Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

OPINION article

Front. Dev. Psychol., 20 August 2025

Sec. Social and Emotional Development

Volume 3 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fdpys.2025.1638327

Early markers of atypical socio-moral development

  • 1Department of Educational Sciences, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
  • 2Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, University of Trento, Trento, Italy

Introduction

Human social relationships aim to care for mutual wellbeing (Afshordi and Liberman, 2021; Trivers, 1971). From birth, humans are motivated to look longer at social stimuli, as well as to select, engage, and bond with social partners (e.g., Fiske, 2018; Tomasello, 2014). Developmental literature indicates that infants employ intuitive theories that allow them to generate predictions and explanations for different events (e.g., Baillargeon et al., 2016; Carey, 2000; Gelman and Legare, 2011; Spelke, 1990; Spelke and Kinzler, 2007) and that infants understand and recognize enduring relationships between people (Thomsen and Carey, 2013). In her seminal work, Spelke (2022) reviews the evidence for the existence of six distinct “core knowledge” domains: objects, numbers, places, forms, agents, and social beings. Core knowledge systems include innate, abstract knowledge of specific fitness-relevant domains that is ancient, early emerging, invariant across development, and automatically activated, unconscious, and encapsulated. Recent evidence on infants' moral cognition has led some researchers to argue for the existence of core moral knowledge (Buyukozer Dawkins et al., 2019; Surian et al., 2025; Ting et al., 2020), based on the assertion that young infants can represent social goals.

We believe that infants can infer social goals in different actions displaying different degrees of prosociality, such as in basic actions like approaching behaviors, or in more complex actions such as helping behaviors or distributive actions (Hamlin, 2023). Concerning affiliative behaviors, Powell (2021) argued that infants need a concept of social affiliation to use their understanding of rational actions to infer what others value and who they value. This concept is based on the extent to which one individual consistently takes on the goals of another, and it has three features: (a) it can be integrated with existing intuitive psychology based on “naïve utility calculus”, such as an interpersonal utility (Jara-Ettinger et al., 2016) it makes a prediction on infants' expectations of social behaviors and affiliation; and (c) it reflects three domains of social cognition: intuitive psychology, intuitive sociology, and intuitive morality.

The ontogeny of the concept of affiliation is still unknown. Two prominent proposals suggest different origins. First, theoretical nativist views support that young infants possess an innate concept of social affiliation that enables them: (i) to estimate the value that one individual places on a partner's goal, following the theory of utility adoption (Powell, 2022); or (ii) to represent social beings as individuals who exist in systems of relationships to each other and infants themselves, in line with the theoretical framework of core knowledge (i.e., core social being system; see Spelke, 2022). Other researchers argue for the existence of an innate moral core that enables infants to grasp social goals and make evaluations of potential social partners, which would reflect in an early preference for approacher over repellents (Buyukozer Dawkins et al., 2019; Hamlin, 2013; Ting et al., 2020; Woo et al., 2022). Second, theoretical constructivist views postulate that social affiliation is learned through early social interactions with caregivers (Frith and Frith, 2010) and that allostasis is the incentive (Atzil et al., 2018). According to this view, social dyads encourage the acquisition of new behaviors and concepts that are necessary for social affiliation (Atzil et al., 2018). Although some researchers have described affiliative actions as social actions (non-moral), such as speaking or imitating (Liberman et al., 2017; Powell and Spelke, 2013), it seems clear that these actions can be aimed at benefiting another (Dunfield, 2014; Eisenberg, 1986).

Thus, to explore this issue, we focus primarily on recent research examining early preferences for agents performing approaching behavior, which is described as a basic prosocial action. Recent research presented young infants with both affiliative and aggressive actions by investigating their visual or reaching preference based on the social goals (i.e., Geraci et al., 2024; Kanakogi et al., 2013). These findings demonstrated a preference for agents displaying approaching behaviors as compared to aggressive actions, suggesting early emerging inferences of social goals, and a sophisticated evaluation of non-moral forms of prosocial vs. anti-social basic actions. Affiliative behavior can be defined as a basic prosocial action, that is, a non-moral kind of prosocial act aimed at benefiting recipients. The lack of preference for affiliative interactions in infancy could suggest an atypical socio-moral development. Also, it is well-known that specific symptoms in some clinical populations are related to the socio-cognitive impairments (Gliga et al., 2014).

