Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 31 May 2023
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
Volume 17 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1130200

Sex differences in olfactory cortex neuronal loss in aging

  • 1Sheffield Institute for Translational Neuroscience, The University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Medical Genomics Research, King Abdullah International Medical Research Center, King Saud bin Abdulaziz University for Health Sciences, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia
  • 3Department of Life Sciences, Brunel University London, Uxbridge, United Kingdom
  • 4Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, Parma, Italy

Introduction: Aging plays a major role in neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease, and impacts neuronal loss. Olfactory dysfunction can be an early alteration heralding the presence of a neurodegenerative disorder in aging. Studying alterations in olfaction-related brain regions might help detection of neurodegenerative diseases at an earlier stage as well as protect individuals from any danger caused by loss of sense of smell.

Objective: To assess the effect of age and sex on olfactory cortex volume in cognitively healthy participants.

Method: Neurologically healthy participants were divided in three groups based on their age: young (20–35 years; n = 53), middle-aged (36–65 years; n = 66) and older (66–85 years; n = 95). T1-weighted MRI scans acquired at 1.5 T were processed using SPM12. Smoothed images were used to extract the volume of olfactory cortex regions.

Results: ANCOVA analyses showed significant differences in volume between age groups in the olfactory cortex (p ≤ 0.0001). In women, neuronal loss started earlier than in men (in the 4th decade of life), while in men more substantial neuronal loss in olfactory cortex regions was detected only later in life.

Conclusion: Data indicate that age-related reduction in the volume of the olfactory cortex starts earlier in women than in men. The findings suggest that volume changes in olfaction-related brain regions in the aging population deserve further attention as potential proxies of increased risk of neurodegenerative diseases.

1. Introduction

The process of aging has an effect on the regional integrity of the human brain via alterations in levels of hormones, neurotransmitters and neurotrophic factors (Peters, 2006). These aging effects may alter the size as well as the composition of the neural tissue, and are the outcome of a combination of contributing factors such as sex, education and structural biological factors. The impact of these aspects is of clinical interest because they are linked to the mechanisms at the basis of the development of diseases or conditions that might affect individuals’ ability to lead a normal life. However, there is interindividual variation in how different brain regions may be susceptible to disease and how these undergo atrophy (Grajauskas et al., 2019). Importantly, not every brain region is altered to the same extent by the presence of pathology (Trollor and Valenzuela, 2001). Aging plays a major role in fostering neurological disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD), cardiac/cerebrovascular disease, or other pathologies that cause cognitive decline (Cermakova et al., 2015; Liesinger et al., 2018). Neurodegeneration can also lead to olfactory dysfunction that can manifest very early and, in some cases, herald the presence of a neurodegenerative disorder, such as Alzheimer’s disease (Gray et al., 2001; Sohrabi et al., 2012; Devanand et al., 2015). There is, in fact, evidence of early pathological changes, such as β-amyloid and neurofibrillary tangles, in regions responsible for olfactory functioning (Christen-Zaech et al., 2003; Attems and Jellinger, 2006). There also is evidence, however, of olfactory decline during normal aging, in the absence of neurodegeneration (Wang et al., 2016). In addition, olfactory dysfunction has been found to be associated with volume reduction in the olfactory cortex (OCV) and the hippocampus (Bitter et al., 2010) that, in addition to memory function, is also involved in aspects of olfactory processing.

Sex differences have been detected in how olfaction changes over time. Women have higher activation in olfactory regions than men during normal processing (Yousem et al., 1999). Furthermore, women are at an increased risk of AD and have faster brain atrophy in the hippocampus than men (Mazure and Swendsen, 2016; Nebel et al., 2018). Overall, a solid body of studies indicates that disease progression is more rapid among women, and that women, on average, also show longer disease durations (Malpetti et al., 2017; Liesinger et al., 2018). In addition, women also show greater neurovolumetric reduction than men during their fourth and fifth decade of life (Guo et al., 2016) that contributes to the definition of sex-specific trajectories in neurological aging.

The number of studies of the olfactory cortex in aging women is small. Published evidence indicates that women retain integrity of olfactory regions but show volumetric reduction in subsidiary areas involved in secondary olfactory processing, such as the hippocampus, parahippocampus, entorhinal cortex, amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex (Tamnes et al., 2013; Jiang et al., 2014; Sele et al., 2020).

Understanding and measuring volumetric loss in the olfactory cortex and in structures that support olfactory processing is important since these regions are also involved in Alzheimer’s disease, the most common neurodegenerative disease, for which an early olfactory dysfunction might be the reflection of an increased aging-related risk for decline (Attems and Jellinger, 2006; Kocahan and Doğan, 2017). Olfactory loss is also found in other forms of neurodegeneration such as Parkinson’s disease (Rahayel et al., 2012). Studying alterations in the olfactory cortex might help detection of neurodegenerative diseases at an earlier stage than currently done.

