Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 18 October 2019
Sec. Sleep and Circadian Rhythms
This article is part of the Research Topic Local Aspects of Sleep and Wakefulness View all 15 articles

Sleepiness as a Local Phenomenon

  • 1Dipartimento di Scienze Biomediche e Cliniche “L. Sacco”, Università Degli Studi di Milano, Milan, Italy
  • 2Sleep and Epilepsy Center, Neurocenter of Southern Switzerland, Civic Hospital (EOC) of Lugano, Lugano, Switzerland
  • 3Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics and Maternal/Child Sciences, University of Genoa, Genoa, Italy
  • 4Department of Neuroscience (DINOGMI), University of Genoa, Genoa, Italy
  • 5IRCCS, Child Neuropsychiatry Unit, Giannina Gaslini Institute, Genoa, Italy

Sleep occupies a third of our life and is a primary need for all animal species studied so far. Nonetheless, chronic sleep restriction is a growing source of morbidity and mortality in both developed and developing countries. Sleep loss is associated with the subjective feeling of sleepiness and with decreased performance, as well as with detrimental effects on general health, cognition, and emotions. The ideas that small brain areas can be asleep while the rest of the brain is awake and that local sleep may account for at least some of the cognitive and behavioral manifestations of sleepiness are making their way into the scientific community. We herein clarify the different ways sleep can intrude into wakefulness, summarize recent scientific advances in the field, and offer some hypotheses that help framing sleepiness as a local phenomenon.

Introduction

Epidemiological data have shown that, over the last decades, we are seeing a concerning decrease in both the duration and the quality of sleep in developed and developing countries (Dinges, 1995; Broman et al., 1996; Dinges et al., 1997; Liu and Zhou, 2002; Krueger and Friedman, 2009; Maric et al., 2017). The progressive shift toward “24-h societies” has been accompanied by an increase in “sleepiness” and its associated detrimental effects on the individual’s performance, cognition, emotions, and general health (Dinges et al., 1997; Van Dongen et al., 2003a; Chee and Choo, 2004; Banks and Dinges, 2007; Bernert and Joiner, 2007; Knutson et al., 2007; Goel et al., 2009, 2014; Couyoumdjian et al., 2010; Grandner et al., 2010; Krause et al., 2017). Thus, understanding the regulatory mechanisms of sleepiness and their implications for human health is urgent and of utmost importance (Garbarino et al., 2016).

Although the concept of sleepiness might sound intuitive, at a closer look its definition is far from trivial, and neither is the answer to fundamental issues like what sleepiness is from a neurobiological standpoint. Attempts to operationalize the subjective feeling of sleepiness for clinical and research purposes have led to the development of a number of tools, some based on subjective ratings (e.g., the Epworth Sleepiness Scale), others on objective measures like cognitive performance (e.g., reaction time test, driving-simulators) and electroencephalography [e.g., multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) or polysomnography (PSG)]. Despite the reliability of these validated measures, their agreement remains poor as they capture different aspects of sleepiness, differentially influenced by endogenous, exogenous, and situational factors. For tackling and overcoming the complex phenomenon of sleepiness, previous studies have employed and suggested a twofold approach of identifying “sleepy” patients based on combined subjective and objective sleepiness and/or physiological and biochemical biomarkers (Olson et al., 1998; Kritikou et al., 2014); this approach may be more valuable than any single measure of sleepiness (Fleming et al., 2016) but is not yet exhaustive.

In the last few years, science has produced compelling evidence supporting the idea that both sleep and wakefulness are under local regulation (Siclari and Tononi, 2017; Krueger et al., 2019). These ideas were influenced by studies performed during the transition between wake and sleep, where it was found that some brain areas may fall asleep, or awaken, before others (Pigarev et al., 1997; Magnin et al., 2010; Marzano et al., 2013; Sarasso et al., 2014a, b; Siclari et al., 2014). In further support of this view, sleep homeostasis can be modulated on a local level by active or passive tasks or via local synaptic potentiation (Kattler et al., 1994; Miyamoto et al., 2003; Huber et al., 2004, 2006, 2007; De Gennaro et al., 2008; Vyazovskiy and Tobler, 2008; Hanlon et al., 2009; Lesku et al., 2011).

The presence of local sleep was also demonstrated through the observation that slow waves and spindles, the two major spontaneous electroencephalographic oscillations of sleep that arise from complex re-entrant circuits in the thalamocortical system, often occur out-of-phase in different brain regions (Andrillon et al., 2011; Nir et al., 2011).

This case is particularly compelling as both sleep spindles and slow waves are dependent on the hyperpolarization of thalamic relay and cortical neurons, respectively. This occurs during NREM sleep due to the progressive decrease of noradrenergic, serotonergic, and cholinergic neuromodulation from brainstem activating systems. As such, being under the influence of diffuse neuromodulatory systems, their occurrence, particularly for slow waves, has been long assumed to be an ubiquitous feature of virtually every neural cell of the sleeping brain (Steriade et al., 1993) occurring in a remarkably synchronous way (Volgushev et al., 2006).

Even more dramatically, during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, slow waves may coexist with transient local wake-like activity (Rector et al., 2005, 2009; Nobili et al., 2011; Peter-Derex et al., 2015). Similarly, local isles of sleep may intrude upon wakefulness (Rector et al., 2005, 2009; Vyazovskiy et al., 2011a; Hung et al., 2013; Quercia et al., 2018).

We herein clarify the different ways sleep can physiologically intrude into wakefulness, summarize the main findings on this topic, and offer a global framework to interpret sleepiness as a local phenomenon.

The Intrusion of Sleep Into Wakefulness

From a neuro-physiological standpoint, sleep may intrude upon wakefulness in the form of local sleep, electroencephalogram (EEG) slowing, and microsleep.

Local Sleep During Wakefulness

Local sleep is a complex physiological phenomenon occurring within anatomically discrete brain locations (Krueger et al., 2019). Experiments in isolated cortical slabs (Kristiansen and Courtois, 1949), as well as in slice preparations (Steriade et al., 1993) and cell cultures (Corner et al., 2008; Hinard et al., 2012), confirmed that slow waves—a key electrophysiological graphoelement characterizing the sleep state—is essentially an intrinsic property of cortical cells ensembles. Rector et al. (2005) provided the first indirect evidence of local sleep in living intact animals using surface evoked potentials (SEP) in rats. They showed that, while on average, wakefulness is characterized by low SEP amplitude and NREM sleep by high SEP amplitude (Hall and Borbely, 1970), SEP amplitude fluctuates over time during both states and is frequently different between hemispheres and nearby cortical columns. Moreover, the longer a cortical column produces low-amplitude wake-like SEP the more it will begin to produce large-amplitude sleep-like SEP (Rector et al., 2005).

Further indirect evidence in favor of local sleep emerged from cortical multiunit recordings in rats during sleep deprivation (SD): firing rates increased continuously for the first 3 h of SD and showed no further significant change in the last hour. This “ceiling-effect” was interpreted as the consequence of the increase in the number of local neuronal silent periods (or OFF-periods) (Vyazovskiy et al., 2009).

Local sleep had been observed more directly in rats in 2011 (Vyazovskiy et al., 2011a, b). After a period of prolonged wakefulness, cortical neurons tended to fall silent for brief periods, as they do during NREM sleep. These OFF-periods were associated with slow oscillations in the slow/theta range in local field potential (LFP) recordings, as also confirmed by a study applying micro-stimulation during prolonged wakefulness (Vyazovskiy et al., 2013). Local OFF-periods occurred asynchronously across brain regions and increased with time spent awake. Most strikingly, they occurred in behaviorally awake animals, and their presence over motor areas negatively affected motor performance during a sugar pellet reaching task.

