Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 19 September 2019
Sec. Sleep and Circadian Rhythms
This article is part of the Research Topic Sleep and Circadian Rhythms Editor’s Pick 2021 View all 15 articles

Brain Activity Correlates With Cognitive Performance Deterioration During Sleep Deprivation

  • 1Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, United States
  • 2Facultad de Ingeniería Mecánica, Electrónica y Biomédica, Universidad Antonio Nariño, Bogota, Colombia

We studied the correlation between oscillatory brain activity and performance in healthy subjects performing the error awareness task (EAT) every 2 h, for 24 h. In the EAT, subjects were shown on a screen the names of colors and were asked to press a key if the name of the color and the color it was shown in matched, and the screen was not a duplicate of the one before (“Go” trials). In the event of a duplicate screen (“Repeat No-Go” trial) or a color mismatch (“Stroop No-Go” trial), the subjects were asked to withhold from pressing the key. We assessed subjects’ (N = 10) response inhibition by measuring accuracy of the “Stroop No-Go” (SNGacc) and “Repeat No-Go” trials (RNGacc). We assessed their reactivity by measuring reaction time in the “Go” trials (GRT). Simultaneously, nine electroencephalographic (EEG) channels were recorded (Fp2, F7, F8, O1, Oz, Pz, O2, T7, and T8). The correlation between reactivity and response inhibition measures to brain activity was tested using quantitative measures of brain activity based on the relative power of gamma, beta, alpha, theta, and delta waves. In general, response inhibition and reactivity reached a steady level between 6 and 16 h of sleep deprivation, which was followed by sustained impairment after 18 h. Channels F7 and Fp2 had the highest correlation to the indices of performance. Measures of response inhibition (RNGacc and SNGacc) were correlated to the alpha and theta waves’ power for most of the channels, especially in the F7 channel (r = 0.82 and 0.84, respectively). The reactivity (GRT) exhibited the highest correlation to the power of gamma waves in channel Fp2 (0.76). We conclude that quantitative measures of EEG provide information that can help us to better understand changes in subjects’ performance and could be used as an indicator to prevent the adverse consequences of sleep deprivation.

Introduction

This paper explores the effect of sleep deprivation on both cognitive performance and brain activity in healthy subjects. Given that alertness and brain activity are known to diminish in parallel during prolonged periods of wakefulness (Borbély et al., 2016), objective techniques for the assessment of brain activity may enable prevention of some of the common fatal events caused due to human error, and reduce the economic cost of human performance deterioration. For example, performance deterioration due to fatigue can lead to accidents in jobs that frequently require working late at night or for long periods (Leger, 1994; Costa, 1996; Lyznicki et al., 1998). Electroencephalography (EEG) is a widely used method for providing quantitative and better dynamic assessment of the electric activity in the brain (Tatum, 2014). We focus on the relative power of EEG as it has been used to avoid the high variability of the absolute power (Monastra et al., 2001; Kropotov, 2010; Pavlov and Kotchoubey, 2017).

An effective means to monitor human job performance is a long-sought goal, especially ways to detect errors and warn an individual if their ability has been affected by something like sleep deprivation. If detection were possible, the individual could be warned to enact appropriate mitigating countermeasures (Gehring et al., 1993; Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2001). EEG is a powerful candidate for such monitoring, as studies of neural responses to performance errors suggest that the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices are critical to error processing, although the specific roles of those regions are a matter of debate (Bush et al., 2000).

It has been shown that both the intensity of EEG waves upon falling asleep and the desire to sleep are linked to the period of time spent awake (Borbély et al., 2016). The process of becoming increasingly tired throughout the day is attributed to the effects on the brain of the neurotransmitter adenosine. Endogenous adenosine inhibits basal forebrain and mesopontine cholinergic neurons, which play a large part in abrupt shifts in EEG frequency, termed EEG arousal (Steriade et al., 1990). As extracellular adenosine concentration increases, so does inhibition of the cholinergic neurons. There are many reasons why researchers believe that adenosine produces the effects of tiredness. During wakefulness, neural metabolism is much higher than during deep sleep, while adenosine concentration in the brain and neural metabolic activity have been linked. Also, caffeine is known to reduce the effects of adenosine and promote arousal in subjects and their EEG waveforms, by blocking adenosine receptors, Porkka-Heiskanen et al. (1997).

