Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 12 August 2022
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias
Volume 14 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2022.943823

Residual reserve index modifies the effect of amyloid pathology on fluorodeoxyglucose metabolism: Implications for efficiency and capacity in cognitive reserve

Cathryn McKenzie1* Romola S. Bucks1 Michael Weinborn1 Pierrick Bourgeat2 Olivier Salvado3 Brandon E. Gavett1 for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
  • 1School of Psychological Science, The University of Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia
  • 2Australian e-Health Research Centre, Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO) Health and Biosecurity, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 3Data61, Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO), Sydney, NSW, Australia

Background: The residual approach to measuring cognitive reserve (using the residual reserve index) aims to capture cognitive resilience conferred by cognitive reserve, but may be confounded by factors representing brain resilience. We sought to distinguish between brain and cognitive resilience by comparing interactions between the residual reserve index and amyloid, tau, and neurodegeneration [“AT(N)”] biomarkers when predicting executive function. We hypothesized that the residual reserve index would moderate at least one path from an AT(N) biomarker to executive function (consistent with cognitive resilience), as opposed to moderating a path between two AT(N) biomarkers (suggestive of brain resilience).

Methods: Participants (N = 332) were from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. The residual reserve index represented the difference between observed and predicted memory performance (a positive residual reserve index suggests higher cognitive reserve). AT(N) biomarkers were: CSF β-amyloid1–42/β-amyloid1–40 (A), plasma phosphorylated tau-181 (T), and FDG metabolism in AD-specific regions ([N]). AT(N) biomarkers (measured at consecutive time points) were entered in a sequential mediation model testing the indirect effects from baseline amyloid to executive function intercept (third annual follow-up) and slope (baseline to seventh follow-up), via tau and/or FDG metabolism. The baseline residual reserve index was entered as a moderator of paths between AT(N) biomarkers (e.g., amyloid-tau), and paths between AT(N) biomarkers and executive function.

Results: The residual reserve index interacted with amyloid pathology when predicting FDG metabolism: the indirect effect of amyloid → FDG metabolism → executive function intercept and slope varied as a function of the residual reserve index. With lower amyloid pathology, executive function performance was comparable at different levels of the residual reserve index, but a higher residual reserve index was associated with lower FDG metabolism. With higher amyloid pathology, a higher residual reserve index predicted better executive function via higher FDG metabolism.

Conclusion: The effect of the residual reserve index on executive function performance via FDG metabolism was consistent with cognitive resilience. This suggests the residual reserve index captures variation in cognitive reserve; specifically, neural efficiency, and neural capacity to upregulate metabolism to enhance cognitive resilience in the face of greater amyloid pathology. Implications for future research include the potential bidirectionality between neural efficiency and amyloid accumulation.

Introduction

As the global population ages, the number of individuals affected by dementia is expected to grow rapidly (Livingston et al., 2020). Alzheimer’s disease is the leading cause of dementia, and is a major contributor to global disease burden (Nichols et al., 2019). Although the search for disease-modifying treatments has produced some promising candidates (van Bokhoven et al., 2021), prevention is key to reducing the burden of Alzheimer’s disease, and requires an understanding of the biological and environmental factors that confer resistance and resilience across the Alzheimer’s disease continuum (Hampel et al., 2019). “Resistance” refers to the reduced or delayed development of neuropathology (e.g., resistance to Alzheimer’s disease pathology), whereas “resilience” refers to the capacity for the brain to cope with neuropathology (Arenaza-Urquijo and Vemuri, 2020; Stern et al., 2022). For example, high resilience may be reflected by lower than expected neurodegeneration at a given level of Alzheimer’s disease pathology (which we will describe as “brain resilience”), or better than expected cognitive performance at a given level of neurodegeneration (which we will describe as “cognitive resilience”).

A wealth of research shows that modifiable behavioral factors (e.g., education and engagement in cognitively stimulating activities) enhance cognitive resilience in the face of Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology, which then delays or prevents dementia (Livingston et al., 2020; Scheltens et al., 2021). Cognitive reserve is one hypothesized mechanism underlying this cognitive resilience: for a given degree of Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology, an individual with greater cognitive reserve is expected to have better cognitive functioning and show less impaired clinical status compared to an individual with lower cognitive reserve (Stern, 2009; Stern et al., 2022). Although there is substantial support for the theory of cognitive reserve, efforts to conceptualize, operationalize, and understand cognitive reserve are ongoing (Stern et al., 2020).

One approach to measuring cognitive reserve is the residual approach, which operationalizes cognitive reserve as the variance in episodic memory performance that is not explained by demographics and brain integrity, i.e., the difference between observed performance and the performance expected based on demographics and brain integrity. In this way, it may also be considered a measure of the cognitive resilience an individual is expressing at the time of measurement. Compared to proxy measurements of cognitive reserve (e.g., years of education), the residual approach offers a more direct estimate of cognitive reserve and can be measured dynamically (Reed et al., 2010; Bettcher et al., 2019).