More recently, use-dependent, local sleep-like EEG theta events have been found to occur during prolonged wakefulness in humans (Hung et al., 2013). As in the former studies on rodents, also in humans the occurrence of these events over frontal or posterior scalp regions was selectively associated with negative behavioral outcomes on executive functions or visuomotor control tasks, respectively (Bernardi et al., 2015; Quercia et al., 2018).

These findings lead to the intriguing hypothesis that deficits in sensory, psychomotor, and cognitive aspects of behavior after SD may arise as a result of altered neuronal responsiveness to incoming stimuli due to these OFF-periods.

Global and Local EEG Slowing

Under SD conditions, which are similar to what occurs during NREM sleep, the firing rates of neurons in ON-periods, as well as the number and duration of OFF-periods, the number of neurons participating synchronously in OFF-periods and the low-frequency content (particularly in the theta range) of the EEG increase (Vyazovskiy et al., 2011b). At odds with NREM sleep, though, is the fact that during SD these events typically involve an isolated portion of the cortex possibly reflecting the occurrence of more or less widespread local cortical isles of sleep during prolonged wakefulness. In further support of this hypothesis, an fMRI connectivity analysis indicated that prolonged wakefulness is associated with a decrease in measures representing the mean strength of coupling among brain areas, resembling the breakdown in connectivity typical of slow waves sleep (Bernardi et al., 2015; Kaufmann et al., 2016).

In humans (Cajochen et al., 1995; Aeschbach et al., 1997; Finelli et al., 2000; Strijkstra et al., 2003; Fattinger et al., 2017), as in rats (Franken et al., 1991; Vyazovskiy and Tobler, 2005), EEG power in the lower frequency bands (especially theta) progressively increase with the time spent in quiet waking. This slowing can be captured by surface EEG and is paralleled by subjective sleepiness—at least in humans (Aeschbach et al., 1996; Bernardi et al., 2015)—and by a decrease in behavioral performance (Gorgoni et al., 2014; Fattinger et al., 2017). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) measures converge with EEG measures in indicating that SD has severe effects on cortical activity. SD is associated with an increased TMS resting motor threshold and cortical facilitation, at least in females, and these changes clearly predict changes in EEG theta activity (De Gennaro et al., 2007).

The increase in low frequencies in the EEG is associated with the subsequent homeostatic increase of sleep slow-wave activity (SWA) during NREM sleep in both humans (Finelli et al., 2000) and rats (Borbély et al., 1984; Tobler and Borbely, 1986), as well as with the increase of theta and delta power during REM sleep (Tinguely et al., 2006). Notably, superimposed to the homeostatic process, several studies reported a strong circadian modulation of the waking EEG (Aeschbach et al., 1996; Dumont et al., 1999; Cajochen et al., 2002) so that the intrusion of low frequencies is restrained when sustained wakefulness coincides with the biological day, while it is completely free to manifest itself during the biological night (Cajochen et al., 2002). Similarly, cortical excitability assessed using TMS coupled with scalp EEG is robustly correlated with circadian dynamics and with endocrine markers of circadian amplitude (Ly et al., 2016).

Interestingly, these regulatory mechanisms may act locally. The aforementioned increase in low frequencies has a fronto-central predominance, mainly in the theta range during wakefulness (Finelli et al., 2000; Strijkstra et al., 2003) and in the SWA range during subsequent recovery sleep (Cajochen et al., 1999; Finelli et al., 2000; Leemburg et al., 2010), and it can be observed also after repeated partial sleep restriction (Plante et al., 2016).

More recently, studies performed in humans showed that the increase in waking theta EEG activity during SD displayed regional, use-dependent changes (Hung et al., 2013; Gorgoni et al., 2014; Bernardi et al., 2015; Nir et al., 2017). A first study took advantage of high-density EEG technology to show an increase in theta power over left frontal brain regions after a language-based task and over posterior parietal regions after a visuomotor task. The same regions displayed local increases in SWA power during subsequent recovery sleep (Hung et al., 2013). A subsequent study demonstrated that the occurrence of theta waves in task-related regions coincided with specific performance errors in humans (Bernardi et al., 2015). Another study used intra-cranial electrodes in human neurosurgical patients performing a psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) at baseline and during SD. Cognitive lapses involved local state-dependent changes in neuronal activity in the medial temporal lobe (MTL). Specifically, immediately before cognitive lapses the spiking responses of individual neurons were attenuated, delayed, and lengthened while, during cognitive lapses, LFPs showed a relative local increase in slow activity (Nir et al., 2017).

In line with these findings, a study using a driving simulator to evaluate the effect of sleepiness at the wheel, found that a local increase in theta EEG activity over the motor regions (as localized by EEG source modeling techniques) was associated with an increased risk of line departures (Ahlstrom et al., 2017).

Microsleep

In this review, microsleeps are defined as short episodes of sleep-like activity that satisfy criteria for stage 1 sleep (theta replacing alpha rhythm) except for their short duration of up to 15 s (Priest et al., 2001; Blaivas et al., 2007; Hertig-Godeschalk et al., 2019). Usually, blinking artifacts characteristic of full wakefulness disappear, often accompanied by the appearance of slow eye movements. However, behavioral changes, such as eye-closure and nodding-off, are not defining features of microsleeps, as they may or may not be present during microsleep episodes (Torsvall and Akerstedt, 1988; Boyle et al., 2008). Regardless, microsleeps may be associated with significant cognitive impairment—e.g., poorer performance during a continuous task under driving-simulator conditions (Boyle et al., 2008)—and are strictly associated with subjective sleepiness. Indeed, some evidence suggests that microsleeps analysis in MSLT might be a more sensitive and specific test for excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) as compared to MSLT alone (Tirunahari et al., 2003).

Microsleep episodes are more frequent after a sleep-restricted night compared to a normally rested night (Friedman et al., 1978; Horne and Pettitt, 1985; Poudel et al., 2018) and can be followed by a brief recovery in performance (Poudel et al., 2018).

Traditionally, microsleeps have been hypothesized to be global brain phenomena that reflect the transient shutdown of activating systems, with the parallel activation of sleep promoting centers (Sechenova, 2011; Silkis, 2013). However, recent evidence describes microsleep in terms of intermediate states between sleep and wakefulness (Hertig-Godeschalk et al., 2019), possibly reflecting their local nature. Supporting this notion, a recent fMRI study during one night of SD described local decreased activation over frontal, parietal, and occipital associative cortices as well as increased activation in the default mode network (DMN) associated with slow reaction times responses at the PVT (typically reflecting the occurrence of microsleeps), showing how these different patterns of activation and deactivation could depend on circadian phases as well as homeostatic sleep pressure and the interactions between the two (Zhu et al., 2018).

Neurobiology and Neurophysiology of Sleepiness

In simple terms, just as hunger is the physiological need for food, sleepiness can be described as the physiological need for sleep. Very few theoretical constructs about sleepiness are available in the literature (Cluydts et al., 2002; De Valck and Cluydts, 2003). Conceptually, sleepiness is the consequence of an imbalance between the sleep drive (level of activation of sleep circuits) and the wake drive (level of activation of arousal systems) (Cluydts et al., 2002). When wake still prevails but sleep pressure is high we experience sleepiness.

The concept of sleepiness, therefore, closely relates to the concept of “sleep debt.” While Horne originally proposed that sleep debt was uniquely the consequence of the loss of “core” or “obligatory sleep” (referred as the first 4–5 h of sleep) but not of “optional” or “facultative” sleep, which “fills the tedious hours of darkness until sunrise” (Horne, 1985), empirical research indicated that sleep debt accumulates linearly. Although clearly influenced and modulated by circadian factors (Shekleton et al., 2013), according to this line of research, sleep debt may be defined as the cumulative hours of sleep loss with respect to a subject-specific daily need for sleep (Van Dongen et al., 2003b).

