Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 29 July 2025

Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Volume 17 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2025.1543408

Neuromotor functions across the lifespan: percentiles from 6 to 80 years

  • 1Child Development Center, University Children’s Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 2Children’s Research Center, University Children’s Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3Department of Epidemiology and Health Systems, Center for Primary Care and Public Health (Unisanté), University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 4University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

Aim: To investigate the dynamics of neuromotor functions from 6 to 80 years with the Zurich Neuromotor Assessment (ZNA) and to provide reference curves for clinical and research use.

Materials and methods: Neuromotor data on 1620 individuals (828 females) measured with the ZNA and ZNA-2 between 1983 and 2023 were extracted from 11 studies, all performed at the same center in the region of Zurich, Switzerland. Performance on 14 motor tasks was modeled as a function of age and sex while controlling for differences in testing procedures that occurred over the period spanned by the studies. The age of peak performance was identified for each task. Motor performance was converted into standard deviation scores (SDSs) at the task level and combined into the five motor components of the ZNA-2: fine motor, pure motor, balance, gross motor, and contralateral associated movements. The effect of body mass index (BMI) on motor component SDSs was also investigated.

Results: The data showed a rapid increase in motor functions during the first years, particularly before age 10 years, followed by a leveling in performance between the ages of 20 and 40 years. Afterward, a decrease in motor functions was observed in most tasks. However, the age of peak performance and the rate of decline varied greatly between the tasks: motor functions in tasks requiring muscle force deteriorated faster than those involving isolated movements, which showed only mild declines at older ages. Males reached peak performance on average 1 year later than females. High BMI (SDS > 1) was associated with lower balance and poorer gross motor functions.

Discussion: Neuromotor functions undergo dynamic changes throughout the lifespan from early childhood to older adulthood, with peak performances and declines depending on type of motor task and sex. High BMI negatively impacts balance and gross motor functions but not other neuromotor domains. Our findings may inform clinical practice and interventions aimed at optimizing motor functions across the lifespan.

Introduction

Motor performance plays a crucial role during childhood and adolescence: it supports children’s social interactions and academic success (Stodden et al., 2008). However, it remains essential across the entire lifespan for a healthy, active, and independent lifestyle (Haywood and Getchell, 2014). Thus, the comprehensive study of the evolution of motor functions from infancy to old age is important for providing normative data for diagnostic purposes at all ages, tracking changes over time, describing individual trajectories, predicting future motor performance, and informing treatment and interventions (Burton and Miller, 1998; Burton and Rodgerson, 2001).

It is generally accepted that motor functions improve during childhood, stabilize during adulthood, and then gradually decline in older age (Haywood and Getchell, 2014; JafariNasabian et al., 2017). However, the data on motor performance are largely limited to specific age ranges [e.g., childhood (Kakebeeke et al., 2018), or old age (Galhardas et al., 2022; Lanzino et al., 2012; Perera et al., 2014)] or to selected motor tasks (Koerte et al., 2010; Springer et al., 2007). Furthermore, studies often focus on children and adolescents with abnormal or delayed motor development (Folio and Fewell, 2000; Henderson et al., 2007; Kakebeeke et al., 2018) or on the decline of functions in old age (Samuel et al., 2012; Arking, 2006).

What is required is a coherent description of the normal course and variability of motor functions across the entire lifespan. For example, McKay et al. (2017) provided data for 12 fine and gross motor (GM) functions from 3 to 101 years, whereas Springer et al. (2007) focused on static balance between 18 and 99 years. However, the tracking of motor functions for clinical and research purposes would be facilitated by age- and sex-specific percentile reference curves, and these are still lacking. A key requirement for developing such percentiles is the reliable assessment of the same motor functions across the lifespan without variations in test design, motor tasks, or study procedures.

The Zurich Neuromotor Assessment (ZNA) meets these criteria. The test is widely used in clinical practice and research to assess motor skills across various domains, including fine and gross motor functions, balance, and associated movements (Kakebeeke et al., 2018). The ZNA is designed for a broad age range and provides objective measurements of neuromotor performance and quality of movements. Normative data for the ZNA have been published for individuals aged 3–18 years (Kakebeeke et al., 2018), and additional data are available for adults around ages 45 and 65 years (Kakebeeke et al., 2023). However, the ZNA has not been applied beyond age 65, and a comprehensive cross-sectional evaluation from early childhood to old age with percentile-based norms for motor functions is still lacking.

In this study, we provide normative data on neuromotor functions covering a variety of motor tasks over the lifespan in continuous, sex-specific percentile curves. Additionally, we examine the impact of body mass index (BMI) on neuromotor functions.

Materials and methods

This study combines cross-sectional measurements on motor functions collected on 1,620 individuals (828 females) aged between 6.0 and 81.5 years. The motor measurements were selected from 11 studies conducted by our group and at the same center between 1983 and 2023 (Wehrle et al., 2021; Largo et al., 2001b; Largo et al., 2001a; Rousson et al., 2008; Wilhelm et al., 2014; Wehrle et al., 2016; Kakebeeke et al., 2018; Knaier et al., 2023; Kakebeeke et al., 2023). For the studies after 2015, the ZNA-2 was used (Kakebeeke et al., 2023; Kakebeeke et al., 2018; Knaier et al., 2023; see Table 1). BMI measurements were available for 1,040.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Pooled sample characteristics.