The present study used Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) to acquire brain scans to determine the extent of volume loss in olfactory cortex in the course of normal aging. Volumetric measurements were acquired in three age groups, young adults, middle-age adults and older adults, all neurologically healthy, to determine the level of regional neural volume loss related to physiological aging. In light of the sex-specific differences outlined above, we expected to find that women would present with an earlier olfactory cortex volume reduction than men.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants

Neurologically healthy participants were selected from the cohort of volunteers who had participated as healthy controls in several MRI based research projects coordinated by the Department of Neuroscience at the University of Sheffield (UK). These cognitively healthy adult participants were between 20 and 85 years of age. In total, 333 datasets were retrieved and considered for inclusion in this study. The resulting age-based groups were classified as “young”, i.e., aged 20–35 (n = 53), “middle-age”, i.e., aged 36–65 (n = 66) and “older”, i.e., aged 66–85 (n = 93). Clinical profiling via comprehensive neuropsychological testing was available for those participants who were part of the middle-age and older groups, and were 40 years old and older, to confirm their cognitive clinical status as unimpaired (i.e., absence of a cognitive profile was the primary reason for excluding about one third of the datasets originally taken into consideration). This comprehensive testing has been described in detail in previous publications (De Marco et al., 2019, 2017) and the mean scores of the samples included in this study are shown in Table 1. All eligible participants who had completed a 1.5 T MRI protocol inclusive of a three-dimensional T1-weighted brain image and had a complete neuropsychological assessment were included. All procedures were carried out in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written consent was obtained from all participants. Ethical approval and Health Research Authority approval for retrospective data analyses were obtained from the West of Scotland Regional Ethics Committee 5, Ref No.: 19/WS/0177.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Neuropsychological profile of middle-aged and older adults.

2.2. MRI processing

Pre-processing of MRI images was carried out using the most updated version of the Statistical Parametric Mapping software package (SPM), i.e., version 12 (Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, London, UK), running in a Matlab R2016b environment, version 9.1 (Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). The images were segmented using the “new segment” procedure in SPM12 and the overall 3D image of the brain was subdivided into three tissue-classes by separating the intracranial voxels of interest into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Segmented sub-maps of gray matter were then normalized, modulated, and smoothed with an 8-mm Gaussian kernel.

2.3. Extraction of olfactory region volume

In order to extract and calculate individual total intracranial volumes, tissue-class volumes were individually extracted in SPM12 using the individuals’ images containing the segmentation parameters (Malone et al., 2015). Gray-matter volume, white-matter volume and CSF volume were computed in liters and converted to milliliters to be compatible with the unit of measurement of olfactory brain region volumes during data analysis. Summing the volume of the three sub-maps was carried out to calculate each individual’s total intracranial volume (TIV).

Regional volumes (in “ml”) were extracted from all segmented, modulated and smoothed gray-matter images using the “get_totals”1 Matlab function to obtain a measurement of the olfactory brain regions of interest (i.e., thus limited to gray-matter tissue only). These were defined via the SPM12 toolbox Wake Forest University (WFU) PickAtlas (Maldjian et al., 2003) that was used to specify olfaction-related regions from a human-brain atlas. This was the Automated Anatomical Labelling (AAL) atlas that was used as the reference to create region-of-interest (ROI) masks for each individual region in the MNI space (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Automatic extraction was done separately for each brain hemisphere. The outcome of this method was separate volumetric measures for the left and right ROI. Summing the ROI volumes from both hemispheres served to obtain the total ROI volume. Figure 1 shows the region selected for extraction. Although this region represents a single entry of the AAL atlas (i.e., the “olfactory cortex” ROI atlas label), subsequent research has shown that it consists of a complex functional territory responsible for olfactory processing that includes the olfactory tract, the amygdala, the piriform cortex, the anterior perforated substance, the subcallosal area and the anterior cingulate cortex, as per definition of the olfactory cortex provided by a recent study (Fjaeldstad et al., 2017).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Areas representing the olfactory cortex selected for volume extraction.