Prior research has supported the validity of the residual (hereafter referred to as the residual reserve index) as a measure of cognitive reserve; e.g., a higher residual reserve index has been associated with better cognitive performance, slower cognitive decline, and reduced risk for dementia (Bocancea et al., 2021). Importantly, the residual reserve index has been shown to modify the relationship between neuropathology and separate cognitive or clinical outcomes (e.g., Zahodne et al., 2013), which is a key characteristic of cognitive reserve (Stern et al., 2020). Our previous work showed that the residual reserve index interacted with CSF biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease: it was positively associated with cognitive decline when biomarkers were positive for Alzheimer’s disease, but there was no association with cognitive decline when CSF biomarkers were consistent with typical aging (McKenzie et al., 2020). This suggests that cognitive reserve, insofar as it is captured by the residual approach, requires a certain degree of Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology before is it “activated” as a predictor of attenuated cognitive decline.

The residual reserve index contains that which is unexplained, or unknown, about cognitive performance. As cognitive performance is ultimately the product of brain function, the unexplained variance in the residual reserve index may be attributed to “unmeasured” brain factors (Mungas et al., 2021). which we assume represent cognitive reserve, to some degree (Reed et al., 2010). This assumption may be a limitation of the residual approach, as the residual reserve index may be confounded by unmeasured brain factors that are not directly related to cognitive reserve (Stern, 2021). For example, brain reserve and brain maintenance may protect cognitive function as a downstream effect of the brain’s resistance or resilience to neuropathology (Nyberg et al., 2012; Stern et al., 2020), rather than the cognitive resilience to neuropathology imparted by cognitive reserve. Compared to cognitive reserve, brain reserve and maintenance are proposed to have different associations with biological and environmental determinants, and to interact differently with biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology to predict better clinical outcomes (Habeck et al., 2016; Cabeza et al., 2018), making them distinct research concepts in Alzheimer’s disease prevention (Stern et al., 2020). Potential contamination of the residual reserve index by factors such as brain reserve or brain maintenance is therefore an important issue that, to our knowledge, is yet to be resolved.

The present study sought to address this issue and further test the validity of the residual reserve index as a measure of cognitive reserve by (1) locating its protective effects along an Alzheimer’s disease pathological sequence, and (2) evaluating whether this location reflects a protective mechanism consistent with cognitive reserve theory. To do this, we tested the interactions between the residual reserve index and biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease pathology and neurodegeneration within a model based on the modified amyloid cascade hypothesis (Figure 1; Jack et al., 2013) and informed by the AT(N) classification system for Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers (Jack et al., 2016). Longitudinal executive function performance was chosen as the distal cognitive outcome, given it is sensitive to decline in both aging and AD (Buckner, 2004; Puente et al., 2015), and separate from the memory composite used to create the residual reserve index (Reed et al., 2010). AT(N) biomarkers and longitudinal executive function outcomes were measured sequentially (Figure 1A) and entered in a mediation model derived from the modified amyloid cascade hypothesis (Figure 1B). The residual reserve index was measured at baseline (T0) and an interaction term was calculated with each biomarker (Figure 1B). By comparing the interactions between the residual reserve index and AT(N) biomarkers at each stage of this pre-defined Alzheimer’s disease pathological sequence, we can test whether the residual reserve index positively predicts downstream executive function performance, and whether the location of its moderating effect is consistent with cognitive reserve theory.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The moderated sequential mediation model. Biomarkers used were the ratio of CSF β-amyloid1–42 to β-amyloid1–40 (Aβ42/Aβ40) for amyloid pathology; plasma phosphorylated tau 181 (p-tau181) for tau pathology; and uptake of 18Ffluorodeoxyglucose tracer (FDG metabolism) in AD-specific regions of interest for neurodegeneration. Executive function (EF) was measured using ADNI’s composite measure of EF. MEMR denotes the residual reserve index. (A) the timeline of data collection for variables used in the sequential mediation model, from baseline (T0) through to the seventh annual follow-up (T7). (B) The moderated sequential mediation model used to test this study’s hypotheses. EF intercept and slope were latent growth factors; the intercept was estimated at T3, and the slope was estimated using all available time points (see Supplementary Figure 2). MEMR was entered as a moderator of all individual paths in the mediation component of the model. Dashed arrows are used for illustrative purposes, to signify paths that relate to moderation by MEMR; solid arrows relate to mediation relationships. Not shown: demographic variables of baseline age, sex, and education were entered as covariates of ADNI-EF intercept and slope. (C) The indirect effects estimated in (B). The magnitude of an effect (i.e., BAT, BATN, BAN) is the product of the a, b, or c paths shown in (B).

