Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 13 November 2018
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 10 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00342

Cognitive and Brain Activity Changes After Mnemonic Strategy Training in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Evidence From a Randomized Controlled Trial

Sharon S. Simon1* Benjamin M. Hampstead2,3 Mariana P. Nucci4 Fábio L. S. Duran5 Luciana M. Fonseca1 Maria da Graça M. Martin4 Renata Ávila1 Fábio H. G. Porto1 Sônia M. D. Brucki6 Camila B. Martins7 Lyssandra S. Tascone1,5 Edson Amaro Jr.4 Geraldo F. Busatto5 Cássio M. C. Bottino1
  • 1Old Age Research Group (PROTER), Department and Institute of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 2Division of Neuropsychology, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
  • 3VA Ann Arbor Healthcare System, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
  • 4Neuroimagem Funcional – Laboratory of Medical Investigations on Magnetic Resonance Imaging (LIM-44), Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 5Laboratory of Psychiatric Neuroimaging (LIM-21), Department and Institute of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 6Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 7Department of Preventive Medicine, Paulista School of Medicine, Federal University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil

Background: Mnemonic strategy training (MST) has been shown to improve cognitive performance in amnestic mild cognitive impairment (a-MCI), however, several questions remain unresolved. The goal of the present study was to replicate earlier pilot study findings using a randomized controlled design and to evaluate transfer effects and changes in brain activation.

Methods: Thirty patients with a-MCI were randomized into MST or education program. At baseline, participants completed clinical and neuropsychological assessments as well as structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Interventions were administered individually and comprised four sessions, over 2 weeks. MST taught patients to use a three-step process to learn and recall face-name associations. Post-treatment assessment included fMRI, a separate face-name association task, neuropsychological tests, and measures of metamemory. Behavioral (i.e., non-fMRI) measures were repeated after one and 3-months.

Results: Participants in the MST condition showed greater improvement on measures of face-name memory, and increased associative strategy use; effects that were accompanied by increased fMRI activation in the left anterior temporal lobe. While all participants reported greater contentment with their everyday memory following intervention, only the MST group reported significant improvements in their memory abilities. There was no clear indication of far-transfer effects to other neuropsychological tests.

Conclusion: Results demonstrate that patients with a-MCI not only show stimulus specific benefits of MST, but that they appear capable of transferring training to at least some other cognitive tasks. MST also facilitated the use of brain regions that are involved in face processing, episodic and semantic memory, and social cognition, which are consonant with the cognitive processes engaged by training.

Introduction

The world population is aging, which leads to an increase of age-associated conditions such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and related dementias (Jorm and Jolley, 1998; Prince et al., 2013). Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is considered the transitional stage between normal aging and dementia, characterized by cognitive impairment in the absence of impaired daily functioning (Petersen et al., 2001, 2014). The clinical presentation of MCI is classified as amnestic or non-amnestic (Winblad et al., 2004), and those patients with amnestic MCI (a-MCI) are at increased risk of conversion to dementia (Espinosa et al., 2013), especially due to AD (Petersen et al., 2001; Jungwirth et al., 2012). Given the predicted multifold increase in the prevalence rates of dementia worldwide, early identification and treatment of individuals at risk of progressing to dementia is imperative.

There is a growing interest in non-pharmacologic treatments for MCI due to the limited benefits of existing pharmacologic agents (McGhee et al., 2016), and the recognition that cognitive interventions can promote brain plasticity and reduce cognitive impairment (Belleville et al., 2011). Also, older adults who are more engaged in cognitively stimulating activities present reduced risk of cognitive deterioration and dementia (Verghese et al., 2003; Barnes and Yaffe, 2011; Marioni et al., 2012), indicating that is it critical to develop effective cognitive interventions for this population.

Decline in episodic memory is one of the hallmark features of AD (Sperling et al., 2010) and is present in a-MCI, which also show deficits in associative memory (Oedekoven et al., 2015) and strategy use (Ramakers et al., 2010). Mnemonic strategies can be broadly conceptualized as cognitive methods that facilitate the organization and association of new information, thereby enhancing depth of processing (Hampstead et al., 2014). Previous literature suggests that mnemonic strategy training (MST) can be effective for patients with MCI, leading to improvements in different cognitive domains (for reviews Simon et al., 2012; Huckans et al., 2013; Hampstead et al., 2014; Chandler et al., 2016; Mewborn et al., 2017). It is worth mentioning that training gains in older adults are more likely to transfer to similar measures in the same trained cognitive domain or construct (“near-transfer effect”), than to operations in non-trained cognitive abilities or domains (“far-transfer effect”) (Mewborn et al., 2017).

Besides transfer effects to objective cognitive measures, training studies have investigated the effects on subjective aspects of memory, such as metamemory, which includes knowledge, perception, and beliefs about one’s own memory and memory functioning (Flavell and Wellman, 1977; Hertzog et al., 1990). Some studies have reported metamemory gains after cognitive training in MCI patients, including greater satisfaction with everyday memory (Bahar-Fuchs et al., 2017), greater self-report memory ability (Rapp et al., 2002; Belleville et al., 2006), and greater self-report strategy use (Rapp et al., 2002; Troyer et al., 2008; Kinsella et al., 2009; Jean et al., 2010; Bahar-Fuchs et al., 2017). The relation between cognitive training-related gains and metamemory is still unclear and deserves further investigation. We hypothesize that memory training would lead to a more positive feelings about memory abilities, and more knowledge about memory functioning.

Several neuroimaging studies have revealed that MST (re)engages brain regions associated with executive functions and memory, with evidence of increased task-related activity in brain areas not recruited before training in those with MCI (Belleville et al., 2011; Hampstead et al., 2011; Belleville and Bherer, 2012; Hosseini et al., 2014; Balardin et al., 2015). For instance, Belleville et al. (2011) found training-related increase of activation in the right inferior parietal lobule after training several memory techniques. Balardin et al. (2015) found the increase of activation in the ventromedial prefrontal and right superior frontal gyrus after teaching a specific semantic strategy. Regarding MST focused on face-name association, Hampstead et al. (2011) reported an increase of activation in a widespread cerebral network involving medial frontal, parietal, and occipital regions, and the left lateral temporal cortex. Likewise, Hampstead et al. (2012b) demonstrated a partially restorative effect of MST on hippocampal activation in those with MCI. Altogether, these findings provide further evidence of neuroplasticity in those with MCI and suggest that MST enhances memory through a mixture of restorative and compensatory mechanisms.

Everyday memory deficits in MCI typically involves difficulties in recall recent information, such as appointments, duties, localization of personal belongings and names of people (Irish et al., 2011; Yanhong et al., 2013). A promising avenue of research is the MST focused on face-name association, since forgetting names is common and has direct social implications (James et al., 2008; Weaver Cargin et al., 2008). Also, memorizing faces has been considered cognitively challenging due to the uniqueness and arbitrariness of the faces (Werheid and Clare, 2007). Neuroimaging findings suggest that face-name binding implicates associative occipito-temporal cerebral regions with extensive connections to other cortical and subcortical brain regions critical to episodic memory (Sperling et al., 2003; Petrella et al., 2006; Chua et al., 2007). These same regions are known to be affected in MCI (Whitwell et al., 2007). Face-name associative tasks have demonstrated sensitivity to memory impairment related to MCI-AD spectrum (Clare et al., 2002; Parra et al., 2010; Polcher et al., 2017), and deficits in such tasks have been associated to Aβ burden in clinically healthy older adults (Rentz et al., 2011). Thus, training those with MCI to enhance memory for faces and names may yield positive everyday effects (i.e., ecological relevance). While face-name associations have been targeted by many studies, most have used a mixture of techniques (Clare et al., 2009; Jean et al., 2010; Belleville et al., 2011) or are uncontrolled studies (Hampstead et al., 2008), which limit knowledge about whether MST is effective. In addition, although there is some evidence of increase of brain activation in a widespread cerebral network after face-name association training (Hampstead et al., 2011), additional research is necessary to better understand the neural mechanisms underlying intervention effects. Moreover, the existing studies have been limited to North American or Northern European samples but there may be important cultural differences that render MST less appropriate.

In the present study, we detail the cognitive and neuroimaging results of a randomized, controlled, single-blind study in which patients with a-MCI were assigned to either MST (focused on face-name association) or an active control intervention (education program – EP). Our primary aim was to examine the efficacy and persistence of MST for face-name associations in a Brazilian cohort, by examining near-transfer effects to face-name tasks. Secondary aims included: (1) to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms associated with MST near-transfer effects using fMRI; and (2) to investigate far-transfer effects on neuropsychological tests and metamemory. Based on the literature, we hypothesized near-transfer effects that would be accompanied by increase of brain activation and metamemory gains, whereas cognitive far-transfer effects were considered less likely. To enhance near-transfer, our MST program incorporated a novel and ecologically vital step where patients applied the methods using real individuals from their daily lives.