The typical EEG waveform consists of alpha, beta, theta, gamma, and delta waves, which differ in frequency as well as amplitude (Dement, 1974; Tatum, 2014). Alpha, beta, and gamma waves are in a higher frequency range (>8 Hz). In most cases, high-frequency low-amplitude waves are concurrent with higher activity of the brain. When awake, most people exhibit beta and alpha wave patterns. Beta waves have the highest frequency and the lowest amplitude than other waves in an awake, alert individual. They are dominant when a person is not sleeping. During periods of relaxation our brain waves become slower, which is accompanied by an increase in the amplitude of alpha waves. They tend to diminish when the eyes are open or when the brain exercises mental effort. In other words, lower levels of activity are represented by higher levels of alpha waves. Gamma waves are linked to voluntary motor movement, learning, and memory. Delta and theta waves are in a lower frequency range. Delta waves are related to slow-wave sleep and have been found during some continuous-attention tasks (Kirmizi-Alsan et al., 2006). As for the theta waves, high amplitude theta waves correlate with memory function while lower-amplitude ones are linked to decreased alertness and increased drowsiness. Low-frequency (delta and theta) brain activity has been observed to increase as a result of sleep deprivation (Cajochen et al., 1999; Hung et al., 2013; Gorgoni et al., 2014; Fattinger et al., 2017).

Several studies have explored the correlation between sleep deprivation, brain activity, and behavioral performance (Lorenzo et al., 1995; Corsi-Cabrera et al., 1996; Forest and Godbout, 2000; Yokoi et al., 2003; Gorgoni et al., 2014; Bernardi et al., 2015). Significant correlations have been found between the EEG increase in theta activity observed during sleep deprivation and a slowing down of reaction times (Gorgoni et al., 2014), or an increase in behavioral errors (Bernardi et al., 2015; Quercia et al., 2018).

Traditionally, EEG signals are divided in short-time epochs and spectral analysis is applied to the epochs to obtain continuous waves. In this study, we have explored a different approach, in which the different waves were isolated in the time domain using finite impulse response (FIR) filters. The spectral power of each wave was computed for each channel in the 4-min segment while the subjects performed a cognitive task, and normalized to the total spectral power of each channel. This analysis quantifies the brain activity during the entire period of time the subject is undergoing a task, instead of linking it directly to specific instantaneous events (e.g., errors or hits), as it is traditionally done. This method of quantifying brain activity can account for the possible relationship between brain activity at a given moment and the committing of future errors. In other words, this method allows us to correlate the overall brain activity with the overall performance, during a given period of time. This approach is simpler and has led us to find stronger correlations between brain activity and performance.

The EEG provides a practical and quantitative way to assess the electrical activity of the brain. This study seeks to examine the correlation between relative power of EEG waves and performance over a period 4 min, during a cognitive assessment. To evaluate this, we have employed a simple yet robust test that requires the subject to maintain response inhibition and reactivity. This information from the neurophysiological measures of the oscillatory activity of the brain can complement physiological measures used in studies focused on autonomic reactions (Posada-Quintero et al., 2017, 2018), to predict the effect of sleep deprivation on human autonomic response and performance.

The resulting data could lead to a tool for prediction of the drowsiness and fatigue states of a person by measuring the electrical activity of the brain. To the best of our knowledge, no other study has analyzed the correlation between task-related relative EEG activity in the different frequency bands and performance during periods of prolonged wakefulness. This information can be used to predict and reduce risk in environments where it is expected that people suffer from sleep deprivation.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Ten healthy volunteers (three females, seven males; age range 25–35) were enrolled in this study. Participants were required to remain awake for at least 24 h. An experiment manager was always observing the participants, to ensure safety and procedural validity of the experimental protocol. During the 7 days prior to the experiment, participants recorded their sleep patterns in a data sheet, to indicate compliance to the experimental constraints.

Protocol

For participating in the study, the participants were required to halt all consumption of stimulants and depressants beginning 48 h prior to the start of the experiment. Participants were instructed to arrive at the experimental facility, located at the University of Connecticut, within 2 h of waking up on the morning of the study. Participants completed a learning run for the experimental task within the 1st h of arrival. Afterward, they were asked to perform a run of the task every 2 h for the duration of the 24-h period, for a total of 12 runs besides the training run. Participants remained in the building for approximately 25 h to allow for the completion of all 12 runs and training.