Our first hypothesis was that the effect of amyloid pathology on downstream executive function performance would be serially mediated by tau pathology and neurodegeneration. This hypothesis would be supported by a significant indirect effect of amyloid pathology on executive function performance (intercept and/or slope) via tau pathology and, subsequently, neurodegeneration (Figure 1C, bottom). Support for this hypothesis would affirm that our model of the modified amyloid cascade hypothesis (Jack et al., 2013, 2018) is appropriate in this sample, and provides a basis for testing more specific hypotheses about the role of cognitive reserve within the AT(N) framework.

The second hypothesis was that the residual reserve index would preferentially moderate at least one path from an AT(N) biomarker to executive function performance, as opposed to primarily moderating a path between two biomarkers. This hypothesis would be supported by a significant interaction (or, if more than one interaction is significant, an interaction of greatest magnitude) between the residual reserve index and an antecedent biomarker when predicting executive function performance. Support for this hypothesis would suggest the residual reserve index attenuates the effect of one or more AT(N) biomarkers on cognitive performance (consistent with cognitive resilience), rather than attenuating the effect of an upstream AT(N) biomarker on a downstream AT(N) biomarker (consistent with brain resilience or resistance). For example, if the residual reserve index primarily moderates the influence of neurodegeneration on executive function performance, this would further validate the residual reserve index as a measure of cognitive reserve, by indicating it captures variance in cognitive resilience to neuropathology. A contrasting example might be if the residual reserve index primarily moderates the influence of amyloid pathology on tau pathology; this may suggest that the residual reserve index is capturing a brain resistance mechanism consistent with brain maintenance, instead of (or in addition to) cognitive reserve.

Materials and methods

Data used in this study were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database at http://adni.loni.usc.edu/data-samples/access-data/. ADNI was launched in 2003 as a public-private partnership, and is led by Principal Investigator Michael W. Weiner, MD. The goal of ADNI is to improve understanding of the progression of mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease, by combining biological markers, e.g., magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), with clinical and neuropsychological assessment. For more information, please see http://adni.loni.usc.edu/.

Participants

Data were collected by ADNI investigators at 59 sites in North America. For maximum data coverage, the current study used data from the ADNI1, ADNIGO, and ADNI2 phases (N = 2,351 participants). For inclusion in ADNI, participants needed to be aged between 55 and 90 years, in generally good health, and willing to participate in a longitudinal study that included neuroimaging and collection of blood and CSF biomarkers. Full inclusion criteria can be found at http://adni.loni.usc.edu/. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant, per the research ethics requirements at each participating ADNI site.

Neuroimaging biomarkers

Baseline measures of hippocampal, whole brain, white matter hyperintensity, and total intracranial volume, all derived from MRI, were used to create the residual reserve index (Reed et al., 2010). Details of ADNI’s neuroimaging protocols have been described previously (Jack et al., 2008) and can be downloaded from http://adni.loni.usc.edu/. Pre-processed T1-weighted MP-RAGE scans obtained from 3.0-Tesla scanners were downloaded from the ADNI database. The volumes were processed using the longitudinal pipeline in Freesurfer version 6.0.0.1 Total intracranial volume was estimated using an atlas-based spatial normalization procedure in Freesurfer (Buckner et al., 2004).

Pre-processed WMH volumes, obtained from T2-weighted FLAIR scans, were downloaded directly from the ADNI database. The distribution of WMH volumes was strongly positively skewed; therefore, this variable was log-transformed before analyses.

The mean 18Ffluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) standard uptake value ratio across five meta-regions of interest (normalized using the pons as a reference) was used as a measure of neurodegeneration (Jack et al., 2018). Details of ADNI’s FDG PET acquisition methods are available at http://adni.loni.usc.edu/ and have been described previously (e.g., Landau et al., 2010).

Fluid biomarkers

The ratio of β-amyloid1–42 to β-amyloid1–40 (Aβ42/Aβ40) in CSF was used as the measure of amyloid pathology (Jack et al., 2018); lower Aβ42/Aβ40 values indicate greater amyloid pathology. Baseline (T0) Aβ42/Aβ40 measurements were downloaded directly from the ADNI database. The ADNI CSF data collection and Aβ42/Aβ40 analysis methods have been detailed previously (Korecka et al., 2014; Kang et al., 2015). Previous work has shown that, compared to Aβ42 concentration alone, CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 is a more specific measure of Alzheimer’s disease pathology, and is a more robust proxy for cortical Aβ42 deposition (Lewczuk et al., 2014, 2017; Janelidze et al., 2016), possibly because Aβ42/Aβ40 controls for individual variation in total β-amyloid peptides, and is less susceptible to confounding factors (Blennow and Zetterberg, 2018).