For details on the study cohorts, please refer to the previous publications (Wehrle et al., 2021; Largo et al., 2001b; Largo et al., 2001a; Rousson et al., 2008; Wilhelm et al., 2014; Wehrle et al., 2016; Kakebeeke et al., 2018; Knaier et al., 2023; Kakebeeke et al., 2023). Because these studies only covered the ages from 6 to 67.6 years, an additional cohort was recruited to specifically capture data on the elderly. For this purpose, retired employees of the University Children’s Hospital Zurich and their partners were recruited (ZNA-p), with the only inclusion criteria being that they were physically mobile to come to the hospital for testing without walking aids and were free of known neurological diseases. For more details on the ZNA-p, please refer to Supplementary material 1. A few adaptations of the ZNA-2 test protocol were introduced in the ZNA-p, such as a reduction in the number of repetitions in the GM tasks of chair-rise and jumping sideways and the omission of the standing long jump to prevent injuries.

The general inclusion criteria were the absence of any known neurological disease, chronical physical illness, handicap or prematurity. In addition, for children and adolescents the inclusion criterion was attending regular schooling. For the cohort of 65–80 years old subjects: cognitive decline was an exclusion criterion as measured by mini-mental-state-assessment (Folstein et al., 1975). All studies from which data were extracted were approved by the Ethical Commission of the Canton Zurich and were performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki. For studies involving children, all families received a study description, and the primary parent or guardian provided written consent for the participation of the children. Additionally, children from 6 to 14 years gave verbal consent. Adolescents older than 14 years gave written informed consent. All participants older than 18 years provided consent.

Individuals were tested with either the original ZNA (Largo et al., 2001b; Largo et al., 2001a) or the ZNA-2 (Kakebeeke et al., 2018; Kakebeeke et al., 2013). The ZNA (the original form as well as its update ZNA-2) is a standardized procedure to describe neuromotor development in children and adolescents. It measures neuromotor performance for five components: fine motor (FM), pure motor (PM), balance (BA), gross motor (GM), and movement quality in contralateral associated movements (CAMs). An overview of the structure of the test in presented in Table 2. Original ZNA data were accommodated to the ZNA-2 testing procedures. For a comparison between the two versions, see Supplementary material 2. Recently, the ZNA-2 has also been used with adults (Kakebeeke et al., 2023).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of the tasks of the ZNA-2 (Kakebeeke et al., 2019).

Data analysis

Outcomes of interest were timed performance (seconds), jumping distance (centimeters), and an index measuring movement quality called the intensity of CAM (see Kakebeeke et al., 2018 for more details). The intensity of CAM can take 24 possible values between 0 and 5 and is approximately normally distributed over that range in healthy individuals (Kakebeeke et al., 2018).

Three of the 11 studies from which data were extracted featured data from the Zurich Longitudinal Studies (ZLS) (Wehrle et al., 2021). To maintain a cross-sectional design well suited to modeling developmental norms, we randomly selected one data point per individual from the longitudinal measurements by employing unequal sampling probabilities. The sampling probabilities were designed to favor the selection of a single data point among longitudinal measurements (a) in age ranges that were less frequently represented in the cross-sectional studies and (b) at ages where measurements on many other tasks were also available.

Generalized additive models for location, scale, and shape implemented in the gamlss R package (Rigby and Stasinopoulos, 2005) were used to model the evolution of timed performance, jumping distance, and intensity of CAMs as a function of age and sex. This framework provides flexibility in modeling developmental tasks by employing simple models (e.g., linear regression, albeit possibly on some transformed scale) when appropriate, while also accommodating more complex features—such as skewness and heteroscedasticity—when supported by the data. Model selection at each stage was guided by the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) (Schwarz, 1978), which imposes a stronger penalty for model complexity than alternatives like the Akaike information criterion (AIC) (Akaike, 1974). As a result, BIC tends to favor more parsimonious models, leading to smooth percentile curves. When applicable, the models were adjusted for changes in testing procedures (see Supplementary material 2 for a description of differences between ZNA and ZNA-2). Consequently, predicted percentile curves were derived as if all measurements had been taken using the ZNA-2, with the same procedure over the whole lifespan, as defined in Table 2. Timed performance and jumping distance were modeled with a Box-Cox Cole and Green distribution (also called LMS method) (Cole and Green, 1992), which captures changes in median, variability, and skewness and reduces to a lognormal distribution when the shape parameter controlling skewness is fixed to zero. A right-censored lognormal model addressed ceiling effects in SB tasks. CAM intensity was modeled with an interval-censored Gaussian distribution using censoring intervals defined by the mid points between the 24 possible values of intensity. This approach has already been used to model CAM intensity in the ZNA and ZNA-2 (Largo et al., 2001b; Kakebeeke et al., 2018), and is justified both from a clinical and statistical point of view (Gasser and Rousson, 2004; Gasser et al., 2007). The age effect was modeled using penalized splines (Eilers and Marx, 1996) while optimizing the BIC for smoothness. The splines were fitted to the logarithm of age because this resulted in a lower BIC and thus improved fit in nearly all tasks. The model included interactions between splines and sex to provide separate norms for males and females. A pragmatic model selection refined the structure of scale and shape parameters, starting with a simple intercept model and adding age and sex effects only if they improved BIC. The shape parameter was modeled with either the same or a simpler structure than the scale parameter. Outliers were identified by calculating task standard deviation scores (SDSs) and flagging those observations exceeding ±3 SDS. The fitting procedure was then repeated after excluding the previously flagged data until no additional outliers were detected.