Materials and Methods

Experimental Design and Sample Size Calculation

Subject flow is shown in Figure 1 according to the CONSORT diagram (Schulz et al., 2010). Participants were randomly assigned by an independent researcher to one of the two intervention programs (MST or EP). The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical School of University of São Paulo and registered in ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT01978353). All participants were volunteers and provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. During the consent process, subjects were informed that the study examined the efficacy of various programs for memory; however, they were given information about their specific treatment only after randomization (i.e., participants remained blinded to the other condition). To assess immediate training effects, participants were evaluated approximately 1-week after finishing the programs (average 5.6 days), and maintenance of the effects was evaluated in follow-up assessments at 1-month and 3-months.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. CONSORT flowchart for selection of study participants. ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid; ED, education program; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; MST, mnemonic strategy training.

The sample size was calculated a priori with G-Power software V. 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007). We found a large effect size (ηp2 = 0.89) in our previous uncontrolled pilot study of MST for face-name associations (Hampstead et al., 2008) and used it to power the current study, focusing on the immediate post-intervention timepoint since our control condition (i.e., EP) lacked the “active” ingredient of MST. The resulting two-group analysis revealed a sample of eight participants was needed to detect an effect with 0.95 power at an alpha level of 0.05. Since this seemed rather optimistic, we then conducted a sensitivity analysis that demonstrated a total sample of 30 would be sensitive to detect a Cohen’s d = 0.3, at 0.8 power and an alpha of 0.05.

Participants

Thirty patients with a-MCI were included in this study and were recruited from community announcements or referred by the Old Age Research Group at the Institute of Psychiatry, University of São Paulo.

Before admission in the study, participants underwent clinical and neuropsychological evaluations and a MRI examination. Inclusion criteria were: (1) 60 years of age or older, (2) four or more years of education, (3) right handedness, (4) Portuguese as a native language, (5) normal or corrected vision and hearing, and (6) diagnosis of a-MCI, according to Petersen’s criteria (Petersen et al., 2009). MCI diagnosis was operationalized as follows: (1) presence of subjective memory complaint corroborated by an informant; (2) presence of memory impairment (we considered at least 1.5 standard deviation (SD) below the age norms on one memory test or 1.0 SD below norms on more than one memory test); (3) normal general cognitive function; (4) essentially normal functional of daily living (e) absence of dementia. Exclusion criteria included (1) history of severe neurologic or psychiatry disorder, (2) history of alcohol or drug abuse; (3) and MRI contraindication. Medications were carefully evaluated to exclude those who were using agents that affect cognition; however, antidepressants were allowed as long as the patient had been on a stable dose for at least 6 months without significant mood symptoms. None of the patients were using cholinesterase inhibitors or memantine. Moreover, we also incorporated an established biomarker measurement (i.e., brain volumes) at baseline for investigation of structural inter-group differences, and as a methodological caution, since brain atrophy may affect the BOLD signal in MCI-AD (Johnson et al., 2000; Dickerson et al., 2005; Dickerson and Sperling, 2008).

Clinical and Neuropsychological Evaluation

The clinical evaluation was conducted by a geriatric psychiatrist and took place in the presence of the patient and an informant, with the exception of four cases that the informant was contacted by telephone. Participants were assessed with the structured clinical interview of the Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of Older People (CAMDEX) (Roth et al., 1986; Bottino et al., 1999), Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAMA) (Hamilton, 1959), Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) (Montgomery and Asberg, 1979; Dratcu et al., 1987), Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE) (Jorm and Jacomb, 1989; Perroco et al., 2009) and the Bayer Activities of Daily Living Scale (B-ADL) (Hindmarch et al., 1998; Folquitto et al., 2007). In addition, to assess participant’s handedness, we applied the Annett’s Hand Preference Questionnaire (Annett, 1970).

The neuropsychological assessment was performed by a neuropsychologist and included: Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005; Memória et al., 2013), Vocabulary, Matrix Reasoning and Digit Span from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale third edition (WAIS-III) (Wechsler, 1981), Short Cognitive Performance Test (SKT) (Erzigkeit, 2001; Flaks et al., 2009), Stroop Test (Spreen and Strauss, 1998), Phonemic Fluency (letters FAS), Semantic Fluency (animal category) (Benton et al., 1983), Boston Naming Test (BNT) (Kaplan et al., 2001; Miotto et al., 2010), Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF) (Rey, 1999; Oliveira et al., 2004), Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised (HVLT-R) (Brandt, 1991), Logical Memory and Faces from the Wechsler Memory Scale third edition (WMS-III) (Wechsler, 1997; Nascimento, 2005). An estimate Intellectual Quotient (IQ) measure was calculated from Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning’s scores (Ringe et al., 2002). Also, measures of metamemory were evaluated through the Multifactorial Memory Questionnaire (MMQ) (Troyer and Rich, 2002; Simon et al., 2016) and self-report mood symptoms through Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck et al., 1988a,b; Gorenstein et al., 1999).

Intervention Programs

Two interventions were administered to the participants: MST or EP. Each program was administered individually, with participants attending four 1-h sessions, twice a week, over a 2-week period. All information was presented using a 10-inch tablet. The programs were delivered by neuropsychologists following a step-by-step manual.

Mnemonic Strategy Training

Mnemonic strategy training was based on the Ecologically Oriented Neurorehabilitation of Memory (EON-MEM) program (Stringer, 2007). In total, participants were trained in 36 face-name pairs, divided across three training sessions (12 pairs in each session), and in the last session, the participants reviewed all content. Procedures otherwise followed the methods detailed in Hampstead et al. (2008). Briefly, for each face-name pair, patients should follow the “feature-reason-image” process: they were directed to a salient facial feature (visual cue), given a “nickname” linking the facial feature to the name (reason or verbal cue), and then instructed to create a mental image that integrated the feature and reason in a detailed and potentially exaggerated image (e.g., enhancing the salient facial feature so it better stood out). The reasons/verbal cues were phonologically similar or rhymed with the actual name. On each training trial, participants were required to first recall the feature, then the reason, and finally the corresponding name. To reinforce this series of steps, participants were required to recall these steps in order on up to ten training trials per stimulus. A stimulus was removed from circulation if participants recalled this information on three consecutive trials. Once all these initial trials had been completed for each stimulus, all 12 pairs trained during the session were reviewed using the same step-by-step process (same day review). The next training session began with the review of all 12 pairs from the previous session in a different order (delayed review), and then a new set of 12 pairs was trained. The final training session presented all 36 pairs in random order and the participants had to recall the step-by-step process (“feature-reason-image”) across each of three trials.

At the end of each session, patients completed an ecological “generalization step” in order to enhance comprehension and transfer the MST methodology. Participants were required to apply the methodology to real people in their everyday life (e.g., somebody whose name they had trouble recalling or had forgotten at least once). First, they were asked to imagine the face of the person in details, describe it out loud, and then apply the methodology with the therapist – thereby allowing them to create their own associations. In each session, one real-life example was trained and participants were actively encouraged to use the associative methodology in their daily routines.

Education Program

In the EP, the therapist presented and discussed relevant topics, such as: healthy aging, memory functioning, aspects that can interfere in memory (e.g., depression, sleep, and stress), risks and protective factors to dementia, such as healthy lifestyle (e.g., diet, physical exercise); and information regarding MCI and Alzheimer’s disease. Review periods were also integrated at the same time points as in the MST group during which the main points were reinforced. Participants also underwent a content review during the last session. Therapists were not allowed to provide any mnemonic strategy to the participants in this group. Importantly, these materials were designed to result in a session length comparable to the MST program.

Outcome Measures: fMRI and Cognition

fMRI Task

Brain activation related to memory encoding was measured using a block design fMRI paradigm. The paradigm was adapted from elsewhere (Hampstead et al., 2011) and comprised alternating 20 s rest, and 24 s active blocks. There were three types of active blocks, including face-name pairs from (1) the trained list, (2) an untrained (i.e., novel) list, and (3) the two control stimuli repeated in alternation. During active blocks, four face-name pairs were shown for 5 s each, with 1 s interstimulus interval. Each block type occurred three times during each of the three functional runs. In total, 72 pairs were displayed once, 36 from each trained and untrained lists; and the control stimuli were seen a total of 36 times. Patients were instructed to press a button as soon as they saw a face-name pair to ensure that they were attending to the stimuli. They were instructed to memorize the name paired with each face. To avoid order bias, block order was different in each run, and run order was counterbalanced across sessions. Participants were familiarized with the task before each session (using the repeated stimuli) with reminder instructions provided prior to each run. The same procedure was performed at pre and post-intervention fMRI sessions, but outside of the scanner at the 1- and 3-month follow-up.