Tau pathology was measured using plasma phosphorylated tau 181 (p-tau181; Jack et al., 2018). Data from baseline (T0) and the first annual follow-up (T1) were downloaded directly from the ADNI database to be used in the decomposition model and moderated sequential mediation model, respectively. We considered using CSF p-tau181 as our tau biomarker, but decided to use plasma to capitalize on the larger sample size at T1, per ADNI protocol (Kang et al., 2015). Recent studies support the validity of plasma p-tau181 as an Alzheimer’s disease-specific biomarker of cortical tau aggregation and a predictor of longitudinal clinical outcomes (Janelidze et al., 2020; Karikari et al., 2021; Moscoso et al., 2021; Palmqvist et al., 2021).

Neuropsychological data

ADNI’s composite memory measure, ADNI-Mem (Crane et al., 2012), was used as the source of variance to be decomposed to create the baseline (T0) residual reserve index (Figure 1). ADNI-Mem is derived using all available memory tasks, such as the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Rey, 1964), and has good validity as a predictor of conversion from MCI to dementia in the ADNI cohort (Crane et al., 2012).

ADNI’s composite executive function measure, ADNI-EF (Gibbons et al., 2012), was used to model the growth factors (intercept and linear slope) used as distal outcome variables in the moderated sequential mediation. A composite of executive function measures, e.g., Digit Span Backward and Digit Symbol Substitution from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition (Wechsler, 1997), ADNI-EF is more sensitive to change over time in the ADNI sample than its constituent tests (Gibbons et al., 2012). ADNI-EF scores were standardized according to the sample mean and standard deviation at T0, so that change in ADNI-EF scores can be interpreted as change from the T0 mean, in T0 standard deviation units.

Demographic variables were also used in the analyses. These included years of education (full-time equivalent; centered on 12 years) and number of apolipoprotein-E ε4 alleles (APOE4 status; range 0–2), and dichotomous variables representing sex (1 = male, 0 = female), race (1 = African American, 0 = not African American), and ethnicity (1 = Hispanic, 0 = non-Hispanic). Demographic variables were used to define the residual reserve index. Education, sex, and baseline age (centered on the sample average 73.10 years) were also included as covariates of the ADNI-EF intercept and slope.

Statistical analyses

Analyses were performed in Mplus version 8 (Muthén and Muthén, 2017) using two structural equation models (SEMs): (1) the decomposition of episodic memory performance to create the residual reserve index (Figure 2), and (2) the testing of our hypotheses using a moderated sequential mediation model (Figure 1) based on the modified amyloid cascade hypothesis (Jack et al., 2013, 2018). Although it is possible to test moderated mediation models involving latent variables in Mplus, estimating the decomposition and moderated sequential mediation models simultaneously was too computationally intensive due to the number of latent variable interactions involved. Instead, we used factor scores from the decomposition model as observed variables in the moderated sequential mediation model. Given that saved factor scores can be complicated by the presence of factor score indeterminacy, which may be a source of error in subsequent analyses (Rigdon et al., 2019), we used Bayesian analysis to impute 30 sets of plausible values for the residual reserve index, to use in the subsequent moderated sequential mediation model (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2010).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The decomposition model used to define the residual reserve index. Variance in baseline (T0) episodic memory performance (ADNI-Mem) is decomposed into variance explained by indicators of brain integrity (also measured at T0), demographic indicators, and the residual reserve index (MEMR). Rectangles represent observed variables and ovals represent latent variables. Observed brain integrity and demographic variables are contained within single rectangles to simplify the diagram. Parameters are freely estimated unless labeled otherwise. Double-ended arrows represent variance or residual variance. S2 = sample variance (multiplied by measurement error). Not shown: whole brain (WBV) and bilateral hippocampal (HCV) volumes were regressed onto total intracranial volume to correct for head size. Correlations between observed variables were initially freely estimated, then non-significant correlations were constrained to zero to facilitate model convergence. Correlations between MEMR and observed variables were constrained to zero. ADNI-Mem, ADNI’s composite of episodic memory performance; logWMH, log-transformed white matter hyperintensity volume; Aβ42/Aβ40, ratio of CSF β-amyloid1–42 to β-amyloid1–40; p-tau181, plasma phosphorylated tau 181; FDG, uptake of 18Ffluorodeoxyglucose tracer in AD-specific regions of interest; APOE4, number of apolipoprotein-E ε4 alleles.

Maximum likelihood (ML) was the primary method used to estimate the decomposition and moderated sequential mediation models; the fit of ML models was evaluated based on converging evidence from the comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), using standard criteria (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Plausible values from the decomposition model were imputed and saved using Bayesian SEM. The reliability of the plausible values was estimated with intraclass correlation using the “psych” package (Revelle, 2021) in R studio version 2021.09.0 (RStudio Team, 2021) and R version 4.0.4 (R Core Team, 2021).

