Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 01 November 2019
Sec. Brain Imaging Methods
This article is part of the Research Topic Brain-image Based Computation for Supporting Clinical Decision in Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders View all 17 articles

Diminished Frontal Theta Activity During Gaming in Young Adults With Internet Gaming Disorder

\r\nJuri Kim&#x;Juri Kim1†Jinsick Park&#x;Jinsick Park1†Young Min ParkYoung Min Park1DongPyo JangDongPyo Jang1Kee Namkoong,Kee Namkoong2,3Young-Chul Jung,*Young-Chul Jung2,3*In Young Kim*In Young Kim1*
  • 1Department of Biomedical Engineering, Hanyang University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea

Cognitive control is essential for flexible, top-down, goal-directed behavior. Individuals with Internet gaming disorder (IGD) are characterized by impaired prefrontal cortex function and cognitive control. This results in an increase in stimulus-driven habitual behavior, particularly related to pathological gaming. In the present study, we investigated the electroencephalographic (EEG) activity in individuals with IGD. Twenty-four individuals with IGD and 35 healthy control (HC) subjects were recruited. We analyzed their EEG activity while the subjects played their favorite game (30–40 min duration). We compared the band power between the two groups. During gaming, the left frontal theta, alpha, and beta band activities were lower in subjects with IGD than in HCs. Moreover, the left frontal theta power negatively correlated with IGD severity. These results indicate that left frontal theta power could be used as a neurophysiological biomarker for the detection of diminished cognitive control patterns in individuals with IGD.

Introduction

Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is a specific form of Internet addiction characterized by an individual’s impaired control over Internet gaming (Kuss, 2013). As with other addictions, individuals with IGD exhibit salience, mood modification, tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, conflicts, and relapses (Chou and Ting, 2003). In the recent 11th Revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) from the World Health Organization (WHO), a gaming disorder is defined as a pattern of gaming behavior (“digital-gaming” or “video-gaming”) characterized by an impaired control over gaming. Increasing priority is given to gaming over other activities to the extent that gaming takes precedence over other interests and daily activities. The continuation or escalation of gaming persists despite the occurrence of negative consequences (Young, 2018).

Many studies have demonstrated the neurophysiological features of IGD using electroencephalography (EEG). Most previous studies have focused on identifying biomarkers for subjects with IGD in the resting state. A study by Choi et al. (2013) revealed that subjects with IGD had a lower beta band power in all regions and a higher gamma band power in the frontal regions than those of healthy control (HC) subjects. These results were related to impulsivity. In addition, Lee et al. (2014) demonstrated that the absolute beta band power in all brain regions was lower in subjects with IGD who did not have depression, than in those with depression and in HC subjects. The lower beta activity is correlated with the impulsivity and dysfunctional inhibitory control in subjects with IGD without depression. Park et al. reported that the intra-hemispheric coherence values for the theta band between the T4-T6 and P4-O2 electrodes were higher in subjects with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) with comorbid IGD than those in subjects with ADHD without comorbidity (Park et al., 2017). They indicated that the repetitive activation of the brain reward system during continuous gaming may have increased the neuronal connectivity within the parieto-occipital and temporal regions in the subjects with ADHD alone, compared to those with comorbid IGD. These studies provided evidence that the excessive use of Internet games results in cognitive impairment and functional changes in the brain of subjects with IGD.

To prevent the cognitive impairment caused by IGD, it is necessary to monitor the cue-induced neurophysiological responses during gaming. It is assumed that gaming may be associated with excessive rewarding behavior that meets the criteria of addiction. Gaming can elicit a strong motivational state that contributes to cravings and repeated excessive or addictive behaviors (Thalemann et al., 2007). For example, in a study investigating pathological gambling, a significant increase in the mean craving for gambling was found in subjects with pathological gambling who were exposed to visual gambling cues during the assessment period (Crockford et al., 2005). In an IGD study on the cue-reactivity paradigm, the differences in the cue-induced event-related potentials between excessive computer-game players and casual players were significant for game related-cues. However, they were not significant for non-game related cues (Thalemann et al., 2007). Yao et al. (2015) reported inhibition deficits during the performance of a gaming-related Go/No-Go task in subjects with IGD compared with HCs. However, the IGD group did not differ from the HC group in non-gaming-related inhibitory control, as assessed by the Stroop task.