Twenty minutes before starting the task, an EEG cap with ten electrodes was placed on the participant’s head. We used an actiCHamp amplifier (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) with an EasyCap electrode system (EasyCap GmbH, Herrsching-Breitbrunn, Germany). The electrodes were placed in frontal (F), frontal-polar (Fp), temporal (T), parietal (P), and occipital (O) positions, as shown in Figure 1. Electrode gel was used to provide appropriate conductance between the electrode and the skin. Prior to EEG data measurement, the electrodes’ impedance was checked to make sure it was lower than 5 kΩ, to ensure good electrode connection to the scalp. Nine EEG channels were recorded: Fp2, F7, F8, O1, Oz, Pz, O2, T7, and T8, while the subject was performing the error awareness task (EAT) with their eyes open. The input signals were referenced to the ears, band pass filtered with cutoff frequencies of 0.5 and 50 Hz, and digitized at a sampling rate of 200 Hz.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Location of EEG channels recorded. The circles represent the approximate location of the electrode for each channel.

Between runs, leads were disconnected and the subjects were allowed to eat or do any activity other than exercising or sleeping (for example, using a laptop, reading, going to the restroom, talking on the phone, and so forth). To avoid any undesirable influence on sympathetic arousal, subjects had to remain in the building. To ensure adherence to the protocol, food was provided. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Institutional Review Board of the University of Connecticut, and with their approval of the study protocol. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Performance Assessment

For this study, we used the EAT. We presented a serial stream of names of colors in colored fonts. Each name was presented for 900 ms, with 600 ms between names. The test was 5 min long in this study. Participants were trained to respond to each new screen with the space bar. Subjects were requested to press the space bar for “Go” trials and withhold this response for “No-Go” trials. A “Go” trial occurred when the font color matched the color’s name and did not fall into the category of a “No-Go” trial. A “No-Go” trial could be generated by two different circumstances: (1) if the same word was presented on two consecutive trials (repeat “No-go” task), or (2) if the color named did not match the font color (Stroop “No-Go” task) (Hester et al., 2005). Figure 2 illustrates the procedure. The second instance of “No-Go” trial represents a Stroop effect, which induces cognitive stress in the subject (Stroop, 1935). The two withhold conditions were meant to maintain subjects’ vigilant attention so they did not fall back on repetitive behavior. Constantly having to monitor the color match kept subjects alert.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The EAT required subjects to respond to a stream of color names by pressing a key, and to withhold their response based on “No-Go” trial rules.

To assess subject performance, the accuracy of Stroop and Repeat “No-Go” tasks (SNGacc and RNGacc, respectively) and average “Go trial” reaction times (GRT) were computed for every trial. SNGacc and RNGacc constitute measures directly proportional to how vigilantly a subject paid attention. For its part, GRT is a simple measure to assess subject reactivity. Notice that GRT exhibits an inverse relationship to subject performance, as a higher reaction time represents lower performance.

EEG Data Processing

Four minutes of clean EEG data were extracted for each subject during each EAT trial. During EAT, subjects sat in a chair and were instructed not to talk or move. For this reason, the EEG signals were stable and no significant artifact corruption was usually observed. When a few instances of motion artifacts were identified, we removed the corrupted data segments (a fraction of a second) from all the channels. The data segment removed was compensated for by selecting a larger window, so that all the analyzed segments were 4 min long. To assess the brain activity, we computed quantitative measures based on spectral analysis of the nine channels of EEG data. We used five bands, with ranges set at <4 Hz (delta), 4–8 Hz (theta), 8–13 Hz (alpha), 13–30 Hz (beta), and >30 Hz (gamma) in accordance with the literature. The five waves were obtained using FIR filters designed using the Parks-McClellan optimal equiripple approach. The power was computed using the following equation:

P x = 1 L t = 1 L x 2 ( t )

Where x is the signal and L is the length of the signal (4 min). This is equivalent to the mean square of the signal. We used the relative power (to the total power) of these waves as a quantitative measure of brain activity.