Another closely related concept of sleepiness is “sleep inertia,” a physiological condition of subjective drowsiness, decreased alertness, and impaired cognitive and sensory-motor performance that arises during the transition between sleep to wakefulness. It has been shown that the subjective feeling of sleep inertia lasts on average 15–30 min, although objective measure of alertness and performance do not return to waking baseline until 2–4 h after waketime (Jewett et al., 1999). Electroencephalographic, evoked potential, and neuroimaging studies suggested that sleep inertia involves the intrusion of sleep patterns during wakefulness (Trotti, 2017), bringing the concepts of sleepiness and sleep inertia even closer and further corroborating the notion that vigilance states are not necessarily discrete.

But how does this translate from a neurophysiological standpoint? According to the data presented in the previous section, sleepiness may be associated with the occurrence of local sleep during wakefulness in the presence of a positive sleep debt. We will now focus on possible regulatory mechanisms of local sleep and their interaction with global processes.

Local Regulation of Sleep

Great progress has been made in characterizing the brain centers responsible for the orchestration of sleep and wakefulness as global behavioral states (Jones, 2005; Saper et al., 2005; Szymusiak and McGinty, 2008; Siegel, 2009; Brown et al., 2012). Although anatomically widespread, these centers act in a coordinated fashion in modulating whole-brain activity, thus allowing for a clear behavioral distinction between wake and sleep states (for a review see Saper et al., 2001; Scammell et al., 2017; Eban-Rothschild et al., 2018). According to Saper’s flip-flop switch model, sleep regulation depends on a mutually inhibitory interaction between sleep centers and the components of the arousal systems (Saper et al., 2001), located both in cortical and subcortical structures.

Although the described mechanisms may orchestrate sleep globally, sleep is fundamentally an intrinsic property of the cerebral neurons and can be regulated locally at the level of cortical areas as small as cortical columns (Krueger et al., 2008). This columnar state segregation is favored by the fact that the functional intracolumnar connections are denser than intercolumnar connections allowing greater activity and state synchrony between cells pertaining to the same column (Panzeri et al., 2003).

The very first model-based postulation of local sleep was published in 1993 and 1995 by Krueger and Obal (1993) and Krueger et al. (2008), who postulated that sleep begins as a local neuronal group event involving oscillations of inhibition and excitation and is thus “quantal” in nature. From this perspective, these authors considered sleepiness as a statistical phenomenon, the perception of which arises when a sufficient number of neuronal groups become “bistable.”

Coordination of neuronal group sleep results from both neuronal and humoral systems. As proposed by the Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis (SHY; Tononi and Cirelli, 2003, 2006), when neuronal plasticity during wakefulness is increased or decreased in specific brain areas, sleep intensity, as reflected by the amount of SWA, selectively increases or decreases in those areas (Kattler et al., 1994; Huber et al., 2004; Vyazovskiy and Tobler, 2008; Hanlon et al., 2009; Lesku et al., 2011). Indeed, increased synaptic connectivity means more synchronous oscillations and increased slow wave activity. Alternatively (or additionally), local slow wave generation could be due to a change in the excitability or amount of adaptation of individual neurons. Along these lines, a build-up in the need for cellular maintenance could cause individual neurons to show lower excitability and stronger adaptation (Vyazovskiy et al., 2013). OFF-periods would therefore occur locally were most needed, thus providing a potential explanation for local sleep patterns. Similarly, as soon as individual neurons fall below a certain cellular stress threshold, their excitability is restored, leading to a more wake-like pattern of activity.

Considerable evidence also suggests a role for local paracrine signaling pathways in the regulation of both global and local sleep. In this vein, it is known that sleep pressure correlates with the concentration of—among others—nitric oxide (NO) (Gautier-Sauvigné et al., 2005; Kalinchuk et al., 2011), adenosine, and various cytokines (Imeri and Opp, 2009) such as interleukin-1 (IL-1) and tumor necrosis factor (TNF). These substances are synthesized by metabolically or synaptically active cells and are released in a local fashion (Latini and Pedata, 2001; Porkka-Heiskanen and Kalinchuk, 2011). Again, Krueger et al. (1995b) played a pivotal role in delineating this humoral component (Krueger, 2008). They hypothesized that sleep at the neuronal group level is regulated by paracrine substances whose production and catabolism rates are synaptic use-dependent (Krueger et al., 1995a).

According to their model, Adenosine 5′-γ-ThiotriPhosphate (ATP) released during neurotransmission and acting on purine P2 receptors induces the release of IL1 and TNF. These cytokines in turn act on neurons to change their intrinsic properties (Krueger, 2008), directly or indirectly, altering the production of neuroendocrine substances and neurotransmitters, for example the growth hormone releasing hormone and NO, which are known to be involved in sleep-wake regulation (Krueger et al., 1995b). More recent evidences in support of this model have been shown by Nguyen et al. (2019).

Finally, there is another mechanism able to explain regional sleep differences, especially the aforementioned frontal predominance of the low frequency effect. Different regions might be more susceptible to sleep due to intrinsic differences in some of their activating inputs. In other words, even widespread projections from centers regulating sleep globally may present with topographical differences that may affect sleep-wake regulation at the level of cortical macro-areas. For example, recent evidence suggested a region-specific dissociation between cortical noradrenaline levels during the sleep/wake cycle (Bellesi et al., 2016). Compared to the motor cortex, in the medial Prefrontal Cortex (mPFC) noradrenaline levels are higher and changes in its concentration during sleep and wake are slower. Furthermore, SD leads to a decrease in noradrenaline only at the level of mPFC, suggesting that noradrenergic neurons targeting the prefrontal cortex may undergo fatigue earlier or more markedly than other projecting cells from locus coeruleus. An increased susceptibility of noradrenergic projections to the frontal cortex might explain frontal cognitive executive function impairments associated with sleepiness (Jones and Harrison, 2001) and why the frontal lobes display more evident electrophysiological signs of deep sleep after prolonged wakefulness (Plante et al., 2016). These mechanisms may, alone or in combination, explain the occurrence of local sleep during wakefulness, leading to the subjective feeling of sleepiness.

As a last note, it is worth mentioning here that, despite the fact that we mainly focused on the cortex when we tried to explain the origins of local sleep, NREM sleep patterns in vivo emerge from the interplay between the cortex and the thalamus, more specifically the thalamic reticular nucleus (TRN). It has been reviewed recently how cellular and functional TRN heterogeneity may account for some features of local NREM sleep (Vantomme et al., 2019). By experimentally modulating the activation and firing of the TRN neurons, it was indeed possible to rapidly induce slow wave activity (Lewis et al., 2015) as well as sleep spindles (Fernandez et al., 2018) in spatially restricted regions of the cortex. However, at the current state of the art, it remains unclear how the TRN contributes in terms of physiological conditions and what the signals that activate TRN neurons locally are. Further research will need to clarify these aspects and disentangle the causative role—if there is one —of TRN and the cortex in this loop-network.

Interplay Between Local and Global Regulation

Having highlighted local sleep regulatory mechanisms, it remains to be discussed how they interact with the different processes orchestrating sleep in a global fashion.

