Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 10 October 2022
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Older adults with lower working memory capacity benefit from transcranial direct current stimulation when combined with working memory training: A preliminary study

\r\nSara Assecondi,,*Sara Assecondi1,2,3*Rong Hu,Rong Hu2,4Jacob KroekerJacob Kroeker5Gail Eskes&#x;Gail Eskes5†Kim Shapiro,&#x;Kim Shapiro2,3†
  • 1Center for Mind/Brain Sciences—CIMeC, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
  • 2Visual Experience Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
  • 3Center for Human Brain Health (CHBH), University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
  • 4Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, Guangzhou First People’s Hospital, South China University of Technology, Guangzhou, China
  • 5Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada

Aging is a very diverse process: successful agers retain most cognitive functioning, while others experience mild to severe cognitive decline. This decline may eventually negatively impact one’s everyday activities. Therefore, scientists must develop approaches to counteract or, at least, slow down the negative change in cognitive performance of aging individuals. Combining cognitive training and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a promising approach that capitalizes on the plasticity of brain networks. However, the efficacy of combined methods depends on individual characteristics, such as the cognitive and emotional state of the individual entering the training program. In this report, we explored the effectiveness of working memory training, combined with tDCS to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), to manipulate working memory performance in older individuals. We hypothesized that individuals with lower working memory capacity would benefit the most from the combined regimen. Thirty older adults took part in a 5-day combined regimen. Before and after the training, we evaluated participants’ working memory performance with five working memory tasks. We found that individual characteristics influenced the outcome of combined cognitive training and tDCS regimens, with the intervention selectively benefiting old-old adults with lower working memory capacity. Future work should consider developing individualized treatments by considering individual differences in cognitive profiles.

Introduction

Aging is an unavoidable and complex process that affects every person, but with an unpredictable outcome, as it is sensitive to individual differences. Some people age well, retaining most cognitive functioning, while others experience mild to severe cognitive decline that can eventually lead to a pathological condition (Cohen et al., 2019). Delaying cognitive decline with lifestyle choices, such as taking part in cognitively stimulating activities is possible but new approaches (e.g., tDCS combined with cognitive training) are needed to counteract cognitive decline once it appears (World Health Organization, 2019).

This age-related decay of cognitive function can greatly impact the independent living of individuals (Grady, 2012). Thus, it is not surprising that scientists, albeit with contradictory results (for reviews, Shipstead et al., 2012; Soveri et al., 2017a), have been searching for ways to slow or even counteract physiological cognitive decline, often focusing on working memory. Working memory is a cognitive system of limited capacity that declines with age but is crucial to many decision-making processes and everyday life functioning (Engle and Kane, 2003). Working memory training usually involves completing a certain number of sessions of an adaptive working memory task (e.g., the n-back (Kirchner, 1958) is a common choice), in which task difficulty adapts to the individual’s performance to keep it challenging, entertaining, and more effective (Harvey et al., 2018; Flegal et al., 2019; Green et al., 2019).

Individuals can learn new skills up to very old age, promoting plastic brain changes (Pauwels et al., 2018a,b). For example, training executive function can lead to structural changes (increase in gray matter and cortical volume) with a general increase in the volume of frontal and parietal brain areas (for a review, see Nguyen et al., 2019). While being directly influenced by genetic factors (Pearson-Fuhrhop et al., 2009), plasticity is also modulated by individual differences (Jones et al., 2006), such as age, education, or motivation (Barrett et al., 2004; Morais et al., 2018). Higher education has been associated with better working memory abilities (Morais et al., 2018), as has motivation (Mohammed et al., 2017). In addition, initial working memory scores can predict training effectiveness, with low working memory capacity predicting lower training gains after a dual n-back training regimen (Matysiak et al., 2019). Pathological conditions like depression and cognitive decline (Hill et al., 2000) may also impact plasticity.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has also been shown to modulate brain plasticity. TDCS modulates cortical excitability (Nitsche et al., 2008; Stagg and Nitsche, 2011; Kronberg et al., 2020), which in turn may boost specific plasticity, namely that of synaptic pathways activated by task performance (Bikson and Rahman, 2013) or networks with increased oscillatory activity (Reato et al., 2010, 2015). Specifically, tDCS can be highly effective when coupled with a relevant behavioral task (Kim and Ko, 2013; Lapenta et al., 2013; Pirulli et al., 2013; Filmer et al., 2014). For example, one session of 13 min of active (anodal, 1.5 mA) tDCS to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) increased frontal activity, which correlated to enhanced working memory performance during an n-back task in older adults (Cespón et al., 2017, 2019). There was not, however, a net working memory improvement in anodal tDCS compared to the sham condition.

In the context of cognitive training with multiple sessions, researchers find that training regimens combined with tDCS can boost training and transfer gains (Au et al., 2016; Ruf et al., 2017; Cespón et al., 2018) by achieving longer-lasting maintenance of improvements (Au et al., 2017), or by shortening the training duration, without sacrificing training gains (Antonenko et al., 2018; Nissim et al., 2019). For example, older adults receiving 10 sessions of active (anodal, 1.5 mA) tDCS to the DLPFC during cognitive training maintained performance improvements for up to a month, both in the training and a transfer task (Jones et al., 2015b). Similarly, only older adults receiving bilateral tDCS of the DLPFC for 10 sessions of cognitive training maintained their performance benefits up to a month later in comparison to the sham group (Park et al., 2014).

