Mohammad Sadegh Sajjadzadeh, Leila Landi Esfahani, Haajar Nasr Esfahani, Dr. Heydari, Dr. Taghi Javadani, Dr. Mojtaba Jahanifar, Dr. Vahid Ghasemi, Dr. Mark Fine, Mahdi Eslami, and all of the study participants, experts, family education centers, and counseling centers that participated in the field section of this research.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1050164/full#supplementary-material

References

Acevedo, B. P., Aron, A., Fisher, H. E., and Brown, L. L. (2012a). Neural correlates of long-term intense romantic love. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 145–159. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Acevedo, B. P., Aron, A., Fisher, H. E., and Brown, L. L. (2012b). Neural correlates of marital satisfaction and well-being: reward, empathy, and affect. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 9, 20–31.

Google Scholar

Acevedo, B. P., Poulin, M. J., Geher, G., Grafton, S., and Brown, L. L. (2019). The neural and genetic correlates of satisfying sexual activity in heterosexual pair-bonds. Brain Behav. 9:e01289. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allemand, M., Amberg, I., Zimprich, D., and Fincham, F. D. (2007). The role of trait forgiveness and relationship satisfaction in episodic forgiveness. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 26, 199–217. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2007.26.2.199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asoodeh, M. H., Khalili, S., Daneshpour, M., and Lavasani, M. G. (2010). Factors of successful marriage: accounts from self described happy couples. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 5, 2042–2046. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bachand, L. L., and Caron, S. L. (2001). Ties that bind: a qualitative study of happy long-term marriages. Contemp. Family Therapy 23, 105–121. doi: 10.1023/A:1007828317271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, L. R., McNulty, J. K., and VanderDrift, L. E. (2017). Expectations for future relationship satisfaction: unique sources and critical implications for commitment. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 146, 700–721. doi: 10.1037/xge0000299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banissy, M. J., Kanai, R., Walsh, V., and Rees, G. (2012). Inter-individual differences in empathy are reflected in human brain structure. Neuroimage 62, 2034–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basińska, M. A., and Sołtys, M. (2020). Personal resources and flexibility in coping with stress depending on perceived stress in a group of cancer patients. Health Psychol. Rep. 8, 107–119. doi: 10.5114/hpr.2020.93781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basten, U., Biele, G., Heekeren, H. R., and Fiebach, C. J. (2010). How the brain integrates costs and benefits during decision making. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 107, 21767–21772. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908104107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berscheid, E. (2010). Love in the fourth dimension. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billingsley, S., Lim, M.-G., Caron, J., and Harris, A. (2005). Historical overview of criteria for marital and family success. Family Therapy 32, 1–14. doi: 10.4102/curationis.v23i4.736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braithwaite, S. R., Selby, E. A., and Fincham, F. D. (2011). Forgiveness and relationship satisfaction: mediating mechanisms. J. Fam. Psychol. 25, 551–559. doi: 10.1037/a0024526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosch, T., and Sander, D. (2013). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying value-based decision-making: from core values to economic value. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:398. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carnevali, L., Koenig, J., Sgoifo, A., and Ottaviani, C. (2018). Autonomic and brain morphological predictors of stress resilience. Front. Neurosci. 12:228. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, M.-S. (2014). Pathways between attachment and marital satisfaction: the mediating roles of rumination, empathy, and forgiveness. Pers. Individual Differ. 70, 246–251. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, M. S., and Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange and communal relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 37, 12–24. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.1.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coan, J. A., Beckes, L., Gonzalez, M. Z., Maresh, E. L., Brown, C. L., and Hasselmo, K. (2017). Relationship status and perceived support in the social regulation of neural responses to threat. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1574–1583.

Google Scholar

Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., and Davidson, R. J. (2006). Lending a hand: social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychol. Sci. 17, 1032–1039. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coria-Avila, G. A., Herrera-Covarrubias, D., Ismail, N., and Pfaus, J. G. (2016). The role of orgasm in the development and shaping of partner preferences. Socioaffect. Neurosci. Psychol. 6:31815. doi: 10.3402/snp.v6.31815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., and Damasio, H. (1990). Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 41, 81–94. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90144-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, R. D., and Acock, A. (2013). Marital well-being and religiousness as mediated by relational virtue and equality. J. Marriage Family 75, 164–177.

Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Cowell, J. M. (2014). The complex relation between morality and empathy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 337–339.

Google Scholar

Dietz, T., Fitzgerald, A., and Shwom, R. (2005). Environmental values. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 30, 335–372.

Google Scholar

Dodell-Feder, D., Felix, S., Yung, M. G., and Hooker, C. I. (2016). Theory-of-mind-related neural activity for one’s romantic partner predicts partner well-being. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 593–603. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doré, B. P., Zerubavel, N., and Ochsner, K. N. (2015). “Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core systems,” in APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology, Volume 1: Attitudes and Social Cognition, eds M. Mikulincer, P. R. Shaver, E. Borgida, and J. A. Bargh (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association).

Google Scholar

Driver, J., Tabares, A., Shapiro, A. F., and Gottman, J. M. (2012). “Couple interaction in happy and unhappy marriages,” in Gottman laboratory studies: Normal family processes: Growing diversity and complexity, ed. F. Walsh (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 57–77.

Google Scholar

Earp, B. D., and Trafimow, D. (2015). Replication, falsification, and the crisis of confidence in social psychology. Front. Psychol. 6:621. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I. (2013). An empirical review of the neural underpinnings of receiving and giving social support: implications for health. Psychosom. Med. 75, 545–556. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31829de2e7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., de Greck, M., and Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a core neural network in empathy? an fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 903–911. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fincham, F. D., Stanley, S. M., and Beach, S. R. (2007). Transformative processes in marriage: An analysis of emerging trends. J. Marriage Fam. 69, 275–292. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00362.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, D. N., and Pruitt, J. N. (2019). Insights from the study of complex systems for the ecology and evolution of animal populations. Curr. Zool. 66, 1–14. doi: 10.1093/cz/zoz016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fourie, M. M., Hortensius, R., and Decety, J. (2020). Parsing the components of forgiveness: Psychological and neural mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 437–451. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gigy, L., and Kelly, J. B. (1993). Reasons for divorce: perspectives of divorcing men and women. J. Divorce Remarriage 18, 169–188. doi: 10.1300/J087v18n01_08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottman, J. M. (2014). Principia Amoris: the New Science of Love. Milton park: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203081785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottman, J. M., and Krokoff, L. J. (1989). Marital interaction and satisfaction: a longitudinal view. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 57, 47–52. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.57.1.47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottman, J. M., and Tabares, A. (2018). The effects of briefly interrupting marital conflict. J. Marital Fam. Ther. 44, 61–72. doi: 10.1111/jmft.12243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkins, D. N., and Booth, A. (2005). Unhappily ever after: effects of long-term, low-quality marriages on well-being. Soc. Forces 84, 451–471. doi: 10.1353/sof.2005.0103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazan, C., and Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 511–524. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.3.511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazan, C., and Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational framework for research on close relationships. Psychol. Inquiry 5, 1–22. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0501_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heijne, A., Rossi, F., and Sanfey, A. G. (2018). Why we stay with our social partners: neural mechanisms of stay/leave decision-making. Soc. Neurosci. 13, 667–679. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2017.1370010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, G., Engelmann, J. B., Vollberg, M. C., and Tobler, P. N. (2016). How learning shapes the empathic brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 80–85. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1514539112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heino, M. T. J., Knittle, K., Noone, C., Hasselman, F., and Hankonen, N. (2021). Studying behaviour change mechanisms under complexity. Behav. Sci. 11:77.

Google Scholar

Heylighen, F. (2001). The science of self-organization and adaptivity. Encyclopedia Life Support Syst. 5, 253–280. doi: 10.1023/A:1009690407292

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, R. (1949). Families under stress: Adjustment to the crises of war separation and reunion. New York, NY: Harper & Brothers.

Google Scholar

Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: a new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am. Psychol. 44, 513–524. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holland, J. H. (2002). “Complex adaptive systems and spontaneous emergence,” in Complexity and Industrial Clusters: Dynamics and Models in Theory and Practice, eds A. Q. Curzio and M. Fortis (Berlin: Springer).