The decomposition model for the residual reserve index

A structural equation model (Figure 2), adapted from Reed et al. (2010), was used to decompose variance in ADNI-Mem performance into variance explained by demographic variables, variance explained by biomarkers of brain integrity, and a latent variable containing residual variance (MEMR; the residual reserve index). MEMR represents the variance in memory performance that is not explained by brain integrity and demographics. A large, positive residual indicates an individual is performing better than expected based on their demographics and brain integrity; such an individual would be interpreted as having high cognitive reserve.

To ensure MEMR represents a unique component of ADNI-Mem variance, its correlations with all other observed variables in the model were constrained to zero. Reed et al.’s (2010) original model, non-significant correlations between observed variables were fixed to zero to facilitate model convergence. Demographic variables that were small, non-significant predictors of ADNI-Mem were removed to increase parsimony in the subsequent Bayesian analysis. Non-significant brain biomarker variables were not removed: because the concept of “unmeasured” brain features prominently in the interpretation of the residual reserve index (Mungas et al., 2021), it was considered more important to ensure all baseline biomarkers of brain pathology and neurodegeneration were accounted for in the decomposition model.

Bayesian imputation of plausible values

The decomposition model (Figure 2), identified using ML estimation, was run using Bayesian estimation with Markov Chain Monte Carlo sampling. The criterion for convergence was a potential scale reduction factor that stayed below 1.1 for the second 50% of iterations, and remained stable when the total number of iterations was doubled (Gelman and Rubin, 1992; Muthén and Asparouhov, 2011). Fit was evaluated against the criterion of a posterior predictive p-value (PPP) > 0.05 (Muthén and Asparouhov, 2011). ML-derived starting values were entered into the Bayesian model, with one exception necessary to facilitate model fit: whereas the residual variance of ADNI-Mem was fixed to our previously calculated estimate of measurement error of 0.161 (McKenzie et al., 2020) in the ML model, it was freely estimated in the Bayesian model, and a highly informative prior [N(0.161, 0.00002)] was specified instead.

After achieving acceptable convergence and fit, 30 imputations of Bayesian plausible values for the residual reserve index were saved. The reliability of the plausible values was determined using the ICC(2,k); i.e., the ICC based on a mean-rating, absolute-agreement, 2-way random effects model (Shrout and Fleiss, 1979; McGraw and Wong, 1996; see Supplementary material for more information).

The moderated sequential mediation model

This study’s hypotheses were tested using the moderated sequential mediation model shown in Figure 1. This model is based on Andrew Hayes’ PROCESS Model 92 (Hayes, 2017); Mplus syntax was adapted from syntax published by Hayes (2015) and Stride et al. (2015). Results from the 30 imputed datasets were aggregated in Mplus; parameter estimates were pooled across imputations, and standard errors were calculated using the within-imputation standard errors and between-imputation variance in parameter estimates (Schafer, 1997; Muthén and Muthén, 2017). As bootstrapped confidence intervals are not available when analyzing multiple imputed datasets in Mplus, the significance of the mediation effects was evaluated by entering the average plausible values into the moderated mediation model and obtaining bootstrapped bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effects.

Amyloid pathology was modeled as a predictor of ADNI-EF performance (intercept and slope) directly, and indirectly via tau pathology and neurodegeneration mediators. The ADNI-EF intercept was defined by data from the third annual follow-up (T3), i.e., 1 year after neurodegeneration (T2 FDG metabolism) was measured—and the ADNI-EF slope was defined using annual data from T0 to the seventh follow-up (T7). Age, years of education, and sex were entered as covariates of the ADNI-EF intercept and slope. Three indirect effects (Figure 1C) were tested simultaneously within the moderated sequential mediation model: (1) Amyloid pathology → mtau pathology → aneurodegeneration eurode-EF performance (BATN); (2) Amyloid pathology → mtau pathology → au pa-EF performance (BAT); and (3) Amyloid pathology myneurodegeneration → eurod-EF performance (BAN). BATN tests the serial mediation pathway predicted by the amyloid cascade hypothesis (Jack et al., 2013, 2018). BAT tests a mediation pathway that is independent from neurodegeneration; it reflects the degree to which tau pathology mediates the effect of amyloid pathology on ADNI-EF performance via mechanisms other than neurodegeneration. BAN tests a mediation pathway that controls for tau pathology; it reflects the degree to which neurodegeneration mediates the effect of amyloid pathology on ADNI-EF performance via mechanisms independent from tau pathology.

To examine the moderating effects of the residual reserve index, interactions between MEMR and the biomarkers were also tested simultaneously within the model (as shown in Figure 1B). These interactions were entered as predictors of tau pathology, neurodegeneration, and ADNI-EF intercept and slope, to test our hypothesis that the residual reserve index would preferentially moderate a path from an antecedent biomarker to ADNI-EF performance, rather than a path between two AT(N) biomarkers. If a significant interaction was found, the influence of this interaction on the relevant indirect pathway(s) (BATN, BAT, and/or BAN) would be quantified using the index of moderated mediation and by plotting conditional indirect effects (Hayes, 2015).