A bootstrap procedure was used to establish 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the 50th percentile curve by resampling individuals within sex (200 replications) and refitting the norms while fixing the degrees of freedom in the penalized splines. This method also allowed us to generate pointwise 95% CIs for the sex difference at each age to highlight where typical motor functions in males differed significantly from those in females. Additionally, a likelihood ratio test was conducted to assess the sex difference globally across all ages.

For each task, we examined the age at which motor functions began to decline by plotting the first derivative, or slope, of the 50% centile curve against age for males and females. Pointwise 95% CIs for the first derivative were also obtained through the bootstrap procedure. We identified the age at which the first derivative reached zero, indicating peak performance, for each sex and reported the oldest age when it occurred multiple times. Finally, we calculated the rank correlation between the ages of peak performance for males and females across the tasks separately for timed performance or jumping distance and CAMs.

Task SDSs were combined into the five ZNA-2 components after replacing missing task SDSs by the average of SDS available for tasks loading on the same component. In later analyses, each component SDS was weighted inversely to the proportion of imputed task SDS. BMI centiles, adjusted for age and sex, were derived from the subset of data with available BMI measurements using the same approach employed to model motor functions, thus allowing for the calculation of age- and sex-adjusted BMI SDS. Motor component SDSs were modeled as a function of smooth effects of BMI SDS using generalized additive models with the mgcv R package (Wood, 2004) while using weights defined by the number of imputed task SDSs. The impact of BMI on each ZNA-2 component was visualized by plotting predicted motor SDS and its 95% CI against BMI SDS.

Median curves (with 95% bootstrap CI) for males and females obtained in this study were compared with results from McKay et al. (2017) on standing long jump, including 1,000 healthy individuals from Sydney, Australia and with the findings from Springer et al. (2007) for standing on one leg with eyes open on 549 healthy participants from Washington DC. In the latter case, our results were adapted to conform to the procedure and censoring time used in the Springer et al. (2007) study. All statistical analyses were conducted in R software version 4.4.1 (R Core Team, 2024).

Results

The highest proportion of data points excluded during model fitting due to |SDS| > 3 was 1.1%, observed in the repetitive hand movement task with the non-dominant hand. All other tasks had less than 1% of outliers (see Supplementary material 5).

Effects of age and sex

In Figure 1, FM functions are illustrated through the pegboard task (dominant side). A fast increase in motor performance can be seen in both sexes, especially below 10 years. During adolescence, the functional improvement slows down, reaches a maximum in the early thirties followed by a slow but continuous deterioration from the early forties onward. Figure 2 displays the evolution of motor functions for selected tasks loading on several motor components (PM: repetitive hand movement, dominant hand; BA: standing on one leg with eyes closed, non-dominant leg; GM: jumping sideways; and CAM: during alternating foot movements, non-dominant side active). A fast increase in motor performance is also seen in these tasks. However, the onset of aging and the speed of the deterioration in motor function is quite variable from one task to the other. Detailed figures with sex specific percentile curves for all tasks are shown in Supplementary materials 3, 4. Overall, it seems that once peak performance is reached, PM functions remain relatively stable and only suffer a mild decline with increasing age, while FM functions decrease faster. In contrast, the deterioration in motor function following peak performance appears larger in BA and GM, with motor functions in the elderly reaching comparable levels to those of the 6 years olds. The intensity of CAM also seems to increase in some tasks (e.g., alternating foot movements) while others remain relatively unaffected (e.g., pegboard).

FIGURE 1
Scatter plot showing pegboard task completion time (seconds) normalized to the exercise with 10 pegs, against age (years) for the dominant side. Reference curves representing 10th, 50th, and 90th percentiles are overplotted for males and females. In both sexes, time decreases until around age 20, then remains stable, rising again after age 60.

Figure 1. Pegboard (dominant side) timed performance (seconds) as a function of age, with estimated percentile curves (10%, 50%, and 90%) as a fine motor component task. Male data and percentiles are represented with blue dots and curves, and female data and percentiles are represented with red dots and curves. Observed (dots) and predicted timed performance (percentile curves) are normalized to the exercise with 10 pegs, as in the ZNA-2.

FIGURE 2
Scatter plots showing evolution of motor performance along 4 components of the ZNA-2, with addition of reference curves for percentiles 10%, 50% and 90% for males (blue) and females (red). Panel A (top left) displays time performance for repetitive hand movements (pure motor component) on the dominant hand, Panel B (top right) for static balance (balance component) on the non-dominant leg with eyes closed, Panel C (bottom left) for jumping sideways (gross motor component), and Panel D (bottom right) displays the evolution of intensity of contralateral associated movements with increasing age.

