Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 19 October 2020
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Recent Progress and Perspectives in Neurosteroid Research View all 20 articles

Human Cognitive Ability Is Modulated by Aromatase Availability in the Brain in a Sex-Specific Manner

  • 1Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, United States
  • 2Missouri Institute of Mental Health, St. Louis, MO, United States
  • 3National Institute on Alcohol and Alcohol Abuse, Bethesda, MD, United States
  • 4Neuroradiology Unit, Vall d’Hebron University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain
  • 5New York University, Langone Medical Center, New York, NY, United States
  • 6Stony Brook University School of Medicine, Stony Brook, NY, United States
  • 7Brookhaven National Laboratory, Upton, NY, United States

The enzyme aromatase catalyzes the final step in estrogen biosynthesis, converting testosterone to estradiol, and is expressed in the brain of all mammals. Estrogens are thought to be important for maintenance of cognitive function in women, whereas testosterone is thought to modulate cognitive abilities in men. Here, we compare differences in cognitive performance in relation to brain aromatase availability in healthy men and women. Twenty-seven healthy participants were administered tests of verbal learning and memory and perceptual/abstract reasoning. In vivo images of brain aromatase availability were acquired in this sample using positron emission tomography (PET) with the validated aromatase radiotracer [11C]vorozole. Regions of interest were placed bilaterally on the amygdala and thalamus where aromatase availability is highest in the human brain. Though cognitive performance and aromatase availability did not differ as a function of sex, higher availability of aromatase in the amygdala was associated with lower cognitive performance in men. No such relationship was found in women; and the corresponding regression slopes were significantly different between the sexes. Thalamic aromatase availability was not significantly correlated with cognitive performance in either sex. These findings suggest that the effects of brain aromatase on cognitive performance are both region- and sex-specific and may explain some of the normal variance seen in verbal and nonverbal cognitive abilities in men and women as well as sex differences in the trajectory of cognitive decline associated with Alzheimer’s disease.

Introduction

The last and obligatory step in estrogen biosynthesis in all organs and species is catalyzed by the enzyme aromatase (estrogen synthase, Cyp19 gene product), which converts the androgens androstenedione and testosterone, to the estrogens, estrone, and estradiol (Simpson et al., 2002). In reproductively competent women, the ovary is the primary source of circulating estrogens (Simpson, 2003). In both sexes, a major site of extra-gonadal estrogen synthesis is the brain, and it is characterized by widespread but heterogeneous aromatase availability (Biegon et al., 2010a, 2015; Azcoitia et al., 2011; Takahashi et al., 2018). A recent study revealed some region- and sex-specific associations between aromatase availability in the human brain and personality characteristics (Takahashi et al., 2018). To date, however, there are virtually no in vivo human studies testing the relationship between brain aromatase availability in the human brain and basic cognitive functions. The potential role of aromatase in cognition is supported by animal studies and studies in women with breast cancer reporting that administration of aromatase inhibitors (AIs) is linked to cognitive dysfunction (Rosenfeld et al., 2018). The most common deficits in women were seen in executive function and verbal episodic memory performance, although the effects of aromatase manipulation on neurobehavioral function in both animals and humans appeared to be sexually dimorphic (Shay et al., 2018). Further support of the role of aromatase in human cognition comes from postmortem studies in humans, which demonstrate region-specific changes in aromatase levels in Alzheimer’s disease (Ishunina et al., 2005; Prange-Kiel et al., 2016), suggesting that aromatase may be implicated in normal as well as pathological variations in learning and memory.

The development and application of positron emission tomography (PET) tracers for aromatase have afforded the ability to measure its availability in different brain regions noninvasively in the living human brain (Biegon et al., 2010a, 2015; Takahashi et al., 2018). With the use of this technology, it has been demonstrated that the regional distribution pattern of [11C]vorozole is heterogeneous with the highest levels of aromatase availability found in the thalamus and amygdala. In the present study, PET with [11C]vorozole was used to measure aromatase availability in the bilateral amygdala and thalamus of healthy men and women. Blood levels of testosterone and estrogen were also obtained. Participants completed tests of verbal learning and memory and perceptual reasoning in order to explore differing domains in cognitive functioning that utilize both verbal and nonverbal abilities, exploring a sex-specific aromatase–cognition association.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study population comprised 27 healthy adult participants (men, n = 12; women, n = 15), age 21–67 years. All individuals provided written informed consent prior to study participation in accordance with the Institutional Review Board and the Radioactive Drug Research Committee of Stony Brook University/Brookhaven National Laboratory. Participants were excluded for (1) recent or current use of gonadal steroids (including hormonal contraceptives); (2) cigarette smoking (Biegon et al., 2010b, 2012, 2015), recreational drug use, and medications affecting brain function; (3) neurological, psychiatric, or metabolic disorders; and (4) pregnancy in women. During the screening visit, premenopausal women reported the date of their last menstrual period, and PET scans were scheduled to coincide with the early follicular stage.