Previous studies have demonstrated that the an addiction cue can lead to strong motivation. This has led to studies that have examined the differences before and after an addiction cue. However, the aforementioned studies did not describe the gaming cue-induced neurophysiological response during gaming in individuals with IGD. A recent study investigated the changes in the heart rate variability (HRV) patterns during gaming as a potential biomarker for IGD (Hong et al., 2018; Lee et al., 2018). Compared with HCs, subjects with IGD exhibited an altered HRV response while playing an online game. This indicates that the dynamics between executive control and reward-seeking may be out of balance during gaming in these subjects. This prompted us to initiate a study of the cue-induced neurophysiological responses during gaming. EEG can be used to measure the neurophysiological responses in real time with a higher temporal resolution than that of fMRI. In addition, EEG can be correlated with cognitive functions and can measure the neural activity directly, with a multidimensional signal encompassing time, space, frequency, and power (Cohen, 2011). In this study, we used EEG to analyze the cue-induced neurophysiological responses during gaming.

We hypothesized that the differences in the brain functions between individuals with and those without IGD would become apparent during gaming. Furthermore, if the activity in specific regions in the brain would be significantly different between the groups, this would be a beneficial biomarker that could be used to classify IGD. Thus, in this study, we examined the changes in the power of the theta, alpha, and beta bands between the resting state and the gaming state using EEG. We compared them between individuals with and those without IGD to determine their significance. The purpose of this study was to clarify whether the brain activity changed during gaming in individuals with IGD without other psychological problems (e.g., depression and anxiety). Since previous studies have reported a difference in motivation between the resting state and after a game-related cue in a cue-related task in IGD, we further investigated the difference in the brain activity while gaming compared with that at the resting state between the IGD and HC groups.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We recruited 59 right-handed young males. The subjects were examined for their Internet usage patterns and were administered the Young’s Internet Addiction Test (YIAT) (Beard and Wolf, 2001; Young, 2011). Subjects who used the Internet primarily for gaming and whose YIAT scores were more than 50, were classified as the IGD group (n = 24, mean age = 23.3 ± 2.3 years). They were confirmed to have IGD by a psychiatrist according to the IGD diagnostic criteria of the DSM Fifth Edition (Petry and O’Brien, 2013). Subjects who scored below 50 on the YIAT were classified as HCs (n = 35, mean age = 23.2 ± 2.5 years). All subjects in this study frequently played “League of Legends (LOL),” (Riot Games, Los Angeles, CA, United States, 2009). This is the most popular multiplayer online battle arena game in Korea (Nuyens et al., 2016, Statista, 2018). To control for differences in skill between the IGD group and the HC group, we included only those who were ranked above the silver tier. There were six tiers in this game, bronze, silver, gold, platinum, diamond and challenger.

All subjects completed several self-reporting questionnaires assessing comorbid psychiatric symptoms of IGD (Kim et al., 2016). The following self-reporting questionnaires were used:

1. The Beck Depression Inventory to test for depression (Beck et al., 1961).

2. The Beck Anxiety Inventory to test for anxiety (Beck et al., 1988).

3. The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, version 11 to evaluate impulsivity (Patton et al., 1995).

4. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test to identify alcohol-related problems (Reinert and Allen, 2002).

5. The Wender Utah Rating Scale to evaluate for symptoms of ADHD (Ward et al., 1993).

Task and Procedure

Resting-state EEG (PRE) was recorded with the participants in a comfortable sitting position with their eyes open for 5 min (Figure 1). Each subject then played the online game LOL three times, (periods GAME1, GAME2, and GAME3) with 5 min of rest between gaming periods. Following the gaming, resting-state EEG was again recorded for 5 min (POST). The Institutional Review Board approved the protocol for this study (HYI-16-044), and all subjects provided signed informed consent before participating.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental protocol for recording scalp EEG signals.