Results

Of the 2,351 participants whose data were used to create the residual reserve index, data from 332 participants were used in the moderated sequential mediation model. This was the number of participants with non-missing data for amyloid, tau, and neurodegeneration biomarkers. Participant characteristics for the total sample and moderated sequential mediation sample are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

On average, the sample used in the moderated sequential mediation analysis were slightly younger, had a smaller proportion of African American and Hispanic participants, a smaller proportion of individuals with one or more APOE ε4 alleles, slightly higher education, lower amyloid pathology at T0, poorer T0 ADNI-Mem performance, higher T0 and T3 ADNI-EF performance, lower tau pathology at T1, and higher FDG metabolism at T0 and T2. The proportion of individuals diagnosed as cognitively normal or mild cognitive impairment at T0 was also larger in the sample whose data were used in the moderated mediation, and the proportion of individuals diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease was smaller.

The decomposition model for the residual reserve index

The decomposition model (Figure 2) fit well using ML estimation: RMSEA = 0.023, 90% CI (0.013–0.035); CFI = 0.997; TLI = 0.990; SRMR = 0.019. Small, non-significant demographic predictors of ADNI-Mem were ethnicity, β = 0.001, SE = 0.018, p = 0.938, and race, β = 0.009, SE = 0.018, p = 0.634; these variables were removed to improve convergence of the Bayesian model. Although ethnocultural factors play a significant role in the development and progression of Alzheimer’s disease, the majority of the ADNI cohort is non-Hispanic white (Birkenbihl et al., 2020), which limits our ability to find significant independent effects of race or ethnicity on cognitive performance in this sample.

Bayesian imputation of plausible values

The Bayesian decomposition model fit well, PPP = 0.418. Comparison of the standardized Bayesian parameter estimates with the standardized ML estimates indicated good consistency between the two models (see Supplementary material). High agreement was also demonstrated across the 30 sets of imputed MEMR plausible values, ICC(2, 30) = 0.973, 95% CI (0.972, 0.975).

Moderated sequential mediation

Moderation and mediation effects were tested using the moderated sequential mediation model shown in Figure 1B; the results are shown in Figure 3. Model fit was good, RMSEA = 0.030, 90% CI (0.013, 0.043); CFI = 0.982; TLI = 0.980; SRMR = 0.054. The MEMR plausible values were averaged for each subject and entered separately into the moderated sequential mediation model to obtain the bias-corrected bootstrapped (B = 2,000) 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effects that are reported in the bottom-right of Figure 3 (Hayes, 2009). Model fit was also good using the average plausible values, RMSEA = 0.046, 90% CI (0.035, 0.057); CFI = 0.964; TLI = 0.960; SRMR = 0.058.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Unstandardized estimates (95% CI) of the parameters obtained from the moderated sequential mediation model (N = 332). Parameter estimates that are significant at the p < 0.05 level are marked with *. The 95% CIs for the indirect effects (bottom-right of figure) are bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals (2,000 draws), obtained from a model estimated using the average residual reserve index (MEMR) plausible values. All other 95% CIs are symmetric intervals (±1.96 standard errors) obtained by pooling results from 30 imputed datasets. Dashed lines are used for illustrative purposes, to signify paths that relate to the moderating effect of MEMR; solid lines relate to mediation relationships. Biomarkers used were the ratio of CSF β-amyloid1–42 to β-amyloid1–40 (Aβ42/Aβ40) for amyloid pathology; plasma phosphorylated tau 181 (p-tau181) for tau pathology; and uptake of 18Ffluorodeoxyglucose tracer (FDG metabolism) in AD-specific regions of interest for neurodegeneration. ADNI-EF denotes ADNI’s composite measure of executive function. BAT, the amyloid → tau → ADNI-EF indirect effect; BATN, the amyloid fect; B neurodegeneration → ADNI-EF indirect effect; BAN, the amyloid → neurodegeneration → ADNI-EF indirect effect.

Total and direct effects

The total effect of amyloid pathology on ADNI-EF intercept and slope was significant and positive, indicating that lower levels of amyloid pathology were associated with higher predicted ADNI-EF scores at T3, and a slower predicted rate of ADNI-EF decline over 7 years. Approximately half of this total effect remained after accounting for the mediators, indicating a significant direct effect of amyloid on ADNI-EF intercept and slope that was independent of tau pathology and neurodegeneration.