According to Saper’s model (Saper et al., 2005), sleep regulation relies on three main streams: the “homeostatic” (Porkka-Heiskanen et al., 1997, 2000; Huang et al., 2007, 2011, 2014), the “circadian” (Chou et al., 2003; Fuller et al., 2006), and the “cognitive/emotional” (Chou et al., 2002; Sakurai et al., 2005; Yoshida et al., 2006). Each drive can potentially act globally as well as locally. Aside from the well-known homeostatic local sleep modulation discussed above, there is evidence of regional modulation of brain circadian rhythmicity. This has been demonstrated by a recent fMRI study quantifying changes in brain responses to a sustained-attention task across the circadian cycle, during baseline wakefulness, SD, and after recovery sleep (Muto et al., 2016). Subcortical areas exhibited a dominant circadian modulation that closely followed the melatonin profile but had no significant influence on sleep debt. Cortical responses also showed significant circadian rhythmicity, the phase of which varied across brain regions, as well as a widespread negative influence exerted by sleep pressure. The mechanisms of this local modulation are unknown, although the authors suggested the potential role of clock gene expression. Intriguingly, an EEG study showed that circadian rhythms modulate the incidence amplitude, frequency, and slope of slow waves (the latter being the most accurate marker of synaptic strength), with a dominant effect on central and occipital areas (Lazar et al., 2015).

Moreover, global regulatory mechanisms, particularly regarding the homeostatic and the circadian components, may influence local sleep regulation. In this respect, the extent of brain areas displaying sleep features (and thus the associated behavioral impairments and subjective feeling of sleepiness) may rest on the level of synchronization between global regulatory mechanisms. As such, asynchrony and shift of phase between the homeostatic and the circadian drive may result in local sleep without a global state transition (see Figure 1, conceived for schematizing these concepts without fitting any biological data for the sleep drives or for the number of neurons in OFF-periods). Likewise, the cognitive/emotional system may modulate the interaction between the homeostatic and the circadian drives and keep the subject awake despite strong circadian and homeostatic sleep-promoting inputs, accentuating their desynchronization (Horne, 1985).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Interplay between global sleep drivers and cortical neuronal firing. The figure is intended to schematize the concepts described in this work without fitting any biological data for sleep drives or the number of neurons in OFF-periods. Top panels represent the time-course of the circadian and homeostatic drive over 24 h. Bottom panels represent the percentage of neurons in OFF-periods across 24 h. (A) circadian and homeostatic drives under physiological conditions. (B) circadian and homeostatic drives out of phase due to sustained wakefulness. (C) percentage of cortical neurons in OFF-periods under physiological conditions. (D) percentage of cortical neurons in OFF-periods during sustained wakefulness. Red bars: percentage of cortical neurons in OFF-periods. Blue lines: homeostatic drive. Green lines: circadian drive. Gray areas: night period. Yellow areas: sleep. Light-blue areas: sleepiness. The figure has been realized by fitting the mathematical function published in Daan et al. (1984).

In summary, local sleep may arise as an intrinsic property of each regulatory drive of the slow-wave cycle or by the desynchronization between these drives acting globally.

In turn, local sleep may affect brain centers responsible for sleep and wake as global behavioral states. As such, the occurrence of isolated local OFF-periods during wakefulness could subsequently lead to global sleep through the involvement of neuro-modulatory systems responsible for the generation of NREM sleep (Saper et al., 2010; Brown et al., 2012). Similar to focal onset seizures with impaired awareness, local changes in cortical activity may lead to profound global alteration of the vigilance state associated with loss of consciousness through the progressive involvement of other brain regions such as midline subcortical structures including the thalamus, the hypothalamus, and the brainstem (Englot and Blumenfeld, 2009). Recordings in the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO) neurons showed that their firing rates increase during sleep, almost doubling during recovery sleep after SD, but did not increase during prolonged wakefulness. Thus, as homeostatic sleep drive accumulates, it may influence other neurons in the brain, such as the median preoptic neurons, which provide input to the VLPO (Saper et al., 2010). Alternatively, there could be a threshold in the number of areas showing sleep signs during wakefulness, which may imply behavioral impairments and sleepiness at first, and only when passing the threshold of the transition to global sleep. Specifically, it is hypothesized that whole organism sleep is an emergent property of the collective neuronal assemblies (Rector et al., 2009), as when networks of neuronal assemblies are coupled they will tend to synchronize (Roy et al., 2008). As such, when the number of neuronal groups entering the sleep state exceeds a significant threshold, other groups will follow (Rector et al., 2009) thus enabling the full-fledged transition from wakefulness to sleep (see Figure 1, conceived for schematizing these concepts without fitting any biological data for the sleep drives or number of neurons in OFF-periods).

Sleepiness typically arises in conditions of SD and/or prolonged wakefulness. The prevalence of the so called “insufficient sleep syndrome” is estimated to be between 1 and 4% of the population (Ohayon, 2008) and two to four times higher in individuals sleeping less than 6 h per night compared to individuals sleeping between 7 and 8 h per night (Ohayon, 2012).

The great majority of sleep disorders determine sleepiness through the curtailment of total sleep time and/or sleep fragmentation without primarily affecting the function of sleep-promoting centers. Examples are sleep breathing disorders like obstructive sleep apnoea, circadian rhythm sleep-wake disorders like shift-work disorder, sleep related movement disorders like restless leg syndrome, and objective insomnia. Central disorders of hypersomnolence like narcolepsy type 1 and 2 or idiopathic hypersomnia involve instead a pathologic imbalance between sleep promoting and wake promoting pathways in favor of the former, determining an increased sleep need and/or the abrupt intrusion of sleep into wakefulness due to an instability of the switching mechanisms between sleep and wakefulness. Also, parasomnias like sleepwalking and sleep terrors have been associated with subjective daytime sleepiness, via as of yet unknown mechanisms (Carrillo-Solano et al., 2016).

Sleepiness is also related to mental and organic diseases that may directly or indirectly affect sleep (Ohayon, 2012). These disorders may cause a disruption of the sleep-wake schedule due to changes in behavior, dysregulate sleep centers orchestrating sleep due to neurological lesions or more subtle abnormalities, or nociceptive, immunomodulatory, or other modulatory inputs.

Conclusion and Future Directions

While familiar to all on a subjective level, sleepiness is a complex matter that science is just beginning to understand. We have herein summarized how, during prolonged wakefulness, the occurrence of local neuronal OFF-periods may relate to the well-known negative consequences on performance observed in this state. As suggested by the reviewed literature, this phenomenon of local sleep during wake may account for at least some of the cognitive and behavioral manifestations of sleepiness. Under this perspective, sleepiness may reflect the transition between different vigilance states, being an epiphenomenon of these “fluid boundaries” (Sarasso et al., 2014a).

This interpretation is probably the key that will help develop new measures to quantify sleepiness in the near future. From a clinical perspective, high-density EEG, which allows an optimal spatial and temporal resolution to capture local isles of EEG slowing, may represent a valuable technological support. Moreover, a better characterization of the role of the circadian rhythm and of its interaction with other drives that modulate the sleep-wake cycle is warranted. Finally, a promising future line of research will be on the linking of the neurophysiological concepts of local sleep and sleepiness to interindividual variability in susceptibility to sleepiness (Van Dongen et al., 2004; Kuna et al., 2012; Rupp et al., 2012; Goel et al., 2013; Spaeth et al., 2015). This will open the way to a more personalized sleep medicine that will have a considerable impact on human health and promote occupational well-being, benefiting the society as a whole.