Verification of Hormonal Status on Study Day: On the day of the PET study, blood samples were obtained and sent to an outside laboratory (ARUP) for measurement of hormone levels. In men, free testosterone and estradiol levels were obtained to exclude hypogonadism. Serum estradiol (E2) concentration was determined by tandem mass spectrometry (TMS). In order to calculate free testosterone (fT), total testosterone and sex hormone binding globulin (SHBG) were measured by quantitative electrochemiluminescent immunoassay. Adult male reference intervals for fT (47–244 pg/ml) and E2 (10.0–42.0 pg/ml) were provided by ARUP. In women, progesterone and luteinizing hormone (LH) were additionally measured to verify the stage of the menstrual cycle as well as menopausal status. Five of the 15 women were postmenopausal, defined as age above 50 and more than 12 months since the last menstrual period by self-report and confirmed by high levels of LH, and low estradiol and progesterone in the postmenopausal range. Reference values for the various hormones supplied by ARUP included the following: LH females: follicular: 2.4–12.6 IU/L; mid-cycle: 14.0–95.6 IU/L; luteal: 1.0–11.4 IU/L; postmenopausal: 7.7–58.5. Estradiol: follicular phase, 27–122 pg/ml; mid-cycle phase, 95–433 pg/ml; luteal phase, 49–291 pg/ml; postmenopausal, <41 pg/ml. Progesterone: cycle days reference interval (ng/ml) 1–6, ≤0.17; 7–12, <1.35; 13–15, ≤15.63; 16–28, ≤25.55; postmenopausal: ≤0.10 IU/L).

Cognitive Tests

Participants completed the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) (Kramer et al., 2000) by the standard method. The CVLT-II is an individually administered test assessing episodic verbal learning and memory. It measures recall and recognition of two word-lists containing 16 words each recalled over immediate and delayed memory trials. There are five presentation trials followed by an immediate recall of the first list (A), followed by a one-time presentation and immediate recall of the interference list (B). Measures of free and semantically cued recall are obtained after the trial with List B (Short Delay free or cued recall), followed by a 20-min delay (Long Delay Free or Cued Recall) during which the participant cannot engage in verbal tasks. After the delay, a recognition trial is completed during which the participant is asked to identify the items from List A from a larger list that contains distractor words. Following another 10-min delay, a forced-choice trial is administered. We chose select outcome variables aimed at indexing learning through memory [total recall over the five learning trials of list A (Trials 1–5), Short and Delayed Cued Recall, and Short and Delayed Free Recall] as the primary measures of learning on the CVLT-II (Elwood, 1995).

Participants also completed the Matrix Reasoning subtest of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999), which is designed to assess nonverbal abstract problem solving, spatial, and inductive reasoning and is considered a general estimate of nonverbal intelligence. In addition, the word reading subtest of the Wide Range Achievement Test-3 (WRAT-3) (Wilkinson, 1993), an estimate of verbal intelligence also considered to be a valid measure of education level, was administered to ascertain letter and word decoding abilities (Wilkinson, 1993; Manly et al., 2002).

Positron Emission Tomography Scans

The PET images were acquired over a 90-min period using a whole-body positron emission tomograph (Siemen’s HR1, spatial resolution 4.5 mm × 4.5 mm × 4.8 mm, at the center of field of view). Radiotracer synthesis, image acquisition, and PET data analysis were carried out as previously described (Kim et al., 2009; Biegon et al., 2010a). Briefly, subjects were administered [11C]vorozole (3–8 mCi; specific activity >0.1 mCi/nmol at the time of injection) intravenously. A metabolite-corrected arterial plasma input function for [11C]vorozole was obtained from arterial blood samples withdrawn every 2.5 s for the first 2 min (Ole Dich automatic blood sampler) and then at 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 10, 15, 20, 30, 45, 60, and up to 90 min (end of study).