2.4. Data analysis

Statistical data analyses were carried out using the SPSS suite, version 26 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) and the latest version of GraphPad Prism 9 (GraphPad Software San Diego, CA, USA). Descriptive statistics were run to characterize the study population, as shown in Table 2. Initially, the data were analyzed with a 2-by-3, sex-by-age group, factorial ANOVA, to test for the interaction effect between the two predictors. The analyses were then stratified by sex: one-way ANCOVAs were run to compare the volume of the selected brain region of interest among the three age groups, regressing out the influence of education and TIV. Shapiro–Wilk and Kolmogorov–Smirnov test significance levels, together with the inspection of the histograms illustrating the frequency distribution of outcome variables, were considered as part of the diagnostic process to verify if the ROI volumes were normally distributed. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to investigate the association between age, education and TIV, and each of the volumetric measures. The statistical threshold to define the significance level for the ROI volume comparisons was set at p < 0.001.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic characteristics of the participants stratified by sex.

3. Results

Significant differences in education were found among the three groups (both sexes: p = 0.0001, females: p = 0.0001, and males: p = 0.009). Significant differences were found for all group comparisons in females, namely young vs. middle-age (p = 0.003), young vs. older (p < 0.001) and middle-age vs. older (p < 0.001). In males, significant differences were found only in the young vs. older (p = 0.006) comparison. As expected, significant group differences were found for age (both sexes: p = 0.0001, females: p = 0.0001, and males: p = 0.0001).

3.1. Olfactory cortex volume

Olfactory cortex volumes were normally distributed. The two-way ANOVA revealed no statistically significant interaction between sex and age group, neither on the volume of the bilateral ROI (p = 0.458), nor in the left (p = 0.538) or right hemisphere (p = 0.370). One-way ANCOVA analyses showed significant differences in volume among age groups in OCV (p < 0.0001), with a trajectory indicating a negative association with age (Figure 2). Post-hoc tests (appropriately corrected for multiple comparisons) showed significant differences in regional volumes across all age groups in the total, left and right OCV in the female population. In men, significant differences were found only between young and older adults for all extracted olfactory cortex volumes and between middle-age and older adults only in the total and right volumes (Figure 2 and Tables 3, 4 for female and male participants, respectively). Left volumes did not differ between middle-age and older males, although a trend approaching significance emerged from this comparison.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Olfactory cortex volume differences among the three groups: comparisons among the three groups are shown for the female population (above) and male population (below) separately. Females show atrophy earlier in life than males. ***p < 0.001; ****p < 0.0001.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Significant differences in volume of the olfactory regions observed among female age groups.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Significant differences in volume of the olfactory regions observed among male age groups.

Pearson’s correlation analyses showed a significant link between OCV and age, education and TIV for both sexes (r = −0.586, r = 0.359 and r = 0.619, respectively), in females (r = −0.629, r = 0.311 and r = 0.470, respectively) and in males (r = −0.641, r = 0.401 and r = 0.654 respectively, Table 5). Additional analyses showed that this significant association persisted in all groups when controlling for education (females: p = 0.0001, males p = 0.0001) or for TIV (females: p = 0.0001, males: p = 0.0001) separately, and the effect retained its significance in the pairwise tests (Table 5). The statistical comparison of demographic and neurostructural variables between males and females (regardless of age) is included in Supplementary Table 1.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Associations of age, education, and TIV with olfactory cortex.

4. Discussion

This cohort study investigated volumes of the olfactory cortex and of subsidiary areas involved in secondary olfactory processing, and their associations with sex, age, education and TIV in neurologically healthy adults, via the use of MRI imaging techniques. The findings indicate that women show greater OCV reduction than men and at an earlier age than men. Our study showed that loss of volume in olfactory cortex in women occurs approximately around the fourth decade of life, while in men it is only detectable as late as the seventh decade. This finding is aligned with what has been found previously in longitudinal studies that indicated that women showed significantly greater brain atrophy than men in the fourth and fifth decades of their life (Guo et al., 2016). There is an association between age, sex, and overall brain atrophy rate that appears to be faster in women (Hua et al., 2010). It has been suggested that these sex-based brain alteration differences may be the outcome of more basic differences between men and women that may be due to a variety of causes, such as, for example, differences in hormones, brain development, inflammation, or psychosocial stress response (Toga and Thompson, 2003; Koss and Frick, 2017; Mancini et al., 2017). Modifiable lifestyle factors may also play an important role, as we have previously found that in women who are ex-smokers, olfactory memory is associated with severity of cognitive decline (Alotaibi et al., 2022).