Stimuli presented in the fMRI task were taken from Hampstead et al. (2011) and included a total of 74 faces used in an earlier study (Kirwan and Stark, 2004), transformed to grayscale images, and randomly paired with gender-appropriate names in Portuguese (5–7 letters). As mentioned above, 72 face-name pairs were divided into two lists, which were matched for gender, race, approximate age, and emotional valence (positive, negative, and neutral), and the remaining two faces (one of each gender) served as control stimuli. Stimuli presentation and response recording were performed with E-Prime 2.0 software.

Near-Transfer Measures

Face-name recognition task

In order to evaluate near-transfer effects (e.g., cognitive operations related to face-name associations), participants completed the Face-Name Recognition Task (FNRT) 30 min after the fMRI paradigm. The task included all 74 stimuli and involved a four-alternative recognition format (Hampstead et al., 2008). These four choices were (1) the target name, (2) a sex-appropriate name from trained list, (3) a sex-appropriate name from the untrained list, and (4) a sex-appropriate novel name. Since the focus of the current work is transfer effects, three measures were selected from the FNRT focusing in the untrained stimuli: (1) accuracy (% correct), (2) confidence ratings for each response using an anchored 4-point scale (1 = not confident at all, 4 = extremely confident), and (3) reaction time (RT). For the follow-up assessments, the procedures were the same, except for the task being presented outside the MRI scan.

Strategy use task

The frequency of strategy use was assessed though the Strategy Use Task (SUT). This experimental task required the participants to memorize 12 new face-name pairs presented for 15 s each. The participants were only told to remember the name that matched each face; they were not explicitly instructed to use any strategies. After a 20-min delay, the faces were presented one by one and participants were asked to state the appropriate name (cued recall). Participants then performed a three-choice recognition task (options included the target name, a name from a different pair within this same test, and a novel name). After each response, participants were asked “how” they remembered the name. If their responses include any associative link between face and name, one point was provided. Parallel versions were developed and used for each assessment period in this task.

Far-Transfer Measures

Far-transfer effects were assessed through tasks involving episodic memory (not related to face-name material), attention, and metamemory. These constructs were not specifically aimed in the MST, therefore were considered as far transfer measures. The instruments are listed below:

Hopkins verbal learning test - revised

The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test - Revised (HVLT-R) is a widely used word list test involving verbal learning and memory, which includes 12 words from three semantic categories. We analyzed the immediate and delayed recall measures. Parallel versions were used in each study assessment (Brandt, 1991).

Short cognitive test

The Short Cognitive Test (SKT) is a brief neuropsychological battery involving attention and visual episodic memory. We calculated a composite score of attention based on six attention sub-items, and computed the memory scores for immediate and delayed recalls. In each recall task the participant had to remember 12 color drawings of everyday objects. We computed the number of omissions of each recall. Parallel versions were used in the assessments (Erzigkeit, 2001; Flaks et al., 2009).

Multifactorial memory questionnaire

Metamemory was evaluated through the self-report Multifactorial Metamemory Questionnaire (MMQ). The MMQ includes the Contentment scale, assessing participant’s emotions linked to memory performance; Ability scale, to evaluate memory mistakes in everyday life, and Strategy scale, which assess the frequency of using several memory strategies (Troyer and Rich, 2002; Simon et al., 2016).

Neuroimaging Data Acquisition

Magnetic Resonance Imaging

MRI scanning was performed on a 3T Philips Achieva machine MR system. Image acquisition parameters were based on BOLD (Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent) effect: echo planar sequence (EPI GRE) encompassing whole brain (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle 90°, three dummy scans, 40 slices axial oblique AC-PÇ oriented with 3 mm isotropic voxels). Three runs were acquired in the fMRI experiment, each with 199 volumes. A structural 3D T1-weighted scan was acquired immediately after the fMRI acquisition for volumetry brain morphometry (VBM), activation map-coregistration, and normalization procedures described ahead (TR = 7 ms, TE = 3.2 ms, flip angle 8°, 180 slices, 1 mm isotropic voxels). An Axial Fluid-Attenuated Inversion Recovery (FLAIR) scan and susceptibility-weighted (Principles of Echo-Shifting with a Train of Observations – PRESTO) acquisitions – together with T1 acquisition – were used to identify brain alterations. All images followed a quality control protocol and were inspected by a neuroradiologist.

Data Analysis

Functional Neuroimaging

Data processing and statistical analyses were conducted using FMRIB Software Library (FSL) version 6.0 (Smith et al., 2004). Functional volumes were processed by motion correction (MCFLIRT), slice-timing correction using Fourier-space time-series phase-shifting, non-brain removal using BET (Smith, 2002), spatial smoothing (FWHM = 5 mm) and highpass temporal filtering (sigma = 50.0 s), to remove signal drift and low-frequency noise. Time-series statistical analysis was carried out using the general linear model (FILM) with local autocorrelation correction (Woolrich et al., 2004). The functional images were then registered with high resolution structural and standard space images using FLIRT (Jenkinson and Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002). For higher-level analysis of multiple sessions (mean of three runs and time effect) we used a fixed effect model, and for multiple subjects (group mean, group comparison and interaction – ANOVA) the FLAME (FMRIB’S Local Analysis of Mixed Effects) was used (Beckmann et al., 2003; Woolrich et al., 2004; Woolrich, 2008). Z statistic images (Gaussianized T/F) were thresholded using clusters determined by Z-score > 2.3 and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p < 0.05 (Worsley, 2003).

Activation maps were generated for two contrasts. First, to investigate brain activation related to the task, we combined the trained and untrained stimuli from the pre-training scan into a single group of ‘novel’ stimuli [we called ‘novel’ since interventions had yet to take place (Hampstead et al., 2011)]. Activation in response to these stimuli was compared to that for the repeated face-name pairs in the contrast novel > repeated. Second, we used a repeated measure ANOVA, considering factors of time (baseline, 1-week) and group (MST, ED), to identify training effects related to the untrained stimuli, in the contrast: {POST [untrained stimuli > repeated stimuli] > PRE [untrained stimuli > repeated stimuli]} to assess increase of activation after the interventions; and {PRE [untrained stimuli > repeated stimuli] > POST [untrained stimuli > repeated stimuli]} to investigate decrease of activation after the programs. In addition, the percent of BOLD signal change was calculated in the brain areas that emerged in the interactions.

Structural Neuroimaging

Brain atrophy, especially affecting medial temporal structures, is commonly observed in a-MCI (Whitwell et al., 2007; Soldan et al., 2015), with evidence that hippocampus and amygdala present the largest annual decline (Fjell et al., 2009). This pattern of brain atrophy may affect the BOLD signal in MCI-AD spectrum (Johnson et al., 2000; Dickerson et al., 2005; Dickerson and Sperling, 2008), thus volumetric differences between the groups could affect data analysis and interpretation of the fMRI results, particularly due to atrophy affecting the hippocampus and amygdala. Therefore, we examined volumetric measurements for all patients with voxel-based morphometry (VBM) using the Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM8; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, United Kingdom) (Ashburner, 2007) (see Supplementary Material for further information). Based on the known patterns of cortical atrophy in a-MCI, we analyzed total brain volumes (gray and white matter) and regional volumes of the hippocampus and amygdala, predicted a priori to show brain atrophy based on previous research on MCI (Hampstead et al., 2012a). We then compared (one-way ANOVA) these values to those of an equally sized control sample recruited in the same neuroimaging laboratory in order to demonstrate differences in AD related brain regions.

Statistical Analysis

At baseline, comparisons between groups were assessed. It is worth mentioning that between-groups comparisons of brain volumes were treated with the same statistics used for the behavioral data. For categorical variables we used chi-squared tests; and for continuous variables we used the Independent T-Test. In order to compare the groups between the different sessions we used repeated measure ANOVA, considering factors of time (baseline, 1-week, 1-month, and 3-months) and group (MST, ED). More specifically, in order to evaluate training-related changes at post-training and maintenance at follow-up, we compared the baseline with each time point of the study. The significance was set at p ≤ 0.05. In order to verify the effect-sizes, Cohen’s d was calculated. Statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 17.

Results

Brain Volume, Clinical, and Cognitive Characteristics at Baseline

Table 1 presents the demographical, clinical, and neuropsychological characteristics of the participants, with no significant differences between groups except for the immediate recall of SKT-Memory [t(28) = 2.16, p = 0.04].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic, clinical, and neuropsychological characteristics of the sample.

Regarding the brain volume data, the one-way ANOVA revealed that the MCI sample presented significant gray matter volume decrements relative to control sample in regions typically affected by Alzheimer’s pathology, including the bilateral hippocampus [F(1,58) = 14.2, p < 0.001], and amygdala [F(1,58) = 68.9, p < 0.001] (Table 2). Importantly, there were no differences in these volumes, and total brain volume, between the a-MCI treatment groups (Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Volumetric measures in the MCI study sample and in a control sample.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. MRI Volumetry (ml) for each intervention group.