Image Analysis

Time frames were summed over the 90-min scanning period. The summed PET images were co-registered with structural three-dimensional magnetic resonance images of the same subject when available, using PMOD software (PMOD Technologies, Zurich, Switzerland) to confirm the anatomical location of tracer accumulation (Figure 1, right panel). Regions of interest (ROIs) were placed bilaterally on the summed image and then projected onto the dynamic images to obtain regional time activity curves. Regions occurring bilaterally (i.e., at a distance from the midline) were averaged. Carbon-11 concentration in each ROI was divided by the injected dose to obtain the % dose/cm3. A two-compartment model was used to estimate the total tissue distribution volume, VT, which includes free and nonspecifically bound tracer as well as specifically bound tracer (Innis et al., 2007). The four model parameters of the two-compartment model were optimized to obtain the best fit to the ROI data for each participant (Flannery et al., 1990; Innis et al., 2007; Biegon et al., 2010a; Pareto et al., 2013; Logan et al., 2014).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Aromatase availability in the amygdala and cognitive performance in men and women. The PET image on the right (coronal plane, overlaid on MRI) shows the regional distribution of tracer uptake at the level of the amygdala (red circles). The image was pseudocolored using the rainbow scale, with purple/blue at the low end and orange/red at the high end. The graphs on the left show the regression lines between a verbal (top left) and nonverbal (bottom left) task performance and aromatase availability in the amygdala (VT) in men (blue circles) and women (empty circles).

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics software (Version 25, IBM). Independent t-tests were used to determine if men and women differed on age, education estimates, cognitive performance, and aromatase availability. The scores on cognitive performance, as well as aromatase availability in the ROIs, were normally distributed; thus, our primary analyses were five general linear models such that sex (categorical), aromatase availability in the amygdala or thalamus (continuous), and their interaction were entered to predict each of five memory outcomes (CVLT-II outcome: Trials 1–5, Long and Short Delay Free and Cued Recall). The same approach was taken for the Matrix Reasoning subtest. A general linear model was used such that sex (categorical), amygdala or thalamus aromatase availability (continuous), and their interaction were entered to predict the estimate of perceptual reasoning. Next, we performed two-tailed Pearson correlations between aromatase availability in the amygdala or thalamus and cognitive performance both across the whole sample and as a function of sex. We further tested the difference in regression slopes between the separate test-by-sex correlations. The cognitive tests use normative data that have been stratified based on age and/or education level(s) as these factors have been found to be consistently correlated with performance, supporting our approach of separate sex by test correlations. Age and education were used as covariates in these analyses, as increasing age and lower education are associated with poorer performance on tasks of verbal learning and memory (Wilkinson, 1993). For education, we used the raw score on the Reading subtest of the WRAT (Manly et al., 2002) as a valid indicator of grade level (Wilkinson, 1993). Finally, we tested whether plasma testosterone and estradiol levels in our sample affected the aforementioned analyses. For this purpose, first, we conducted partial correlations between the cognitive test scores, aromatase availability, and blood measures of estradiol and testosterone. Second, we entered estradiol and testosterone levels in our general linear models to test whether their presence changes the models. All analyses were considered significant at the p < 0.05 threshold.

Results

There were no significant differences between men and women in age, education estimates, and aromatase availability in the amygdala and thalamus (Table 1). Likewise, there were no differences between men and women on their performance on the verbal or nonverbal cognitive tasks (Table 2). However, amygdala aromatase availability correlated with cognitive performance scores (Table 3). Analyses showed that in men only, lower amygdala aromatase availability was associated with better performance on the CVLT-II—recall following a short or long delay (r = -0.57 to -0.66, p < 0.05). Indeed, the interaction of correlation trend lines, sex × aromatase availability in the amygdala predicted free recall performance following a short delay (free: F1,22 = 5.76, p = 0.025; cued: F1,22 = 6.51 p = 0.018, Figure 1 top left). Similar results were found for recall after a long delay (free: F1,22 = 4.85, p = 0.038) with a trend for Long Delay Cued Recall (F1,22 = 3.16, p = 0.089). The same pattern was revealed on the nonverbal test, Matrix Reasoning (F1,21 = 5.32, p = 0.035) (Figure 1, bottom left). In the thalamus, there were no correlations between aromatase availability and cognitive performance in men or women (men, r = -0.08 to 0.30, p > 0.35; women, r = -0.46–0.25, p > 0.09). Plasma levels of estradiol and testosterone did not correlate with aromatase availability in either the amygdala or thalamus, nor with any of the cognitive test scores (p > 0.05). Furthermore, adding blood measures of estrogen and/or testosterone to the aforementioned general linear models did not change the results.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Age, reading ability, and aromatase availability in the brains of men and women.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparisons between male and female performance on tests of perceptual reasoning and verbal learning and memory.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of Pearson’s correlations between aromatase amygdala availability and measures of abstract reasoning and verbal learning and memory.