Figure 2. Percentile curves (10%, 50%, and 90%) and data points displaying the changes over age for males (blue) and females (red) in (A) pure motor – repetitive hand movements, dominant hand; (B) balance – standing on one leg, eyes closed, non-dominant leg; (C) gross motor – jumping sideways; the amount of jumps in the 65–80 year olds was multiplied with two; and (D) quality of movement – contralateral associated movements (CAMs) during alternating foot movements, non-dominant side active.

Figures illustrating the gap between the 50th centile of males and females at each age are provided in Supplementary material 6. They also report the p-value of the likelihood ratio test comparing models with and without sex differences. A table summarizing important findings from these figures is available in Supplementary material 7. Virtually all tasks featured a statistically significant sex difference, especially at younger ages. With the exception of standing long jump where males did perform much better than females, the magnitude of the sex difference in other tasks was small when compared to the (sex-adjusted) between-subject variability depicted on the norms. In FM, females performed consistently better than males in pegboard and beads, while males usually performed better in the bolt task (though only after 50 years on the dominant side). In PM, males were performing better on all repetitive tasks while females performed better on alternating and sequential tasks. The only exception was alternating hand movements, where no significant sex difference could be observed on either side (p ≥ 0.181). Females performed better on all BA tasks, although the difference was only statistically significant during childhood. Furthermore, males were consistently better on GM tasks. As children, the boys generally had more CAMs than girls of the same age. However, as adults the differences became largely insignificant for most items, persisting only on sequential finger movements (dominant hand), a task that females also performed faster than males. For further details on the differences in sex on the tasks, refer to Supplementary materials 4, 6, 7.

Age of peak performance

Because motor performance increases rapidly during childhood and decreases in old age, it is of particular interest to identify the age at which motor performance reaches its peak. This peak age is defined as the point where the first derivative of the 50% centile curve equals zero, and lies between 20 and 25 years for standing long jump (Figure 3A). Plots of the first derivative of the 50th centile curve for all tasks are provided in Supplementary material 8. Overall, the age of peak performance lies between the early twenties and the mid-thirties. However, maximum performance in most BA and GM tasks appears to be reached first, approximately between 20 and 25 years, whereas performance in other motor dimensions still improves for a few more years. Excepting pegboard, males appeared to reach peak performance at the same age or slightly older than did females on all other tasks. The median difference in peak performance age between males and females was +1 year (interquartile range 0–2 years). Additionally, the rank correlation between the peak performance ages in males and females across the tasks was remarkably high at 0.97 and 0.96 for timed performance or jumping distances and for CAMs, respectively (Figure 3B). For further details about the timing of peak performance and associated sex differences, see Supplementary material 8.

FIGURE 3
Panel A shows average change in standing long jump distance (first derivative of 50% percentile curve) over two months versus age for males and females. Positive changes refer to improved performance while negative changes refer to degraded performance. Improvements in jumping distance decline steadily until 20-25 years, with jumping performance declining thereafter. Panel B shows peak performance age by sex for each timed performance task. Task labels are colored according to the component they belong to (fine motor, pure motor, balance or graoss motor). The rank correlation between peak performance age of males and females is 0.97.

Figure 3. Age of peak performance. (A) First derivative or slope of 50% centile with 95% confidence interval for standing long jump as a function of age, for males (blue) and females (red). The y-axis value refers to the expected change in jumping distance that occurs over a 2 months’ period (i.e., ±1 month around each age). The age of peak performance is reached when the curve crosses 0. (B) Peak performance age for males (x-axis) and females (y-axis) for all tasks. Rank correlation between ages of peak performance in males and females for timed performance/jumping distance. For abbreviations see Table 2; task labels BLTnd, RHDd, AFTnd, AHDd/nd, SFGnd, and SBCnd are not shown for readability.

Effect of BMI on ZNA-2 components

The BMI of our sample increases during youth and adolescence conforming to the Swiss and WHO norms on weight and height. After 45 years, the 50th percentile for BMI remained stable for females at 23.3 kg/m2, but it still increased slightly in males, from 24.8 kg/m2 at 45 years to 25.5 kg/m2 at 80 years. At 45 years, percentiles 84.1% and 97.7% of BMI (corresponding to +1 and +2 SDSs) were equal to 29.2 and 36.7 for males, and 27.5 and 34.6 for females, respectively. As shown in Figure 4, motor performance in FM, PM, and CAM was not significantly associated with BMI. BA and GM showed no BMI effect on performance up to a BMI of approximately +1 SDS (equivalent to percentile 84.1%). However, a higher BMI had a deleterious impact on both BA and GM performance.

FIGURE 4
Five graphs illustrating the relationship between BMI z-score and predicted SDS across different motor components with 95% confidence intervals. No effect of BMI can be depicted on SDS of fine motor (panel A), pure motor (panel B) and intensity of contralateral asssociated movements (panel E). In contrast, a sharp decrease in balance (panel C) and gross motor performance (panel D) is seen when BMI exceeds 1 SDS.

Figure 4. Predicted effects of BMI SDS on fine motor (FM, A), pure motor (PM, B), balance (BA, C), gross motor (GM, D), and contralateral associated movements (CAMs, E). The shaded areas correspond to the 95% confidence intervals.