Statistics

A total of 45 measures of brain activity were computed—the power of the five bands in each of the nine channels of EEG. As a measure of subject performance, average GRT, and Stroop No-Go and Repeat No-Go accuracies (SNGacc and RNGacc, respectively) were computed for every run of every subject.

To evaluate the significance of the differences in the measures of EEG and performance during the experiment, we performed multiple comparisons of the indices between the 12 runs. The normality of each index was tested using the one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (Massey, 1951; Miller, 1956; Marsaglia et al., 2003). Given that all measures were normally distributed, the one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to test for significant differences between runs. The Bonferroni method was used for correction of multiple comparisons.

Correlation Analysis

We computed the Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) between mean values of measures of performance (GRT, SNGacc, and RNGacc) and the 45 measures of brain activity (from EEG) of the participants (over the 12 runs of the 24-h test). The t-test was used to assess the statistical significance of the correlation coefficient (the null hypothesis was that the product moment correlation coefficient was zero) (Spiegel, 1961).

Results

The time duration between the waking time and the first session was less than 40 min. This short time duration allows us to expect that possible circadian effects are negligible because of the small difference in the start time of the experiment across subjects. The mean starting time of the first session was 8:25 AM. Figure 3 shows the plots of the topographic maps of the scalp data field in a 2-D circular view (looking down at the top of the head) using interpolation on a fine Cartesian grid. Note that each wave is plotted in a different scale, shown at the bottom of the figure. The rows represent the hours of sleep deprivation at the time of the measurement, and the columns represent the EEG waves considered in this study. The topographic maps are the result of averaging all the subjects together. In this study, gamma and beta waves were more prominent in the front throughout the 24 h. Alpha waves were more prominent in the parietal region, with noticeable changes during the 24 h of the experiment. Theta waves exhibited higher power in the parietal, occipital, and temporal regions, with an apparent reduction noticeable after 16 h. Delta waves presented higher power in the parietal, occipital, and temporal regions than it did in the other lobes and their power increased toward the end of the 24-h period.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Averaged scalp topographies of the gamma, beta, alpha, theta and delta waves of the subjects performing the EAT for the 12 runs during the 24 h of testing.

Figure 4 shows the change in performance measures during the 24 h of sleep deprivation, along with the quantitative measure of EEG with which they exhibited the highest correlation. Significant differences between the runs are marked. Figure 5 shows the correlation plot for those pairs of measures. Figures 4A,B show that SNGacc and RNGacc indices exhibited stable values for a majority of the 1st 14 h of the experiment, followed by a sustained drop in value after 18 h. RNGacc showed a significant decrease at 22 h compared to most of the runs before 18 h, and a decrease at 24 h compared to the first 14 h runs. RNGacc exhibited a peak value at 14 h. GRT at 6, 8, and 10 h was significantly lower than the measurement at 2 h. GRT increased after 14 h. GRT can be summarized as 4 h of learning period after which the subjects reached a plateau where the average reaction time stabilized between 6 and 16 h, ending in a final increase in the reaction time, possibly related to subjects’ tiredness.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean ± Standard Error of (A) RNGacc, (B) SNGacc, and (C) GRT measures and their most-correlated quantitative measure of EEG. Symbols denote significant difference: to run 1; ⊢ to runs 1, 2 and 7; ‡ to runs 1, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 9; # to run 7; and † to runs 1 and 2.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlation of (A) RNGacc and power of theta wave of channel F7, (B) SNGacc and power of theta wave of channel F7, and (C) GRT and power of gamma wave of channel T8.

Using quantitative analysis of EEG data, we performed correlation analysis between brain activity and performance. Table 1 includes the results for the correlation analysis of all quantitative measures of EEG available in this study (five waves times nine channels), to the three measures of performance obtained from the subjects performing the EAT. Higher overall correlation to the brain activity was found for the SNGacc and RNGacc, compared to GRT. The highest correlation was found between the SNGacc measure and the theta waves of the F7 channel (r = 0.84). Such wave exhibited the highest correlation to the RNGacc (r = 0.82). RNGacc and SNGacc were found to be significantly correlated with theta waves of all the channels except the frontal polar (Fp2). These measures were also found to be inversely correlated to delta waves in all lobes except for frontal (F) channels and the frontal polar channel (Fp). As for the gamma and beta waves, RNGacc and SNGacc were only found to be significantly correlated to those waves in frontal channels (F). These two measures of performance were not found to be correlated to alpha waves of any channel.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Correlation (r) between EEG measures and performance measures.