Discussion

Here, we have used [11C]vorozole, a thoroughly validated radiotracer for brain aromatase (3; 6; 18, 21; 22) in conjunction with PET to examine the relationship between aromatase availability in high-density regions (amygdala and thalamus) and cognitive abilities in healthy subjects. Our data show that brain aromatase availability predicted individual differences in verbal and nonverbal cognitive performance in men but not in women. Men with lower amygdala levels of aromatase had better recall for a list of words (Short Delay Free and Cued Recall and Long Delay Free Recall), on the CVLT-II. Similarly, men with lower aromatase in the amygdala also performed better on the Matrix Reasoning test. These effects were not dependent on plasma levels of estradiol and testosterone.

Animal studies suggest that brain aromatase availability is higher in males than in females and is modulated by changes in testosterone levels (Abdelgadir et al., 1994). As in our previous studies on this cohort (Biegon et al., 2010a, 2015), there were no statistically significant differences in aromatase availability in the amygdala as a function of sex, in line with previous human studies that reported comparable levels of brain aromatase and gene expression in men and women (Steckelbroeck et al., 1999; Stoffel-Wagner et al., 1999). There were also no significant effects of age or hormonal status on aromatase in the brain or any other organ beside the ovary, confirming the organ- and tissue-specific regulation of aromatase expression (Bulun and Simpson, 1994; Biegon et al., 2010a, 2015). Imaging studies suggest that even in the absence of behavioral sex differences, there are clear sex-dependent activations in regions of the brain associated with memory tasks (De Vries, 2004; Gillies and McArthur, 2010), which could be attributed to sex-linked levels of estrogens, androgens, and their receptors in the brain (Cahill, 2006). Therefore, it is not unexpected that despite the absence of sex-based differences in the amygdala aromatase availability and in cognitive performance, the relationship between the two is nonetheless sexually dimorphic.

In this regard, it is important to note that aromatase activity, while giving rise to estrogen, also decreases testosterone levels. In humans, testosterone has been shown to enhance spatial performance in men, whereas estradiol has been shown to enhance verbal memory in women (Matousek and Sherwin, 2010). Furthermore, postmortem studies in brains of men who died with Alzheimer’s disease consistently show large declines in testosterone levels, which correlated with levels of amyloid, a disease pathological marker (Rosario et al., 2004, 2011).

An additional compelling reason to support the sex-specific relationship between aromatase in the amygdala and cognitive function in our cohort, composed of mostly premenopausal women, is that in men, in whom circulating estrogen levels are low, aromatase-dependent production of estrogens from androgens is the main source of estrogens in the brain. This is not true in reproductively competent women, in whom brain levels of estrogen derive from local production as well as peripheral estrogens produced in the ovary, which diffuse freely into the brain. Since ovarian and brain aromatase expression are regulated independently (Rosario et al., 2004) via organ-specific promoter control (Golovine et al., 2003; Rosario et al., 2011), it is to be expected that androgenic and estrogenic modulation of brain function will be regional as well as more tightly correlated with local aromatase availability in men relative to women (Bulun et al., 2003; Golovine et al., 2003).