EEG Data Acquisition and Pre-processing

EEG data were acquired using a Waveguard 64 EEG sensor-cap (CA-105, ANT-Neuro, Enscheda, Netherlands). The signals were sampled at a frequency of 1024 Hz and we used the active G1/G2 ground reference (BRAINBOX EEG-1166 system, Braintronics, Almere, Netherlands) for the ground and reference (G1: FPz, G2: AFz). As a reference, a single channel with bipolar electrodes was attached to the mastoids. Possible line noise artifacts were removed from the data with a notch filter at 60 Hz in the central frequency, IIR notch filter in filter type, and 9.9 Hz in bandwidth. The impedance of each electrode was maintained below 10 kiloohms.

Artifact removal was performed using the EEGLAB toolbox (Version 14.1.1b, Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience) in Matlab (Version R2016b, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States) (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). Data were filtered using the EEGLAB filter function pop_eegfiltnew; high pass, 1 Hz; low pass, 30 Hz. The filtering option in EEGLAB uses linear finite impulse response (FIR) filtering with a second-order Hamming window. It was applied to the EEG signals to eliminate the noise from the subjects’ movement, breathing, and muscle electrical activity. After artifact removal, the signals were re-referenced with a common average of potentials at 61 electrodes, except for the ground and reference electrodes, by applying the pop reref algorithm. After the application of the common average re-reference (CAR) method, independent component analysis (ICA) was performed by applying the pop runica algorithm. After the ICA analysis, to remove the eye-blink noise, we removed components with a large weight (typically 1 and 2 components) in Fp1 and Fp2 while having an eye-blink pattern. A time-frequency representation was generated by a Morlet wavelet transform with a wavelet length of three cycles, a window size of 3000 ms, and a shift time of 100 ms (Akin, 2002). Four frequency bands were integrated: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), and beta (13–30 Hz). We focused on the theta, alpha, and beta bands in this study.

EEG Analysis

A percentage value was used to normalize the individuals’ baseline, because the absolute amplitude of EEG oscillations is as dependent on skull conductivity and geometry as it is on neuronal dynamics. The change in the EEG power during the task was calculated as a relative change in the average EEG power between the gaming task and baseline, as follows:

P e r c e n t a g e v a l u e = ( P o w e r ( T a s k ) - P o w e r ( B a s e l i n e P R E ) ) * 100 P o w e r ( B a s e l i n e P R E )

As shown in Figure 2, we divided the activity at 61 sites into 13 regions by averaging within each region (according to the international 10-10 system): prefrontal (FP1, FP2), left frontal (F7, F3, AF7, AF3, F5), frontal (Fz, F1, F2), right frontal (F4, F8, AF4, AF8, F6), left central (FC5, C3, CP5, FC3, C5, CP3), central (FC1, FC2, Cz, CP1, CP2, FCz, C1, C2, CPz), right central (FC6, C4, CP6, FC4, C6, CP4), left temporal (T7, FT7, TP7), right temporal (T8, FT8, TP8), left parietal (P7, P3, P5, PO5, PO3, PO7), parietal (Pz, P1, P2), right parietal (P4, P8, P6, PO4, PO6, PO8), and occipital (POz, O1, Oz, O2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Representation of the 13 regions from 61 electrodes in the analyses.