Since there is evidence that preverbal infants positively and negatively evaluate agents who have previously acted prosocially and antisocially, we examine in more depth the relationship between social affiliation and the moral sense, concluding with brief remarks on the importance of identifying early markers for impairments in socio moral functioning.

Infants' preferences for approaching behaviors

Approach is a basic behavior that is prompted by and indicates attraction to a person or object for both adults (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 1993; Chen and Bargh, 1999) and infants (Gergely et al., 1995; Martin et al., 2012; Sommerville and Woodward, 2010; Woodward, 1998). Previous studies on fetuses and newborns revealed early (and maybe not learned) predispositions to detect (and prefer) social stimuli, such as faces (e.g., Reid et al., 2017; Turati et al., 2006; Valenza et al., 1996) and gazes (Farroni et al., 2004, 2006). More recent research on newborns found a visual preference for non-human objects (Orioli et al., 2018) and human figures (Roberti et al., 2024) moving according to trajectories approaching and receding from their body, compared to trajectories directed to the space around their body, suggesting an early predisposition toward social closeness. Intriguingly, these findings suggest an innate mechanism aimed at detecting and preferring agents who perform approaching behaviors.

Another line of research sheds light on how visual and motion cues can trigger infants' detection of social affiliation. It is well-known that synchronized motion by animate characters prompts social affiliation in adults (Hove and Risen, 2009) and infants (Cirelli et al., 2014), and we also know that this is interpreted by infants as a sign of social affiliation (Geraci and Franchin, 2021; Powell and Spelke, 2013). Recent studies demonstrated that infants use similar behaviors and preferences to infer affiliation between others (Liberman et al., 2017, 2021), as well as patterns of imitation (Powell and Spelke, 2017, 2018a,b). A recent study aimed to investigate infants‘ intention-based evaluations of distributive actions, by using both preferential looking and reaching paradigms, found a visual preference not only for fair over unfair distributors but also for more affiliative over less affiliative agents from nine months of age (Geraci et al., 2022a). Importantly, another recent study found an early visual preference for approaching over repulsing actions, as well as a developmental shift between 4 and 8 months of age (Geraci et al., 2022b). Specifically, Geraci et al. (2022b) presented infants with familiarization events displaying a basic interaction involving two agents: one of them (the “repulser”) moved toward the other (the “approacher”), which reacted by simply moving toward the repulser without contacting it. In the test phase, by using both preferential looking and preferential reaching paradigms, they found that 8-month-olds looked longer at the approacher than at the repulser, whereas 4-month-olds showed no preference. Taken together, these findings prompt an early-emerging concept of social affiliation, since preverbal infants between 4 and 8 months of age show a visual preference for affiliative agents in different scenarios.

Social affiliation and moral sense

There are some reasons to consider that affiliative or approaching behaviors are non-moral forms of prosocial actions, which are different from more complex and moral prosocial actions, such as helping behaviors or distributive actions (Hamlin, 2013; Surian et al., 2025; Ting et al., 2020). First, some researchers found early visual preferences for either affiliative or prosocial agents by using the same scenarios, and usually these preferences toward affiliative agents are like those for prosocial agents to the point that no significant difference between conditions emerges (Geraci et al., 2022a,b; Geraci and Surian, 2023; Strid and Meristo, 2020). Certainly, these results reflect a positive evaluation of affiliative behaviors, which is similar to that of agents displaying prosocial behaviors (Hamlin et al., 2007; Hamlin and Wynn, 2011; Surian and Franchin, 2017a,b). A recent study on newborns found that five-day-old newborns attend more to prosocial interactions both for simple, clearly non-moral forms of prosocial vs. anti-social acts (approach vs. avoidance), and for interactions that are more complex and that possess some moral value (helping vs. hindering) (Geraci et al., 2025). Thus, early detection of agents' social goals triggers the evaluation process, suggesting possible coevolution and cooperation between social beings and moral cores (Hamlin, 2023). Taken together, these findings shed light on possible different mechanisms that are involved when infants evaluate different actions: (a) an intention-based attribution for non-moral prosocial acts, and (b) a value-based judgment for moral prosocial acts.