Our results further suggest that extra-gonadal (i.e., brain) estrogen synthesis and testosterone metabolism, mediated by aromatase, is implicated in verbal and nonverbal cognitive processes and therefore reveals a previously unappreciated sex-dependent relationship between aromatase and cognitive function in humans. Men with lower amygdala levels of aromatase, expected to result in lower estrogen and higher testosterone levels, had better recall for a list of words on the CVLT during short and long delay following encoding. These findings also resonate with a report showing that aromatase inhibition before and during a learning task improved working memory in male rats (Alejandre-Gomez et al., 2007). To date, there have been very few in vivo studies of brain aromatase and behavior in humans, yet recently published studies show that individual differences in brain aromatase availability are associated with individual differences in personality traits, with some sex-specific findings (Takahashi et al., 2018; Biegon et al., 2020). This is the first in vivo study to show that individual differences in aromatase availability correspond to cognitive performance, including memory. While the amygdala is a brain region best known for modulation of emotion, it is also thought to play a major role in higher cognition (Schaefer and Gray, 2007; Janak and Tye, 2015), and some of these effects are sex-dependent (Cahill, 2010; Carre et al., 2013; Shvil et al., 2014). Based on several studies and theories in the last decades, it is asserted in the literature that amygdala function is implicated in long- and short-term memory, abstract reasoning, and attention vigilance during mentally demanding cognitive tasks such as used in this paper. Even in the absence of emotion triggers during neutral cognitive tasks, attention vigilance and suppression of emotion are needed, implicating the amygdala in the output of every cognitive demand [(for good reviews, see Schaefer and Gray (2007) and Janak and Tye (2015)]. Lastly, robust sex differences have been reported in the functional connectivity of the human amygdala, specifically cortical connections, (Kilpatrick et al., 2006) further suggesting that the effects of varying levels of aromatase in the human amygdala on cognitive function are also likely to be sex-dependent. Notably, we have not observed significant sex difference in verbal learning and memory in our relatively small cohort, although better performance on CVLT in women is a consistent finding across age groups (Kramer et al., 2003; McCarrey et al., 2016; Graves et al., 2017). However, the absolute difference is also consistently small (∼10%, ibid), and studies reporting this difference as significant need a much larger sample (more than 400 subjects/sex, ibid).

Conclusion

This study used PET with [11C]vorozole to document for the first time the association between availability of aromatase in the amygdala and individual differences in cognition in healthy men and women. We demonstrate a clear sex-specific relationship between aromatase levels in the amygdala and cognitive performance, showing that men with lower aromatase availability in the amygdala had better verbal memory and spatial reasoning performance than men having higher amygdala aromatase availability. These findings also suggest that the cognitive impact of brain aromatase is both region- and sex-specific, potentially contributing to the normal variation of cognitive performance in healthy men and women.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board and the Radioactive Drug Research Committee of Stony Brook University/Brookhaven National Laboratory. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AB and NA-K conceived the idea and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All other authors read and edited the final manuscript. In addition, SK was responsible for tracer production. DP and JL performed PET image data analysis and modeling. RP-C conducted statistical analyses and writing. SM assisted in statistical analyses and writing. G-JW was the study physician. JF secured the funding.

Funding

This study was supported by NIH 1R21EB012707 (AB is the Principal Investigator). In addition, RP-C was supported by a training grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (5T32DA007135-32 to RP). SM was supported by 1K01DA037452.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abdelgadir, S. E., Resko, J. A., Ojeda, S. R., Lephart, E. D., McPhaul, M. J., and Roselli, C. E. (1994). Androgens regulate aromatase cytochrome P450 messenger ribonucleic acid in rat brain. Endocrinology 135, 395–401. doi: 10.1210/endo.135.1.8013375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alejandre-Gomez, M., Garcia-Segura, L. M., and Gonzalez-Burgos, I. (2007). Administration of an inhibitor of estrogen biosynthesis facilitates working memory acquisition in male rats. Neurosci. Res. 58, 272–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2007.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azcoitia, I., Yague, J. G., and Garcia-Segura, L. M. (2011). Estradiol synthesis within the human brain. Neuroscience 191, 139–147.