Statistical Analysis

Independent-sample t-tests were performed to compare the demographic, clinical, and behavioral variables between the two groups. The differences in the EEG data were analyzed by means of a mixed-model analysis of variance (mixed ANOVA). To evaluate the between-group differences in the EEG percentage values in the theta, alpha, and beta bands in 13 ROIs, we used the gaming period (during gaming sessions 1-3 and post) as the within-subjects factor, and the group (IGD or HC group) as the between-subjects factor. Post hoc comparisons were applied to determine whether the differences between the groups (independent t-tests) and between certain periods (paired t-tests) were statistically significant. To avoid multiple comparison problems including three bands and 13 ROIs, a false discovery rate (FDR) correction algorithm was applied to the p-values (Groppe et al., 2011). We used an alpha value of 0.05 (2-tailed significance) and an FDR value of 0.2 for the statistics that corresponded to a control of the maximum proportion of false positives among the rejected null hypotheses of <20% (Genovese et al., 2002). The p-values as well as corrected p-values were included as part of the results. In addition, to determining the relationship between the EEG percentage value in each band and the severity of IGD, we conducted Pearson’s correlation analysis between the left frontal percentage value and the YIAT score for each band. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (version 21; IBM, Inc., NY, United States).

Results

Demographics, Clinical Characteristics, and Behavioral Results of the Subjects

The demographics, clinical characteristics, and behavioral results of the subjects are presented in Table 1. The groups did not differ in age or IQ. The subjects in the IGD group scored significantly higher in the tests for severity of the online game-related problems (YIAT: t = 12.088, p < 0.001; K-scale: t = 5.500, p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics, clinical characteristics, and behavioral results of the subjects.

The two groups did not differ significantly in self-reported depression, anxiety, alcohol-related problems, and ADHD symptoms. However, the subjects in the IGD group scored significantly higher than those in the HC group in the tests for impulsivity (BIS: t = 3.352, p < 0.005). The gaming time during the experiment was approximately 30 min, and there was no difference between the two groups.

EEG Activity

Figure 3 displays the scalp topographies of the theta, alpha, and beta bands for the IGD and HC groups. For all bands analyzed by the mixed-model ANOVA, the main effect of the group was found to differ significantly between the groups (Figure 4). The data satisfied both normality and homogeneity of variance. In the theta band, left frontal: F(1,57) = 12.713, p = 0.001, corrected p = 0.039, effect size = 0.182; frontal: F(1,57) = 4.701, p = 0.034, corrected p = 0.165, effect size = 0.076; left central: F(1,57) = 8.707, p = 0.005, corrected p = 0.065, effect size = 0.133; central: F(1,57) = 6.610, p = 0.013, corrected p = 0.126, effect size = 0.104; right central: F(1,57) = 6.619, p = 0.013, corrected p = 0.101, effect size = 0.104; left temporal: F(1,57) = 5.318, p = 0.025, corrected p = 0.162, effect size = 0.085 (effect size as partial eta-squared). In the alpha band, left frontal: F(1,57) = 4.614, p = 0.036, corrected p = 0.156, effect size = 0.075. In the beta band, left frontal: F(1,57) = 10.702, p = 0.002, corrected p = 0.039, effect size = 0.158; left temporal: F(1,57) = 4.598, p = 0.036, corrected p = 0.1404, effect size = 0.078; left central: F(1,57) = 4.834, p = 0.032, corrected p = 0.178, effect size = 0.075 (mixed ANOVA; p value <0.05, FDR correction; corrected p value <0.2). The percentage values for all measured bands were lower in the IGD group than those in the HC group. For all bands, the left frontal region differed significantly between the groups.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Topographical maps of the percentage values in the (A) theta, (B) alpha, and (C) beta bands in the healthy control (HC) subjects and those with Internet gaming disorder (IGD) during different periods (PRE, GAME1, GAME2, GAME3, and POST). Red represents a higher value, while blue represents a lower value. The IGD group demonstrated reduced percentage values in the theta, alpha, and beta bands compared to those in the HC group. Scales display% for percentage power.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The bar graph represents the results of mixed ANOVA for the 13 regions in the (A) theta, (B) alpha, and (C) beta bands. The X-axis represents the period (GAME1, GAME2, GAME3, and POST) and Y-axis represents percentage values for the 13 regions (light gray: HC, dark gray: IGD). The bar graph is located upward at percentage values greater than 0 and downward at percentage values less than zero. The red box indicates a region with a significant difference for the group effect (mixed ANOVA; p < 0.05, FDR correction; corrected p < 0.2). The post hoc results from the independent t-tests between the groups are shown as asterisks (). Error bars indicate standard error. p < 0.05.