Second, following a developmental perspective, social evaluation seems to emerge earlier than the use of prosocial behaviors for affiliative inferences, in line with prior findings on infants' social evaluations of different prosocial actions as young as 3 months of age (Hamlin et al., 2010; Geraci and Surian, 2023). Prior studies found that younger infants did not expect the recipient of helping behaviors to approach the helper over the hinder (Hamlin et al., 2007; Kuhlmeier et al., 2003). Otherwise, older infants who were presented with scenarios depicting recipients' approaching behaviors toward prosocial or antisocial agents demonstrated their expectations about others' positive social attitudes in different kinds of prosocial interactions, such as helping behaviors (e.g., Hamlin et al., 2007; Kuhlmeier et al., 2003), distributive actions (e.g., Geraci and Surian, 2011; Strid and Meristo, 2020), and defending/interfering behaviors (e.g., Geraci, 2020; Kanakogi et al., 2017).

Third, from a comparative perspective, De Roni et al. (2023) explored newborn chick preferences for affiliative behaviors by using the same stimuli used on human infants (Geraci et al., 2022b). In particular, newborn chicks were presented with events displaying a basic interaction, in which at first Agent A approached Agent B slowly, and then Agent B rejected Agent A, by hitting it. This pattern of actions was repeated three times by displaying the same dynamic cues (i.e., speed and trajectories), showing that Agent A, even after being rejected, expressed the same desire to get close to Agent B again. Certainly, Agent A performed a voluntary positive interaction with Agent B (Eisenberg et al., 2006), while Agent B showed a negative interaction with Agent A (Geraci and Simion, 2022; Kanakogi et al., 2013, 2017). Like infants between 4 and 8 months of age, newborn chicks also showed a preference for the approacher agents, demonstrating an early sensitivity to both social goals and roles. Certainly, the precocity of the concept of social affiliation in both human and non-human social species suggests the existence of an evolutionary root for core social cognition (Spelke, 2022), but also for moral core (for human infants see Hamlin, 2013; Ting et al., 2020; Surian et al., 2025; for non-human species see Brosnan, 2023). Crucially, both newborn chicks and human infants demonstrated the ability to identify and prefer the best social-cooperative partner by inferring agents' social goals and attributing positive or negative valence to their actions. Non-human species' social preferences can hardly be due to the approval of agents for acting as they should (i.e., in terms of morality by considering whether actions are “right” or “wrong”) but might reflect a more basic inference that the approaching agent followed both social and prosocial goals. To choose the best social partner, infants and newborn chicks must distinguish and evaluate two different agents' social goals: approaching (positive disposition) or repulsing (negative disposition) (Kuhlmeier et al., 2003). Thus, we believe that both social and moral cores are involved in social contexts and together cooperate to navigate the social world. Future research could assess pre-term newborns visual preference toward events displaying social and prosocial goals to know early markers of atypical development (Dean et al., 2021; Sansavini et al., 2011), starting from the recent developmental views posing that we are born hardwired to be social and moral.

Early markers of atypical socio-moral development

To our knowledge, the frequency of prosocial behaviors was almost similar between atypical (e.g., Autism Spectrum Disorder, ASD) and neurotypical groups, even if some studies revealed differing patterns, strategies, and moderating factors from autistic children to their comparison groups (for a systematical review regarding for autistic children's prosocial behaviors, see Ryan-Enright et al., 2022). In particular, it seems plausible that the lack of attention toward social events, and a failure to prefer prosocial events over antisocial ones, may indicate not only atypical visual attention to social stimuli, as a potential early marker of social and communicative deficits (as in ASD; Di Giorgio et al., 2016, 2021; Filipe et al., 2018), but also impairments of socio-moral functioning (Calkins and Keane, 2009; Clarkson et al., 2023). Crucially, infant studies demonstrated that decreased preferential tracking of the human face soon after birth is associated with later Callous-Unemotional (CU) traits, as a precursor of psychopathy, supporting the hypothesis that reduced attention to social stimuli (i.e., faces with direct gazes) in infancy can have downstream consequences for the socio-affective development (Bedford et al., 2015; Dadds et al., 2011).