Google Scholar

Holland, J. H. (2006). Studying complex adaptive systems. J. Syst. Sci. Complexity 19, 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s11424-006-0001-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Homer, P. M., and Kahle, L. R. (1988). A structural equation test of the value-attitude-behavior hierarchy. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 638–646. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horn, A. B., Samson, A. C., Debrot, A., and Perrez, M. (2019). Positive humor in couples as interpersonal emotion regulation: a dyadic study in everyday life on the mediating role of psychological intimacy. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 36, 2376–2396.

Google Scholar

Ioannidis, K., Askelund, A. D., Kievit, R. A., and van Harmelen, A. L. (2020). The complex neurobiology of resilient functioning after childhood maltreatment. BMC Med. 18:32. doi: 10.1186/s12916-020-1490-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izuma, K. (2012). The social neuroscience of reputation. Neurosci. Res. 72, 283–288.

Google Scholar

Jakubiak, B. K. (2022). Affectionate touch in satisfying and dissatisfying romantic relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 39, 2287–2315.

Google Scholar

Kapogiannis, D., Deshpande, G., Krueger, F., Thornburg, M. P., and Grafman, J. H. (2014). Brain networks shaping religious belief. Brain Connect 4, 70–79. doi: 10.1089/brain.2013.0172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karimi, R., Bakhtiyari, M., and Arani, A. M. (2019). Protective factors of marital stability in long-term marriage globally: a systematic review. Epidemiol. Health 41:e2019023. doi: 10.4178/epih.e2019023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karney, B. R., and Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: a review of theory, method, and research. Psychol. Bull. 118, 3–34. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karney, B. R., and Bradbury, T. N. (2020). Research on marital satisfaction and stability in the 2010s: Challenging conventional wisdom. J. Marr. Fam. 82, 100–116.

Google Scholar

Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., and Fehr, E. (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science 314, 829–832. doi: 10.1126/science.1129156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladyman, J., Lambert, J., and Wiesner, K. (2013). What is a complex system? Eur. J. Philos. Sci. 3, 33–67.

Google Scholar

Ladyman, J., and Wiesner, K. (2020). What is a Complex System?. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Google Scholar

Lavner, J. A., Karney, B. R., and Bradbury, T. N. (2016). Does couples’ communication predict marital satisfaction, or does marital satisfaction predict communication? J. Marriage Fam. 78, 680–694.

Google Scholar

Lawler, E. J., and Thye, S. R. (1999). Bringing emotions into social exchange theory. Annu. Rev. Soc. 25, 217–244.

Google Scholar

Levin, I. P., Xue, G., Weller, J. A., Reimann, M., Lauriola, M., and Bechara, A. (2012). A neuropsychological approach to understanding risk-taking for potential gains and losses. Front. Neurosci. 6:15. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levinger, G. (1976). A social psychological perspective on marital dissolution. J. Soc. Iss. 32, 21–47.

Google Scholar

Levinger, G. (1965). Marital cohesiveness and dissolution: An integrative review. J. Marr. Fam. 27, 19–28.

Google Scholar

Lewin, R. (1999). Complexity: Life at the Edge of Chaos. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Lewis, R. A., and Spanier, G. B. (1979). “Theorizing about the quality and stability of marriage,” in Contemporary Theories About the Family, eds W. Burr, R. Hill, and I. Nye (New York, NY: Free Press). doi: 10.1037/fam0000217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Chen, Q., Lu, J., and Qiu, J. (2017). Brain structural bases of tendency to forgive: evidence from a young adults sample using voxel-based morphometry. Sci. Rep. 7:16856. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16868-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 259–289.