Importantly, and leading to the rationale for the present report, the outcomes of interventions using simultaneous working memory training and brain stimulation are not always successful (Berryhill and Jones, 2012; Perceval et al., 2016; Horne et al., 2021). TDCS applications require the choice of many parameters, including the position and size of the electrodes and the duration and intensity of the stimulation current. There is also variability in the working memory training task chosen, its duration, and in the scheduling of the training (Berryhill, 2017; Hurley and Machado, 2018; Weller et al., 2020). Moreover, anatomical parameters (head anatomy, inter-, and intra-cranial volumes) that change with age determine the tDCS current distribution in the brain (Opitz et al., 2015; Antonenko et al., 2020). Finally, the initial level of performance also plays a role in the effectiveness of brain stimulation. TDCS appears to selectively benefit individuals with low baseline performance (Tseng et al., 2012; Hsu et al., 2014; Benwell et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2019), with initial capacity modulating the effectiveness of tDCS just as it modulates that of cognitive training (Matysiak et al., 2019; Krebs et al., 2021).

It has been suggested that tDCS effects are state-dependent, i.e., they are influenced by brain state at the time of stimulation (Krause et al., 2013; Krause and Cohen Kadosh, 2014; Benwell et al., 2015). We previously found (Assecondi et al., 2021) that the outcome of combined cognitive training and tDCS is sensitive to individual differences in young adults. We considered two aspects of such differences: initial performance in working memory capacity and strategy use. In particular, baseline abilities and motivation (Jones et al., 2015a; Katz et al., 2017) can contribute to shaping the outcome of combined tDCS and training protocols. To what extent these factors influence performance in older individuals remains unclear (but see Krebs et al., 2021 for a recent report) and forms the basis of the experimental question addressed in the present report.

Thus, this project aimed to assess the effectiveness of a working memory training regimen combined with tDCS in older adults. Thirty individuals were randomly assigned to one of two groups receiving either SHAM or ACTIVE stimulation, and all participants entered a 5-day training regimen using an adaptive n-back paradigm. In line with previous findings (Assecondi et al., 2021; Krebs et al., 2021), we hypothesized that individuals would respond differentially to the intervention depending on their initial individual cognitive characteristics. Specifically, we predicted that individuals with lower working memory capacity would benefit more from the combined training regimen than those with high working memory capacity.

Materials and methods

Participants

Two of the thirty older adults recruited withdrew because of health issues. All the remaining 28 participants (mean age 67.9 ± 6.1 yr, range 56–76, 14 females) had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, a Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA) score above 26 (Nasreddine et al., 2005), no previous brain injury, no history of epilepsy or depression, were not taking medications likely to alter cortical excitability, had not received brain stimulation or cognitive training in the previous 6 months, and fulfilled safety inclusion criteria for brain stimulation (Antal et al., 2017). The study was approved by the University of Birmingham’s Ethics Committee, and informed consent was obtained before taking part in the experiment. Individuals were compensated £100. Participants were randomly allocated to one of the two stimulation groups, receiving either ACTIVE or placebo (SHAM) stimulation. Because of the age spread of recruited individuals, a posteriori participants were further split into younger older adults (YO or young-old) and older older adults (OO or old-old), based on a median split of their age (median age was 69.5 yr). This resulted in two groups (ACTIVE; SHAM) for each AGE (YO; OO).

Transcranial direct current stimulation

2 mA tDCS was administered using an 8-channel device (Starstim, Neuroelectrics®) for 20 min. Two circular electrodes (NG Pistim, Neuroelectrics, area = 3.14 cm2), filled with conductive gel (SignaGel, ParkerLabs), were placed over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC, F4 on the 10–20 system, anodal electrode), and the contralateral supraorbital site (Fp1, cathodal electrode). Impedance always remained below 20 kOhm. Both protocols (ACTIVE and SHAM) included 20 min of either 2 mA or 0 mA preceded and followed by 30 s ramping up and down (total stimulation time = 21 min). Simulations showing the distribution of the electric field and current density obtained with this procedure are reported in Supplementary material. Participants were randomly assigned to either the ACTIVE or the SHAM tDCS condition with blinding efficacy and side effects recorded via questionnaire.

Working memory training: Adaptive spatial n-Back

Working memory was trained with N-IGMA (Ploughman et al., 2019), a proprietary naturalistic adaptive spatial n-Back (ANBACK) task designed to stimulate participants’ engagement (see Supplementary material for an example of the stimuli used). Participants were requested to track a series of locations and indicate whether the current location was a match or not of the location presented at “n” trials ago, following the method of Verhaeghen and Basak (2005). The inter-trial duration was 3 s, and participants responded by pressing the left (“match”) or the right (“no match”) arrow on the computer keyboard with their dominant hand. Each training session consisted of 20 blocks, and each participant started at “n” = 1 and moved to “n”+1 every time their performance was at or above 90% accuracy. Motivation was encouraged by feedback after every response and a short text after every block. The average “n” during a session (n¯) was the dependent variable. The total duration of the training session was approximately 20 min, with variability from individual to individual due to the duration of self-paced breaks. The working memory training always started at the same time as the tDCS, with an average duration (across sessions and participants) of 21 min and 39 s (as such, the training could last a few seconds more than the stimulation).