We also found significant main effects for the period for all bands. The theta band differed significantly in 12 regions (every region except the prefrontal region). The alpha band differed significantly in all 13 regions and the beta band differed significantly in 10 regions (all but the left parietal, right parietal, and occipital regions). The interactions between the groups and the period were not statistically significant in any region.

Correlation Analyses

To investigate whether the activity in the left frontal region could be a biomarker for IGD, we analyzed the relationship between the YIAT score and the power of each band in the left frontal region. We found significant negative correlations between the YIAT score and percentage value in the left frontal region in the theta, alpha, and beta bands in several periods. In the theta band, GAME1: r = −0.353, p = 0.006; GAME2: r = −0.328, p = 0.011; GAME3: r = −0.305, p = 0.018; POST: r = −0.279, p = 0.032 (Figure 5). In the alpha band, GAME1: r = −0.264, p = 0.043; GAME2: r = −0.259, p = 0.047. In the beta band, GAME1: r = −0.257, p = 0.049; GAME2: r = −0.263, p = 0.044.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlation analyses between the YIAT score and the percentage value in the left frontal region in all subjects in the (A) theta, (B) alpha, and (C) beta bands. The red boxes indicate statistical significance (p < 0.05), indicating periods with a weak correlation between the YIAT score and percentage value in the left frontal region. Significant negative correlations were observed in all periods for the theta band, during GAME1 and GAME2 periods for the alpha band, and during GAME1 and GAME2 periods for the beta band. p < 0.05.

Discussion and Conclusion

We investigated the differences in the EEG response patterns in the theta, alpha, and beta bands between the HC and IGD groups during the gaming periods. We recruited individuals with IGD who exhibited only impulsivity, without other accompanying mental illnesses (depression, anxiety, alcohol use disorder [AUD], or ADHD). This is the first study to acquire real-time scalp EEG signals and analyzed longitudinal data during playing the online game LOL, the subjects’ primary game. The subjects with IGD had significantly lower theta, alpha, and beta band powers in the left frontal region than those of the HCs during gaming. The association between impaired cognitive control and IGD during gaming is represented by the lower frontal theta activity. This is associated with less cognitive control, whereas the lower frontal alpha activity suggests a lower load on the working memory. It is likely that validation and application of meaningful clinical indicators for IGD diagnosis and treatment will be possible through the addiction-related features of individuals with IGD that appear during gaming. In addition, the left frontal theta activity, which correlates with the severity of addiction, is likely to be used to discriminate individuals with IGD during gaming in real-time.

The frontal theta activity has been shown to be related to cognitive control (Brand et al., 2014; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014). Cognitive control is an executive function in the control system that allows us to regulate our behavior so that it is planned, goal-oriented, flexible, and effective (Shallice and Burgess, 1996; Jurado and Rosselli, 2007; Anderson et al., 2008). IGD is associated with impaired cognitive control, characterized as a behavioral addiction or impulse-control disorder (Dong et al., 2011, 2012, 2013). Additionally, increased alpha activity is related to the load on the working memory (Gundel and Wilson, 1992; Gevins et al., 1997; Klimesch, 2012). Katahira et al. (2018) suggested that the amplitudes of frontal theta and alpha activity were significantly smaller under conditions of boredom (accuracy 99.7%) than under flow conditions (accuracy 54.4%). Our results indicate that lower frontal theta activity can be interpreted as decreased cognitive control, while lower frontal alpha activity can be interpreted as a lower working memory load condition. Furthermore, it is assumed that the individuals with IGD are gaming under boredom conditions, characterized by lower frontal activity of both theta and alpha waves. The reduced frontal beta power in individuals with IGD is associated with impaired inhibitory control and reward feedback (Choi et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2014; HajiHosseini and Holroyd, 2015).