We believe that infants looking reactions toward prosocial and antisocial events could be early markers for impairments in socio-moral functioning (i.e., conduct disorders) or deficits in specific stages of socio-moral information processing (e.g., perception, cognition, and regulation; Blair, 2013; Salekin, 2017). In line, previous developmental studies suggested the possibility for an earlier diagnosis, by indicating two main potential sources of early-onset of antisocial tendency (Tremblay, 2012) neuropsychological deficits, such as poor verbal and executive functions, which emerge in utero or shortly after birth and cause persistent adjustment problems (Moffitt, 1993); family-related risks that prevent very young children from acquiring the skills and resources needed to inhibit antisocial impulses (Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1990). Moreover, some researchers supported that some individuals may be genetically predisposed to exhibit such traits (Glenn, 2019; Mills-Koonce et al., 2015).

In addition, infants' visual or behavioral reactions could reflect early signs of impairments in the development of empathy, which plays a foundational role in guiding appropriate moral behavior and social understanding. Several researchers have demonstrated that empathic responses to distress exhibited during the first year of life predicted subsequent prosocial behavior, greater social competence, and reduced aggression at 18 and 36 months (Davidov et al., 2021; Paz et al., 2021, 2022, 2024). In particular, to understand the behavior of infants later diagnosed with ASD, Paz et al. (2024) considered several features of infants' behavioral responses: a less empathic concern for a distressed other, no evidence of elevated self-distress, at 6 months (but not at later ages) a looking behavior that shifts away more from the distressed other. This result is in line with the findings of Chawarska et al. (2013) who found that infants later diagnosed with ASD looked less at the face of a female actor at 6 months, and with the findings of Jones and Klin (2013), who demonstrated that their attention to an actor's eyes declined from 2 to 6 months. Thus, low empathic abilities evaluated based on different behavioral responses exhibited by children in the early years of life are not exclusive to ASD and may predict other developmental difficulties, including aggression and Callous-Unemotional traits (Paz et al., 2021; Waller et al., 2020). Detecting such signs early enables targeted interventions that can prevent the onset of problematic behaviors and promote empathy, self-regulation, cooperation, and, in general, a well-functioning and adaptive socio-moral development.

Discussion

Understanding the developmental roots of social affiliation is key to uncovering how infants begin to navigate complex social environments. On the other hand, identifying early markers of difficulties in socio-moral functioning is crucial to fostering healthy social and moral development in children. Future research should aim to clarify the interplay between innate and learned components of affiliation, shedding light on how these early capacities shape lifelong social functioning. Finally, this knowledge could contribute to the improvement of early interventions for children at risk of socio-moral impairments.

Author contributions

AG: Writing – original draft, Resources, Investigation, Software, Visualization, Data curation, Validation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing, Project administration, Methodology, Conceptualization, Supervision. EB: Investigation, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft. LF: Investigation, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Visualization, Project administration, Methodology, Validation, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Afshordi, N., and Liberman, Z. (2021). Keeping friends in mind: development of friendship concepts in early childhood. Soc. Dev. 30, 331–342. doi: 10.1111/sode.12493

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Atzil, S., Gao, W., Fradkin, I., and Barrett, L. F. (2018). Growing a social brain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 624–636. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0384-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., and Bian, L. (2016). Psychological reasoning in infancy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 159–186. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedford, R., Pickles, A., Sharp, H., Wright, N., and Hill, J. (2015). Reduced face preference in infancy: a developmental precursor to callous-unemotional traits? Biol. Psychiatry 78, 144–150. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. R. (2013). The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 786–799. doi: 10.1038/nrn3577

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, S. F. (2023). A comparative perspective on the human sense of justice. Evol. Hum. Behav. 44, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.12.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buyukozer Dawkins, M., Sloane, S., and Baillargeon, R. (2019). Do infants in the first year of life expect equal resource allocations? Front. Psychol. 10:116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., and Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: II. arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 65:5. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.1.5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Calkins, S. D., and Keane, S. P. (2009). Developmental origins of early antisocial behavior. Dev. Psychopathol. 21, 1095–1109. doi: 10.1017/S095457940999006X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, S. (2000). The origin of concepts. J. Cogn. Dev. 1, 37–41. doi: 10.1207/S15327647JCD0101N_3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chawarska, K., Macari, S., and Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M., and Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 215–224. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025002007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., and Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 17, 1003–1011. doi: 10.1111/desc.12193

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarkson, E., Jasper, J. D., Rose, J. P., Gaeth, G. J., and Levin, I. P. (2023). Increased levels of autistic traits are associated with atypical moral judgments. Acta Psychol. 235:103895. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2023.103895