Outcome measures

An individual’s working memory capacity score was calculated by averaging performance in five tasks, all relying on different aspects of working memory. We used a Change Detection task (CD), a spatial (SNBACK) and a visual n-back (VNBACK), and the digit span backward (DSB) and forward (DSF). Details on how these tasks were implemented and an example of the stimuli are provided in Supplementary material.

First, we derived a task-dependent performance measure for each task (d-prime for the CD, the visual VNBACK, the SNBACK, and the maximum span for the DSF and DSB) at baseline and post-test. We then standardized performance separately for each task, based on the entire sample at baseline and averaged them for each person, obtaining a composite working memory capacity score (WMC score)1; see Figure 1 for a description of the procedure. Finally, we calculated changes in composite WMC scores (WMCPOST-WMCPRE) after training.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Diagram of the procedure to calculate composite working memory capacity (WMC) scores. Dependent variables measuring working memory performance were extracted from each task, standardized, and averaged, obtaining a composite score for each participant. The obtained distribution of composite scores is shown for each age and stimulation group.

Questionnaires

To control for individual differences in participants’ lifestyles and attitudes that could influence the training outcome, we administered a series of questionnaires listed in Table 1. Before each training session, we asked participants about their attitude and expectation toward the intervention. After each training session, participants reported on side effects of the stimulation, and, at the end of the last session, we asked them about the strategy used during training, if any, and to guess if they were in the ACTIVE or SHAM group (to confirm the efficacy of the blinding procedure).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of the questionnaires administered to participants, and where they were filled in (at home or in the lab).

Procedure

A flowchart of the procedure is shown in Figure 2. On the first day (TIME 0—PRE) participants were invited to the lab and asked to sign a consent form. After being screened for tDCS safety (Antal et al., 2017), color blindness (Farnsworth, 1943) and cognitive impairment (MOCA; Nasreddine et al., 2005), individuals were randomly assigned to stimulation groups. Then, participants received various questionnaires to complete at home (see Table 1) and were administered the computerized outcome tests to assess baseline cognitive performance. Finally, participants familiarized themselves with the stimulation procedure and asked questions. This first session took approximately 2 h and was administered on a Friday.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flowchart of the procedure. CT, cognitive training.

After the weekend following the initial session,2 participants returned to the lab to start the cognitive training phase. After having the tDCS cap fitted, participants answered four questions about their mental status (level of alertness, sadness, motivation to train, and expectation on the intervention) on a continuous scale from 1 to 100, and then began 20 min of training (see section “Working memory training: Adaptive spatial n-back”) while receiving tDCS (ACTIVE or SHAM). At the end of the session, participants provided feedback on any perceived side effects of the stimulation. The entire session lasted approximately 45 min and was repeated for five consecutive days.

Participants returned to the lab for the outcome assessment (same tasks as at TIME 0) 48 h after the last training session (TIME 6—POST). At the end of the session, participants were debriefed on the experiment and were asked to guess their assigned stimulation group (ACTIVE or SHAM) to confirm the efficacy of the blinding procedure.

Data analysis

Data analysis was performed using JASP (JASP Team, 2020), and R (R Core Team, 2020).

We used parametric t-tests to compare groups at baseline with Hedges effect sizes (Hg) to account for small sample sizes (Hedges, 1981).

To evaluate training gains, we performed a 3-way mixed analysis of co-variance (between subject factors: STIMULATION (ACTIVE, SHAM) and AGE (YO, OO); within-subject factor: SESSION (TIME1, TIME2, TIME3, TIME4, TIME5); covariate: WMC score at baseline), on the dependent training variable [the average “n” level reached during a session (n¯)]. Significant interactions were followed up with a simple effects analysis within each age range.

To evaluate outcome performance, we used a 3-way mixed analysis of variance [between subject factors: STIMULATION (ACTIVE, SHAM) and AGE (YO, OO); within-subject factor: SESSION (TIME0, TIME6)] to analyze the effect of the interaction between age, training, and stimulation on the composite outcome measure (WMC score). Effect sizes were quantified with partial eta square (ηp2). We used Spearman’s ρ to quantify correlations. To test for non-random associations between categorical variables Fisher’s exact test was used, which is suitable for small sample sizes.

Results

Baseline differences and blinding

Tabulated statistics and descriptive data are presented in Supplementary material. Participants in the ACTIVE and SHAM groups showed no significant differences in age, years of education, MoCA scores, sleepiness, overall quality of life, depression, and familiarity with technology, either within AGE level or overall (all ps > 0.05). Fisher’s exact test showed no significant association between the assigned condition and the self-reported perceived stimulation, indicating that subjects were blind to the stimulation group (p = 0.673) (see Supplementary material for reported side effects). We also found no systematic differences in baseline WMC scores between ACTIVE and SHAM groups, within each AGE level. Thus, there were no significant differences between groups at baseline to bias the analysis. However, overall the YO and OO group differed in their WMC scores with OO having lower scores than YO [OO: −0.240 ± 0.652, YO: 0.207 ± 0.467; t (26) = 2.108, p = 0.045, Hg = 0.775]. Statistics and descriptive data for the two age groups are reported in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic and baseline characteristics of the sample divided by AGE, for each STIMULATION group.