Google Scholar

Liu, D., Piao, Y., Ma, R., Zhang, Y., Guo, W., Zuo, L., et al. (2021). Actor and partner effects of touch: touch-induced stress alleviation is influenced by perceived relationship quality of the couple. Front. Psychol. 12:661438. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.661438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, S., Ma, R., Liu, X., Zhang, C., Chen, Y., Jin, C., et al. (2019). Using transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) to improve romantic relationships can be a promising approach. Front. Psychol. 10:365. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, S.-S., Zhang, J.-T., Song, K.-R., Zhao, R., Fang, R.-H., Wang, L.-B., et al. (2022a). Connectome-based prediction of marital quality in husbands’ processing of spousal interactions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 17, 1055–1067. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsac034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, S.-S., Zhang, J.-T., Wang, L.-B., Song, K.-R., Yao, S.-T., Fang, R.-H., et al. (2022b). Efficient brain connectivity reconfiguration predicts higher marital quality and lower depression. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 17, 323–335. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCall, C., Tipper, C. M., Blascovich, J., and Grafton, S. T. (2012). Attitudes trigger motor behavior through conditioned associations: neural and behavioral evidence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 841–849. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCubbin, H. I., and Patterson, J. M. (1982). “Family adaptation to crises,” in Family stress, coping and social support, eds H. McCubbin, A. Cauble, and J. Patterson (Springfield, IL: Thomas), 26–47.

Google Scholar

Menzies-Toman, D. A., and Lydon, J. E. (2005). Commitment-motivated benign appraisals of partner transgressions: do they facilitate accommodation? J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 22, 111–128.

Google Scholar

Morriss, J., Bell, T., Johnstone, T., Van Reekum, C. M., and Hill, J. (2019). Social domain based modulation of neural responses to threat: the different roles of romantic partners versus friends. Soc. Neurosci. 14, 398–408. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2018.1486735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., and Griffin, D. W. (1996). The self-fulfilling nature of positive illusions in romantic relationships: love is not blind, but prescient. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 1155–1180. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.6.1155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oba, K., Noriuchi, M., Atomi, T., Moriguchi, Y., and Kikuchi, Y. (2016). Memory and reward systems coproduce ‘nostalgic’ experiences in the brain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1069–1077. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohtsubo, Y., Matsunaga, M., Himichi, T., Suzuki, K., Shibata, E., Hori, R., et al. (2020). Role of the orbitofrontal cortex in the computation of relationship value. Soc. Neurosci. 15, 600–612.

Google Scholar

Ohtsubo, Y., Matsunaga, M., Tanaka, H., Suzuki, K., Kobayashi, F., Shibata, E., et al. (2018). Costly apologies communicate conciliatory intention: an fMRI study on forgiveness in response to costly apologies. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 249–256.

Google Scholar

Ortigue, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Patel, N., Frum, C., and Lewis, J. W. (2010). Neuroimaging of love: fMRI meta-analysis evidence toward new perspectives in sexual medicine. J. Sex Med. 7, 3541–3552. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2010.01999.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Panksepp, J. B. (2013). Toward a cross-species understanding of empathy. Trends Neurosci. 36, 489–496. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peels, R. (2019). Replicability and replication in the humanities. Res. Integrity Peer Rev. 4, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F., and Friston, K. J. (2018). Hierarchical active inference: a theory of motivated control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 294–306.

Google Scholar

Phan, K. L., Sripada, C. S., Angstadt, M., and McCabe, K. (2010). Reputation for reciprocity engages the brain reward center. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 107, 13099–13104.

Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Lee, W. (2019). A neuroscientific perspective on basic psychological needs. J. Pers. 87, 102–114.

Google Scholar

Ricciardi, E., Rota, G., Sani, L., Gentili, C., Gaglianese, A., Guazzelli, M., et al. (2013). How the brain heals emotional wounds: the functional neuroanatomy of forgiveness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:839. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbloom, M. H., Schmahmann, J. D., and Price, B. H. (2012). The functional neuroanatomy of decision-making. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 24, 266–277.

Google Scholar

Rusbult, C. E., Coolsen, M. K., Kirchner, J. L., and Clarke, J. A. (2006). “Commitment,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Personal Relationships, eds A. L. Vangelisti and D. Perlman (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 615–636.

Google Scholar

Schilke, O., Reimann, M., and Cook, K. S. (2013). Effect of relationship experience on trust recovery following a breach. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 110, 15236–15241. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1314857110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain 132, 617–627. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., and Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 81–96.