Atrophy in the olfactory cortex in the elderly may predict future illness. A recent study has shown that olfactory dysfunction is detected in a high percentage of elderly participants (Loudghi et al., 2019). Additionally, olfactory dysfunction has been observed in a good proportion of patients with Alzheimer’s disease and some researchers have even suggested that it might represent an early symptom of this disease (Doty, 2009; Wilson et al., 2009; Murphy, 2019). Moreover, some studies indicated that olfactory-related brain regions such as the olfactory bulb, the olfactory tract, the olfactory epithelium, the olfactory cortex and the entorhinal cortex are all significantly atrophic in AD (Devanand et al., 2007; Buschhüter et al., 2008; Thomann et al., 2009; Arnold et al., 2010; Al-Otaibi et al., 2020). The territory responsible for olfactory functioning is particularly susceptible to proteinopathies, including amyloidosis and tauopathy (Ubeda-Bañon et al., 2020), and it is anatomically contiguous to the mediotemporal regions responsible for episodic memory processing that are prominently affected by neurodegenerative processes in AD. This makes olfactory functioning a mechanistically suitable candidate of interest for the detection of those abnormalities triggered by AD. In addition, olfactory testing is feasible in clinical settings (Su et al., 2021) and is typically well-received by testees. Furthermore, investigating abnormal functioning in a clinical population may allow researchers to consolidate the neuroscientific frameworks of olfactory (and, more generally, sensory) perception at its various stages, for instance by distinguishing the mechanisms of perception from those of smell recognition and retrieval of smell-related semantic knowledge. The use of functional MRI could be particularly informative to this end, as it would allow the separation of these computational stages with ad hoc experimental manipulations. Similarly, expanding the study of anatomical properties of olfactory regions via other methodologies (e.g., cortical thickness and structural covariance) could provide additional evidence that is, to some degree, complementary to that informed by regional volumes. Resorting to multiple approaches could help shed light on trajectories of neuroanatomical aging. For instance, some researchers have found that gray matter increases until it reaches the plateau in the third decade of life, while other studies have reported that a decrease in volume initiates after the second decade of life (Raz et al., 2004; Shen et al., 2013).

Education has been previously found to be associated with gray matter volume (Boller et al., 2017; Kang et al., 2021), although longitudinal evidence appears to indicate that it has no specific effect on atrophy rates (Nyberg et al., 2021). Moreover, the findings described in these voxel-based studies do not reveal any association between educational attainment and the anatomical integrity of the olfactory cortex. Our results point at a significant association between years of education and the volume of the olfactory cortex in both females and males, but these models were uncorrected for age or TIV, and, for this reason, indicate a general, rather than a specific association. There is evidence that education is associated with better retained cognitive performance in aging, particularly memory performance when this was measured several decades after school discontinuation (Schneeweis et al., 2014), a finding that is in line with the hypothesis that education contributes to better cognitive reserve (Stern, 2009). However, higher levels of education may also contribute to foster brain reserve by limiting brain atrophy and, in turn, resulting in better cognitive performance. Additionally, there is also evidence that functional brain network decline in older adults without college education is greater than older adults with college education (Chan et al., 2021).

The anatomy of olfactory functioning spans across multiple brain regions. The olfactory cortex is highly connected to the orbitofrontal cortex (Du et al., 2020). The olfactory cortex, moreover, includes part of the entorhinal cortex as well as part of the amygdala (Price, 1985; Carmichael et al., 1994; Seubert et al., 2013). These, in turn, are in direct connection with the hippocampus and the parahippocampal cortex (Naya, 2016; McDonald and Mott, 2017). Odorant memory retrieval and emotional activation are supported by the hippocampus and the amygdala (Rolls, 2001; Kubota et al., 2020), while the processes of recall and recognition are sustained by the hippocampal/parahippocampal complex and the orbitofrontal cortex (Rolls, 2001; Iizuka et al., 2021). This shows how important it is to monitor OCV changes in aging, because this region is involved in processing olfaction and emotion, it is connected to memory-related regions, and could be the object of a working hypothesis related to potential mechanisms of detection of neurodegenerative diseases such as AD. Fine-grained inspection of atrophy patterns that extend from the mediotemporal lobe to the medial prefrontal regions that are functionally connected with the primary olfactory cortex (Zhou et al., 2019) in particular, could be a viable clinical procedure that leads to suspecting abnormal olfactory functioning. This could be added to the clinical diagnostic algorithms currently in place to detect the presence of neurological abnormalities in middle-aged and older adults.