Training gains

Following previous findings in young (Assecondi et al., 2021) and older adults (Krebs et al., 2021), we hypothesized that individuals entering the training regimen with lower working memory capacity would benefit more from the intervention.

Performance across groups is depicted in Figure 3. The effect of the baseline WMC scores covariate was significant [F(1, 23) = 10.736, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.318], with higher scores corresponding to enhanced training performance. There was also a significant main effect of SESSION [F(4, 92) = 11.276, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.329] and a significant STIMULATION × AGE interaction [F(1, 23) = 10.846, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.320], after controlling for initial working memory. Simple effects analyses revealed that ACTIVE stimulation was more effective than SHAM at increasing performance (p = 0.027) in the OO group, while the opposite was found in the YO, with a larger increase in performance found in the SHAM condition compared to ACTIVE (p = 0.016).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Training performance (n¯) for each training day, for the SHAM and ACTIVE groups, within each AGE level. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.

To summarize, our finding supports the hypothesis that the benefit of combined training regimens is inversely proportional to initial working memory capacity. Importantly, old-old individuals with lower capacity (OO) benefit the most in training gains from the combination of training and brain stimulation relative to cognitive training alone, with this advantage evident from DAY1.

Outcome tasks

To assess the overall impact of combined tDCS and working memory training on working memory, we assessed changes in WMC score after five consecutive days of training (POST). Analysis of individual tasks is reported in Supplementary material.

The analysis of the change in WMC score revealed a significant main effect of SESSION [F(1, 24) = 45.502, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.655] and of AGE [F(1, 24) = 4.665, p = 0.041, ηp2 = 0.163]. Follow-up analysis showed that WMC scores were significantly higher at post than at pretest, and YO scores were significantly higher than OO scores, regardless of the stimulation. To summarize, overall working memory evaluated 2 days after the end of 5 days of practice, increased in both age groups, with overall higher scores in young-old, but this increase was not modulated by stimulation. This result could be ascribed to an overall benefit of the training or due to practice effects or both.

Strategy use

In a previous study with young adults, we found that strategy plays an important role in combined working memory training and stimulation regimens (Assecondi, 2021). Therefore, we collected self-reports of strategy use at the end of training by asking participants two questions, with answers ranging on a scale from 1 to 10: (1) if they used a strategy; and, if so, (2) how efficient they felt the strategy was in improving their performance.

Overall, we found no significant association between STIMULATION groups and frequency of strategy use, meaning that individuals in the ACTIVE and SHAM groups were equally likely to develop a strategy. We found a difference between the perceived efficacy of the strategy between the ACTIVE and SHAM group, however, (tWelch = 2.239, p = 0.037, Hg = 0.889), with the SHAM group perceiving the strategy to be more efficient (ACTIVE: 7.12 ± 0.41; SHAM: 8.22 ± 0.28). However, the rating of the strategy’s effectiveness did not correlate with training gains (Spearman’s ρ = −0.053, p = 0.819, N = 20).

When looking at the frequency of strategy use separately for YO and OO individuals (see Supplementary material for details), however, we found a significant association between STIMULATION and strategy use for OO individuals only (Fisher’s exact p = 0.005), meaning that OO individuals receiving active stimulation were more likely to use a strategy than those not receiving active stimulation.

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effectiveness of improving working memory in older individuals by combining working memory training and transcranial direct current stimulation. We employed a 5-day training regimen, where participants completed 20 min of combined working memory training and tDCS (SHAM or ACTIVE) on five consecutive weekdays. Two days after the end of the intervention, we evaluated training effects on an overall composite WMC score. We found that, whereas all individuals improved their performance during training, tDCS concurrent with working memory training selectively benefitted old-old individuals with lower working memory capacity. We also found that performance during training was significantly better when there was no stimulation in young-old adults, who showed higher working memory scores.

Non-invasive brain stimulation, in our case tDCS, is emerging as a “state-dependent” intervention, with its effectiveness modulated by the pre-existing individual’s cortical excitation (Krause et al., 2013; Hsu et al., 2014; Krause and Cohen Kadosh, 2014; Esposito et al., 2021, 2022). The relation between excitation and performance has been proposed as a left-skewed inverted U-shaped curve, with a maximum point corresponding to optimal performance. Cognitive functioning within the curve is optimal when there is a balance between inhibition and excitation to allow sufficient flexibility for acquiring new skills (plasticity) but equally sufficient stability to resist noise (e.g., distractions) and to maintain new information (Krause et al., 2013; Krause and Cohen Kadosh, 2014).