Author Contributions

AC and SD’A wrote the first draft of the manuscript. OG, SG, LN, and SS wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by “Sinergia” CRSII3_160803/1 of the Swiss National Science Foundation to SD’A.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Aeschbach, D., Cajochen, C., Landolt, H., and Borbely, A. A. (1996). Homeostatic sleep regulation in habitual short sleepers and long sleepers. Am. J. Physiol. 270, R41–R53.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Aeschbach, D., Matthews, J. R., Postolache, T. T., Jackson, M. A., Giesen, H. A., and Wehr, T. A. (1997). Dynamics of the human EEG during prolonged wakefulness: evidence for frequency-specific circadian and homeostatic influences. Neurosci. Lett. 239, 121–124. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(97)00904-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahlstrom, C., Jansson, S., and Anund, A. (2017). Local changes in the wake electroencephalogram precedes lane departures. J. Sleep Res. 26, 816–819. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrillon, T., Nir, Y., Staba, R. J., Ferrarelli, F., Cirelli, C., Tononi, G., et al. (2011). Sleep spindles in humans: insights from intracranial EEG and unit recordings. J. Neurosci. 31, 17821–17834. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2604-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banks, S., and Dinges, D. F. (2007). Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 3, 519–528.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bellesi, M., Tononi, G., Cirelli, C., and Serra, P. A. (2016). Region-specific dissociation between cortical noradrenaline levels and the sleep/wake cycle. Sleep 39, 143–154. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernardi, G., Siclari, F., Yu, X., Zennig, C., Bellesi, M., Ricciardi, E., et al. (2015). Neural and behavioral correlates of extended training during sleep deprivation in humans: evidence for local, task-specific effects. J. Neurosci. 35, 4487–4500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4567-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernert, R. A., and Joiner, T. E. (2007). Sleep disturbances and suicide risk: a review of the literature. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 3, 735–743. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s1248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blaivas, A. J., Patel, R., Hom, D., Antigua, K., and Ashtyani, H. (2007). Quantifying microsleep to help assess subjective sleepiness. Sleep Med. 8, 156–159. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borbély, A. A., Tobler, I., and Hanagasioglu, M. (1984). Effect of sleep deprivation on sleep and EEG power spectra in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 14, 171–182. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(84)90186-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyle, L. N., Tippin, J., Paul, A., and Rizzo, M. (2008). Driver Performance in the Moments Surrounding a Microsleep. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 11, 126–136. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2007.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broman, J. E., Lundh, L. G., and Hetta, J. (1996). Insufficient sleep in the general population. Neurophysiol. Clin. 26, 30–39. doi: 10.1016/0987-7053(96)81532-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. E., Basheer, R., Mckenna, J. T., Strecker, R. E., and Mccarley, R. W. (2012). Control of sleep and wakefulness. Physiol. Rev. 92, 1087–1187. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Brunner, D. P., Krauchi, K., Graw, P., and Wirz-Justice, A. (1995). Power density in theta/alpha frequencies of the waking EEG progressively increases during sustained wakefulness. Sleep 18, 890–894. doi: 10.1093/sleep/18.10.890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Khalsa, S. B., Wyatt, J. K., Czeisler, C. A., and Dijk, D. J. (1999). EEG and ocular correlates of circadian melatonin phase and human performance decrements during sleep loss. Am. J. Physiol. 277, R640–R649.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cajochen, C., Wyatt, J. K., Czeisler, C. A., and Dijk, D. J. (2002). Separation of circadian and wake duration-dependent modulation of EEG activation during wakefulness. Neuroscience 114, 1047–1060. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00209-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrillo-Solano, M., Leu-Semenescu, S., Golmard, J.-L., Groos, E., and Arnulf, I. (2016). Sleepiness in sleepwalking and sleep terrors: a higher sleep pressure? Sleep Med. 26, 54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chee, M. W., and Choo, W. C. (2004). Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci. 24, 4560–4567. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0007-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, T. C., Bjorkum, A. A., Gaus, S. E., Lu, J., Scammell, T. E., and Saper, C. B. (2002). Afferents to the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus. J. Neurosci. 22, 977–990. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.22-03-00977.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chou, T. C., Scammell, T. E., Gooley, J. J., Gaus, S. E., Saper, C. B., and Lu, J. (2003). Critical role of dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus in a wide range of behavioral circadian rhythms. J. Neurosci. 23, 10691–10702. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.23-33-10691.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cluydts, R., De Valck, E., Verstraeten, E., and Theys, P. (2002). Daytime sleepiness and its evaluation. Sleep Med. Rev. 6, 83–96. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2002.0191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corner, M. A., Baker, R. E., and Van Pelt, J. (2008). Physiological consequences of selective suppression of synaptic transmission in developing cerebral cortical networks in vitro: differential effects on intrinsically generated bioelectric discharges in a living ‘model’ system for slow-wave sleep activity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1569–1600. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couyoumdjian, A., Sdoia, S., Tempesta, D., Curcio, G., Rastellini, E., De Gennaro, L., et al. (2010). The effects of sleep and sleep deprivation on task-switching performance. J. Sleep Res. 19, 64–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00774.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daan, S., Beersma, D. G., and Borbely, A. A. (1984). Timing of human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. Am. J. Physiol. 246, R161–R183.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

De Gennaro, L., Fratello, F., Marzano, C., Moroni, F., Curcio, G., Tempesta, D., et al. (2008). Cortical plasticity induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation during wakefulness affects electroencephalogram activity during sleep. PLoS One 3:e2483. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Gennaro, L., Marzano, C., Veniero, D., Moroni, F., Fratello, F., Curcio, G., et al. (2007). Neurophysiological correlates of sleepiness: a combined TMS and EEG study. Neuroimage 36, 1277–1287. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Valck, E., and Cluydts, R. (2003). Sleepiness as a state-trait phenomenon, comprising both a sleep drive and a wake drive. Med. Hypotheses 60, 509–512. doi: 10.1016/s0306-9877(02)00444-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinges, D. F. (1995). An overview of sleepiness and accidents. J. Sleep Res. 4, 4–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1995.tb00220.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinges, D. F., Pack, F., Williams, K., Gillen, K. A., Powell, J. W., Ott, G. E., et al. (1997). Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep 20, 267–277.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dumont, M., Macchi, M. M., Carrier, J., Lafrance, C., and Hebert, M. (1999). Time course of narrow frequency bands in the waking EEG during sleep deprivation. Neuroreport 10, 403–407. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199902050-00035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eban-Rothschild, A., Appelbaum, L., and De Lecea, L. (2018). Neuronal Mechanisms for Sleep/Wake Regulation and Modulatory Drive. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 937–952. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Englot, D. J., and Blumenfeld, H. (2009). Consciousness and epilepsy: why are complex-partial seizures complex? Prog. Brain Res. 177, 147–170. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17711-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fattinger, S., Kurth, S., Ringli, M., Jenni, O. G., and Huber, R. (2017). Theta waves in children’s waking electroencephalogram resemble local aspects of sleep during wakefulness. Sci. Rep. 7:11187. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11577-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez, L. M., Vantomme, G., Osorio-Forero, A., Cardis, R., Béard, E., and Lüthi, A. (2018). Thalamic reticular control of local sleep in mouse sensory cortex. eLife 7:e39111. doi: 10.7554/eLife.39111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finelli, L. A., Baumann, H., Borbely, A. A., and Achermann, P. (2000). Dual electroencephalogram markers of human sleep homeostasis: correlation between theta activity in waking and slow-wave activity in sleep. Neuroscience 101, 523–529. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00409-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, W. E., Ferouz-Colborn, A., Samoszuk, M. K., Azad, A., Lu, J., Riley, J. S., et al. (2016). Blood biomarkers of endocrine, immune, inflammatory, and metabolic systems in obstructive sleep apnea. Clin. Biochem. 49, 854–861. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiochem.2016.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franken, P., Dijk, D. J., Tobler, I., and Borbely, A. A. (1991). Sleep deprivation in rats: effects on EEG power spectra, vigilance states, and cortical temperature. Am. J. Physiol. 261, R198–R208.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Friedman, L., Bergmann, B. M., and Rechtschaffen, A. (1978). Effects of sleep deprivation on sleepiness, sleep intensity, and subsequent sleep in the rat. Sleep 1, 369–391. doi: 10.1093/sleep/1.4.369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuller, P. M., Gooley, J. J., and Saper, C. B. (2006). Neurobiology of the sleep-wake cycle: sleep architecture, circadian regulation, and regulatory feedback. J. Biol. Rhythms 21, 482–493. doi: 10.1177/0748730406294627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garbarino, S., Lanteri, P., Durando, P., Magnavita, N., and Sannita, W. G. (2016). Co-Morbidity, mortality, quality of life and the healthcare/welfare/social costs of disordered sleep: a rapid review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 13:E831. doi: 10.3390/ijerph13080831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gautier-Sauvigné, S., Colas, D., Parmantier, P., Clement, P., Gharib, A., Sarda, N., et al. (2005). Nitric oxide and sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 9, 101–113.