Trained-Relates Changes

Near-Transfer Effects

Untrained stimuli (FNRT)

Regarding accuracy, after 1-week there was no main effect of time [F(1,28) = 2.41, p = 0.13], or group [F(1,28) = 0.05, p = 0.81]; however, we observed a significant time-by-group interaction [F(1,28) = 4.04, p = 0.05; d = 0.35) (Figure 2A). This interaction was driven by accuracy improvement for the MST [t(14) = −2.27, p = 0.03] but not the ED group [t(14) = 0.36, p = 0.71]. This pattern was more evident at follow-up evaluations, when we observed significant time-by-group interactions (1-month: [F(1,28) = 6.38, p = 0.01]; and 3-months: [F(1,27) = 8.38, p = 0.007]), and greater effect-sizes (1-month: d = 0.64; 3-month: d = 0.75), suggesting that near transfer-effects persisted at follow-up. Analysis of RT revealed a main effect of time [F(1,28) = 8.83, p = 0.006] and time-by-group interaction, indicating a slower performance in the MST group at post-intervention [F(1,28) = 8.41, p = 0.007; d = 0.79] (Figure 2B). However, no interactions were observed at 1-month [F(1,28) = 3.06, p = 0.09; d = 0.17] or 3-months follow-up [F(1,27) = 1.38, p = 0.25; d = 0.27].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Performance in the FNRT. (A) Accuracy; (B) confidence level of response; (C) reaction time. Represents p < 0.05 indicating a time-by-group interaction. Error bars represent standard error mean. FNRT, Face-name recognition task; MST, memory strategy training.

For confidence (Figure 2C), there was a main effect of time at 1-week [F(1,28) = 6.87, p = 0.01] but no effect of group [F(1,28) = 0.27, p = 0.60] or time-by-group interaction [F(1,28) = 0.21, p = 0.88; d = 0.21]. Instead, an interaction emerged at 1-month [F(1,28) = 4.34, p = 0.04; d = 0.89], indicating greater confidence in the MST group; which dissipated at 3-months follow-up [F(1,27) = 1.26, p = 0.27; d = 0.49].

Frequency of strategy use (SUT)

The frequency of strategy use for new face-name pairs was measured in the context of free recall and recognition. One-week after the programs, there was a time-by-group interaction for the recognition task [F(1,28) = 18.53, p < 0.001; d = 1.33] (Figure 3A), but not for the cued recall [F(1,28) = 2.63, p = 0.11; d = 0.17] (Figure 3B). The interaction for the recognition task was driven by the fact that only the MST group showed significant improvement after the intervention [t(14) = −5.85, p < 0.001], whereas the control group did not [t(14) = −1.58, p = 0.13]. Moreover, at 1-month follow-up, we observed an interaction for both free recall [F(1,28) = 7.58, p = 0.01; d = 0.67] and recognition [F(1,28) = 11.51, p = 0.002; d = 1.33], however, it dissipated after 3-months [cued recall: F(1,27) = 2.19, p = 0.15; d = 0.20; recognition: F(1,17) = 3.46, p = 0.07; d = 0.58].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Frequency of strategy use. (A) SUT – Free recall; (B) SUT – Recognition Task. MST, mnemonic strategy training; SUT, strategy use task. Represents; p < 0.05 indicating time-group interactions. Error bars represent standard error mean.

Far-Transfer Effects

Neuropsychological tests (HVLT-R and SKT)

Regarding HVLT-R immediate recall, at 1-week there was no main effect of time [F(1,28) = 0.26, p = 0.61], group [F(1,28) = 0.18, p = 0.66], or interaction [F(1,28) = 0.10, p = 0.74; d = 0.18] at any time point (1-month: [F(1,28) = 0.45, p = 0.50; d = 0.13]; 3-month: [F(1,27) = 0.52, p = 0.47; d = 0.29]) (Figure 4A). For the HVLT-R delayed recall, at post-intervention we observed a main effect of time [F(1,28) = 5.70, p = 0.02], but no effect of group [F(1,28) = 1.31, p = 0.26] or time-by-group interaction [F(1,28) < 0.03, p = 0.95; d = 0.31] at any follow-up assessment [1-month: F(1,28) = 0.23, p = 0.63; d = 0.69; 3-months: F(1,27) = 0.01, p = 0.91; d = 0.40] (Figure 4B).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Cognitive performance in HVLT-R. (A) Immediate recall; (B) delayed recall. HVLT-R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised; MST, mnemonic strategy training. Error bars represent standard error mean.

For the SKT-Attention score, there was no effect of time [F(1,28) = 2.14, p = 0.15], group [F(1,28) = 0.23, p = 0.63], or group-by-time interaction [F(1,28) > 1; p = 0.93; d = 0.46] (Figure 5A). Although we did not observe a main effect of group in the SKT-Memory immediate recall [F(1,28) > 1; p = 0.93], there was a main effect of time [F(1,28) = 7.74; p = 0.01] and group-by-time interaction [F(1,28) = 4.23; p = 0.04; d = 0.08], indicating a slightly better performance (fewer omissions) in the MST group at post-intervention (Figure 5B). Moreover, in the SKT-Memory delayed recall, there was no effect of time [F(1,28) = 1.82; p = 0.18] or group [F(1,28) = 1.96; p = 0.17] though there was a trend for the interaction [F(1,28) = 3.58; p = 0.06; d = 0.9] (Figure 5C). It is worth mentioning that SKT was not re-assessed at follow-up due to a limited number of test versions.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Cognitive performance in SKT. (A) SKT-Attention score; (B) SKT-Memory immediate recall; (C) SKT-Memory delayed recall. MST, mnemonic strategy training; SKT, Short Cognitive Test. Represents p < 0.05 indicating a time-by-group interaction. Error bars represent standard error mean.

Metamemory questionnaire (MMQ)

In the MMQ-Feelings post-training subscale, an effect of time [F(1,28) = 12.43, p > 0.001] suggested that both groups reported greater contentment about their own memory after the programs (Figure 6A). However, there was no main effect of group [F(1,28) = 0.01, p = 0.91] or interaction [F(1,28) = 0.15, p = 0.69; d = 0.08], including at the follow-up assessments [1-month: F(1,28) = 0.37, p = 0.54; d = 0.02; 3-months: F(1,27) = 1.46, p = 0.23; d = 0.03]. For MMQ-Ability, although there was no main effect of group at post-training [F(1,28) = 0.16, p = 0.90]; we observed an effect of time [F(1,28) = 6.47, p = 0.01] and time-by-group interaction [F(1,28) = 7.16, p = 0.01; d = 0.23], indicating that only the MST group reported greater memory ability in everyday life after the programs; however, this did not persist at follow-up evaluations [1-month: F(1,28) = 0.65, p = 0.42; d = 0.05; 3-months: F(1,27) = 1.09, p = 0.30; d = 0.05] (Figure 6B). In the MMQ-Strategy, there was no main effect of time [F(1,28) = 0.28, p = 0.60], group [F(1,28) = 0.68, p = 0.41], nor time-by-group interaction [F(1,28) = 0.96, p = 0.33; d = 0.41] either at post-treatment or at follow-up [1-month: F(1,28) = 0.75, p = 0.39; d = 0.17; 3-months: F(1,27) = 0.18, p = 0.67; d = 0.62] (Figure 6C).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Performance in metamemory questionnaire. (A) MMQ-Contentment scale; (B) MMQ-Ability scale; (C) MMQ-Strategy scale. MMQ, Multifactorial Memory Questionnaire; MST, mnemonic strategy training. Represents p < 0.05 indicating a time-by-group interaction. Error bars represent standard error mean.

Neuroimaging Data

Brain Activation Related to the fMRI Task

At pre-training, increase of activation in several brain foci was evident when subjects were encoding novel relative to repeated face-name stimuli, including widespread bilateral regions within the occipital and parietal cortices, the cerebellar vermis, intraparietal sulcus and posterior portions of the fusiform gyrus (consistent with the fusiform face area, e.g., Solomon-Harris et al., 2016). Regarding the frontal cortex, we observed the activation in bilateral regions such as inferior frontal and precentral gyri; in left areas, such as middle frontal, orbital gyrus, and anterior insula; and in medial structures, including the anterior cingulate gyrus and medial frontal gyrus. Finally, increased activity was also found in the bilateral hippocampal formation, amygdala and putamen (Figure 7 and Table 4A).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 7. Brain activation related to the fMRI task (prior to intervention). 3D maps: mean of activation in the contrast Novel stimuli > Repeated stimuli. LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; n = 30; Z-score: >2.30; p < 0.05.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Statistical information of significant clusters highlighted prior to intervention.

In order to investigate group differences in brain activation related to encoding of stimuli, the groups were compared in the same contrast (novel > repeated stimuli). We found that the MST group showed greater activation only in the right cuneus, part of the primary visual cortex (Table 4B).