Aging is associated with a weaker cortical excitability (Rossini et al., 1992; Pitcher et al., 2003; Oliviero et al., 2006; Ferreri et al., 2017). Importantly, however, engaging in a task increases cortical excitability. It is illustrative to compare a young individual with an older individual. The former will have higher WMC scores, stronger cortical excitability, and resources to engage with the task; the latter will likely have lower WMC scores, weaker cortical excitability, hence less cortical activation, and fewer resources available for the task. The younger individual is likely closer to its optimal performance point (maximum on the u-shaped curve). When receiving tDCS, their cortical excitability will increase, potentially pushing this individual over their optimal performance point, resulting in worse performance. On the other hand, the older adult is likely further away from their optimal performance point; therefore, receiving tDCS will only push them closer to their maximum of the u-shaped curve, resulting in better performance. This model agrees with the inverted effect of tDCS we found in young-old and old-old adults. Our findings also agree with both the cognitive training and the tDCS literature, showing that training-related changes in performance correlate with initial WMC scores. Whereas cognitive training alone seems more effective in young-old adults (Berryhill and Jones, 2012; Foster et al., 2017), the benefits of tDCS, when added to cognitive training, seem to be specific to old-old adults (Tseng et al., 2012; Gözenman and Berryhill, 2016; Assecondi et al., 2021; Krebs et al., 2021). Identifying the neural mechanism that explains this selective tDCS benefit warrants further research (Tseng et al., 2012).

A working memory training regimen of 5 days is relatively short with respect to typical cognitive training studies. Despite this short training period, we also assessed overall working memory improvements, as measured via a composite working memory score. This composite WMC score includes tasks that share information processing components and engage similar neural circuitry (Dahlin et al., 2008). Research has shown that change in performance after training can be due to either a change in capacity or a change in efficiency, depending on available capacity (von Bastian and Oberauer, 2014). Whereas working memory scores improved overall, we found no significant differences in composite score due to stimulation. It is worth noting that the WMC score is calculated from different types of tasks, which could be considered to range from near- to far- transfer tasks. Lack of transfer effects has been reported before (Waris et al., 2015; Soveri et al., 2017a,b) and, in our study, possibly exacerbated by the short duration of the training program.

We found that old-old individuals receiving active stimulation were more likely to use a strategy than those not receiving active stimulation. We can hypothesize that tDCS to the frontal area may facilitate executive functions, and therefore, promote strategy use. We acknowledge though that this statement is highly speculative, particularly considering our sample size, and further research is needed to test this hypothesis.

Regarding limitations, we acknowledge that our sample size was small (Button et al., 2013), however, we took several steps to support our findings, including implementing a model that accounts for subject variability (in age and initial WMC score). We did not evaluate cognitive function after a period of no contact; we, therefore, cannot say if there was a modulatory effect in maintaining the training gain for a more extended period. Although it will be useful to replicate our results in a larger sample size, we note that we did replicate results found in a young adult population and that our findings are consistent with the literature (Assecondi, 2021; Krebs et al., 2021).

To conclude, we found that tDCS during working memory training selectively benefits old-old adults with lower initial working memory capacity; importantly, we also show that the benefit of training in older adults is predicted by baseline working memory capacity (Au et al., 2022). The combined intervention appears to be a viable method to improve working memory in young and older adults, the latter being crucial given the large cohort of older adults experiencing both normal and abnormal (e.g., dementia) cognitive decline.

Further investigation of transfer benefits is needed in longer-term training studies along with an investigation of the role of strategy development. Finally, it will be essential to investigate the neurophysiological correlates of training-related changes, e.g., using EEG, to understand if these are caused by an improvement in a specific working memory component, such as in capacity or general strategy use. Our research adds to the body of literature that stresses the importance of interindividual differences to explain the variability in the outcome of tDCS studies. Importantly, it shows how this variability can be exploited to maximize the outcome of cognitive training interventions. A better understanding of the mechanism underlying successful training and transfer will likely benefit the design of subject-specific training and training regimens in general.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the University of Birmingham. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SA: conceptualization, methodology, validation, data collection and curation, formal analysis, investigation, writing—original draft and review and editing, and funding acquisition. RH: data collection and writing—review and editing. JK: methodology and data collection and analyses. GE: conceptualization, methodology, resources, writing—review and editing, and funding acquisition. KS: conceptualization, methodology, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This project was supported by an MRC Proximity to Discovery grant awarded to SA and KS and a Vice-President Research and Innovation International Seed Fund (VPRIIS Fund) 2019–2020/Dept. Psychiatry: Multi-modal approaches to cognitive enhancement in aging and brain disease” awarded to GE. SA was supported by Fondazione Cassa di Risparmio Trento e Rovereto (CARITRO). RH’s tenure at the University of Birmingham was supported by a Guangzhou Planned Project of Science and Technology grant (grant no. 2016201604030018) awarded to KS, a project of Guangzhou General Science and Technology Project of Health and Family Planning (grant no. 20191A011004) awarded to RH and a project of Innovative Clinical Technology of Guangzhou (grant no. 2019GX06). Publication fees were covered by the Library Services Open Access budgets of the University of Birmingham.

Acknowledgments

We thank the 1,000 Elderly cohort and all the elderly volunteers who were interested in our study.

Conflict of interest

A patent application has been submitted by the University of Birmingham and Dalhousie University with SA, KS, and GE named as inventors.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2022.1009262/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ DVz = (DV-μ)/σ, where DVz is the standardized dependent variable, DV is the original dependent variable, μ and σ are the mean and standard deviation of the DV over the entire sample at baseline (28 participants).
  2. ^ One participant let 45 days pass between baseline testing and the first training session because of unforeseen family circumstances conflicting with scheduling. The participant was included in the analysis because we did not expect a significant change in their cognitive function over 45 days. As an additional check, repeating the analysis of the training data without that participant did not change the significance of the results.