Google Scholar

Goel, N., Abe, T., Braun, M. E., and Dinges, D. F. (2014). Cognitive workload and sleep restriction interact to influence sleep homeostatic responses. Sleep 37, 1745–1756. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goel, N., Basner, M., Rao, H., and Dinges, D. F. (2013). Circadian rhythms, sleep deprivation, and human performance. Prog. Mol. Biol. Transl. Sci. 119, 155–190. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-396971-2.00007-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goel, N., Rao, H., Durmer, J. S., and Dinges, D. F. (2009). Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin. Neurol. 29, 320–339. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1237117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorgoni, M., Ferlazzo, F., Ferrara, M., Moroni, F., D’Atri, A., Fanelli, S., et al. (2014). Topographic electroencephalogram changes associated with psychomotor vigilance task performance after sleep deprivation. Sleep Med. 15, 1132–1139. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandner, M. A., Hale, L., Moore, M., and Patel, N. P. (2010). Mortality associated with short sleep duration: the evidence, the possible mechanisms, and the future. J. Sleep Med. Rev. 14, 191–203. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, R. D., and Borbely, A. A. (1970). Acoustically evoked potentials in the rat during sleep and waking. Exp. Brain Res. 11, 93–110.

Google Scholar

Hanlon, E. C., Faraguna, U., Vyazovskiy, V. V., Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2009). Effects of skilled training on sleep slow wave activity and cortical gene expression in the rat. Sleep 32, 719–729. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.6.719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertig-Godeschalk, A., Skorucak, J., Malafeev, A., Achermann, P., Mathis, J., and Schreier, D. R. (2019). Microsleep episodes in the borderland between wakefulness and sleep. Sleep doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz163 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hinard, V., Mikhail, C., Pradervand, S., Curie, T., Houtkooper, R. H., Auwerx, J., et al. (2012). Key electrophysiological, molecular, and metabolic signatures of sleep and wakefulness revealed in primary cortical cultures. J. Neurosci. 32, 12506–12517. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2306-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horne, J., and Pettitt, A. N. (1985). High incentive effects on vigilance performance during 72 hours of total sleep deprivation. Acta Psychol. 58, 123–139. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(85)90003-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horne, J. A. (1985). Sleep function, with particular reference to sleep deprivation. Ann. Clin. Res. 17, 199–208.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Huang, Z. L., Urade, Y., and Hayaishi, O. (2007). Prostaglandins and adenosine in the regulation of sleep and wakefulness. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 7, 33–38. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2006.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Z. L., Urade, Y., and Hayaishi, O. (2011). The role of adenosine in the regulation of sleep. Curr. Top. Med. Chem. 11, 1047–1057.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Huang, Z. L., Zhang, Z., and Qu, W. M. (2014). Roles of adenosine and its receptors in sleep-wake regulation. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 119, 349–371. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801022-8.00014-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, R., Esser, S. K., Ferrarelli, F., Massimini, M., Peterson, M. J., and Tononi, G. (2007). TMS-induced cortical potentiation during wakefulness locally increases slow wave activity during sleep. PLoS One 2:e276. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., Ferrarelli, F., Riedner, B. A., Peterson, M. J., et al. (2006). Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1169–1176. doi: 10.1038/nn1758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., and Tononi, G. (2004). Local sleep and learning. Nature 430, 78–81. doi: 10.1038/nature02663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, C. S., Sarasso, S., Ferrarelli, F., Riedner, B., Ghilardi, M. F., Cirelli, C., et al. (2013). Local experience-dependent changes in the wake EEG after prolonged wakefulness. Sleep 36, 59–72. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imeri, L., and Opp, M. R. (2009). How (and why) the immune system makes us sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 199–210. doi: 10.1038/nrn2576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jewett, M. E., Wyatt, J. K., Ritz-De Cecco, A., Khalsa, S. B., Dijk, D. J., and Czeisler, C. A. (1999). Time course of sleep inertia dissipation in human performance and alertness. J. Sleep Res. 8, 1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1999.00128.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K., and Harrison, Y. (2001). Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 5, 463–475. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. E. (2005). From waking to sleeping: neuronal and chemical substrates. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 26, 578–586. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2005.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalinchuk, A. V., Mccarley, R. W., Porkka-Heiskanen, T., and Basheer, R. (2011). The time course of adenosine, nitric oxide (NO) and inducible NO synthase changes in the brain with sleep loss and their role in the non-rapid eye movement sleep homeostatic cascade. J. Neurochem. 116, 260–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.07100.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kattler, H., Dijk, D. J., and Borbely, A. A. (1994). Effect of unilateral somatosensory stimulation prior to sleep on the sleep EEG in humans. J. Sleep Res. 3, 159–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1994.tb00123.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, T., Elvsåshagen, T., Alnæs, D., Zak, N., Pedersen, P. Ø, Norbom, L. B., et al. (2016). The brain functional connectome is robustly altered by lack of sleep. Neuroimage 127, 324–332. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, K. L., Spiegel, K., Penev, P., and Van Cauter, E. (2007). The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 163–178. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, A. J., Simon, E. B., Mander, B. A., Greer, S. M., Saletin, J. M., Goldstein-Piekarski, A. N., et al. (2017). The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 404–418. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kristiansen, K., and Courtois, G. (1949). Rhythmic electrical activity from isolated cerebral cortex. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1, 265–272. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(49)90038-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kritikou, I., Basta, M., Vgontzas, A. N., Pejovic, S., Liao, D., Tsaoussoglou, M., et al. (2014). Sleep apnoea, sleepiness, inflammation and insulin resistance in middle-aged males and females. ERJ 43, 145–155. doi: 10.1183/09031936.00126712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, J. M. (2008). The role of cytokines in sleep regulation. Curr. Pharm. Des. 14, 3408–3416. doi: 10.2174/138161208786549281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, J. M., Nguyen, J. T., Dykstra-Aiello, C. J., and Taishi, P. (2019). Local sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 43, 14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2018.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, J. M., Obál, F. Jr., Kapás, L., and Fang, J. (1995a). Brain organization and sleep function. Behav. Brain Res. 69, 177–185. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00015-l

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, J. M., Takahashi, S., Kapás, L., Bredow, S., Roky, R., Fang, J., et al. (1995b). Cytokines in sleep regulation. Adv. Immunol. 5, 171–188.