Comparison with other studies

For the standing long jump, the data presented here are nearly identical with the data gathered by McKay et al. (2017), which are integrated and displayed in Figure 5A. Small differences can be detected, but these differences are mostly due to how the data were handled: McKay et al. (2017) calculated the average jumping distance within distinct age groups, whereas our modeling approach considered age as a continuous variable. Although Springer et al. (2007) did not assess balance during childhood, their adult balance data show similar results to ours between 25 and 60 years, as seen on Figure 5B. Some differences can be seen in the elderly, where the US sample performed worse than the Swiss but still showed the same sex effect; the performance in females dropped earlier. Their mean BMI (18–99 years) was 27.07 compared to a mean BMI (18–80 years) of 23.54 in this study.

FIGURE 5
Graphs comparing age-related performance in standing long jump and standing on one leg obtained in our study with data from external studies. The long jump (panel A) shows a good agreement between our study and that of McKay et al (2017), especially regarding age of peak performance and sex difference. Panel B shows some discrepancies between our results and those of Springer et al (2007) above 60 years, where ceiling effect is less impactful. However, those differences could be explained by the higher BMI in the cohort of Springet et al., which may have contributed to lower balance performance.

Figure 5. (A) Median curves of the standing long jump from this study (solid lines, with pointwise 95% confidence interval) compared to the data from the online calculator of the McKay et al. (2017) study (dashed lines). Note that our norms stop at 65 years because participants in the ZNM-p did not perform the standing long jump. (B) Average time standing on one leg with eyes open (dominant side) from this study (solid lines, with pointwise 95% confidence interval) compared to the data from the Springer et al. (2007) study (dashed lines). Our results were obtained by simulating timed performance from the balance model and then censoring the simulated values at 45 s to ensure comparability with the Springer et al. (2007) study. Males in blue, females in red.

Discussion

This study is the first to present percentile curves for several neuromotor functions between 6 and 80 years of age for both sexes. The changes across the lifespan were investigated in tasks measuring FM, PM, BA, and GM functions and CAMs.

The findings confirm significant functional improvements in young children, a slowdown during adolescence and adulthood, and a decline in older ages (Haywood and Getchell, 2014; Arking, 2006). Notably, PM and FM tasks show pronounced improvements during youth, with peak performances occurring between 25 and 35 years of age. BA and GM tasks also exhibit a marked increase during childhood and adolescence mostly reaching peak performance in the early twenties followed by a decline. These changes are particularly evident in balance tasks, which depend on multiple body systems that evolve with age. Beside muscular strength and coordination, proprioceptive, visual, vestibular, and auditory perceptual systems all contribute to balance function (Haywood and Getchell, 2014). Each of these systems undergoes specific aging processes. A decrease in muscle mass causes a deterioration in the capacity to stand upright (Haywood and Getchell, 2014; Springer et al., 2007). At greater ages, a decline in force variability amplitude hinders rapid adaptations to balance corrections when standing on one leg (Brunner et al., 2007). And finally, older adults become increasingly dependent on their visual system for balance, shown by the fact that BA with eyes closed deteriorates more rapidly than BA with eyes open, as was also shown by Springer et al. (2007).

For the GM tasks of jumping sideways, chair rising, and standing long jump, the increase in performance during childhood and adolescence is attributed to the increase in muscle mass in adolescents, especially in males. Peak performances in jumping tasks are attained at early adult age and earlier by females than by males: Peak performance in jumping sideways occurs at 19 years for females and 20 years for males, and in standing long jump at 21 years for females and 25 years for males (see Figure 3B). The subsequent decline in performance can be attributed to a reduction in the size of the fast muscle fibers (Hepple and Rice, 2016; JafariNasabian et al., 2017; Deschenes, 2004; Brunner et al., 2007). In contrast, the chair rise test starts to deteriorate only after 35 years because the slower muscle fibers start to deteriorate later (Hepple and Rice, 2016).

The late decline in performance of PM and FM in comparison to BA and GM was striking. Additionally, the 50th percentile of PM and FM at old age (around 80) was functionally always better, i.e., faster than at the starting point of 6 years of age. It looks as if – once learnt – performance is preserved on a high level. Presumably the high maintenance of PM and FM is accredited to the lifelong commitment and daily use of the hands and feet. The change of CAMs over age follows a similar course: at the beginning the children (between 6 and 10 years) have a lot of CAMs, they disappear during adolescence and during aging they reappear, but are less abundant than in the young children (Kakebeeke et al., 2023).

Tasks on which females are always better than males are the pegboard, beads, and sequential finger movements. Tasks on which males are always better than females are repetitive movements of hands, fingers, and feet. The consistency in the sex difference observed between repetitive PM tasks and sequential PM and FM tasks suggests that these functions are probably linked to biological sex. This also applies to movement quality; females always have fewer CAMs than do males except on the bolts task. In BA, females always perform better than males until adolescence. At older ages, sex differences could not be reliably estimated due to ceiling effects and/or a sparsity of data. In the GM tasks, males always perform better than the females, which can be attributed to the males having more muscle mass (10%) than the females (Bredella, 2017). However, the differences between the sexes are generally small in comparison to the high variability within the sexes, yet they remain highly statistically significant, which is why we present sex-specific percentiles.