Some limitations should be acknowledged. In the literature, different terms are used to indicate the olfactory cortex and, similarly, the anatomical definition of the olfactory cortex is not uniform. Some state that the olfactory cortex includes the piriform cortex (also called pyriform or prepyriform cortex) that is the largest sub-region deputed to olfactory processing (Wilson, 2009). Others define the primary olfactory cortex as including the anterior olfactory nucleus, the pyriform cortex, the periamygdaloid and amygdala complex region, and the rostral entorhinal cortex (Doty, 2017). Others consider the primary olfactory cortex as including the piriform cortex, the anterior olfactory nucleus, the anterior perforated substance, the olfactory tubercle, the anterior portion of periamygdaloid cortex and the amygdala (Wang et al., 2010). In this study, we used the definition of olfactory cortex originally proposed by Dejerine (1895) and used recently in another study (Fjaeldstad et al., 2017), and this includes the olfactory tract, the amygdala, the piriform cortex, the anterior perforated substance, the subcallosal area and the anterior cingulate cortex. This more comprehensive definition might have facilitated the emergence of sex-related differences in these structures, and might have helped highlight a more precise pattern of associations between the predictors and regional volumes. In this respect, this line of research would greatly benefit from the definition of methodological gold standards that can inform researchers on the most adequate choices, e.g., the use of specific ROIs or the inclusion of certain correction factors in the analyses. In addition, it is still possible that the pattern of findings may have been affected by intervenient variables that were not part of the study design. Epidemiological evidence indicates that olfactory dysfunction is influenced by modifiable lifestyle aspects such as exposure to air pollution and toxins, and smoking habits (Yang and Pinto, 2016). As far as chronic smoking is concerned, however, this is not linked to atrophy in the regions responsible for olfactory processing (Sutherland et al., 2016). It is still possible, however, that other mechanisms might be at play in accounting for at least a portion of the findings described in this study. A further limitation in our study, finally, is the absence of data from a validated test of olfactory functioning (e.g., based on olfactory discrimination or recognition).

In conclusion, the data of the current study suggest that age-related reduction in the volume of the olfactory cortex is detectable earlier in women than in men. The findings suggest that monitoring volume changes in olfaction-related brain regions in the aging population may be valuable to detect risk of neurodegenerative diseases, especially in individuals at greater risk. Additionally, more studies are needed to establish a causal link between changes in olfactory functioning and pathological alterations in olfactory cortex during the preclinical phase of AD. This would promote the design of hypothesis-based clinical studies with the purpose of defining clinical guidelines that can help people recognize preclinical abnormalities that could be further investigated by secondary-care specialists. These changes would emerge alongside those that have already been established in other areas of psychological functioning such as semantic memory or perceptual speed (Amieva et al., 2008; Payton et al., 2020) and would help us define the timeline of the olfactory cascade (i.e., when changes in behavioral functioning occur in relation, for instance, to regional atrophy), contributing to outlining a more general and multidimensional profile of risk.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the West of Scotland Regional Ethics Committee 5, Ref. No: 19/WS/0177. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MMA carried out data analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MDM contributed to the study design, data analysis, and critical review of the manuscript. AV conceived and designed the study, contributed to the data acquisition, critical review, and finalization of the manuscript. All authors approved the submitted version of this manuscript.

Funding

MMA was funded by a scholarship by King Abdullah International Medical Research Center, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1130200/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; SPM, statistical parametric mapping; ROI, region of interest; OCV, olfactory cortex volume; TIV, total intracranial volume.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www0.cs.ucl.ac.uk/staff/g.ridgway/vbm/get_totals.m

References

Alotaibi, M., Lessard-Beaudoin, M., Busch, K., Loudghi, A., Gaudreau, P., and Graham, R. K. (2022). Olfactory dysfunction associated with cognitive decline in an elderly population. Exp. Aging Res. [Online ahead of print]. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2022.2160597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Otaibi, M., Lessard-Beaudoin, M., Castellano, C.-A., Gris, D., Cunnane, S. C., and Graham, R. K. (2020). Volumetric MRI demonstrates atrophy of the olfactory cortex in AD. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 17, 904–915. doi: 10.2174/1567205017666201215120909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amieva, H., Le Goff, M., Millet, X., Orgogozo, J. M., Pérès, K., Barberger-Gateau, P., et al. (2008). Prodromal Alzheimer’s disease: successive emergence of the clinical symptoms. Ann. Neurol. 64, 492–498. doi: 10.1002/ana.21509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, S. E., Lee, E. B., Moberg, P. J., Stutzbach, L., Kazi, H., Han, L.-Y., et al. (2010). Olfactory epithelium amyloid-β and paired helical filament-tau pathology in Alzheimer disease. Ann. Neurol. 67, 462–469. doi: 10.1002/ana.21910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Attems, J., and Jellinger, K. A. (2006). Olfactory tau pathology in Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Clin. Neuropathol. 25, 265–271.