Brain Activation Related to the Training

Brain activation related to the training effect was investigated through the repeated measure ANOVA. Our findings revealed that after the interventions the MST group showed a greater increase of activation than EP in multiple regions of the left anterior temporal lobe. The peak of activation occurred in the left superior temporal sulcus (STS) (Brodmann area 38) with signal change extending to the middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and superior temporal gyrus (STG) (Table 5 and Figure 8A). In addition, BOLD signal in the STS increased in the MST group, but declined in the EP group (Figure 8B).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Statistical information of significant clusters highlighted in the ANOVAs.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 8. Changes in task-related brain activation and % of BOLD signal. (A) 3D maps: mean of activation revealed by the ANOVAs. Increase of brain activation (fMRI task) related to the untrained stimuli, in the contrast Post [Untrained stimuli > Repeated stimuli] > Pre [Untrained stimuli > Repeated stimuli]. LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; n = 30; Z-score: >2.30; p < 0.05; (B) graph: % of BOLD signal at pre and post intervention in the superior temporal sulcus (region of peak of activation), EP, education program; MST, mnemonic strategy training.

Discussion

In this randomized, controlled, single-blind study we report the efficacy of MST focused on face-name stimuli in patients with a-MCI. Our experimental design allowed us to observe that learning mnemonic strategies were more beneficial to memory performance than learning non-specific contents in an active control group (EP). We found that only after MST there was a significant improvement in memory performance for untrained face-name pairs, as well as increase of strategy use when recalling new face-name pairs, which persisted at follow-up. Interestingly, only participants in the MST group reported an increase of self-rated memory ability at post-intervention. Despite such findings, we did not observe consistent changes in neuropsychological measures of attention or episodic memory (SKT and HVLT-R), except for the SKT immediate recall.

Our neuroimaging data corroborate the behavioral findings, showing that only after MST there was an increase of task-related brain activation in brain regions known to be implicated in several of the cognitive processes trained. It is relevant to highlight that our findings were not confounded by between-group differences in terms of brain atrophy despite the presence of significantly reduced volume in key MTL regions. Such findings underscore our conclusion that MST may be efficacious in a-MCI due to AD.

Near-Transfer Effects

Our findings are highly suggestive of near-transfer effect after cognitive training, which is in line with several studies with healthy older adults and MCI patients (for a recent review, see Mewborn et al., 2017; and a recent work Belleville et al., 2018). We found that 1-week after the programs, the MST group presented a greater improvement in accuracy for untrained stimuli, which were maintained at follow-up evaluations. Curiously, the MST group’s confidence increased as they were further removed from training. This likely reflects (1) an initial non-specific benefit of EP that rapidly dissipated at follow up and (2) providing patients with “tools” (i.e., MST) to overcome an area of weakness has long-term benefit. The observed differences in accuracy (MST better than EP) and reaction time (MST slower than EP) further support this idea. While slowed reaction time may initially seem counterintuitive, it fits with the inherently slower and more effortful nature of using mnemonic strategies (see Hampstead et al., 2014 for discussion).

Moreover, the data regarding the frequency of strategy use (SUT) support the hypothesis that MST group transferred the strategy use to a new context, and maintained it after 1-month. However, the frequency of strategy use decreased to baseline level after 3-months, which suggest that booster sections are desirable to keep mnemonic strategies. It is relevant to note that objective strategy use outcomes after MST are still scarce in the literature, and it is a critical aspect to be further investigated, especially because people with MCI shows impaired strategy use (Ramakers et al., 2010).

Beside our behavioral data, the neuroimaging findings provided robust evidence of near-transfer effect after MST, since only the training group showed increase of brain activation in a left lateral temporal area when encoding untrained face-name pairs, which is consistent with previous studies (Belleville and Bherer, 2012). This area included the left STS, STG, and MTG, traditionally implicated in auditory and language processing, but critically involved in social cognition and face processing (Hein and Knight, 2008; Deen et al., 2015). The STS has been linked to theory of mind (Deen et al., 2015), mirror neuron system (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Molenberghs et al., 2010), process of social cues (Perrett et al., 1992), face processing (Pitcher et al., 2011), face recognition (Sliwinska and Pitcher, 2018) and perception of physical features such as attractiveness. Similarly, the MTG is implicated in observation of facial expressions (Sato et al., 2012), and STG in the perception of emotions and changeable characteristics of a face (Bigler et al., 2007). Interestingly, these aspects are consistent with the visual cues (i.e., “Feature”) that participants learned in the face-name association training. Part of the cues provided relied on physical characteristics, but others in the emotion expression of the face, which may also integrate cognitive processes related to social cognition and empathy. It is likely that our generalization step may have contributed to the near-transfer effect observed, since we reinforced the associative strategy with real and meaningful examples; nevertheless further investigations are necessary to test this hypothesis. In addition, the left STS serves for multiple functions supporting semantic memory and associative thinking (Liebenthal et al., 2014), and the MTG has been linked to semantic memory retrieval (Tranel et al., 1997; Chao et al., 1999), and demanding semantic decisions (Whitney et al., 2011). Thus, it is possible that MST enhanced new learning/memory via additional semantic processing; an approach that could leverage the relatively better preserved semantic network in those with MCI (Murphy et al., 2008). It is worth mentioning that the pattern of activation related to the task (i.e., prior to training) showed activation in a widespread bilateral network, including target areas relevant to episodic memory (i.e., hippocampal formation and amygdala) (Whitney et al., 2011; Soldan et al., 2015), and face processing (i.e., fusiform face area) (Solomon-Harris et al., 2016), indicating that our fMRI task was successful. The brain changes related to transfer effects are coherent with the intervention format and involved the activation of new alternative brain areas. Our finding agrees with models of brain compensation in aging and suggests that maintaining optimal memory functioning relies on both increased activation of specialized areas and recruitment of new alternative brain networks (Cabeza, 2002; Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008).

Far-Transfer Effects

It is well established that far-transfer effects are more challenging to be demonstrated than near-transfer effects (Simons et al., 2016; Mewborn et al., 2017). In our study we observed limited evidence of transfer effects on neuropsychological tests of attention and memory (not involving face-name associations). Specifically, there were no changes on measures of SKT-attention, contrary to a previous study (Brum et al., 2009), which can be explained by the fact that the patients were relatively intact at baseline and we did not have any explicit attentional component in the training program. Likewise, there were no changes on the HVLT-R, though this is likely understandable since the MST approach did not teach generalization to word lists (note though that Belleville et al., 2011 have shown MST-based improvement on word lists using a semantic clustering approach that is appropriate for word lists). As discussed earlier (Hampstead et al., 2014), the format of some memory tests may be incompatible with the slower and more demanding nature of MST (i.e., it may be difficult for patients to apply strategies when faced with a stimulus every second). We did, however, see some tenuous evidence of improvement on the SKT during both immediate (p = 0.04) and delayed memory (p = 0.06) that was specific to the MST group. It is possible that participants were able to use the “FRI” approach to remember the objects used as stimuli in the SKT, though this awaits empirical confirmation. In hindsight, it is possible our selected outcome measures were not optimal for assessing far-transfer so we encourage future studies to thoroughly consider the correspondence between the training approach and cognitive processes being assessed by selected outcome measures. As we previously stated, it is probably unreasonable to expect a single cognitively oriented treatment to effectively address all cognitive domains (or even multiple aspects of a given domain; Hampstead et al., 2014) since, by comparison, one would not consider a medication ineffective if it reduced low density lipoproteins (LDLs) but not very LDLs or failed to increase high density lipoproteins (HDLs). We maintain that transfer effects will be most likely when the training condition approximates deficits encountered in everyday life; an approach that would readily facilitate the clinical translation of research.

Regarding metamemory, both groups reported greater satisfaction with their own memory after intervention, which is both consistent (Troyer et al., 2008; Kinsella et al., 2009) and inconsistent with other studies (Jean et al., 2010; Bahar-Fuchs et al., 2017). One explanation to this finding is that cognitive training and education may have an effect on psychological wellbeing (e.g., providing a sense of control) and arise from interacting with study team members and participating in an intervention program. Our finding that only the MST group reported improved self-rated memory ability again suggests a training specific, near-transfer effect that aligns with prior research (Rapp et al., 2002; Belleville et al., 2006), though this is certainly not a universal finding (Troyer et al., 2008; Kinsella et al., 2009; Jean et al., 2010; Bahar-Fuchs et al., 2017). It is possible that metamemory gains facilitate the transfer to everyday life, since metamemory has been associated with everyday memory performance (McDougall and Kang, 2003). However, this effect dissipated after 1-month, which may suggest that four sessions of MST have an immediate effect on metamemory, but are not enough to maintain the subjective sense of memory-ability improvement over the time. Thus, booster sessions would clearly be needed prior to 1 month to maintain such effects. Although, we found improvement in the frequency of the strategy use on the SUT, we did not observe changes on the self-rated strategy use, which is at odds with previous reports (Rapp et al., 2002; Troyer et al., 2008; Kinsella et al., 2009; Jean et al., 2010; Bahar-Fuchs et al., 2017). In retrospect, we suspect that the focused nature of our intervention explains this discrepancy since these other studies a range of strategies that would be more likely captured by the MMQ-Strategy scale.