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dadds, M. R., Jambrak, J., Pasalich, D., Hawes, D. J., and Brennan, J. (2011). Impaired attention to the eyes of attachment figures and the developmental origins of psychopathy. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 238–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02323.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidov, M., Paz, Y., Roth-Hanania, R., Uzefovsky, F., Orlitsky, T., Mankuta, D., et al. (2021). Caring babies: concern for others in distress during infancy. Dev. Sci. 24:e13016. doi: 10.1111/desc.13016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Roni, P., Geraci, A., Simion, F., and Regolin, L. (2023). Sensitivity to the role of an animated agent from observed interactions in newborn chicks (Gallus gallus). R. Soc. Open Sci. 10:210020. doi: 10.1098/rsos.210020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, B., Ginnell, L., Boardman, J. P., and Fletcher-Watson, S. (2021). Social cognition following preterm birth: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 124, 151–167. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Giorgio, E., Frasnelli, E., Rosa Salva, O., Luisa Scattoni, M., Puopolo, M., Tosoni, D., et al. (2016). Difference in visual social predispositions between newborns at low-and high-risk for autism. Sci. Rep. 6:26395. doi: 10.1038/srep26395

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Giorgio, E., Rosa-Salva, O., Frasnelli, E., Calcagnì, A., Lunghi, M., Scattoni, M. L., et al. (2021). Abnormal visual attention to simple social stimuli in 4-month-old infants at high risk for Autism. Sci. Rep. 11:15785. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-95418-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunfield, K. A. (2014). A construct divided: prosocial behavior as helping, sharing, and comforting subtypes. Front. Psychol. 5:958. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00958

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic Emotion, Cognition, and Behavior. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., and Spinrad, T. L. (2006). “Emotion-related regulation: shaping emotional development and social competence,” in Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3 (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons), 646–718.

Google Scholar

Farroni, T., Massaccesi, S., Pividori, D., and Johnson, M. H. (2004). Gaze following in newborns. Infancy 5, 39–60. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0501_2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farroni, T., Menon, E., and Johnson, M. H. (2006). Factors influencing newborns' preference for faces with eye contact. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 95, 298–308. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.08.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Filipe, M. G., Watson, L., Vicente, S. G., and Frota, S. (2018). Atypical preference for infant-directed speech as an early marker of autism spectrum disorders? A literature review and directions for further research. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 32, 213–231. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2017.1342694

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T. (2018). Social Beings: Core Motives in Social Psychology. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Frith, U., and Frith, C. (2010). The social brain: allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has been. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 365, 165–176. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, S. A., and Legare, C. H. (2011). Concepts and folk theories. Annu Rev. Anthropol. 40, 379–398. doi: 10.1146/annurev-anthro-081309-145822

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A. (2020). How do toddlers evaluate defensive actions toward third parties?. Infancy 25, 910–926. doi: 10.1111/infa.12367

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., Benavides-Varela, S., Nascimben, C., Simion, F., and Di Giorgio, E. (2024). Evaluations of aggressive chasing interactions by 7-month-old infants. Aggress. Behav. 50:e22174. doi: 10.1002/ab.22174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., and Franchin, L. (2021). Do toddlers expect that agents help similar or dissimilar needy agents? Infant Child Dev. 30:e2247. doi: 10.1002/icd.2247

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., Regolin, L., Simion, F., and Surian, L. (2022b). Infants' preferences for approachers over repulsers shift between 4 and 8 months of age. Aggress. Behav. 48, 487–499. doi: 10.1002/ab.22033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., and Simion, F. (2022). Evaluation of prosocial actions performed by dynamic shapes at 17 months of age. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 19, 785–809. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2021.1957823

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., Simion, F., and Surian, L. (2022a). Infants' intention-based evaluations of distributive actions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 220:105429. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., and Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: infants' reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Dev. Sci. 14, 1012–1020. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01048.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., and Surian, L. (2023). Intention-based evaluations of distributive actions by 4-month-olds. Infant Behav. Dev. 70:101797. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2022.101797

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geraci, A., Surian, L., Tina, L. G., and Hamlin, J. K. (2025). Human newborns spontaneously attend to prosocial interactions. Nat. Commun. 16:6304. doi: 10.1038/s41467-025-61517-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., and Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56, 165–193. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-H

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenn, A. L. (2019). Early life predictors of callous-unemotional and psychopathic traits. Infant Ment. Health J. 40, 39–53. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21757