The highest absolute correlation of GRT was with the gamma and delta waves of channel Fp2 (0.76). GRT was significantly correlated to gamma, beta, and alpha waves of channels T8 and Fp2. This measure of performance was also found to be moderately correlated to alpha and delta waves of channel F7.

Discussion

We have implemented a task that allows simultaneous evaluation of response inhibition and reactivity, as components of overall cognitive performance of subjects undergoing sleep deprivation. This test has been used previously to study the relationship between autonomic reactions and performance on young and healthy subjects (Posada-Quintero et al., 2017). In this study, we have used quantitative measures of EEG for the assessment of oscillatory brain activity. Results allowed us to observe the changes on the different waves (covering different frequency ranges) during the 24 h of sleep deprivation, and how the power of those waves correlates with changes in the measures of response inhibition and reactivity. This led to the conclusion that these quantitative measures of EEG provide valuable information that could potentially be used to prevent an individual from inadvertently performing undesirable actions when sleep deprived.

Remarkably, two channels contained oscillatory power that was the most correlated to the indices of performance: channels F7 and Fp2. Specifically, the power of the theta waves of channel F7 exhibited the highest correlation to RNGacc (r = 0.82) and SNGacc (r = 0.84), related to subjects’ response inhibition. The power of the gamma and delta waves of channel Fp2 exhibited the highest correlation to the measure of reactivity, GRT (r = 0.76). This is an interesting observation because if a reduced set of channels of EEG is sufficient for providing a neurophysiological objective measure that is able to track the effects of sleep deprivation, it increases the feasibility of deploying the EEG for practical applications.

In this study, theta waves exhibited high correlation with the “No-Go” trials, assessed with the RNGacc and SNGacc measures. Theta waves have been linked to a subject’s voluntary repression of provoked responses, as they have been found to increase in circumstances where individuals are intentionally trying to inhibit a reaction (Kirmizi-Alsan et al., 2006). Our findings corroborate this relationship between subjects’ inhibitory responses and theta waves, particularly high in the frontal channels.

Beta waves are linked to intense brain activity, stress, active thinking, and focus, among other circumstances (Lalo et al., 2007; Baumeister et al., 2008). The power of the beta waves in the frontal channels was moderately correlated to subjects’ sustained response inhibition (RNGacc and SNGacc measures). This behavior of power of the beta waves suggests that subjects’ active thinking and focus were diminished after 14 h of sleep deprivation, and it might have contributed to the reduction in performance related to vigilant attention.

We found that gamma waves in the frontal polar (Fp2) and right temporal (T8) channels were significantly correlated to subjects’ reaction time (GRT) throughout the 24-h experiment. Gamma waves in frontal channels (F1 and F2) were related to subjects’ vigilant attention (RNGacc and SNGacc). Gamma waves are present during short-term memory matching of recognizable elements presented to the sight, and other sensory stimuli (Kisley and Cornwell, 2006; Kanayama et al., 2007). With some controversy, these waves are thought to be implicated in the conscious perception (Gray, 1999; Vanderwolf, 2000). We observed that the power of the gamma waves mostly correlated to the rapidness the subjects exhibited in making reaction decisions, which involves memory, matching, and motor functions.

Alteration of the power in the delta waves observed in this study (Figure 3). was inversely correlated with the RNGacc and SNGacc measures (beyond −0.81), mainly in the occipital, parietal, and temporal channels (especially O1, O2, and PZ). They were also inversely correlated to GRT in the frontal polar channel (−0.76). The measure of reactivity, GRT, seemed to be more linked to the delta oscillatory activity of the right side of the brain, as higher correlation was found in channel T8.

As for the alpha waves, it seems like they moved from the frontal to the parietal lobe during the experiment. Alpha waves are reportedly found predominantly in posterior sides of the head, and are reduced with drowsiness and sleep (Palva and Palva, 2007). Alpha waves are also linked to inhibitory control (Kamarajan et al., 2004; Klimesch, 2012). Alpha waves only exhibited significant correlation to GRT in channel Fp2 (−0.71).