Google Scholar

Biegon, A., Alexoff, D. L., Kim, S. W., Logan, J., Pareto, D., and Schlyer, D. (2015). Aromatase imaging with [N-methyl-11C]vorozole PET in healthy men and women. J. Nucl. Med. 56, 580–585. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.114.150383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biegon, A., ALia-Klein, N., Alexoff, D. L., Fowler, J. S., Kim, S. W., Logan, J., et al. (2020). Relationship of estrogen synthesis capacity in the brain with obesity and self-control in men and women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117(37):22962–22966 doi: 10.1073/pnas.2006117117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biegon, A., Alia-Klein, N., and Fowler, J. S. (2012). Potential contribution of aromatase inhibition to the effects of nicotine and related compounds on the brain. Front. Pharmacol. 3:185. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2012.00185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biegon, A., Kim, S. W., Alexoff, D. L., Jayne, M., Carter, P., Hubbard, B., et al. (2010a). Unique distribution of aromatase in the human brain: in vivo studies with PET and [N-methyl-11C]vorozole. Synapse 64, 801–807. doi: 10.1002/syn.20791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biegon, A., Kim, S. W., Logan, J., Hooker, J. M., Muench, L., and Fowler, J. S. (2010b). Nicotine blocks brain estrogen synthase (aromatase): in vivo positron emission tomography studies in female baboons. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 774–777. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulun, S. E., and Simpson, E. R. (1994). Regulation of aromatase expression in human tissues. Breast Cancer Res. Treat. 30, 19–29. doi: 10.1007/bf00682738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulun, S. E., Sebastian, S., Takayama, K., Suzuki, T., Sasano, H., and Shozu, M. (2003). The human CYP19 (aromatase P450) gene: update on physiologic roles and genomic organization of promoters. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 86, 219–224. doi: 10.1016/s0960-0760(03)00359-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahill, L. (2006). Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 477–484. doi: 10.1038/nrn1909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahill, L. (2010). Sex influences on brain and emotional memory: the burden of proof has shifted. Prog. Brain Res. 186, 29–40.

Google Scholar

Carre, J. M., Murphy, K. R., and Hariri, A. R. (2013). What lies beneath the face of aggression? Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 224–229. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Vries, G. J. (2004). Minireview: sex differences in adult and developing brains: compensation, compensation, compensation. Endocrinology 145, 1063–1068. doi: 10.1210/en.2003-1504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elwood, R. W. (1995). the california verbal learning test: psychometric characteristics and clinical application. Neuropsychol. Rev. 5, 173–201. doi: 10.1007/bf02214761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flannery, B. P., Press, W., Teukolsky, S., and Vetterline, W. T. (1990). Numerical Recipes in C: the Art of Scientific Computing. Cambridge: Cambridge Univeristy Press.

Google Scholar

Gillies, G. E., and McArthur, S. (2010). Estrogen actions in the brain and the basis for differential action in men and women: a case for sex-specific medicines. Pharmacol. Rev. 62, 155–198. doi: 10.1124/pr.109.002071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golovine, K., Schwerin, M., and Vanselow, J. (2003). Three different promoters control expression of the aromatase cytochrome p450 gene (cyp19) in mouse gonads and brain. Biol. Reprod. 68, 978–984. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod.102.008037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graves, L. V., Moreno, C. C., Seewald, M., Holden, H. M., Uttarwar, V., and McDonald, C. R. (2017). Effects of age and gender on recall and recognition discriminability. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 32, 972–979. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innis, R. B., Cunningham, V. J., Delforge, J., Fujita, M., Gjedde, A., and Gunn, R. N. (2007). Consensus nomenclature for in vivo imaging of reversibly binding radioligands. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 27, 1533–1539. doi: 10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishunina, T. A., van Beurden, D., van der Meulen, G., Unmehopa, U. A., Hol, E. M., Huitinga, I., et al. (2005). Diminished aromatase immunoreactivity in the hypothalamus, but not in the basal forebrain nuclei in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 173–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janak, P. H., and Tye, K. M. (2015). From circuits to behaviour in the amygdala. Nature 517, 284–292. doi: 10.1038/nature14188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilpatrick, L. A., Zald, D. H., Pardo, J. V., and Cahill, L. F. (2006). Sex-related differences in amygdala functional connectivity during resting conditions. NeuroImage 30, 452–461. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. W., Biegon, A., Katsamanis, Z. E., Ehrlich, C. W., Hooker, J. M., Shea, C., et al. (2009). Reinvestigation of the synthesis and evaluation of [N-methyl-(11)C]vorozole, a radiotracer targeting cytochrome P450 aromatase. Nucl. Med. Biol. 36, 323–334. doi: 10.1016/j.nucmedbio.2008.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, J. H., Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test—2nd Edition, San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Kramer, J. H., Yaffe, K., Lengenfelder, J., and Delis, D. C. (2003). Age and gender interactions on verbal memory performance. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 9, 97–102 doi: 10.1017/s1355617703910113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, J., Kim, S. W., Pareto, D., Telang, F., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., et al. (2014). Kinetic analysis of [11C]vorozole binding in the human brain with positron emission tomography. Mol. Imag. 13, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Manly, J. J., Jacobs, D. M., Touradji, P., Small, S. A., and Stern, Y. (2002). Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 341–348. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702813157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matousek, R. H., and Sherwin, B. B. (2010). Sex steroid hormones and cognitive functioning in healthy, older men. Horm. Behav. 57, 352–359. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarrey, A., Yang, A., Kitner-Triolo, M., Ferrucci, L., and Resnick, S. (2016). Sex differences in cognitive trajectories in clinically normal older adults. Psychol. Aging 31, 166–175. doi: 10.1037/pag0000070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pareto, D., Biegon, A., Alexoff, D., Carter, P., Shea, C., Muench, L., and Xu, Y. (2013). In vivo imaging of brain aromatase in female baboons: [11C]vorozole kinetics and effect of the menstrual cycle. Mol. Imag. 12, 518–524.