Google Scholar

Bitter, T., Gudziol, H., Burmeister, H. P., Mentzel, H. J., Guntinas-Lichius, O., and Gaser, C. (2010). Anosmia leads to a loss of gray matter in cortical brain areas. Chem. Senses 35, 407–415. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boller, B., Mellah, S., Ducharme-Laliberté, G., and Belleville, S. (2017). Relationships between years of education, regional grey matter volumes, and working memory-related brain activity in healthy older adults. Brain Imaging Behav. 11, 304–317. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9621-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschhüter, D., Smitka, M., Puschmann, S., Gerber, J. C., Witt, M., Abolmaali, N. D., et al. (2008). Correlation between olfactory bulb volume and olfactory function. NeuroImage 42, 498–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmichael, S. T., Clugnet, M.-C., and Price, J. L. (1994). Central olfactory connections in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 346, 403–434. doi: 10.1002/cne.903460306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cermakova, P., Eriksdotter, M., Lund, L. H., Winblad, B., Religa, P., and Religa, D. (2015). Heart failure and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Intern. Med. 277, 406–425. doi: 10.1111/joim.12287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, M. Y., Han, L., Carreno, C. A., Zhang, Z., Rodriguez, R. M., LaRose, M., et al. (2021). Long-term prognosis and educational determinants of brain network decline in older adult individuals. Nat. Aging 1, 1053–1067. doi: 10.1038/s43587-021-00125-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christen-Zaech, S., Kraftsik, R., Pillevuit, O., Kiraly, M., Martins, R., Khalili, K., et al. (2003). Early olfactory involvement in Alzheimer’s Disease. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 30, 20–25. doi: 10.1017/S0317167100002389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Marco, M., Beltrachini, L., Biancardi, A., Frangi, A. F., and Venneri, A. (2017). Machine-learning support to individual diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment using multimodal MRI and cognitive assessments. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 31, 278–286. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Marco, M., Ourselin, S., and Venneri, A. (2019). Age and hippocampal volume predict distinct parts of default mode network activity. Sci. Rep. 9:16075. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-52488-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dejerine, J. (1895). Anatomie des Centres Nerveux. Wien: Rueff.