Google Scholar

Krueger, J. M., and Obal, F. (1993). A neuronal group theory of sleep function. J. Sleep Res. 2, 63–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1993.tb00064.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, J. M., Rector, D. M., Roy, S., Van Dongen, H. P., Belenky, G., and Panksepp, J. (2008). Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 910–919. doi: 10.1038/nrn2521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, P. M., and Friedman, E. M. (2009). Sleep duration in the United States: a cross-sectional population-based study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 169, 1052–1063. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuna, S. T., Maislin, G., Pack, F. M., Staley, B., Hachadoorian, R., Coccaro, E. F., et al. (2012). Heritability of performance deficit accumulation during acute sleep deprivation in twins. Sleep 35, 1223–1233. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latini, S., and Pedata, F. (2001). Adenosine in the central nervous system: release mechanisms and extracellular concentrations. J. Neurochem. 79, 463–484. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00607.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazar, A. S., Lazar, Z. I., and Dijk, D.-J. (2015). Circadian regulation of slow waves in human sleep: topographical aspects. Neuroimage 116, 123–134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leemburg, S., Vyazovskiy, V. V., Olcese, U., Bassetti, C. L., Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2010). Sleep homeostasis in the rat is preserved during chronic sleep restriction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 15939–15944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002570107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesku, J. A., Vyssotski, A. L., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., Wilzeck, C., and Rattenborg, N. C. (2011). Local sleep homeostasis in the avian brain: convergence of sleep function in mammals and birds? Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 2419–2428. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, L. D., Voigts, J., Flores, F. J., Schmitt, L. I., Wilson, M. A., Halassa, M. M., et al. (2015). Thalamic reticular nucleus induces fast and local modulation of arousal state. eLife 4:e08760. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X., and Zhou, H. (2002). Sleep duration, insomnia and behavioral problems among Chinese adolescents. Psychiatry Res. 111, 75–85. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00131-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ly, J. Q., Gaggioni, G., Chellappa, S. L., Papachilleos, S., Brzozowski, A., Borsu, C., et al. (2016). Circadian regulation of human cortical excitability. Nat. Commun. 7:11828. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnin, M., Rey, M., Bastuji, H., Guillemant, P., Mauguiere, F., and Garcia-Larrea, L. (2010). Thalamic deactivation at sleep onset precedes that of the cerebral cortex in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 3829–3833. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909710107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maric, A., Montvai, E., Werth, E., Storz, M., Leemann, J., Weissengruber, S., et al. (2017). Insufficient sleep: enhanced risk-seeking relates to low local sleep intensity. Ann. Neurol. 82, 409–418. doi: 10.1002/ana.25023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzano, C., Moroni, F., Gorgoni, M., Nobili, L., Ferrara, M., and De Gennaro, L. (2013). How we fall asleep: regional and temporal differences in electroencephalographic synchronization at sleep onset. Sleep Med. 14, 1112–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyamoto, H., Katagiri, H., and Hensch, T. (2003). Experience-dependent slow-wave sleep development. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 553–554. doi: 10.1038/nn1064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muto, V., Jaspar, M., Meyer, C., Kussé, C., Chellappa, S. L., Degueldre, C., et al. (2016). Local modulation of human brain responses by circadian rhythmicity and sleep debt. Science 353, 687–690. doi: 10.1126/science.aad2993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, J., Gibbons, C. M., Dykstra-Aiello, C., Ellingsen, R., Koh, K. M. S., Taishi, P., et al. (2019). Interleukin-1 receptor accessory proteins are required for normal homeostatic responses to sleep deprivation. J. Appl. Physiol. 127, 770–780. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00366.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nir, Y., Andrillon, T., Marmelshtein, A., Suthana, N., Cirelli, C., Tononi, G., et al. (2017). Selective neuronal lapses precede human cognitive lapses following sleep deprivation. Nat. Med. 23, 1474–1480. doi: 10.1038/nm.4433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nir, Y., Staba, R. J., Andrillon, T., Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C., Fried, I., et al. (2011). Regional slow waves and spindles in human sleep. Neuron 70, 153–169. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nobili, L., Ferrara, M., Moroni, F., De Gennaro, L., Russo, G. L., Campus, C., et al. (2011). Dissociated wake-like and sleep-like electro-cortical activity during sleep. Neuroimage 58, 612–619. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohayon, M. M. (2008). From wakefulness to excessive sleepiness: what we know and still need to know. Sleep Med. Rev. 12, 129–141. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohayon, M. M. (2012). Determining the level of sleepiness in the American population and its correlates. J. Psychiatr. Res. 46, 422–427. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, L., Cole, M., and Ambrogetti, A. (1998). Correlations among Epworth sleepiness scale scores, multiple sleep latency tests and psychological symptoms. J. Sleep Res. 7, 248–253. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1998.00123.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panzeri, S., Petroni, F., Petersen, R. S., and Diamond, M. E. (2003). Decoding neuronal population activity in rat somatosensory cortex: role of columnar organization. Cereb. Cortex 13, 45–52. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.1.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peter-Derex, L., Magnin, M., and Bastuji, H. (2015). Heterogeneity of arousals in human sleep: a stereo-electroencephalographic study. Neuroimage 123, 229–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pigarev, I. N., Nothdurft, H. C., and Kastner, S. (1997). Evidence for asynchronous development of sleep in cortical areas. Neuroreport 8, 2557–2560. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199707280-00027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plante, D. T., Goldstein, M. R., Cook, J. D., Smith, R., Riedner, B. A., Rumble, M. E., et al. (2016). Effects of partial sleep deprivation on slow waves during non-rapid eye movement sleep: a high density EEG investigation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1436–1444. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.10.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porkka-Heiskanen, T., and Kalinchuk, A. V. (2011). Adenosine, energy metabolism and sleep homeostasis. Sleep Med. Rev. 15, 123–135. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2010.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porkka-Heiskanen, T., Strecker, R. E., and Mccarley, R. W. (2000). Brain site-specificity of extracellular adenosine concentration changes during sleep deprivation and spontaneous sleep: an in vivo microdialysis study. Neuroscience 99, 507–517. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00220-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porkka-Heiskanen, T., Strecker, R. E., Thakkar, M., Bjorkum, A. A., Greene, R. W., and Mccarley, R. W. (1997). Adenosine: a mediator of the sleep-inducing effects of prolonged wakefulness. Science 276, 1265–1268. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5316.1265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poudel, G. R., Innes, C. R. H., and Jones, R. D. (2018). Temporal evolution of neural activity and connectivity during microsleeps when rested and following sleep restriction. Neuroimage 174, 263–273. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priest, B., Brichard, C., Aubert, G., Liistro, G., and Rodenstein, D. O. (2001). Microsleep during a simplified maintenance of wakefulness test. A validation study of the OSLER test. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 163, 1619–1625. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.163.7.2007028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quercia, A., Zappasodi, F., Committeri, G., and Ferrara, M. (2018). Local use-dependent sleep in wakefulness links performance errors to learning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:122. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rector, D. M., Schei, J. L., Van Dongen, H. P., Belenky, G., and Krueger, J. M. (2009). Physiological markers of local sleep. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 1771–1778. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06717.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rector, D. M., Topchiy, I. A., Carter, K. M., and Rojas, M. J. (2005). Local functional state differences between rat cortical columns. Brain Res. 1047, 45–55. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, S., Krueger, J. M., Rector, D. M., and Wan, Y. (2008). A network model for activity-dependent sleep regulation. J. Theor. Biol. 253, 462–468. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.03.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rupp, T. L., Wesensten, N. J., and Balkin, T. J. (2012). Trait-like vulnerability to total and partial sleep loss. Sleep 35, 1163–1172. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakurai, T., Nagata, R., Yamanaka, A., Kawamura, H., Tsujino, N., Muraki, Y., et al. (2005). Input of orexin/hypocretin neurons revealed by a genetically encoded tracer in mice. Neuron 46, 297–308. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saper, C. B., Cano, G., and Scammell, T. E. (2005). Homeostatic, circadian, and emotional regulation of sleep. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 92–98. doi: 10.1002/cne.20770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saper, C. B., Chou, T. C., and Scammell, T. E. (2001). The sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness. Trends Neurosci. 24, 726–731. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)02002-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saper, C. B., Fuller, P. M., Pedersen, N. P., Lu, J., and Scammell, T. E. (2010). Sleep state switching. Neuron 68, 1023–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarasso, S., Pigorini, A., Proserpio, P., Gibbs, S. A., Massimini, M., and Nobili, L. (2014a). Fluid boundaries between wake and sleep: experimental evidence from Stereo-EEG recordings. Arch. Ital. Biol. 152, 169–177. doi: 10.12871/0002982920142311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarasso, S., Proserpio, P., Pigorini, A., Moroni, F., Ferrara, M., De Gennaro, L., et al. (2014b). Hippocampal sleep spindles preceding neocortical sleep onset in humans. Neuroimage 86, 425–432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scammell, T. E., Arrigoni, E., and Lipton, J. O. (2017). Neural Circuitry of Wakefulness and Sleep. Neuron 93, 747–765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sechenova, I. M. (2011). Rossiiskii Fiziologicheskii Zhurnal. Transl. Silkis 97, 472–482.