References

Antal, A., Alekseichuk, I., Bikson, M., Brockmöller, J., Brunoni, A. R., Chen, R., et al. (2017). Low intensity transcranial electric stimulation: Safety, ethical, legal regulatory and application guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 1774–1809. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonenko, D., Grittner, U., Saturnino, G., Nierhaus, T., Thielscher, A., and Flöel, A. (2020). Inter-individual and age-dependent variability in simulated electric fields induced by conventional transcranial electrical stimulation. NeuroImage 224:117413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonenko, D., Külzow, N., Sousa, A., Prehn, K., Grittner, U., and Flöel, A. (2018). Neuronal and behavioral effects of multi-day brain stimulation and memory training. Neurobiol. Aging 61, 245–254. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assecondi, S. (2021). Impact of tDCS on working memory training is enhanced by strategy instructions in individuals with low working memory capacity. Sci. Rep. 11:5531. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84298-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assecondi, S., Hu, R., Eskes, G., Pan, X., Zhou, J., and Shapiro, K. (2021). Impact of tDCS on working memory training is enhanced by strategy instructions in individuals with low working memory capacity. Sci. Rep. 11:5531.

Google Scholar

Au, J., Karsten, C., Buschkuehl, M., and Jaeggi, S. M. (2017). Optimizing transcranial direct current stimulation protocols to promote long-term learning. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 1, 65–72. doi: 10.1007/s41465-017-0007-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Au, J., Katz, B., Buschkuehl, M., Bunarjo, K., Senger, T., Zabel, C., et al. (2016). Enhancing working memory training with transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 1–14. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00979

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Au, J., Smith-Peirce, R. N., Carbone, E., Moon, A., Evans, M., Jonides, J., et al. (2022). Effects of multisession prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation on long-term memory and working memory in older adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 1015–1037. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01839

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., and Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychol. Bull. 130:553. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benwell, C. S. Y., Learmonth, G., Miniussi, C., Harvey, M., and Thut, G. (2015). Non-linear effects of transcranial direct current stimulation as a function of individual baseline performance: Evidence from biparietal tDCS influence on lateralized attention bias. Cortex 69, 152–165. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berryhill, M. E. (2017). Longitudinal tDCS: Consistency across working memory training studies. AIMS Neurosci. 4:71. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.2.71

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berryhill, M. E., and Jones, K. T. (2012). tDCS selectively improves working memory in older adults with more education. Neurosci. Lett. 521, 148–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.05.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bikson, M., and Rahman, A. (2013). Origins of specificity during tDCS: Anatomical, activity-selective, and input-bias mechanisms. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:688. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S. J., et al. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 365–376. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cespón, J., Miniussi, C., and Pellicciari, M. C. (2018). Interventional programmes to improve cognition during healthy and pathological ageing: Cortical modulations and evidence for brain plasticity. Ageing Res. Rev. 43, 81–98. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2018.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cespón, J., Rodella, C., Miniussi, C., and Pellicciari, M. C. (2019). Behavioural and electrophysiological modulations induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy elderly and Alzheimer’s disease patients: A pilot study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 130, 2038–2052. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cespón, J., Rodella, C., Rossini, P. M., Miniussi, C., and Pellicciari, M. C. (2017). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation promotes frontal compensatory mechanisms in healthy elderly subjects. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:420. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, R. A., Marsiske, M. M., and Smith, G. E. (2019). Neuropsychology of aging. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 167, 149–180. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-804766-8.00010-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahlin, E., Neely, A. S., Larsson, A., Bäckman, L., and Nyberg, L. (2008). Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science 320, 1510–1512. doi: 10.1126/science.1155466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engle, R. W., and Kane, M. J. (2003). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 44, 145–199. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44005-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esposito, M., Ferrari, C., Fracassi, C., Miniussi, C., and Brignani, D. (2022). Responsiveness to left-prefrontal tDCS varies according to arousal levels. Eur. J. Neurosci. 55, 762–777. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esposito, M., Mauri, P., Panizza, L., Mazza, V., Miniussi, C., and Brignani, D. (2021). Baseline levels of alertness influence tES effects along different age-related directions. Neuropsychologia 160:107966. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farnsworth, D. (1943). The Farnsworth-Munsell 100-hue and dichotomous tests for color vision*. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 33, 568–578. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.33.000568