The study also examines age and sex differences by reporting the age of peak performance for various tasks. Males mature on average 1 year later than females in all the functions, as shown by the high rank correlation (r = 0.97) between the ages of peak performance in males and females. Males also exhibit more CAMs than females of the same age, thus confirming that females mature earlier (Tanner, 1978; Tanner et al., 1983; Thomas and French, 1985; Haywood and Getchell, 2014).

Although higher BMI did not significantly affect PM, FM, or CAM performances, it negatively impacted BA and GM at BMI > 1 SDS. The deleterious effect of higher BMI on motor performance in antigravity tasks emphasizes the importance of maintaining a healthy weight from an early age to support motor performance (Chivers et al., 2013; Haywood and Getchell, 2014).

Some limitations need to be considered when interpreting these curves. Despite pooling data from 11 studies, resulting in the analysis of motor measurements from 1,620 individuals, the density of data is not uniform. Many data points are available for the developmentally dynamic phases, whereas for ages from 25 to 65 years, the data density is lower (Figures 1, 2). Further, we focused on obtaining smooth reference curves for motor functions over the whole lifespan and used a strong smoothness penalty (BIC) to model the age trend. This resulted in smoothed percentiles that provide an acceptable good fit globally, but may occasionally lack precise fit within some age ranges. Also, although the age of peak performance is an interesting metric with which to compare the speed of maturation between males and females, it is not well defined on tasks in which the degradation in motor performance due to aging is not pronounced. Consequently, its estimation may be imprecise.

The representativeness of data for neuromuscular function across the lifespan is often discussed, and several recruitment biases have been acknowledged, especially in elderly populations (Banack et al., 2019; Enzenbach et al., 2019). The motor measurements performed on children and adolescents from the Zurich area were obtained from diverse samples stratified by socioeconomic status (SES) (Kakebeeke et al., 2018; Largo et al., 2001b; Largo et al., 2001a). Adults around 45 years and around 65 years were characterized in detail and shown to be representative of the Swiss population (Kakebeeke et al., 2023). However, recruiting elderly participants poses challenges because healthier individuals are substantially more likely to volunteer, leading to a selection bias. In particular, the ZNA-p subsample included former employees of the University Children’s Hospital Zurich who were physically mobile. Due to the inclusion criteria this subpopulation is not completely representative of the general Swiss population at the same age. Nevertheless, the ZNA-p subsample aligned with the findings from the Swiss Health Survey (Bundesamt für Statistik, 2022), especially on BMI distribution (see Supplementary material 1). In contrast, representativity with respect to SES is more difficult to assess because the number of years of education, a proxy for SES in the elderly, shows large disparities between males and females. In fact, no single definition of SES applies from childhood to old age. Therefore, it was not possible to investigate any SES effect on motor performance across the wide age range covered in this study.

Despite these limitations, the neuromotor data presented in this study are suitable to serve as normative standards. In fact, comparisons with the reference values reveal similarities in key motor domains such as BA and GM (e.g., McKay et al., 2017). Some differences are likely due to varying protocols. The difference in BA in the elderly may be attributed to the higher BMI in the US sample (Springer et al., 2007), which is in line with our findings about high BMI effects. Thus, our findings reflect typical motor development across the lifespan, and the curves may be used beyond Switzerland.

The percentiles for neuromotor functions across the lifespan presented here may serve several of the purposes suggested by Burton and Miller (1998) and Burton and Rodgerson (2001). For example, the differing changes over the lifespan exhibited by GM and FM functions indicate which functions are more prone to aging than others. For clinicians, the norm data and percentile curves support norm-based diagnostics that allow individual motor performance in one or more domains to be interpreted relative to age-specific reference values. In fact, significantly below-average values (<10th percentile) are of clinical importance, as they may indicate functionally meaningful impairments. For example, in children and adolescents, one of the key diagnostic criteria for the diagnosis of developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is performance below a cut-off on a norm-based assessment (Kakebeeke et al., 2018), for which the ZNA-2 is used. We also assume that the norms could be clinically valuable in older adults, especially in the age group over 65 years, for estimating the risk of falling or determining the indication for therapeutic interventions and assistive devices in the case of FM impairments. Moreover, tracking a decline in motor performance over time may provide diagnostic insights into emerging neurological conditions. To distinguish such disease-related decline from the normal age-related changes in performance, sex- and age-specific percentile curves, as shown in Supplementary material 3, are essential.

Conclusion

This article presents percentiles for 14 neuromotor tasks of the ZNA-2 performed by participants from 6 to 80 years. Performance on most tasks featured an increase in motor functions during childhood followed by a decrease in performance during adulthood. The performance decline started in the early twenties; it was more pronounced in balance and GM functions but began later and was less pronounced in FM and PM functions. Particular precautions should be taken in judging the neuromotor performance of individuals with high BMI (>percentile 84.1% or >1 SDS) because the impact of gravity on weight-sensitive functions overrides motor function.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in this article/Supplementary materials. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Kantonale Ethikkommission Zürich. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