Performance and physiology are expected to be as sensitive to circadian rhythms as to sleep deprivation in a 24-h period of wakefulness. However, sleep deprivation is known to cause an overall increase in reaction time and increased errors of omission and commission (Lim and Dinges, 2008). By the end of this study, at about 8 AM, the effect of the circadian rhythm should, in theory, cause a recovery of the subject’s responsiveness and response inhibition. Instead, Figure 4 shows a continued trend of increase in the GRT and decrease in response inhibition measures (RNGacc and SNGacc). This indicates that participants are more affected by sleep deprivation than by the circadian rhythm. The same interpretation can be made with the measures of brain activity.

Multiple studies have reported that low-frequency waves, mainly theta waves, increase as a result of sleep deprivation (Cajochen et al., 1999; Hung et al., 2013; Gorgoni et al., 2014; Fattinger et al., 2017). There is also evidence of a moderate positive correlation between theta waves and the committing of errors (Lorenzo et al., 1995; Corsi-Cabrera et al., 1996; Forest and Godbout, 2000; Yokoi et al., 2003; Gorgoni et al., 2014; Bernardi et al., 2015). In those studies, the power of the waves was normalized to the power of the first run. As we were interested in studying the changes in the waves over increasing time of sleep deprivation, and the correlation of the overall brain activity to performance, we did not normalize to the power in the first run to avoid changing the “system gain” for each subject. Instead, for quantifying the power of the bands we used the relative power of the bands with respect to the total power. We hypothesize that this difference in approach may have led to different trends in theta waves. Although we found a strong positive correlation between the power of delta waves and the impairment of performance (maximum r = 0.81), we found a strong negative correlation between the power of theta waves and performance (maximum r = −0.84). Note that delta waves exhibited much higher power (ranging 0.1–0.22) compared to theta waves (ranging 0.06–0.15) (Figure 3). As delta waves increase by the end of the experiment, it produces an overall decrease in the relative power of theta waves. Figure 6 shows the absolute power of theta waves normalized to the power of the first run, from 16 to 24 h of sleep deprivation. The expected increase in theta waves as a result of sleep deprivation concomitant with the impairment of performance is also observed in our data. In spite of the different analytical approach (absolute vs. relative power), our results are substantially in line with previous findings indicating that sleep deprivation is associated with a relative increase of low-frequency (vs. high-frequency) activity that is in turn correlated with performance impairment.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Increase in the absolute power of theta waves in all channels toward the end of the 24-h experiment.

The limited sample size and characteristics of the set of participants (ten healthy subjects) limit the conclusiveness of this study. This was a quite demanding study protocol requiring 24 h of sleep deprivation. To draw general conclusions, more data are needed. We acknowledge that given the many correlations relevant for the study, there is a good chance that random data would yield significant correlation (p < 0.05). The analysis of gender differences is not possible with the current data set and needs to be explored in future studies. Also, EAT measures of performance are certainly linked to reactivity and attention, but do not fully replicate the kinds of tasks a subject encounters in real life. It should be noted that despite the unavoidable variability between subjects’ skills and training conditions, and a limited sample size, we found considerable consistency in measures of performance throughout the ten subjects.

Conclusion

Alteration of the power of theta and beta waves in frontal channels followed a trend similar to measures of response inhibition, measured by accuracy in performing the EAT, whereas the changes of power in gamma and delta waves in the frontal polar channel resembled more the changes in reactivity, measured by reaction time. We conclude that quantitative measures of EEG provide information to better understand changes in subject performance and could be used to prevent the adverse consequences of sleep deprivation. Furthermore, objective neurophysiological measures can provide valuable information to systems intended to assess a human’s readiness to perform.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Institutional Review Board of The University of Connecticut, with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of The University of Connecticut.