The left region of the brain is related to analytical and strategic thinking, and the LOL game requires strategic thinking. In this study, the HC group showed higher left frontal theta power than those in the IGD group during LOL gaming. In other words, based on the left frontal theta activity, it seems that the HC subjects performed the LOL game with more strategic thinking than individuals with IGD.

Imaging studies on addictive behavior have identified the key role of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), both through its regulation of the limbic reward regions and its involvement in higher-order executive functions (Goldstein and Volkow, 2011). PFC dysfunction is associated with activation of the limbic system, which results in addictive behavior (Crews and Boettiger, 2009). In other words, impaired executive functions contribute to an imbalance between the frontal cortical functions and a hyperactive limbic system that drives impulsive behavior (Bechara, 2005). Lee et al. reported that weaker dorsolateral prefrontal activation is correlated with higher cognitive impulsivity in IGD. The accuracy of the Stroop task was lower in the addiction group than in the normal group (Lee et al., 2015). Koepp et al. (1998) reported that the frontal striatal and limbic brain regions involved in the dopamine mesolimbic pathway are associated with the urge to play games and the reward mechanism.

In terms of reinforcement learning in a gaming environment, the player recognizes the current state and selects actions that maximize reward among the selectable actions (Montague, 1999). A feature of reinforcement learning is that when a player selects an action, the environment changes by his/her reactions to it. In the LOL game, it is difficult for a player to know what another player is thinking. There are those who strive to think strategically and obtain information based on the characteristics of the LOL game; however, there are also those who seek only immediate rewards. Players who only pursue immediate rewards are reward-seeking, and this can easily lead to addiction. In the brain, goal-directed control predicts a future situation based on the given information, establishes a strategy to achieve the goal, and acts as planned. In contrast, stimulus-driven control is not based on the given information and the participant responds to the situation without strategy or planning (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). Repetitive stimulus-driven control results in individuals losing control over their gaming behavior with a loss of control over gaming.

This study has several limitations. First, as the scalp EEG was recorded during real-time online gaming, it is possible that there was noise in the EEG signals due to physical movements (i.e., wrist movement, neck motion). To reduce this effect, we instructed subjects not to engage in activities unrelated to gaming. Additionally, various filters were used to remove the noise from the data in the analytic processes. Future studies should consider additional biological signals that can reflect the subject’s physical condition (i.e., EMG, PPG), which can be effective tools for establishing objective indicators during gaming. In addition, only male subjects were included in this study, which is the reason that generalization of the results of the current study may be limited. Finally, the changes in the scalp EEG signals in response to specific game events were not evaluated in this study. Future studies, including an analysis of the behavioral characteristics of individuals with IGD during specific game events and their accompanying scalp EEG signal responses, may expand on our present study.

There are several strengths associated with this study. First, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to compare EEG activity recorded in individuals with IGD and HC subjects during gaming. Most previous studies reported EEG activity measured in the resting state, but we concluded that it is important to observe the reaction of participants to gaming cues in order to understand the addiction (Choi et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2014; Park et al., 2017). In addition, we made considerable efforts to recruit subjects who only exhibited impulsivity, in order to control for other factors. Almost all previous studies conducted in patients with IGD included subjects who had comorbid disorders (depression, anxiety, AUD, and ADHD).

In summary, based on the findings reported here we conclude that an association exists between impaired cognitive control and IGD during gaming. Our data demonstrated that individuals with IGD had significantly lower theta, alpha, and beta power in the left frontal region than that of the HCs while gaming. The collective data on theta, alpha, and beta activity suggest that the low theta and alpha activity indicated a boredom state, while the low beta activity reflected impulsivity and impaired inhibitory control during gaming. In addition, the degree of left frontal theta power suppression was found to be correlated with the severity of IGD during gaming. These findings suggest that reduced percentage values for the responses in the theta, alpha, and beta bands in the left frontal region during gaming have the potential to be neurobiological markers for IGD.

Ethics Statement

All subjects provided written informed consent before beginning with the experiment. Moreover, the experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Hanyang University (IRB number: HYI-16-044).