Google Scholar

Prange-Kiel, J., Dudzinski, D. A., Prols, F., Glatzel, M., Matschke, J., and Rune, G. M. (2016). Aromatase expression in the hippocampus of AD patients and 5xFAD Mice. Neural. Plast. 2016:9802086.

Google Scholar

Rosario, E. R., Chang, L., Stanczyk, F. Z., and Pike, C. J. (2004). Age-related testosterone depletion and the development of Alzheimer disease. JAMA 292, 1431–1432. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.12.1431-b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosario, E. R., Chang, L., Head, E. H., Stanczyk, F. Z., and Pike, C. J. (2011). Brain levels of sex steroid hormones in men and women during normal aging and in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 604–613. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenfeld, C. S., Shay, D. A., and Vieira-Potter, V. J. (2018). Cognitive effects of aromatase and possible role in memory disorders. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne) 9:610. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2018.00610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, A., and Gray, J. R. (2007). A role for the human amygdala in higher cognition. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 355–363.

Google Scholar

Shay, D. A., Vieira-Potter, V. J, and Rosenfeld, C. S. (2018). Sexually dimorphic effects of aromatase on neurobehavioral responses. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 11:374. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shvil, E., Sullivan, G. M., Schafer, S., Markowitz, J. C., Campeas, M., Wager, T. D., et al. (2014). Sex differences in extinction recall in posttraumatic stress disorder: a pilot fMRI study. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 113, 101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, E. R. (2003). Sources of estrogen and their importance. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 86, 225–230. doi: 10.1016/s0960-0760(03)00360-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, E. R., Clyne, C., Rubin, G., Robertson, K., Britt, K., Speed, C., et al. (2002). Aromatase–a brief overview. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 64, 93–127. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.64.081601.142703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steckelbroeck, S., Heidrich, D. D., Hans, V. H., Schramm, J., Bidlingmaier, F., and Klingmuller, D. (1999). Characterization of aromatase cytochrome P450 activity in the human temporal lobe. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 84, 2795–2801. doi: 10.1210/jc.84.8.2795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoffel-Wagner, B., Watzka, M., Schramm, J., Bidlingmaier, F., and Klingmuller, D. (1999). Expression of CYP19 (aromatase) mRNA in different areas of the human brain. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 70, 237–241. doi: 10.1016/s0960-0760(99)00114-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, K., Hosoya, T., Onoe, K., Takashima, T., Tanaka, M., Ishii, A., et al. (2018). Association between aromatase in human brains and personality traits. Sci. Rep. 8:16841.

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence., San Antonio, TX: Psychological Cooporation.

Google Scholar

Wilkinson, G. (1993). The Wide-Range Achievement Test 3: Administration Manual. Wilminton, DE: Wide-Range Inc.

Google Scholar

Keywords: aromatase, [11C]vorozole, estrogen, testosterone, cognition, PET, amygdala, human brain

Citation: Alia-Klein N, Preston-Campbell RN, Kim SW, Pareto D, Logan J, Wang G-J, Moeller SJ, Fowler JS and Biegon A (2020) Human Cognitive Ability Is Modulated by Aromatase Availability in the Brain in a Sex-Specific Manner. Front. Neurosci. 14:565668. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.565668

Received: 03 July 2020; Accepted: 14 September 2020;
Published: 19 October 2020.

Edited by:

Vance L. Trudeau, University of Ottawa, Canada

Reviewed by:

Thierry D. Charlier, University of Rennes 1, France
Neil James MacLusky, University of Guelph, Canada

Copyright © 2020 Alia-Klein, Preston-Campbell, Kim, Pareto, Logan, Wang, Moeller, Fowler and Biegon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anat Biegon, anat.biegon@stonybrookmedicine.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.