TK: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AC: Data curation, Formal Analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Visualization. VR: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Supervision. FW: Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. OJ: Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation, grant number 32003B_153273, the Maiores Foundation, the Velux Stiftung, and the Stiftung für das Kind Giedion Risch.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the participants for their contribution to this database. We thank numerous colleagues who have contributed to the Zurich Neuromotor Studies, including Remo H. Largo, Elisa Knaier, Elisabeth Kälin, and Theo Gasser. We also thank Simon Milligan for proofreading the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2025.1543408/full#supplementary-material

References

Akaike, H. (1974). New look at statistical-model identification. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. 19, 716–723. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arking, R. (2006). The biology of aging - observations and principles. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Banack, H. R., Kaufman, J. S., Wactawski-Wende, J., Troen, B. R., and Stovitz, S. D. (2019). Investigating and remediating selection bias in geriatrics research: The selection bias toolkit. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 67, 1970–1976. doi: 10.1111/jgs.16022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bredella, M. A. (2017). Sex differences in body composition. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 1043, 9–27. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-70178-3_2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunner, F., Schmid, A., Sheikhzadeh, A., Nordin, M., Yoon, J., and Frankel, V. (2007). Effects of aging on Type II muscle fibers: A systematic review of the literature. J. Aging Phys. Act 15, 336–348. doi: 10.1123/japa.15.3.336

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bundesamt für Statistik (2022). Schweizerische Gesundheitsbefragung. Neuchâtel: Bundesamt für Statistik.

Google Scholar

Burton, A. W., and Miller, D. A. (1998). Movement skill assessment. Leeds: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Burton, A. W., and Rodgerson, R. W. (2001). New perspectives on the assessment of movement skills and motor abilities. Adapt. Phys. Act. Q. 18, 347–365. doi: 10.1123/apaq.18.4.347

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chivers, P., Larkin, D., Rose, E., Beilin, L., and Hands, B. (2013). Low motor performance scores among overweight children: Poor coordination or morphological constraints? Hum. Mov. Sci. 32, 1127–1137. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2013.08.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, T. J., and Green, P. J. (1992). Smoothing reference centile curves - the LMS method and penalized likelihood. Stat. Med. 11, 1305–1319. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780111005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deschenes, M. R. (2004). Effects of aging on muscle fibre type and size. Sports Med. 34, 809–824. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200434120-00002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eilers, P. H. C., and Marx, B. D. (1996). Flexible smoothing with B-splines and penalties. Stat. Sci. 11, 89–121. doi: 10.1214/ss/1038425655

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Enzenbach, C., Wicklein, B., Wirkner, K., and Loeffler, M. (2019). Evaluating selection bias in a population-based cohort study with low baseline participation: The LIFE-Adult-Study. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 19:135. doi: 10.1186/s12874-019-0779-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Folio, M., and Fewell, R. (2000). Peabody developmental motor Scales-2. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and Mchugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Galhardas, L., Raimundo, A., Del Pozo-Cruz, J., and Marmeleira, J. (2022). Physical and motor fitness tests for older adults living in nursing homes: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:5058. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19095058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasser, T., and Rousson, V. (2004). Modelling neuromotor ratings with floor-effects. Stat. Med. 23, 3641–3653. doi: 10.1002/sim.1914

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasser, T., Rousson, V., Caflisch, J., and Largo, R. (2007). Quantitative reference curves for associated movements in children and adolescents. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 608–614. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00608.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haywood, K., and Getchell, N. (2014). Life span motor development, 6th Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Henderson, S. E., Sugden, D. A., Barnett, A. L., Petermann, F., Bös, K., and Kastner, J. (2007). Movement assessment battery for children - 2nd Edn (Movement ABC-2) - deutschsprachige adaptation. London: Harcourt Assessment.

Google Scholar

Hepple, R. T., and Rice, C. L. (2016). Innervation and neuromuscular control in ageing skeletal muscle. J. Physiol. 594, 1965–1978. doi: 10.1113/JP270561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

JafariNasabian, P., Inglis, J. E., Reilly, W., Kelly, O. J., and Ilich, J. Z. (2017). Aging human body: Changes in bone, muscle and body fat with consequent changes in nutrient intake. J. Endocrinol. 234, R37–R51. doi: 10.1530/JOE-16-0603

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakebeeke, T. H., Caflisch, J., Chaouch, A., Rousson, V., Largo, R. H., and Jenni, O. G. (2013). Neuromotor development in children. Part 3: Motor performance in 3- to 5-year-olds. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 55, 248–256. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakebeeke, T. H., Caflisch, J., Largo, R. H., and Jenni, O. G. (2019). Zürcher neuromotorik-2. Zürich: Akademie Für das Kind Giedion Risch.