Google Scholar

Song, H., Zhang, Y., Zuo, L., Chen, X., Cao, G., d’Oleire Uquillas, F., et al. (2018). Improving relationships by elevating positive illusion and the underlying psychological and neural mechanisms. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:526. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberg, J. M., Heller, W., and Miller, G. A. (2013). Hierarchical brain networks active in approach and avoidance goal pursuit. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:284. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, J. R., and Baker, R. M. (2019). Complexity theory: an overview with potential applications for the social sciences. Systems 7:4.

Google Scholar

Ueda, R., Yanagisawa, K., Ashida, H., and Abe, N. (2018). Executive control and faithfulness: only long-term romantic relationships require prefrontal control. Exp. Brain Res. 236, 821–828. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5181-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valk, S. L., Bernhardt, B. C., Trautwein, F.-M., Böckler, A., Kanske, P., Guizard, N., et al. (2017). Structural plasticity of the social brain: differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training. Sci. Adv. 3:e1700489. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallacher, R. R., and Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re doing? action identification and human behavior. Psychol. Rev. 94, 3–15.

Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, M. P., and Sporns, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 683–696.

Google Scholar

Verschure, P. F., Pennartz, C. M., and Pezzulo, G. (2014). The why, what, where, when and how of goal-directed choice: neuronal and computational principles. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369:20130483. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., Zhao, Y., Li, J., Lai, H., Qiu, C., Pan, N., et al. (2020). Neurostructural correlates of hope: dispositional hope mediates the impact of the SMA gray matter volume on subjective well-being in late adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15, 395–404. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, R. L. (1980). Strategic behavioral marital therapy: toward a model for assessment and intervention. Adv. Family Intervention Assessment Theory 1, 229–271.

Google Scholar

Weisz, E., and Zaki, J. (2018). Motivated empathy: a social neuroscience perspective. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 24, 67–71.

Google Scholar

Whisman, M. A., Dixon, A. E., and Johnson, B. (1997). Therapists’ perspectives of couple problems and treatment issues in couple therapy. J. Family Psychol. 11, 361–366.

Google Scholar

Witherington, D. C. (2011). Taking emergence seriously: the centrality of circular causality for dynamic systems approaches to development. Hum. Dev. 54, 66–92.

Google Scholar

Xu, X., Brown, L., Aron, A., Cao, G., Feng, T., Acevedo, B., et al. (2012). Regional brain activity during early-stage intense romantic love predicted relationship outcomes after 40 months: an fMRI assessment. Neurosci. Lett. 526, 33–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, Z. F., and Hsieh, S. (2019). Neurocognitive mechanism of human resilience: a conceptual framework and empirical review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:5123.

Google Scholar

Yeh, H.-C., Lorenz, F. O., Wickrama, K., Conger, R. D., and Elder, G. H. Jr. (2006). Relationships among sexual satisfaction, marital quality, and marital instability at midlife. J. Family Psychol. 20, 339–343.

Google Scholar

Yoder, K. J., and Decety, J. (2018). The neuroscience of morality and social decision-making. Psychol. Crime Law 24, 279–295.

Google Scholar

Younger, J., Aron, A., Parke, S., Chatterjee, N., and Mackey, S. (2010). Viewing pictures of a romantic partner reduces experimental pain: involvement of neural reward systems. PLoS One 5:e13309. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J., and Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 675–680. doi: 10.1038/nn.3085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: complex adaptive system, marital quality, marital relationship, marital stability, neuroscience, theories of marriage

Citation: Nikrahan GR (2023) Theory of brain complexity and marital behaviors: The application of complexity science and neuroscience to explain the complexities of marital behaviors. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1050164. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1050164

Received: 21 September 2022; Accepted: 14 February 2023;
Published: 07 March 2023.

Edited by:

Yasemin Sorakın, Near East University, Cyprus

Reviewed by:

Ezgi Ulu, Near East University, Cyprus
Basak Baglama, Cyprus International University, Cyprus

Copyright © 2023 Nikrahan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gholam Reza Nikrahan, nikrahan@cfu.ac.ir, rezanikrahan@yahoo.com

Download