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreri, F., Guerra, A., Vollero, L., Ponzo, D., Maatta, S., Mervaala, E., et al. (2017). Age-related changes of cortical excitability and connectivity in healthy humans: Non-invasive evaluation of sensorimotor network by means of TMS-EEG. Neuroscience 357, 255–263. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filmer, H. L., Dux, P. E., and Mattingley, J. B. (2014). Applications of transcranial direct current stimulation for understanding brain function. Trends Neurosci. 37, 742–753. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flegal, K. E., Ragland, J. D., and Ranganath, C. (2019). Adaptive task difficulty influences neural plasticity and transfer of training. NeuroImage 188, 111–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, J. L., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Draheim, C., Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. W. (2017). Do the effects of working memory training depend on baseline ability level? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 43, 1677–1689. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gözenman, F., and Berryhill, M. E. (2016). Working memory capacity differentially influences responses to tDCS and HD-tDCS in a retro-cue task. Neurosci. Lett. 629, 105–109. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.06.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 491–505. doi: 10.1038/nrn3256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, S. C., Bavelier, D., Kramer, A. F., Vinogradov, S., Ansorge, U., Ball, K. K., et al. (2019). Improving methodological standards in behavioral interventions for cognitive enhancement. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 3, 2–29. doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0115-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, P. D., McGurk, S. R., Mahncke, H., and Wykes, T. (2018). Controversies in computerized cognitive training. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 907–915. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. J. Educ. Stat. 6, 107–128. doi: 10.2307/1164588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, R. D., Backman, L., and Stigsdotter-Neely, A. (2000). Cognitive Rehabilitation in Old Age. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Horne, K. S., Filmer, H. L., Nott, Z. E., Hawi, Z., Pugsley, K., Mattingley, J. B., et al. (2021). Evidence against benefits from cognitive training and transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy older adults. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 146–158. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00979-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, T.-Y., Tseng, P., Liang, W.-K., Cheng, S.-K., and Juan, C.-H. (2014). Transcranial direct current stimulation over right posterior parietal cortex changes prestimulus alpha oscillation in visual short-term memory task. NeuroImage 98, 306–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurley, R., and Machado, L. (2018). Using transcranial direct current stimulation to improve verbal working memory: A detailed review of the methodology. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 40, 790–804. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2018.1434133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

JASP Team (2020). JASP (Version 0.14.1)[Computer software]. New York, NY: JASP team.

Google Scholar

Johns, M. W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 14, 540–545. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. T., Gözenman, F., and Berryhill, M. E. (2015a). The strategy and motivational influences on the beneficial effect of neurostimulation: A tDCS and fNIRS study. NeuroImage 105, 238–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. T., Stephens, J. A., Alam, M., Bikson, M., and Berryhill, M. E. (2015b). Longitudinal neurostimulation in older adults improves working memory. PLoS One 10:e0121904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S., Nyberg, L., Sandblom, J., Stigsdotter Neely, A., Ingvar, M., Magnus Petersson, K., et al. (2006). Cognitive and neural plasticity in aging: General and task-specific limitations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 864–871. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, B., Au, J., Buschkuehl, M., Abagis, T., Zabel, C., Jaeggi, S. M., et al. (2017). Individual differences and long-term consequences of tDCS-augmented cognitive training. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 1498–1508. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, G.-W., and Ko, M.-H. (2013). Facilitation of corticospinal tract excitability by transcranial direct current stimulation combined with voluntary grip exercise. Neurosci. Lett. 548, 181–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.05.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirchner, W. K. (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 352–358. doi: 10.1037/h0043688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, B., and Cohen Kadosh, R. (2014). Not all brains are created equal: The relevance of individual differences in responsiveness to transcranial electrical stimulation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:25. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, B., Márquez-Ruiz, J., and Cohen Kadosh, R. (2013). The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation: A role for cortical excitation/inhibition balance? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:602. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krebs, C., Peter, J., Wyss, P., Brem, A.-K., and Klöppel, S. (2021). Transcranial electrical stimulation improves cognitive training effects in healthy elderly adults with low cognitive performance. Clin. Neurophysiol. 132, 1254–1263. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2021.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kronberg, G., Rahman, A., Sharma, M., Bikson, M., and Parra, L. C. (2020). Direct current stimulation boosts hebbian plasticity in vitro. Brain Stimul. 13, 287–301. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lapenta, O. M., Minati, L., Fregni, F., and Boggio, P. S. (2013). Je pense donc je fais: Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates brain oscillations associated with motor imagery and movement observation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:256. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matysiak, O., Kroemeke, A., and Brzezicka, A. (2019). Working memory capacity as a predictor of cognitive training efficacy in the elderly population. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:126. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohammed, S., Flores, L., Deveau, J., Hoffing, R. C., Phung, C., Parlett, C. M., et al. (2017). The benefits and challenges of implementing motivational features to boost cognitive training outcome. J. Cogn. Enhanc. Integr. Theory Pract. 1, 491–507. doi: 10.1007/s41465-017-0047-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morais, R. M., Pera, M. V., Ladera, V., Oliveira, J., and García, R. (2018). Individual differences in working memory abilities in healthy adults. J. Adult Dev. 25, 222–228. doi: 10.1007/s10804-018-9287-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z., Phillips, N., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, L., Murphy, K., and Andrews, G. (2019). Cognitive and neural plasticity in old age: A systematic review of evidence from executive functions cognitive training. Ageing Res. Rev. 53:100912. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.100912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nissim, N. R., O’Shea, A., Indahlastari, A., Kraft, J. N., von Mering, O., Aksu, S., et al. (2019). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation paired with cognitive training on functional connectivity of the working memory network in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:340. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliviero, A., Profice, P., Tonali, P. A., Pilato, F., Saturno, E., Dileone, M., et al. (2006). Effects of aging on motor cortex excitability. Neurosci. Res. 55, 74–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2006.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Opitz, A., Paulus, W., Will, S., Antunes, A., and Thielscher, A. (2015). Determinants of the electric field during transcranial direct current stimulation. NeuroImage 109, 140–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S.-H., Seo, J.-H., Kim, Y.-H., and Ko, M.-H. (2014). Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults. Neuroreport 25:122. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauwels, L., Chalavi, S., and Swinnen, S. P. (2018b). Aging and brain plasticity. Aging 10, 1789–1790. doi: 10.18632/aging.101514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauwels, L., Chalavi, S., Gooijers, J., Maes, C., Albouy, G., Sunaert, S., et al. (2018a). Challenge to promote change: The neural basis of the contextual interference effect in young and older adults. J. Neurosci. 38, 3333–3345. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2640-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson-Fuhrhop, K. M., Kleim, J. A., and Cramer, S. C. (2009). Brain plasticity and genetic factors. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 16, 282–299. doi: 10.1310/tsr1604-282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perceval, G., Flöel, A., and Meinzer, M. (2016). Can transcranial direct current stimulation counteract age-associated functional impairment? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 65, 157–172. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirulli, C., Fertonani, A., and Miniussi, C. (2013). The role of timing in the induction of neuromodulation in perceptual learning by transcranial electric stimulation. Brain Stimul. 6, 683–689. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, J. B., Ogston, K. M., and Miles, T. S. (2003). Age and sex differences in human motor cortex input–output characteristics. J. Physiol. 546, 605–613. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.029454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ploughman, M., Eskes, G. A., Kelly, L. P., Kirkland, M. C., Devasahayam, A. J., Wallack, E. M., et al. (2019). Synergistic benefits of combined aerobic and cognitive training on fluid intelligence and the role of IGF-1 in chronic stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 33, 199–212. doi: 10.1177/1545968319832605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team. (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Reato, D., Bikson, M., and Parra, L. C. (2015). Lasting modulation of in vitro oscillatory activity with weak direct current stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 113, 1334–1341. doi: 10.1152/jn.00208.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reato, D., Rahman, A., Bikson, M., and Parra, L. C. (2010). Low-intensity electrical stimulation affects network dynamics by modulating population rate and spike timing. J. Neurosci. 30, 15067–15079. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2059-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossini, P. M., Desiato, M. T., and Caramia, M. D. (1992). Age-related changes of motor evoked potentials in healthy humans: Non-invasive evaluation of central and peripheral motor tracts excitability and conductivity. Brain Res. 593, 14–19. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91256-E