Less severe amyloid pathology was also uniquely associated with less severe tau pathology at T1 and less severe neurodegeneration at T2, such that an individual with amyloid pathology that is one standard deviation below the sample mean (i.e., an Aβ42/Aβ40 value one standard deviation above the mean) would be expected to have an approximately one-third standard deviation reduction in tau pathology and neurodegeneration, respectively. The interaction between amyloid pathology and MEMR was also a significant predictor of neurodegeneration (Figure 4). While higher MEMR was associated with more severe neurodegeneration (i.e., lower FDG metabolism) at less severe levels of amyloid pathology, this relationship was inverted at more severe levels of amyloid pathology, such that higher MEMR was associated with less severe neurodegeneration (i.e., higher FDG metabolism). No other significant MEMR interactions were found.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Effect of amyloid pathology on FDG metabolism as a function of the residual reserve index. Amyloid pathology (Aβ42/Aβ40) was modeled as a continuous variable and is separated into higher pathology (–1 SD), average pathology (0 SD), and lower pathology (+1 SD) for illustration purposes. Aβ42/Aβ40, ratio of CSF β-amyloid1–42 to β-amyloid1–40; FDG, uptake of 18Ffluorodeoxyglucose tracer in AD-specific regions of interest; MEMR, the residual reserve index.

Indirect effects

Of the indirect effects, BAN and BATN were significant (Figure 3), indicating that, in this sample, there were three different pathways by which amyloid pathology predicted ADNI-EF intercept and slope: (1) via mechanisms that are independent from downstream tau pathology and neurodegeneration (i.e., direct effects), (2) serially through tau pathology and neurodegeneration (i.e., BATN), and (3) through neurodegeneration independent from tau pathology (i.e., BAN). Given that the only significant MEMR interaction was the interaction between MEMR and amyloid pathology when predicting FDG metabolism (i.e., MEMR moderated the amyloid → FDG metabolism direct effect), the rest of this section will focus on interpreting the conditional BAN indirect effects.

Conditional amyloid → fluorodeoxyglucose metabolism → executive function effects

To examine how the BAN indirect effect depends on MEMR (the residual reserve index), the index of moderated mediation (IMMAN) was calculated (Hayes, 2015). The index of moderated mediation was significantly different from zero for the ADNI-EF intercept, IMMAN = −0.070, 95% bootstrapped CI (−0.158, −0.035), and the ADNI-EF slope, IMMAN = −0.009, 95% bootstrapped CI (−0.023, −0.004), indicating that the magnitude of BAN was negatively associated with the residual reserve index. Figure 5 illustrates how the interaction between amyloid pathology and the residual reserve index, when predicting neurodegeneration, alters the downstream ADNI-EF outcomes.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Model-predicted ADNI-EF intercept and slope derived from conditional BAN indirect effects. Amyloid pathology (T0) is defined by continuous variable Aβ42/Aβ40, which is separated into higher pathology (–1 SD), average pathology (0 SD), and lower pathology (+1 SD) for illustration purposes. Colored lines represent the downstream neurodegeneration (i.e., FDG metabolism) predicted by the interaction between amyloid pathology Aβ42/Aβ40 and the residual reserve index (MEMR) at T0. Solid vs. dashed lines denote the chosen levels of MEMR. The vertical dashed line denotes the ADNI-EF intercept, modeled to succeed measurement of the antecedent biomarkers. BAN, the amyloid eneurodegeneration → ADNI-EF indirect effect; FDG, uptake of 18Ffluorodeoxyglucose tracer in AD-specific regions of interest; MEMR, the residual reserve index; Aβ42/Aβ40, ratio of CSF β-amyloid1–42 to β-amyloid1–40.

Higher MEMR was associated with higher predicted ADNI-EF performance at T3 across all levels of amyloid pathology, although the association between MEMR and the T3 ADNI-EF performance became stronger as amyloid pathology became more severe. MEMR was not associated with change in ADNI-EF slope at average and more severe levels of amyloid pathology. At less severe levels of amyloid pathology, the rate of change in ADNI-EF appeared to depend somewhat on MEMR but, overall, ADNI-EF rate of change was predicted to remain fairly stable at low T0 levels of amyloid burden, regardless of the value of the residual reserve index.

These results suggest that higher residual reserve index values predict relative preservation of future executive function performance, but the mechanism by which this protection happens depends on baseline amyloid pathology. When amyloid pathology at T0 is less severe, a higher T0 residual reserve index relates to relatively lower FDG metabolism at T2, and a similar executive function performance at T3 compared to lower values of the residual reserve index. In contrast, when amyloid pathology at T0 is more severe, the protective effect of the residual reserve index at T0 is related to relatively higher FDG metabolism at T2 and, subsequently, better executive function performance at T3.