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gliga, T., Jones, E. J., Bedford, R., Charman, T., and Johnson, M. H. (2014). From early markers to neuro-developmental mechanisms of autism. Dev. Rev. 34, 189–207. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottfredson, M. R., and Hirschi, T. (1990).In A General Theory Of Crime. Stanford University Press. doi: 10.1515/9781503621794

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: evidence for an innate moral core. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 186–193. doi: 10.1177/0963721412470687

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamlin, J. K. (2023). Core morality? Or merely core agents and social beings? A response to spelke's what babies know. Mind Lang. 38, 1323–1335. doi: 10.1111/mila.12487

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamlin, J. K., and Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cogn. Dev. 26, 30–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., and Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450, 557–559. doi: 10.1038/nature06288

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., and Bloom, P. (2010). Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Dev. Sci. 13, 923–929. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00951.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hove, M. J., and Risen, J. L. (2009). It's all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 27, 949–960. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jara-Ettinger, J., Gweon, H., Schulz, L. E., and Tenenbaum, J. B. (2016). The naïve utility calculus: computational principles underlying commonsense psychology. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 589–604. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, W., and Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature 504, 427–431. doi: 10.1038/nature12715

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanakogi, Y., Inoue, Y., Matsuda, G., Butler, D., Hiraki, K., and Myowa-Yamakoshi, M. (2017). Preverbal infants affirm third-party interventions that protect victims from aggressors. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0037. doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanakogi, Y., Okumura, Y., Inoue, Y., Kitazaki, M., and Itakura, S. (2013). Rudimentary sympathy in preverbal infants: preference for others in distress. PLoS ONE 8:e65292. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065292

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., and Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychol. Sci. 14, 402–408. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01454

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberman, Z., Kinzler, K. D., and Woodward, A. L. (2021). Origins of homophily: infants expect people with shared preferences to affiliate. Cognition 212:104695. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104695

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., and Kinzler, K. D. (2017). The origins of social categorization. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 556–568. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A., Onishi, K. H., and Vouloumanos, A. (2012). Understanding the abstract role of speech in communication at 12 months. Cognition 123, 50–60. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.12.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills-Koonce, W. R., Wagner, N. J., Willoughby, M. T., Stifter, C., Blair, C., Granger, D. A., and Family Life Project Key, Investigators (2015). Greater fear reactivity and psychophysiological hyperactivity among infants with later conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 56, 147–154. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol. Rev. 100, 674–701.

Google Scholar

Orioli, G., Filippetti, M. L., Gerbino, W., Dragovic, D., and Farroni, T. (2018). Trajectory discrimination and peripersonal space perception in newborns. Infancy 23, 252–267. doi: 10.1111/infa.12207

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paz, Y., Davidov, M., Orlitsky, T., Roth-Hanania, R., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (2022). Developmental trajectories of empathic concern in infancy and their links to social competence in early childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Disciplines 63, 762–770. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13516

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paz, Y., Orlitsky, T., Roth-Hanania, R., Zahn-Waxler, C., and Davidov, M. (2021). Predicting externalizing behavior in toddlerhood from early individual differences in empathy. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 62, 66–74. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13247

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paz, Y., Roth-Hanania, R., Gabis, L. V., Orlitsky, T., Zilka-Cohen, N., Zahn-Waxler, C., et al. (2024). Predicting autism from young Infants' empathic responding: a prospective study. Dev. Psychopathol. 37, 1416–1430. doi: 10.1017/S0954579424001226

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, L., and Spelke, E. (2017). The role of perceptual access in infants' third party evaluations of imitation. PsyAxiv preprint. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/63tyq_v1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, L. J. (2021). Individual differences provide new insight into infant looking. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 925–926. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, L. J. (2022). Adopted utility calculus: origins of a concept of social affiliation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17, 1215–1233. doi: 10.1177/17456916211048487

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, L. J., and Spelke, E. S. (2013). Preverbal infants expect members of social groups to act alike. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, E3965–E3972. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1304326110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, L. J., and Spelke, E. S. (2018a). Human infants' understanding of social imitation: inferences of affiliation from third party observations. Cognition 170, 31–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, L. J., and Spelke, E. S. (2018b). Third-party preferences for imitators in preverbal infants. Open Mind 2, 61–71. doi: 10.1162/opmi_a_00018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, V. M., Dunn, K., Young, R. J., Amu, J., Donovan, T., and Reissland, N. (2017). The human fetus preferentially engages with face-like visual stimuli. Curr. Biol. 27, 1825–1828. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberti, E., Addabbo, M., Colombo, L., Porro, M., and Turati, C. (2024). Newborns' perception of approach and withdrawal from biological movement: a closeness story. Infancy 29, 22–30. doi: 10.1111/infa.12565