Google Scholar

Kakebeeke, T. H., Chaouch, A., Caflisch, J., Eichelberger, D. A., Wehrle, F. M., and Jenni, O. G. (2023). Comparing neuromotor functions in 45- and 65-year-old adults with 18-year-old adolescents. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1286393. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1286393

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kakebeeke, T. H., Knaier, E., Chaouch, A., Caflisch, J., Rousson, V., Largo, R. H., et al. (2018). Neuromotor development in children. Part 4: New norms from 3 to 18 years. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 60, 810–819. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13793

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knaier, E., Chaouch, A., Caflisch, J. A., Rousson, V., Wehrle, F. M., Kakebeeke, T. H., et al. (2023). Secular trends in motor performance in Swiss children and adolescents from 1983 to 2018. Front. Public Health 11:1095586. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1095586

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koerte, I., Eftimov, L., Laubender, R. P., Esslinger, O., Schroeder, A. S., Ertl-Wagner, B., et al. (2010). Mirror movements in healthy humans across the lifespan: Effects of development and ageing. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 52, 1106–1112. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03766.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanzino, D. J., Conner, M. N., Goodman, K. A., Kremer, K. H., Petkus, M. T., and Hollman, J. H. (2012). Values for timed limb coordination tests in a sample of healthy older adults. Age Ageing 41, 803–807. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Largo, R. H., Caflisch, J. A., Hug, F., Muggli, K., Molnar, A. A., Molinari, L., et al. (2001a). Neuromotor development from 5 to 18 years. Part 1: Timed performance. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 436–443. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201000810

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Largo, R. H., Caflisch, J. A., Hug, F., Muggli, K., Molnar, A. A., and Molinari, L. (2001b). Neuromotor development from 5 to 18 years. Part 2: Associated movements. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 444–453. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201000822

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McKay, M. J., Baldwin, J. N., Ferreira, P., Simic, M., Vanicek, N., Burns, J., et al. (2017). Reference values for developing responsive functional outcome measures across the lifespan. Neurology 88, 1512–1519. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003847

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perera, S., Studenski, S., Newman, A., Simonsick, E., Harris, T., Schwartz, A., et al. (2014). Are estimates of meaningful decline in mobility performance consistent among clinically important subgroups? (Health ABC study). J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 69, 1260–1268. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2024). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. reference index. Vienna: R Core Team.

Google Scholar

Rigby, R. A., and Stasinopoulos, D. M. (2005). Generalized additive models for location, scale and shape. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. C Appl. Stat. 54, 507–544. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9876.2005.00510.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousson, V., Gasser, T., Caflisch, J., and Largo, R. (2008). Reliability of the zurich neuromotor assessment. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 60–72. doi: 10.1080/13854040601076702

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Samuel, D., Wilson, K., Martin, H. J., Allen, R., Sayer, A. A., and Stokes, M. (2012). Age-associated changes in hand grip and quadriceps muscle strength ratios in healthy adults. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 24, 245–250. doi: 10.1007/BF03325252

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–465. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Springer, B. A., Marin, R., Cyhan, T., Roberts, H., and Gill, N. W. (2007). Normative values for the unipedal stance test with eyes open and closed. J. Geriatr. Phys. Ther. 30, 8–15. doi: 10.1519/00139143-200704000-00003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stodden, D. F., Goodway, J. D., Langendorfer, S. J., Roberton, M. A., Rudisill, M. E., Garcia, C., et al. (2008). A developmental perspective on the role of motor skill competence in physical activity: An emergent relationship. Quest 60, 290–306. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2008.10483582

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanner, J. M. (1978). Foetus into man: Physical growth from conception to maturity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Tanner, J. M., Landt, K. W., Cameron, N., Carter, B. S., and Patel, J. (1983). Prediction of adult height from height and bone age in childhood. A new system of equations (TW Mark II) based on a sample including very tall and very short children. Arch. Dis. Child 58, 767–776. doi: 10.1136/adc.58.10.767

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, J. R., and French, K. E. (1985). Gender differences across age in motor performance a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 98, 260–282. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.98.2.260

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wehrle, F. M., Caflisch, J., Eichelberger, D. A., Haller, G., Latal, B., Largo, R. H., et al. (2021). The importance of childhood for adult health and development-study protocol of the zurich longitudinal studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:612453. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.612453

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wehrle, F. M., Kaufmann, L., Benz, L. D., Huber, R., O’Gorman, R. L., Latal, B., et al. (2016). Very preterm adolescents show impaired performance with increasing demands in executive function tasks. Early Hum. Dev. 92, 37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.10.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilhelm, I., Kurth, S., Ringli, M., Mouthon, A. L., Buchmann, A., Geiger, A., et al. (2014). Sleep slow-wave activity reveals developmental changes in experience-dependent plasticity. J. Neurosci. 34, 12568–12575. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0962-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, S. N. (2004). Stable and efficient multiple smoothing parameter estimation for generalized additive models. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 99, 673–686. doi: 10.1198/016214504000000980

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: motor development, aging, lifespan, Zurich Neuromotor Assessment (ZNA), body mass index (BMI), percentiles of neuromotor performance

Citation: Kakebeeke TH, Caflisch J, Chaouch A, Rousson V, Wehrle FM and Jenni OG (2025) Neuromotor functions across the lifespan: percentiles from 6 to 80 years. Front. Aging Neurosci. 17:1543408. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2025.1543408

Received: 11 December 2024; Accepted: 02 July 2025;
Published: 29 July 2025.

Edited by:

Simon M. Danner, Drexel University, United States

Reviewed by:

Luca Petrigna, University of Catania, Italy
Guillaume Caron, Université Paris Cité, France

Copyright © 2025 Kakebeeke, Caflisch, Chaouch, Rousson, Wehrle and Jenni. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tanja H. Kakebeeke, dGFuamEua2FrZWJlZWtlQGtpc3BpLnV6aC5jaA==

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.