Author Contributions

JK, JP, YP, DJ, KN, Y-CJ, and IK conceived and designed the experiments. JK and YP conducted the experiments. JP and DJ analyzed the data. KN, YJ, and IK wrote the manuscript. All authors revised and approved the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the Original Technology Research Program for Brain Science through the National Research Foundation of Korea funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT and Future Planning (NRF-2015 M3C7A1064789).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Akin, M. (2002). Comparison of wavelet transform and FFT methods in the analysis of EEG signals. J. Med. Syst. 26, 241–247. doi: 10.1023/A:1015075101937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, V., Anderson, P., and Jacobs, R. (2008). Executive Functions and the Frontal Lobes: A Lifespan Perspective. NewYork, NY: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Beard, K. W., and Wolf, E. M. (2001). Modification in the proposed diagnostic criteria for internet addiction. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 4, 377–383. doi: 10.1089/109493101300210286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1458–1463. doi: 10.1038/nn1584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Epstein, N., and Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56, 893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, M., Young, K. S., and Laier, C. (2014). Prefrontal control and Internet addiction: a theoretical model and review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:375. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J. S., Park, S. M., Lee, J., Hwang, J. Y., Jung, H. Y., Choi, S. W., et al. (2013). Resting-state beta and gamma activity in internet addiction. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 89, 328–333. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, T. J., and Ting, C. C. (2003). The role of flow experience in cyber-game addiction. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 6, 663–675. doi: 10.1089/109493103322725469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X. (2011). It’s about time. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:2. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crews, F. T., and Boettiger, C. A. (2009). Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 93, 237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crockford, D. N., Goodyear, B., Edwards, J., Quickfall, J., and el-Guebaly, N. (2005). Cue-induced brain activity in pathological gamblers. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 787–795. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G. H., DeVito, E. E., Du, X. X., and Cui, Z. Y. (2012). Impaired inhibitory control in ‘internet addiction disorder’: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 203, 153–158. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G. H., Shen, Y., Huang, J., and Du, X. X. (2013). Impaired error-monitoring function in people with internet addiction disorder: an event-related fMRI study. Eur. Addict. Res. 19, 269–275. doi: 10.1159/000346783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G. H., Zhou, H., and Zhao, X. (2011). Male internet addicts show impaired executive control ability: evidence from a color-word stroop task. Neurosci. Lett. 499, 114–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genovese, C. R., Lazar, N. A., and Nichols, T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15, 870–878. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gevins, A., Smith, M. E., McEvoy, L., and Yu, D. (1997). High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. Cereb. Cortex 7, 374–385. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.4.374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, R. Z., and Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 652–669. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groppe, D. M., Urbach, T. P., and Kutas, M. (2011). Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: a critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology 48, 1711–1725. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01273.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gundel, A., and Wilson, G. F. (1992). Topographical changes in the ongoing EEG related to the difficulty of mental tasks. Brain Topogr. 5, 17–25. doi: 10.1007/BF01129966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