Although we observed limited far-transfer effects in our study, as hypothesized, it is especially encouraging that our brief four-session training led to significant improvements in objective and subjective memory measures, as well as robust increase of task-related brain activation. Our findings are in line with previous reports on the underlying mechanism of near-transfer effect of MST (Belleville et al., 2011; Hampstead et al., 2011, 2012b). We believe that the success of our program is related to the focused and intense nature of the training since we provided hundreds of trials in order to reinforce the MST approach. Moreover, we believe the ‘generalization step’ helped participants consolidate the laboratory based training since participants learned to apply the steps using real-life examples.

As with any study, there are some limitations that warrant discussion. First, the findings reported herein are based on a relatively modest sample size, which limits the power of analysis, and calls for replication with larger groups. We are, however, encouraged by the general replication of results across studies. Second, although we showed that our sample presented significant more brain atrophy than a control sample, we classified a-MCI according to clinical criteria, and not based on direct amyloid- and tau-based biomarker data (e.g., PET or CSF). It is critical that future studies include MCI population characterized also based on such AD-related biomarker data in order to better clarify the efficacy of MST in MCI individuals with the same underlying pathological process (i.e., MCI due to AD). Third, due to the sample size, our study was not able to address moderating factors that may predict intervention response, which is critical for identifying those MCI individuals most likely to benefit from cognitive training (Belleville et al., 2018). Last, although others have used education (Vidovich et al., 2015) as an active control condition, we were not able to control for face-name exposure between the groups.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this randomized controlled study indicates that patients with a-MCI can learn to apply new strategies, which engage task-relevant brain regions, thereby joining other recent works in this area (Belleville et al., 2011; Hampstead et al., 2011, 2012a,b; Rosen et al., 2011; Balardin et al., 2015). Future studies should build on these encouraging findings to investigate and facilitate transfer effects to meaningful everyday life activities.

Author Contributions

SS, CB, BH, RÁ, and EA: conceptualization. SS, MN, LF, and FP: data collection. FD, LT, and MN: data processing. SS, MN, MM, FD, and CM: data analysis. SS and MN: visualization. SS, CB, and GB: resources. CB, BH, GB, and EA: supervision. SS: wrote the original draft. SS, CB, BH, GB, EA, MN, FD, LF, MM, RÁ, FP, SB, CM, and LT: reviewed and edited the draft.

Funding

The study was funded by São Paulo Research Foundation – FAPESP (2012/51699-1) and SS received scholarships from the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel/Brazil (CAPES-PROEX), Science without Borders (CAPES – 99999.002392/2015-01), and Lemann Foundation. GB acknowledges FAPESP (2012/50329-6, 2012/50010-0) and CNPQ-Brazil, and BH acknowledges VA Merit Review Award (IRX001534) and the NIH/NIA funded Michigan Alzheimer’s Disease Center (5P30AG053760) for supporting their work during the development of this work.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We dedicate this article to Prof. Dr. Cássio M. C. Bottino (in memoriam), for supporting cognitive interventions in Brazil, and for mentoring this project at the Old Age Research Group – PROTER, Institute and Department of Psychiatry, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil. We express our gratitude to Alessandra Peck for her excellent administrative assistance, and the support of the neuroimaging laboratories (LIM-21 and LIM-44). We also thank Prof. Candida H. P. de Camargo for her relevant contributions to the project, Prof. Kirk Daffner for the valuable scientific teachings throughout this study, and Prof. Sylvie Belleville for the invitation to present the preliminary results of this study at University of Montreal, and valuable suggestions. Last, we are grateful to the participants who gave their time so generously to this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00342/full#supplementary-material

References

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Annett, M. (1970). A classification of hand preference by association analysis. Br. J Psychol. 61, 303–321. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1970.tb01248.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahar-Fuchs, A., Webb, S., Bartsch, L., Clare, L., Rebok, G., Cherbuin, N., et al. (2017). Tailored and adaptive computerized cognitive training in older adults at risk for dementia: a randomized controlled trial. J. Alzheimers Dis. 60, 889–911. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balardin, J. B., Batistuzzo, M. C., Martin, M. A. G., Sato, J. R., Smid, J., Porto, C., et al. (2015). Differences in prefrontal cortex activation and deactivation during strategic episodic verbal memory encoding in mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:147. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, D. E., and Yaffe, K. (2011). The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 10, 819–828. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70072-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., and Steer, R. A. (1988a). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56, 893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Carbin, M. G. (1988b). Psychometric properties of the beck depression inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 8, 77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, C. F., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. M. (2003). General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI. Neuroimage 20, 1052–1063. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00435-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belleville, S., and Bherer, L. (2012). Biomarkers of cognitive training effects in aging. Curr. Transl. Geriatr. Exp. Gerontol. Rep. 1, 104–110. doi: 10.1007/s13670-012-0014-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belleville, S., Clément, F., Mellah, S., Gilbert, B., Fontaine, F., et al. (2011). Training-related brain plasticity in subjects at risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 134(Pt 6), 1623–1634. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belleville, S., Gilbert, B., Fontaine, F., Gagnon, L., Ménard, E., and Gauthier, S. (2006). Improvement of episodic memory in persons with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: evidence from a cognitive intervention program. Dement Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 22, 486–499. doi: 10.1159/000096316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belleville, S., Hudon, C., Bier, N., Brodeur, C., Gilbert, B., and Grenier, S. (2018). MEMO+: Efficacy, durability and effect of cognitive training and psychosocial intervention in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. J Am. Geriatr. Soc. 66, 655–663. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benton, A. L., de Hamsher, K. S., and Sivan, A. B. (1983). Multilingual Aphasia Examination, 3rd Edn. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.

Google Scholar

Bigler, E. D., Mortensen, S., Neeley, E. S., Ozonoff, S., Krasny, L., Johnson, M., et al. (2007). Superior temporal gyrus, language function, and autism. Dev. Neuropsychol. 31, 217–238. doi: 10.1080/87565640701190841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bottino, C. M. C., Almeida, O. P., Tamai, S., Forlenza O. V., Scalco, M. Z., and Carvalho, I. A. M. (1999). Entrevista Estruturada Para Diagnóstico De Transtornos MentaisEm Idosos. São Paulo: PROTER, Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina da USP.

Google Scholar

Brandt, J. (1991). The hopkins verbal learning test: development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clin. Neuropsychol. 5, 125–142. doi: 10.1080/13854049108403297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brum, P. S., Forlenza, O. V., and Yassuda, M. S. (2009). Cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: impact on cognitive and functional performance. Dement Neuropsychol. 3, 124–131. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30200010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the harold model. Psychol. Aging 17, 85–100. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chandler, M. J., Parks, A. C., Marsiske, M., Rotblatt, L. J., and Smith, G. E. (2016). Everyday impact of cognitive interventions in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 26, 225–251. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9330-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, L. L., Haxby, J. V., and Martin, A. (1999). Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 913–919. doi: 10.1038/13217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chua, E. F., Schacter, D. L., Rand-Giovannetti, E., and Sperling, R. A. (2007). Evidence for a specific role of the anterior hippocampal region in successful associative encoding. Hippocampus 17, 1071–1080. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clare, L., van Paasschen, J., Evans, S. J., Parkinson, C., Woods, R. T., and Linden, D. E. (2009). Goal-oriented cognitive rehabilitation for an individual with mild cognitive impairment: behavioural and neuroimaging outcomes. Neurocase 15, 318–331. doi: 10.1080/13554790902783116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clare, L., Wilson, B. A., Carter, G., Roth, I., and Hodges, J. R. (2002). Relearning face-name associations in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 16, 538–547. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.4.538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deen, B., Koldewyn, K., Kanwisher, N., and Saxe, R. (2015). Functional organization of social perception and cognition in the superior temporal sulcus. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4596–609. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickerson, B. C., Salat, D. H., Greve, D. N., Chua, E. F., Rand-Giovannetti, E., Rentz, D. M., et al. (2005). Increased hippocampal activation in mild cognitive impairment compared to normal aging and Ad. Neurology 65, 404–411. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000171450.97464.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickerson, B. C., and Sperling, R. A. (2008). Functional abnormalities of the medial temporal lobe memory system in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: insights from functional MRI studies. Neuropsychologia 46, 1624–1635. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dratcu, L., da Costa, Riberito, L., and Calil, H. M. (1987). Depression assessment in brazil. the first application of the montgomery-asberg depression rating scale. Br. J. Psychiatry 150, 797–800. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erzigkeit, H. (2001). Skt: A Short Cognitive Performance Test for Assessing Deficits of Memory and Attention, 23rd Edn. Erlanger: Geromed GmbH.