Google Scholar

Shekleton, J. A., Rajaratnam, S. M. W., Gooley, J. J., Van Reen, E., Czeisler, C. A., and Lockley, S. W. (2013). Improved neurobehavioral performance during the wake maintenance zone. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 9, 353–362. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.2588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siclari, F., Bernardi, G., Riedner, B. A., Larocque, J. J., Benca, R. M., and Tononi, G. (2014). Two distinct synchronization processes in the transition to sleep: a high-density electroencephalographic study. Sleep 37, 1621–1637. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siclari, F., and Tononi, G. (2017). Local aspects of sleep and wakefulness. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 44, 222–227. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, J. M. (2009). The neurobiology of sleep. Semin. Neurol. 29, 277–296.

Google Scholar

Silkis, I. G. (2013). Generation of ponto-geniculo-occipital (pgo) waves as a possible cause of impairments to visual perception during microsleep. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 43, 122–128. doi: 10.1007/s11055-012-9701-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaeth, A. M., Dinges, D. F., and Goel, N. (2015). Phenotypic vulnerability of energy balance responses to sleep loss in healthy adults. Sci. Rep. 5:14920. doi: 10.1038/srep14920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steriade, M., Nunez, A., and Amzica, F. (1993). A novel slow (< 1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J. Neurosci. 13, 3252–3265. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.13-08-03252.1993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strijkstra, A. M., Beersma, D. G., Drayer, B., Halbesma, N., and Daan, S. (2003). Subjective sleepiness correlates negatively with global alpha (8-12 Hz) and positively with central frontal theta (4-8 Hz) frequencies in the human resting awake electroencephalogram. Neurosci. Lett. 340, 17–20. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00033-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szymusiak, R., and McGinty, D. (2008). Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and arousal. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1129, 275–286. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinguely, G., Finelli, L. A., Landolt, H. P., Borbely, A. A., and Achermann, P. (2006). Functional EEG topography in sleep and waking: state-dependent and state-independent features. Neuroimage 32, 283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tirunahari, V. L., Zaidi, S. A., Sharma, R., Skurnick, J., and Ashtyani, H. (2003). Microsleep and sleepiness: a comparison of multiple sleep latency test and scoring of microsleep as a diagnostic test for excessive daytime sleepiness. Sleep Med. 4, 63–67. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(02)00250-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobler, I., and Borbely, A. A. (1986). Sleep EEG in the rat as a function of prior waking. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 64, 74–76. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(86)90044-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2003). Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res. Bull. 62, 143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2006). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med. Rev. 10, 49–62. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torsvall, L., and Akerstedt, T. (1988). Extreme sleepiness: quantification of EOG and spectral EEG parameters. Int. J. Neurosci. 38, 435–441. doi: 10.3109/00207458808990704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trotti, L. M. (2017). Waking up is the hardest thing I do all day: sleep inertia and sleep drunkeness. Sleep Med. Rev. 35, 76–84. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dongen, H. P., Baynard, M. D., Maislin, G., and Dinges, D. F. (2004). Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep 27, 423–433.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Van Dongen, H. P., Maislin, G., Mullington, J. M., and Dinges, D. F. (2003a). The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep 26, 117–126. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dongen, H. P., Rogers, N. L., Dinges, D. F. J. S., and Rhythms, B. (2003b). Sleep debt: theoretical and empirical issues. Sleep Biol. Rhythm 1, 5–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1446-9235.2003.00006.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vantomme, G., Osorio-Forero, A., Lüthi, A., and Fernandez, L. M. J. (2019). Regulation of Local Sleep by the Thalamic Reticular Nucleus. Front. Neurosci. 13:576. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volgushev, M., Chauvette, S., Mukovski, M., and Timofeev, I. (2006). Precise long-range synchronization of activity and silence in neocortical neurons during slow-wave sleep. J. Neurosci. 26, 5665–5672. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0279-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C., and Tononi, G. (2011a). Electrophysiological correlates of sleep homeostasis in freely behaving rats. Prog. Brain Res. 193, 17–38. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00002-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Olcese, U., Hanlon, E. C., Nir, Y., Cirelli, C., and Tononi, G. (2011b). Local sleep in awake rats. Nature 472, 443–447. doi: 10.1038/nature10009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Olcese, U., Cirelli, C., and Tononi, G. (2013). Prolonged wakefulness alters neuronal responsiveness to local electrical stimulation of the neocortex in awake rats. J. Sleep Res. 22, 239–250. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Olcese, U., Lazimy, Y. M., Faraguna, U., Esser, S. K., Williams, J. C., et al. (2009). Cortical firing and sleep homeostasis. Neuron 63, 865–878. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., and Tobler, I. (2005). Theta activity in the waking EEG is a marker of sleep propensity in the rat. Brain Res. 1050, 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., and Tobler, I. (2008). Handedness leads to interhemispheric EEG asymmetry during sleep in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 969–975. doi: 10.1152/jn.01154.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshida, K., Mccormack, S., Espana, R. A., Crocker, A., and Scammell, T. E. (2006). Afferents to the orexin neurons of the rat brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 494, 845–861. doi: 10.1002/cne.20859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, Y., Xi, Y., Fei, N., Liu, Y., Zhang, X., Liu, L., et al. (2018). Dynamics of cerebral responses to sustained attention performance during one night of sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 27, 184–196. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: local sleep, sleepiness, microsleep, EEG slowing, sleep loss, prolonged wakefulness, OFF-periods, performance

Citation: D’Ambrosio S, Castelnovo A, Guglielmi O, Nobili L, Sarasso S and Garbarino S (2019) Sleepiness as a Local Phenomenon. Front. Neurosci. 13:1086. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01086

Received: 20 March 2019; Accepted: 26 September 2019;
Published: 18 October 2019.

Edited by:

Michele Bellesi, University of Bristol, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Vincenzo Muto, University of Liège, Belgium
Valentina Alfonsi, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2019 D’Ambrosio, Castelnovo, Guglielmi, Nobili, Sarasso and Garbarino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sasha D’Ambrosio, sasha.dambrosio@unimi.it; Anna Castelnovo, anna.castelnovo@eoc.ch; Simone Sarasso, simone.sarasso@unimi.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.