Author Contributions

HP-Q performed the experiments, data analysis, and wrote a draft of the manuscript. NR and JB performed the experiments and edited the manuscript. AO-C performed the data analysis and edited the manuscript. KC conceptualized the study and edited the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Baumeister, J., Barthel, T., Geiss, K. R., and Weiss, M. (2008). Influence of phosphatidylserine on cognitive performance and cortical activity after induced stress. Nutr. Neurosci. 11, 103–110. doi: 10.1179/147683008X301478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernardi, G., Siclari, F., Yu, X., Zennig, C., Bellesi, M., Ricciardi, E., et al. (2015). Neural and behavioral correlates of extended training during sleep deprivation in humans: evidence for local, task-specific effects. J. Neurosci. 35, 4487–4500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4567-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borbély, A. A., Daan, S., Wirz-Justice, A., and Deboer, T. (2016). The two-process model of sleep regulation: a reappraisal. J. Sleep Res. 25, 131–143. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, G., Luu, P., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Foy, R., and Dijk, D.-J. (1999). Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res. Online 2, 65–69.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Corsi-Cabrera, M., Arce, C., Ramos, J., Lorenzo, I., and Guevara, M. A. (1996). Time course of reaction time and EEG while performing a vigilance task during total sleep deprivation. Sleep 19, 563–569. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.7.563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, G. (1996). The impact of shift and night work on health. Appl. Ergon. 27, 9–16. doi: 10.1016/0003-6870(95)00047-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dement, W. C. (1974). Some Must Watch While Some Must Sleep. New York, NY: WH Freeman.

Google Scholar

Fattinger, S., Kurth, S., Ringli, M., Jenni, O. G., and Huber, R. (2017). Theta waves in children’s waking electroencephalogram resemble local aspects of sleep during wakefulness. Sci. Rep. 7:11187. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11577-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forest, G., and Godbout, R. (2000). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance and EEG spectral analysis in young adults. Brain Cogn. 43, 195–200.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., and Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychol. Sci. 4, 385–390. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorgoni, M., Ferlazzo, F., Ferrara, M., Moroni, F., D’Atri, A., Fanelli, S., et al. (2014). Topographic electroencephalogram changes associated with psychomotor vigilance task performance after sleep deprivation. Sleep Med. 15, 1132–1139. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. M. (1999). The temporal correlation hypothesis of visual feature integration: still alive and well. Neuron 24, 31–47. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80820-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hester, R., Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Shpaner, M., and Garavan, H. (2005). Neural mechanisms involved in error processing: a comparison of errors made with and without awareness. Neuroimage 27, 602–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, C.-S., Sarasso, S., Ferrarelli, F., Riedner, B., Ghilardi, M. F., Cirelli, C., et al. (2013). Local experience-dependent changes in the wake EEG after prolonged wakefulness. Sleep 36, 59–72. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamarajan, C., Porjesz, B., Jones, K. A., Choi, K., Chorlian, D. B., Padmanabhapillai, A., et al. (2004). The role of brain oscillations as functional correlates of cognitive systems: a study of frontal inhibitory control in alcoholism. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 51, 155–180. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanayama, N., Sato, A., and Ohira, H. (2007). Crossmodal effect with rubber hand illusion and gamma-band activity. Psychophysiology 44, 392–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00511.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirmizi-Alsan, E., Bayraktaroglu, Z., Gurvit, H., Keskin, Y. H., Emre, M., and Demiralp, T. (2006). Comparative analysis of event-related potentials during Go/NoGo and CPT: decomposition of electrophysiological markers of response inhibition and sustained attention. Brain Res. 1104, 114–128. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kisley, M. A., and Cornwell, Z. M. (2006). Gamma and beta neural activity evoked during a sensory gating paradigm: effects of auditory, somatosensory and cross-modal stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 2549–2563. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 606–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kropotov, J. D. (2010). Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Lalo, E., Gilbertson, T., Doyle, L., Di Lazzaro, V., Cioni, B., and Brown, P. (2007). Phasic increases in cortical beta activity are associated with alterations in sensory processing in the human. Exp. Brain Res. 177, 137–145. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0655-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leger, D. (1994). The cost of sleep-related accidents: a report for the national commission on sleep disorders research. Sleep 17, 84–93. doi: 10.1093/sleep/17.1.84

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, J., and Dinges, D. F. (2008). Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1129, 305–322. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo, I., Ramos, J., Arce, C., Guevara, M. A., and Corsi-Cabrera, M. (1995). Effect of total sleep deprivation on reaction time and waking EEG activity in man. Sleep 18, 346–354.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lyznicki, J. M., Doege, T. C., Davis, R. M., and Williams, M. A. (1998). Sleepiness, driving, and motor vehicle crashes. Council on scientific affairs, American medical association. JAMA 279, 1908–1913.