Google Scholar

Espinosa, A., Alegret, M., Valero, S., Vinyes-Junqué, G., Hernández, I., Mauleón, A., et al. (2013). A longitudinal follow-up of 550 mild cognitive impairment patients: evidence for large conversion to dementia rates and detection of major risk factors involved. J. Alzheimers Dis. 34, 769–780. doi: 10.3233/JAD-122002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Fennema-Notestine, C., McEvoy, L. K., Hagler, D. J., Holland, D., et al. (2009). One-year brain atrophy evident in healthy aging. J. Neurosci. 29, 15223–15231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3252-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaks, M. K., Forlenza, O. V., Pereira, F. S., Viola, L. F., and Yassuda, M. S. (2009). Short cognitive performance test: diagnostic accuracy and education bias in older brazilian adults. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 24, 301–306. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acp033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flavell, J. H., and Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. Perspectives on the Development of Memory and Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Folquitto, J. C., Bustamante, S. E., Barros, S. B., Azevedo, D., Lopes, M. A., Hototian, S. R., et al. (2007). The bayer: activities of daily living scale (B-Adl) in the differentiation between mild to moderate dementia and normal aging. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 29, 350–353. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462006005000037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorenstein, C., Andrade, L., Vieira Filho, A. H., Tung, T. C., and Artes, R. (1999). Psychometric properties of the portuguese version of the beck depression inventory on brazilian college students. J. Clin. Psychol. 55, 553–562. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199905)55:5<553::AID-JCLP3>3.0.CO;2-D

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1959). The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 32, 50–55. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampstead, B. M., Gillis, M. M., and Stringer, A. Y. (2014). Cognitive rehabilitation of memory for mild cognitive impairment: a methodological review and model for future research. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 135–151. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampstead, B. M., Sathian, K., Moore, A. B., Nalisnick, C., and Stringer, A. Y. (2008). Explicit memory training leads to improved memory for face-name pairs in patients with mild cognitive impairment: results of a pilot investigation. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 883–889. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampstead, B. M., Sathian, K., Phillips, P. A., Amaraneni, A., Delaune, W. R., and Stringer, A. Y. (2012a). Mnemonic strategy training improves memory for object location associations in both healthy elderly and patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a randomized, single-blind study. Neuropsychology 26, 385–399. doi: 10.1037/a0027545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampstead, B. M., Stringer, A. Y., Stilla, R. F., Giddens, M., and Sathian, K. (2012b). Mnemonic strategy training partially restores hippocampal activity in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Hippocampus 22, 1652–1658. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampstead, B. M., Stringer, A. Y., Stilla, R. F., Deshpande, G., Hu, X., Moore, A. B., et al. (2011). Activation and effective connectivity changes following explicit-memory training for face-name pairs in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 25, 210–222. doi: 10.1177/1545968310382424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, G., and Knight, R. T. (2008). Superior temporal sulcus – It’s my area: or is it? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 2125–2136. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertzog, C., Dixon, R. A., and Hultsch, D. F. (1990). Relationships between metamemory, memory predictions, and memory task performance in adults. Psychol. Aging 5, 215–227. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.5.2.215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hindmarch, I., Lehfeld, H., de Jongh, P. and Erzigkeit, H. (1998). The bayer activities of daily living scale (B-Adl). Dement Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 9(Suppl. 2), 20–26. doi: 10.1159/000051195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hosseini, S. M., Kramer, J. H., and Kesler, S. R. (2014). Neural correlates of cognitive intervention in persons at risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:231. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huckans, M., Hutson, L., Twamley, E., Jak, A., Kaye, J., and Storzbach, D. (2013). Efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation therapies for mild cognitive impairment (Mci) in older adults: working toward a theoretical model and evidence-based interventions. Neuropsychol. Rev. 23, 63–80. doi: 10.1007/s11065-013-9230-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irish, M., Lawlor, B. A., Coen, R. F., and O’Mara, S. M. (2011). Everyday episodic memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a preliminary investigation. BMC Neurosci. 12:80. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-80

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, L. E., Fogler, K. A., and Tauber, S. K. (2008). Recognition memory measures yield disproportionate effects of aging on learning face-name associations. Psychol. Aging 23, 657–664. doi: 10.1037/a0013008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jean, L., Simard, M., Wiederkehr, S., Bergeron, M. E., Turgeon, Y., Hudon, C., et al. (2010). Efficacy of a cognitive training programme for mild cognitive impairment: results of a randomised controlled study. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 20, 377–405. doi: 10.1080/09602010903343012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. (2001). A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 5, 143–156. doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. C., Saykin, A. J., Baxter, L. C., Flashman, L. A., Santulli, R. B., McAllister, T. W., et al. (2000). The relationship between FMRI activation and cerebral atrophy: comparison of normal aging and Alzheimer disease. Neuroimage 11, 179–187. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorm, A. F., and Jacomb, P. A. (1989). The informant questionnaire on cognitive decline in the elderly (Iqcode): socio-demographic correlates, reliability, validity and some norms. Psychol. Med. 19, 1015–1022. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700005742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorm, A. F., and Jolley, D. (1998). The incidence of dementia: a meta-analysis. Neurology 51, 728–733. doi: 10.1212/WNL.51.3.728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jungwirth, S., Zehetmayer, S., Hinterberger, M., Tragl, K. H., and Fischer, P. (2012). The validity of amnestic MCI and non-amnestic MCI at Age 75 in the prediction of Alzheimer’s dementia and vascular dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr. 24, 959–966. doi: 10.1017/S1041610211002870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (2001). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Kinsella, G. J., Mullaly, E., Rand, E., Ong, B., Burton, C., Price, S., et al. (2009). Early intervention for mild cognitive impairment: a randomised controlled trial. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 80, 730–736. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.148346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirwan, C. B., and Stark, C. E. (2004). Medial temporal lobe activation during encoding and retrieval of novel face-name pairs. Hippocampus 14, 919–930. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liebenthal, E., Desai, R. H., Humphries, C., Sabri, M., and Desai, A. (2014). The functional organization of the left STS: a large scale meta-analysis of pet and FMRI studies of healthy adults. Front. Neurosci. 8:289. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marioni, R. E., Valenzuela, M. J., van den Hout, A., Brayne, C., Matthews, F. E., Mrc Cognitive, et al. (2012). Active cognitive lifestyle is associated with positive cognitive health transitions and compression of morbidity from age sixty-five. PLoS One 7:e50940. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDougall, G. J., and Kang, J. (2003). Memory self-efficacy and memory performance in older males. Int. J. Mens Health 2, 131–147. doi: 10.3149/jmh.0202.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGhee, D. J., Ritchie, C. W., Zajicek, J. P., and Counsell, C. E. (2016). A review of clinical trial designs used to detect a disease-modifying effect of drug therapy in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. BMC Neurol. 16:92. doi: 10.1186/s12883-016-0606-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Memória, C. M., Yassuda, M. S., Nakano, E. Y., and Forlenza, O. V. (2013). Brief screening for mild cognitive impairment: validation of the brazilian version of the montreal cognitive assessment. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28, 34–40. doi: 10.1002/gps.3787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mewborn, C. M., Lindbergh, C. A., and Stephen Miller, L. (2017). Cognitive interventions for cognitively healthy, mildly impaired, and mixed samples of older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized-controlled trials. Neuropsychol. Rev. 27, 403–439. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9350-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miotto, E. C., Sato, J., Lucia, M. C., Camargo, C. H., and Scaff, M. (2010). Development of an adapted version of the boston naming test for portuguese speakers. Rev. Bras Psiquiatr. 32, 279–282. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462010005000006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molenberghs, P., Brander, C., Mattingley, J. B., and Cunnington, R. (2010). The role of the superior temporal sulcus and the mirror neuron system in imitation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1316–1326. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, S. A., and Asberg, M. (1979). A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br. J. Psychiatry 134, 382–389. doi: 10.1192/bjp.134.4.382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, K. J., Troyer, A. K., Levine, B., and Moscovitch, M. (2008). Episodic, but not semantic, autobiographical memory is reduced in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 46, 3116–3123. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nascimento, E. (2005). Wais-Iii: Escala De Inteligência Wechsler Para Adultos – Manual Técnico. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo.

Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cummings, J. L., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, moca: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oedekoven, C. S., Jansen, A., Keidel, J. L., Kircher, T., and Leube, D. (2015). The influence of age and mild cognitive impairment on associative memory performance and underlying brain networks. Brain Imaging Behav. 9, 776–789. doi: 10.1007/s11682-014-9335-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, M., Rigoni, M., Andretta, I., and Moraes, J. F. (2004). Validação do teste figuras complexas de rey na população brasileira. Aval. Psicol. 3, 33–38.