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruf, S., Fallgatter, A. J., and Plewnia, C. (2017). Augmentation of working memory training by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Sci. Rep. 7:876. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01055-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychol. Bull. 138:628. doi: 10.1037/a0027473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soveri, A., Antfolk, J., Karlsson, L., Salo, B., and Laine, M. (2017a). Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 1077–1096. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1217-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soveri, A., Karlsson, E. P. A., Waris, O., Grönholm-Nyman, P., and Laine, M. (2017b). Pattern of near transfer effects following working memory training with a dual n-back task. Exp. Psychol. 64, 240–252. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stagg, C. J., and Nitsche, M. A. (2011). Physiological Basis of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. Neuroscientist 17, 37–53. doi: 10.1177/1073858410386614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, P., Hsu, T.-Y., Chang, C.-F., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., Muggleton, N. G., et al. (2012). Unleashing potential: Transcranial direct current stimulation over the right posterior parietal cortex improves change detection in low-performing individuals. J. Neurosci. 32, 10554–10561. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0362-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhaeghen, P., and Basak, C. (2005). Ageing and switching of the focus of attention in working memory: Results from a modified n-back task. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sec. A 58, 134–154. doi: 10.1080/02724980443000241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Bastian, C. C., and Oberauer, K. (2014). Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: A review. Psychol. Res. 78, 803–820. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0524-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., Itthipuripat, S., and Ku, Y. (2019). Electrical Stimulation Over Human Posterior Parietal Cortex Selectively Enhances the Capacity of Visual Short-Term Memory. J. Neurosci. 39, 528–536. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1959-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waris, O., Soveri, A., and Laine, M. (2015). Transfer after Working Memory Updating Training. PLoS One 10:e0138734. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weller, S., Nitsche, M. A., and Plewnia, C. (2020). Enhancing cognitive control training with transcranial direct current stimulation: A systematic parameter study. Brain Stimul. 13, 1358–1369. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO] (1996). Whoqol-bref introduction, administration, scoring and generic version of the assessment?: Field trial version. Available Online at: https://www.who.int/publications-detail-redirect/WHOQOL-BREF (accessed Jul 17, 2021).

Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO] (2019). Risk Reduction of Cognitive Decline and Dementia: WHO Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Zigmond, A. S., and Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 67, 361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS), tDCS—transcranial direct current stimulation, working memory training (WMT), plasticity, older adults, individual differences

Citation: Assecondi S, Hu R, Kroeker J, Eskes G and Shapiro K (2022) Older adults with lower working memory capacity benefit from transcranial direct current stimulation when combined with working memory training: A preliminary study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:1009262. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.1009262

Received: 01 August 2022; Accepted: 20 September 2022;
Published: 10 October 2022.

Edited by:

Hakuei Fujiyama, Murdoch University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Jesus Cespon, Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, Spain
Nicole R. Nissim, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Copyright © 2022 Assecondi, Hu, Kroeker, Eskes and Shapiro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sara Assecondi, c2FyYS5hc3NlY29uZGlAdW5pdG4uaXQ=, c2FyYS5hc3NlY29uZGlAZ21haWwuY29t

These authors share senior authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.