Google Scholar

Marsaglia, G., Tsang, W. W., and Wang, J. (2003). Evaluating kolmogorov’s distribution. J. Stat. Softw. 8, 1–4.

Google Scholar

Massey, F. J. Jr. (1951). The kolmogorov-smirnov test for goodness of fit. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 46, 68–78.

Google Scholar

Miller, L. H. (1956). Table of percentage points of kolmogorov statistics. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 51, 111–121. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1956.10501314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monastra, V. J., Lubar, J. F., and Linden, M. (2001). The development of a quantitative electroencephalographic scanning process for attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder: reliability and validity studies. Neuropsychology 15, 136–144. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.15.1.136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palva, S., and Palva, J. M. (2007). New vistas for alpha-frequency band oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 30, 150–158. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlov, Y. G., and Kotchoubey, B. (2017). EEG correlates of working memory performance in females. BMC Neurosci. 18:26. doi: 10.1186/s12868-017-0344-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porkka-Heiskanen, T., Strecker, R. E., Thakkar, M., Bjørkum, A. A., Greene, R. W., and McCarley, R. W. (1997). Adenosine: a mediator of the sleep-inducing effects of prolonged wakefulness. Science 276, 1265–1268. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5316.1265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posada-Quintero, H. F., Bolkhovsky, J. B., Qin, M., and Chon, K. H. (2018). Human performance deterioration due to prolonged wakefulness can be accurately detected using time-varying spectral analysis of electrodermal activity. Hum. Factors 60, 1035–1047. doi: 10.1177/0018720818781196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posada-Quintero, H. F., Bolkhovsky, J. B., Reljin, N., and Chon, K. H. (2017). Sleep deprivation in young and healthy subjects is more sensitively identified by higher frequencies of electrodermal activity than by skin conductance level evaluated in the time domain. Front. Physiol. 8:409. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2017.00409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quercia, A., Zappasodi, F., Committeri, G., and Ferrara, M. (2018). Local use-dependent sleep in wakefulness links performance errors to learning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:122. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegel, M. R. (1961). Theory and Problems of Statistics. Schaum’s Outline Series in Mathematics. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Steriade, M., Datta, S., Paré, D., Oakson, G., and Curró Dossi, R. C. (1990). Neuronal activities in brain-stem cholinergic nuclei related to tonic activation processes in thalamocortical systems. J. Neurosci. 10, 2541–2559. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.10-08-02541.1990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tatum, W. O. (2014). Ellen R. Grass lecture: extraordinary EEG. Neurodiagnostic J. 54, 3–21. doi: 10.1080/21646821.2014.11079932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullsperger, M., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Subprocesses of performance monitoring: a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage 14, 1387–1401. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderwolf, C. H. (2000). Are neocortical gamma waves related to consciousness? Brain Res. 855, 217–224. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(99)02351-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yokoi, M., Aoki, K., Shimomura, Y., Iwanaga, K., Katsuura, T., and Shiomura, Y. (2003). Effect of bright light on EEG activities and subjective sleepiness to mental task during nocturnal sleep deprivation. J. Physiol. Anthropol. Appl. Hum. Sci. 22, 257–263. doi: 10.2114/jpa.22.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: electroencephalography, error awareness test, sleep deprivation, performance, reactivity, response inhibition

Citation: Posada-Quintero HF, Reljin N, Bolkhovsky JB, Orjuela-Cañón AD and Chon KH (2019) Brain Activity Correlates With Cognitive Performance Deterioration During Sleep Deprivation. Front. Neurosci. 13:1001. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01001

Received: 16 April 2019; Accepted: 04 September 2019;
Published: 19 September 2019.

Edited by:

Yuval Nir, Tel Aviv University, Israel

Reviewed by:

Giulio Bernardi, IMT School for Advanced Studies Lucca, Italy
Simone Sarasso, University of Milan, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Posada-Quintero, Reljin, Bolkhovsky, Orjuela-Cañón and Chon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hugo F. Posada-Quintero, h.posada@engr.uconn.edu; hugo.posada-quintero@uconn.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.