HajiHosseini, A., and Holroyd, C. B. (2015). Sensitivity of frontal beta oscillations to reward valence but not probability. Neurosci. Lett. 602, 99–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.06.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, S. J., Lee, D., Park, J., Namkoong, K., Lee, J., Jang, D. P., et al. (2018). Altered heart rate variability during gameplay in internet gaming disorder: the impact of situations during the game. Front. Psychiatry 9:429. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jurado, M. B., and Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katahira, K., Yamazaki, Y., Yamaoka, C., Ozaki, H., Nakagawa, S., and Nagata, N. (2018). EEG correlates of the flow state: a combination of increased frontal theta and moderate frontocentral alpha rhythm in the mental arithmetic task. Front. Psychol. 9:300. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, N. R., Hwang, S. S., Choi, J. S., Kim, D. J., Demetrovics, Z., Kiraly, O., et al. (2016). Characteristics and psychiatric symptoms of internet gaming disorder among adults using self-reported DSM-5 criteria. Psychiatry Invest. 13, 58–66. doi: 10.4306/pi.2016.13.1.58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 606–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koepp, M. J., Gunn, R. N., Lawrence, A. D., Cunningham, V. J., Dagher, A., Jones, T., et al. (1998). Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature 393, 266–268. doi: 10.1038/30498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuss, D. J. (2013). Internet gaming addiction: current perspectives. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 6, 125–137. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S39476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, D., Hong, S. J., Jung, Y. C., Park, J., Kim, I. Y., and Namkoong, K. (2018). Altered heart rate variability during gaming in internet gaming disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 21, 259–267. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Hwang, J. Y., Park, S. M., Jung, H. Y., Choi, S. W., Kim, D. J., et al. (2014). Differential resting-state EEG patterns associated with comorbid depression in internet addiction. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Lee, S., Chun, J. W., Cho, H., Kim, D. J., and Jung, Y. C. (2015). Compromised prefrontal cognitive control over emotional interference in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 18, 661–668. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montague, P. R. (1999). Reinforcement learning: an introduction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 360–360. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01331-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuyens, F., Deleuze, J., Maurage, P., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., and Billieux, J. (2016). Impulsivity in multiplayer online battle arena gamers: preliminary results on experimental and self-report measures. J. Behav. Addict. 5, 351–356. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J. H., Hong, J. S., Han, D. H., Min, K. J., Lee, Y. S., Kee, B. S., et al. (2017). Comparison of QEEG findings between adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) without comorbidity and ADHD comorbid with internet gaming disorder. J. Korean Med. Sci. 32, 514–521. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2017.32.3.514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., and Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petry, N. M., and O’Brien, C. P. (2013). Internet gaming disorder and the DSM-5. Addiction 108, 1186–1187. doi: 10.1111/add.12162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinert, D. F., and Allen, J. P. (2002). The alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): a review of recent research. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 26, 272–279. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02534.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shallice, T., and Burgess, P. (1996). The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 351, 1405–1411. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Statista (2018). League of Legends: Number of League of Legends Monthly Active Users (MAU) from 2011 to 2016 (in millions). Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/317099/number-lol-registered-users-worldwide/ (accessed Oct 25, 2018).

Google Scholar

Thalemann, R., Wolfling, K., and Grusser, S. M. (2007). Specific cue reactivity on computer game-related cues in excessive gamers. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 614–618. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.3.614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, M. F., Wender, P. H., and Reimherr, F. W. (1993). The wender utah rating scale: an aid in the retrospective diagnosis of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 150, 885–890. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.6.885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, Y. W., Wang, L. J., Yip, S. W., Chen, P. R., Li, S., Xu, J. S., et al. (2015). Impaired decision-making under risk is associated with gaming-specific inhibition deficits among college students with internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry Res. 229, 302–309. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. (2018). WHO Adds Gaming Disorder to ICD-11. Available at: https://www.jwatch.org/fw114289/2018/06/19/who-adds-gaming-disorder-icd-11 (accessed June 19, 2018).

Google Scholar

Young, K. S. (2011). Clinical Assessment of Internet-Addicted Clients: A Handbook and Guide to Evaluation and Treatment. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 19–34.

Google Scholar

Keywords: EEG, left frontal cortex, cognitive control, theta power, internet gaming disorder

Citation: Kim J, Park J, Park YM, Jang D, Namkoong K, Jung Y-C and Kim IY (2019) Diminished Frontal Theta Activity During Gaming in Young Adults With Internet Gaming Disorder. Front. Neurosci. 13:1183. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01183

Received: 04 January 2019; Accepted: 18 October 2019;
Published: 01 November 2019.

Edited by:

Feng Feng, Peking Union Medical College Hospital (CAMS), China

Reviewed by:

Koichi Sameshima, University of São Paulo, Brazil
Saskia Sophie Steinmann, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Kim, Park, Park, Jang, Namkoong, Jung and Kim. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Young-Chul Jung, eugenejung@yuhs.ac; In Young Kim, iykim@hanyang.ac.kr

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.