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan-Enright, T., O'Connor, R., Bramham, J., and Taylor, L. K. (2022). A systematic review of autistic children's prosocial behaviour. Res. Aut. Spectr. Disord. 98:102023. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2022.102023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salekin, R. T. (2017). Research review: What do we know about psychopathic traits in children?. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 1180–1200. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12738

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., and Caselli, M. C. (2011). Preterm birth: neuropsychological profiles and atypical developmental pathways. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 17, 102–113. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.1105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommerville, J. A., and Woodward, A. L. (2010). “The link between action production and action processing in infancy,” in Naturalizing Intention in Action, eds. F. Grammont, D. Legrand, and P. Livet (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 67–89.

Google Scholar

Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cogn. Sci. 14, 29–56. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spelke, E. S. (2022). What Babies Know: Core Knowledge and Composition, Vol. 1. Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oso/9780190618247.001.0001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spelke, E. S., and Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Dev. Sci. 10, 89–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00569.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strid, K., and Meristo, M. (2020). Infants consider the distributor's intentions in resource allocation. Front. Psychol. 11:596213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.596213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Surian, L., and Franchin, L. (2017a). Infants reason about deserving agents: a test with distributive actions. Cogn. Dev. 44:496. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Surian, L., and Franchin, L. (2017b). Toddlers selectively help fair agents. Front:Psychol. 8:944. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00944

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Surian, L., Parise, E., and Geraci, A. (2025). Core moral concepts and the sense of fairness in human infants. Hum. Nat. 36, 121–142. doi: 10.1007/s12110-025-09490-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomsen, L., and Carey, S. (2013). “Core cognition of social relations,” in Navigating The Social World: What Infants, Children, And Other Species Can Teach Us, eds. M. R. Banaji, and S. A. Gelman (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 17–22. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.003.0004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ting, F., Buyukozwer Dawkins, M., Stavans, M., and Baillargeon, R. (2020). “Principles and concepts in early moral cognition,” in The Social Brain: A Developmental Perspective, ed. J. Decety (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 41–65. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/11970.003.0005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 44, 187–194. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, R. E. (2012). “Development of antisocial behavior during childhood,” in, Handbook Of Life-Course Criminology: Emerging Trends And Directions For Future Research, eds.C. Gibson, and M. Krohn (New York, NY: Springer), 3–19. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5113-6_1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q. Rev. Biol. 46, 35–57. doi: 10.1086/406755

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turati, C., Macchi Cassia, V., Simion, F., and Leo, I. (2006). Newborns' face recognition: role of inner and outer facial features. Child Dev. 77, 297–311. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00871.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M., and Umiltà, C. (1996). Face preference at birth. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 22:892. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.22.4.892

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Waller, R., Wagner, N. J., Barstead, M. G., Subar, A., Petersen, J. L., Hyde, J. S., et al. (2020). A meta-analysis of the associations between callous-unemotional traits and empathy, prosociality, and guilt. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 75:101809. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101809

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woo, B. M., Tan, E., and Hamlin, J. K. (2022). Human morality is based on an early-emerging moral core. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 4, 41–61. doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121020-023312

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodward, H. (1998). Reflective journals and portfolios: learning through assessment. Assess. Eval. High. Educ. 23, 415–423. doi: 10.1080/0260293980230408

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: early markers, typical development, socio-moral development, infancy, social evaluation

Citation: Geraci A, Borzì E and Franchin L (2025) Early markers of atypical socio-moral development. Front. Dev. Psychol. 3:1638327. doi: 10.3389/fdpys.2025.1638327

Received: 30 May 2025; Accepted: 31 July 2025;
Published: 20 August 2025.

Edited by:

Ilaria Grazzani, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Bianca Filippi, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Geraci, Borzı̀ and Franchin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alessandra Geraci, YWxlc3NhbmRyYS5nZXJhY2lAdW5pY3QuaXQ=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.