Google Scholar

Parra, M. A., Abrahams, S., Logie, R. H., L. G. Méndez, Lopera, F., and Della Sala, S. (2010). Visual short-term memory binding deficits in familial Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 133, 2702–2713. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrett, D. I., Hietanen, J. K., Oram, M. W., and Benson, P. J. (1992). Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 335, 23–30. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perroco, T. R., Bustamante, S. E., Moreno Model, P., Hototian, S. R., Lopes, M. A., Azevedo, D., et al. (2009). Performance of brazilian long and short iqcode on the screening of dementia in elderly people with low education. Int. Psychogeriatr. 21, 531–538. doi: 10.1017/S1041610209008849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Caracciolo, B., Brayne, C., Gauthier, S., Jelic, V., and Fratiglioni, L. (2014). Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J. Intern. Med. 275, 214–228. doi: 10.1111/joim.12190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., et al. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 58, 1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Roberts, R. O., Knopman, D. S., Boeve, B. F., Geda, Y. E., Ivnik, R. J., et al. (2009). Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch. Neurol. 66, 1447–1455. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrella, J. R., Krishnan, S., Slavin, M. J., Tran, T. T., Murty, L., and Doraiswamy, P. M. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment: evaluation with 4-T functional MR imaging. Radiology 240, 177–186. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2401050739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Dilks, D. D., Saxe, R. R., Triantafyllou, C., and Kanwisher, N. (2011). Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective cortical regions. Neuroimage 56, 2356–2363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polcher, A., Frommann, I., Koppara, A., Wolfsgruber, S., Jessen, F., and Wagner, M. (2017). Face-name associative recognition deficits in subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 56, 1185–1196. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prince, M., Bryce, R., Albanese, E., Wimo, A., Ribeiro, W., et al. (2013). The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement 9, 63.e2–75.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramakers, I. H., Visser, P. J., Aalten, P., Maes, H. L., Lansdaal, H. G., Meijs, C. J., et al. (2010). The predictive value of memory strategies for Alzheimer’s disease in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 25, 71–77. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acp093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapp, S., Brenes, G., and Marsh, A. P. (2002). Memory enhancement training for older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a preliminary study. Aging Ment Health 6, 5–11. doi: 10.1080/13607860120101077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rentz, D. M., Amariglio, R. E., Becker, J. A., Frey, M., Olson, L. E., Frishe, K., et al. (2011). Face-name associative memory performance is related to amyloid burden in normal elderly. Neuropsychologia 49, 2776–2783. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 177–182. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rey, A. (1999). Teste De Cópia E Reprodução De Memória De Figuras Geométricas Complexas. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo.

Google Scholar

Ringe, W. K., Saine, K. C., Lacritz, L. H., Hynan, L. S., and Cullum, C. M. (2002). Dyadic short forms of the wechsler adult intelligence scale-iii. Assessment 9, 254–260. doi: 10.1177/1073191102009003004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, A. C., Sugiura, L., Kramer, J. H., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Cognitive training changes hippocampal function in mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 26(Suppl. 3), 349–357. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-0009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, M., Tym, E., Mountjoy, C. Q., Huppert, F. A., Hendrie, H., Verma, S., et al. (1986). Camdex. A standardised instrument for the diagnosis of mental disorder in the elderly with special reference to the early detection of dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 149, 698–709. doi: 10.1192/bjp.149.6.698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, W., Toichi, M., Uono, S., and Kochiyama, T. (2012). Impaired social brain network for processing dynamic facial expressions in autism spectrum disorders. BMC Neurosci. 13:99. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, K. F., Moher, D., and Altman, D. G. (2010). Consort 2010 comments. Lancet 376, 1222–1223. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61879-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, S. S., Avila, R. T., Vieira, G., and Bottino, C. M. C. (2016). Metamemory and aging: psychometric properties of the brazilian version of the multifactorial memory questionnaire for elderly. Dement Neuropsychol. 10, 113–126. doi: 10.1590/S1980-5764-2016DN1002007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, S. S., Yokomizo, J. E., and Bottino, C. M. (2012). Cognitive intervention in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1163–1178. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2016). Do brain-training programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17, 103–186. doi: 10.1177/1529100616661983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sliwinska, M. W., and Pitcher, D. (2018). TMS demonstrates that both right and left superior temporal sulci are important for facial expression recognition. Neuroimage 183, 394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural Mr Image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl. 1), S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soldan, A., Pettigrew, C., Lu, Y., Wang, M. C., Selnes, O., Albert, M., et al. (2015). Relationship of medial temporal lobe atrophy, apoe genotype, and cognitive reserve in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp. 36, 2826–2841. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solomon-Harris, L. M., Rafique, S. A., and Steeves, J. K. (2016). Consecutive TMS-fMRI reveals remote effects of neural noise to the “occipital face area”. Brain Res. 1650, 134–141. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.08.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R., Chua, E., Cocchiarella, A., Rand-Giovannetti, E., Poldrack, R., Schacter, D. L., et al. (2003). Putting names to faces: successful encoding of associative memories activates the anterior hippocampal formation. Neuroimage 20, 1400–1410. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00391-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., Dickerson, B. C., Pihlajamaki, M., Vannini, P., LaViolette, P. S., Vitolo, O. V., et al. (2010). Functional alterations in memory networks in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuromolecular Med. 12, 27–43. doi: 10.1007/s12017-009-8109-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreen, O., and Strauss, E. (1998). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Stringer, A. Y. (2007). Ecologically Oriented Neurorehabilitation of Memory (Eon-Mem). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Tranel, D., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. R. (1997). A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia 35, 1319–1327. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00085-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troyer, A. K., Murphy, K. J., Anderson, N. D., Moscovitch, M., and Craik, F. I. (2008). Changing everyday memory behaviour in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a randomised controlled trial. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 18, 65–88. doi: 10.1080/09602010701409684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Troyer, A. K., and Rich, J. B. (2002). Psychometric properties of a new metamemory questionnaire for older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 57, 19–27. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.1.P19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A., Kuslansky, G., et al. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N. Engl. J. Med. 348, 2508–2516. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidovich, M. R., Lautenschlager, N. T., Flicker, L., Clare, L., McCaul, K., and Almeida, O. P. (2015). The pace study: a randomized clinical trial of cognitive activity strategy training for older people with mild cognitive impairment. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 23, 360–372. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2014.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weaver Cargin, J., Collie, A., Masters, C., and Maruff, P. (2008). The nature of cognitive complaints in healthy older adults with and without objective memory decline. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 30, 245–257. doi: 10.1080/13803390701377829

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale- Revised. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsles Memory Scale, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Werheid, K., and Clare, L. (2007). Are faces special in Alzheimer’s disease? Cognitive conceptualisation, neural correlates, and diagnostic relevance of impaired memory for faces and names. Cortex 43, 898–906. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70689-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitney, C., Kirk, M., O’Sullivan, J., Lambon, M. A., Ralph, and Jefferies, E. (2011). The neural organization of semantic control: TMS evidence for a distributed network in left inferior frontal and posterior middle temporal gyrus. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1066–1075. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitwell, J. L., Petersen, R. C., Negash, S., Weigand, S. D., Kantarci, K., Ivnik, R. J., et al. (2007). Patterns of atrophy differ among specific subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 64, 1130–1138. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.8.1130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L. O., et al., (2004). Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment. J. Intern. Med. 256, 240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolrich, M. (2008). Robust group analysis using outlier inference. Neuroimage 41, 286–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolrich, M. W., Behrens, T. E., Beckmann, C. F., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. M. (2004). Multilevel linear modelling for fmri group analysis using bayesian inference. Neuroimage 21, 1732–1747. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worsley, K. (2003). “Statistical analysis of activation images,” in Functional Mri: An Introduction to Methods, eds P. Jezzard, P. Matthews, and S. Smith (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Yanhong, O., Chandra, M., and Venkatesh, D. (2013). Mild cognitive impairment in adult: a neuropsychological review. Ann. Indian Acad. Neurol. 16, 310–318. doi: 10.4103/0972-2327.116907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: memory training, cognitive rehabilitation, mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, neuroimaging, functional MRI

Citation: Simon SS, Hampstead BM, Nucci MP, Duran FLS, Fonseca LM, Martin MdGM, Ávila R, Porto FHG, Brucki SMD, Martins CB, Tascone LS, Amaro E Jr, Busatto GF and Bottino CMC (2018) Cognitive and Brain Activity Changes After Mnemonic Strategy Training in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Evidence From a Randomized Controlled Trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:342. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00342

Received: 13 March 2018; Accepted: 10 October 2018;
Published: 13 November 2018.

Edited by:

Milica S. Prostran, University of Belgrade, Serbia

Reviewed by:

Hui-Jie Li, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China
Elisabetta Farina, Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi Onlus (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2018 Simon, Hampstead, Nucci, Duran, Fonseca, Martin, Ávila, Porto, Brucki, Martins, Tascone, Amaro, Busatto and Bottino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sharon S. Simon, sharon.sanzsimon@gmail.com

Download