Google Scholar

Devanand, D. P., Lee, S., Manly, J., Andrews, H., Schupf, N., Doty, R. L., et al. (2015). Olfactory deficits predict cognitive decline and Alzheimer dementia in an urban community. Neurology 84, 182–189. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devanand, D. P., Pradhaban, G., Liu, X., Khandji, A., De Santi, S., Segal, S., et al. (2007). Hippocampal and entorhinal atrophy in mild cognitive impairment: prediction of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 68, 828–836. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000256697.20968.d7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doty, R. L. (2009). The olfactory system and its disorders. Semin. Neurol. 29, 74–81. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1124025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doty, R. L. (2017). Sense of Smell, in: Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.06559-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, J., Rolls, E. T., Cheng, W., Li, Y., Gong, W., Qiu, J., et al. (2020). Functional connectivity of the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Cortex 123, 185–199. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fjaeldstad, A., Fernandes, H. M., Van Hartevelt, T. J., Gleesborg, C., Møller, A., Ovesen, T., et al. (2017). Brain fingerprints of olfaction: a novel structural method for assessing olfactory cortical networks in health and disease. Sci. Rep. 7:42534. doi: 10.1038/srep42534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grajauskas, L. A., Siu, W., Medvedev, G., Guo, H., D’Arcy, R. C. N., and Song, X. (2019). MRI-based evaluation of structural degeneration in the ageing brain: pathophysiology and assessment. Ageing Res. Rev. 49, 67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2018.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, A. J., Staples, V., Murren, K., Dhariwal, A., and Bentham, P. (2001). Olfactory identification is impaired in clinic-based patients with vascular dementia and senile dementia of Alzheimer type. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 16, 513–517. doi: 10.1002/gps.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, J. Y., Isohanni, M., Miettunen, J., Jääskeläinen, E., Kiviniemi, V., Nikkinen, J., et al. (2016). Brain structural changes in women and men during midlife. Neurosci. Lett. 615, 107–112. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hua, X., Hibar, D. P., Lee, S., Toga, A. W., Jack, C. R. Jr., Weiner, M. W., et al. (2010). Sex and age differences in atrophic rates: an ADNI study with n=1368 MRI scans. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 1463–1480. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iizuka, N., Masaoka, Y., Kubota, S., Sugiyama, H., Yoshida, M., Yoshikawa, A., et al. (2021). Entorhinal cortex and parahippocampus volume reductions impact olfactory decline in aged subjects. Brain Behav. 11:e02115. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Sachdev, P., Lipnicki, D. M., Zhang, H., Liu, T., Zhu, W., et al. (2014). A longitudinal study of brain atrophy over two years in community-dwelling older individuals. NeuroImage 86, 203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, D. W., Wang, S.-M., Na, H.-R., Kim, N.-Y., Lim, H. K., and Lee, C. U. (2021). Differential impact of education on gray matter volume according to sex in cognitively normal older adults: whole brain surface-based morphometry. Front. Psychiatry 12:644148. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.644148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kocahan, S., and Doğan, Z. (2017). Mechanisms of Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis and prevention: the brain, neural pathology, N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors, Tau protein and other risk factors. Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci. 15, 1–8. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2017.15.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koss, W. A., and Frick, K. M. (2017). Sex differences in hippocampal function: sex differences in hippocampal function. J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 539–562. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubota, S., Masaoka, Y., Sugiyama, H., Yoshida, M., Yoshikawa, A., Koiwa, N., et al. (2020). Hippocampus and parahippocampus volume reduction associated with impaired olfactory abilities in subjects without evidence of cognitive decline. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:556519. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.556519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liesinger, A. M., Graff-Radford, N. R., Duara, R., Carter, R. E., Hanna Al-Shaikh, F. S., Koga, S., et al. (2018). Sex and age interact to determine clinicopathologic differences in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 136, 873–885. doi: 10.1007/s00401-018-1908-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loudghi, A., Alotaibi, M., Lessard-Beaudoin, M., Gris, D., Busch, K., Gaudreau, P., et al. (2019). Unawareness of olfactory dysfunction in older adults. Int. J. Neurol. Neurother. 6:86. doi: 10.23937/2378-3001/1410086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., and Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage 19, 1233–1239. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00169-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, I. B., Leung, K. K., Clegg, S., Barnes, J., Whitwell, J. L., Ashburner, J., et al. (2015). Accurate automatic estimation of total intracranial volume: a nuisance variable with less nuisance. NeuroImage 104, 366–372. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malpetti, M., Ballarini, T., Presotto, L., Garibotto, V., Tettamanti, M., Perani, D., et al. (2017). Gender differences in healthy aging and Alzheimer’s Dementia: a 18 F-FDG-PET study of brain and cognitive reserve. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4212–4227. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancini, A., Gaetani, L., Di Gregorio, M., Tozzi, A., Ghiglieri, V., Calabresi, P., et al. (2017). Hippocampal neuroplasticity and inflammation: relevance for multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. Demyelinating Disord. 2:2. doi: 10.1186/s40893-017-0019-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazure, C. M., and Swendsen, J. (2016). Sex differences in Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Lancet Neurol. 15, 451–452. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00067-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, A. J., and Mott, D. D. (2017). Functional neuroanatomy of amygdalohippocampal interconnections and their role in learning and memory. J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 797–820. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, C. (2019). Olfactory and other sensory impairments in Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 15, 11–24. doi: 10.1038/s41582-018-0097-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naya, Y. (2016). Declarative association in the perirhinal cortex. Neurosci. Res. 113, 12–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2016.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nebel, R. A., Aggarwal, N. T., Barnes, L. L., Gallagher, A., Goldstein, J. M., Kantarci, K., et al. (2018). Understanding the impact of sex and gender in Alzheimer’s disease: a call to action. Alzheimers Dement. 14, 1171–1183. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., Magnussen, F., Lundquist, A., Baaré, W., Bartrés-Faz, D., Bertram, L., et al. (2021). Educational attainment does not influence brain aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 118:e2101644118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2101644118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payton, N. M, Rizzuto, D., Fratiglioni, L., Kivipelto, M., Bäckman, L., and Laukka, E. J. (2020). Combining cognitive markers to identify individuals at increased dementia risk: influence of modifying factors and time to diagnosis. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 26, 785–797. doi: 10.1017/S1355617720000272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, R. (2006). Ageing and the brain. Postgrad. Med. J. 82, 84–88. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.2005.036665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, J. L. (1985). Beyond the primary olfactory cortex: olfactory-related areas in the neocortex, thalamus and hypothalamus. Chem. Senses 10, 239–258. doi: 10.1093/chemse/10.2.239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahayel, S., Frasnelli, J., and Joubert, S. (2012). The effect of Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease on olfaction: a meta-analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 231, 60–74. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.02.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, N., Gunning-Dixon, F., Head, D., Rodrigue, K. M., Williamson, A., and Acker, J. D. (2004). Aging, sexual dimorphism, and hemispheric asymmetry of the cerebral cortex: replicability of regional differences in volume. Neurobiol. Aging 25, 377–396. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00118-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T. (2001). The rules of formation of the olfactory representations found in the orbitofrontal cortex olfactory areas in primates. Chem. Senses 26, 595–604. doi: 10.1093/chemse/26.5.595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneeweis, N., Skirbekk, V., and Winter-Ebmer, R. (2014). Does education improve cognitive performance four decades after school completion? Demography 51, 619–643. doi: 10.1007/s13524-014-0281-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sele, S., Liem, F., Mérillat, S., and Jäncke, L. (2020). Decline variability of cortical and subcortical regions in aging: a longitudinal study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:363. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seubert, J., Freiherr, J., Djordjevic, J., and Lundström, J. N. (2013). Statistical localization of human olfactory cortex. NeuroImage 66, 333–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, J., Kassir, M. A., Wu, J., Zhang, Q., Zhou, S., Xuan, S. Y., et al. (2013). MR volumetric study of piriform-cortical amygdala and orbitofrontal cortices: the aging effect. PLoS One 8:e74526. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sohrabi, H. R., Bates, K. A., Weinborn, M. G., Johnston, A. N. B., Bahramian, A., Taddei, K., et al. (2012). Olfactory discrimination predicts cognitive decline among community-dwelling older adults. Transl. Psychiatry 2:e118. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, B., Bleier, B., Wei, Y., and Wu, D. (2021). Clinical implications of psychophysical olfactory testing: assessment, diagnosis, and treatment outcome. Front. Neurosci. 15:646956. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.646956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutherland, M. T., Riedel, M. C., Flannery, J. S., Yanes, J. A., Fox, P. T., Stein, E. A., et al. (2016). Chronic cigarette smoking is linked with structural alterations in brain regions showing acute nicotinic drug-induced functional modulations. Behav. Brain Funct. 12:16. doi: 10.1186/s12993-016-0100-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamnes, C. K., Walhovd, K. B., Dale, A. M., Østby, Y., Grydeland, H., Richardson, G., et al. (2013). Brain development and aging: overlapping and unique patterns of change. NeuroImage 68, 63–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomann, P. A., Dos Santos, V., Seidl, U., Toro, P., Essig, M., and Schröder, J. (2009). MRI-derived atrophy of the olfactory bulb and tract in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 17, 213–221. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toga, A. W., and Thompson, P. M. (2003). Mapping brain asymmetry. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 37–48. doi: 10.1038/nrn1009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trollor, J. N., and Valenzuela, M. J. (2001). Brain ageing in the new millennium. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 35, 788–805. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00969.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ubeda-Bañon, I., Saiz-Sanchez, D., Flores-Cuadrado, A., Rioja-Corroto, E., Gonzalez-Rodriguez, M., Villar-Conde, S., et al. (2020). The human olfactory system in two proteinopathies: Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Transl. Neurodegener. 9:22. doi: 10.1186/s40035-020-00200-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Eslinger, P. J., Doty, R. L., Zimmerman, E. K., Grunfeld, R., Sun, X., et al. (2010). Olfactory deficit detected by fMRI in early Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res. 1357, 184–194. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Sun, X., and Yang, Q. X. (2016). Early aging effect on the function of the human central olfactory system. J. Gerontol. A. Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 72, 1007–1014. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, D. A. (2009). Olfactory Cortex Physiology, in: Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 95–100. doi: 10.1016/B978-008045046-9.01690-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Boyle, P. A., Buchman, A. S., and Bennett, D. A. (2009). Olfactory impairment in presymptomatic Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1170, 730–735. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04013.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., and Pinto, J. M. (2016). The epidemiology of olfactory disorders. Curr. Otorhinolaryngol. Rep. 4, 130–141. doi: 10.1007/s40136-016-0120-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yousem, D. M., Maldjian, J. A., Siddiqi, F., Hummel, T., Alsop, D. C., Geckle, R. J., et al. (1999). Gender effects on odor-stimulated functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Res. 818, 480–487. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)01276-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, G., Lane, G., Cooper, S. L., Kahnt, T., and Zelano, C. (2019). Characterizing functional pathways of the human olfactory system. Elife 8:e47177. doi: 10.7554/eLife.47177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, sex, atrophy, olfactory cortex, neuroimaging, neuronal loss

Citation: Alotaibi MM, De Marco M and Venneri A (2023) Sex differences in olfactory cortex neuronal loss in aging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1130200. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1130200

Received: 22 December 2022; Accepted: 28 March 2023;
Published: 31 May 2023.

Edited by:

Yang Jiang, University of Kentucky, United States

Reviewed by:

Claire Joy Hanley, Swansea University, United Kingdom
Mazlyfarina Mohamad, National University of Malaysia, Malaysia

Copyright © 2023 Alotaibi, De Marco and Venneri. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Annalena Venneri, annalena.venneri@brunel.ac.uk

Download