Discussion

The current study aimed to test the validity of the residual reserve index as a measure of cognitive reserve by (1) locating its protective effects within a sequential mediation model based on the modified amyloid cascade hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease, and (2) evaluating whether this location reflects a protective mechanism consistent with cognitive reserve theory. Our first hypothesis was supported: the effect of baseline amyloid pathology on future executive function performance was partially serially mediated by tau pathology and neurodegeneration, although the tau-independent (amyloid → mneurodegeneration → eexecutive function) pathway explained the greatest proportion of variance in the total amyloid effect. Our second hypothesis, that the residual reserve index would preferentially moderate at least one path from an antecedent biomarker to executive function performance, was not supported. The only significant interaction was the moderation by the T0 residual reserve index of the path between T0 amyloid pathology and consequent neurodegeneration at T2, which was measured using FDG metabolism. Overall, there was a negative association between amyloid pathology and FDG metabolism, but the magnitude of this effect varied depending on the residual reserve index.

Although the location of the residual reserve index’s strongest interaction effect is inconsistent with our hypothesis, the residual reserve index still showed a downstream protective effect on executive function at T3 (i.e., the executive function intercept) via its association with FDG metabolism, which is consistent with the cognitive resilience that is assumed to be conferred by cognitive reserve. When T0 amyloid pathology was less severe, a higher T0 residual reserve index predicted relatively lower T2 FDG metabolism, but there was little difference in predicted T3 executive function performance at varying levels of the residual reserve index. These results suggest that, at less severe levels of amyloid pathology, individuals with a higher residual reserve index are expected to show lower FDG metabolism and a similar level of future executive function performance, relative to those with a lower residual reserve index. At increasingly severe levels of T0 amyloid pathology, a higher residual reserve index predicted higher T2 FDG metabolism, which consequently predicted better T3 executive function.

While a negative association between amyloid pathology and FDG metabolism in AD-affected regions is well established in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease dementia (Mosconi et al., 2009; Nordberg et al., 2010), some studies show a positive association between amyloid burden and FDG metabolism in individuals with MCI, as well as APOE ε4 carriers and amyloid-positive individuals with intact cognition (Cohen et al., 2009; Kadir et al., 2012; Johnson et al., 2014; Oh et al., 2014; Yi et al., 2014; Kemppainen et al., 2015), which may indicate there is a compensatory increase in metabolism early in the Alzheimer’s disease pathological continuum that has disappeared by the time the dementia syndrome manifests. To support the contention that an increase in metabolism is compensatory in nature, the increase should be associated with improved cognitive outcomes (Apostolova et al., 2018), and this was demonstrated in a study by Ossenkoppele et al. (2014), in which amyloid positivity was associated with higher FDG metabolism, which subsequently predicted higher episodic memory performance, in cognitively normal older adults. Our positive association between the residual reserve index and FDG metabolism at higher levels of amyloid pathology could be interpreted as a compensatory upregulation of metabolism since it benefits downstream executive function performance.

If we consider FDG metabolism as a proxy for neurodegeneration (where lower metabolism represents greater neurodegeneration), we might expect that, at a given degree of cognitive function or clinical severity, individuals with higher cognitive reserve will show lower FDG metabolism (indicative of greater neurodegeneration) compared to individuals with lower cognitive reserve, because they are able to maintain cognitive performance in the face of more severe brain changes (Stern et al., 1992). Studies using education as a proxy for cognitive reserve tend to demonstrate this pattern of poorer brain integrity at higher levels of education (Perneczky et al., 2006; Garibotto et al., 2008; Kemppainen et al., 2008). Ewers et al. (2013) found that, while controlling for cognitive status, the association between education and FDG metabolism changed depending on level of amyloid pathology, though they found the opposite pattern to ours: higher education was associated with lower FDG metabolism in amyloid positive participants, and higher FDG metabolism in amyloid negative participants. There are a few possible explanations for our different findings. First, we predicted future executive function performance, rather than controlling for cognitive performance. As such, we are reporting the association between the residual reserve index and FDG metabolism that predicts variability in downstream executive function performance. Second, we did not select our sample based on pathological severity or cognitive performance at baseline, meaning there may be more variation in the expression of cognitive reserve compared to a sample of clinically normal older adults. Third, our results may highlight an important distinction between education and the residual reserve index: whereas education is a static estimate of cognitive reserve over the lifetime, the residual reserve index is intended to capture an individual’s expression of cognitive reserve (specifically, the cognitive resilience they are exhibiting) at the time of measurement.

Our findings suggest that the residual reserve index represents variability in neural efficiency and capacity, which are two of the proposed mechanisms underlying cognitive reserve (Barulli and Stern, 2013). Briefly, efficiency is defined as the degree of neural activity required to function, and capacity as the maximum level of neural activity that can be utilized in the face of increasing functional demands or neuronal injury. Relevant to our study is the hypothetical model of cognitive reserve and amyloid accumulation presented by Jagust and Mormino (2011). Based on growing evidence suggesting that higher metabolic activity accelerates amyloid accumulation, the authors proposed that cognitive reserve slows the deposition of cortical amyloid through an association with greater neural efficiency; then, once a threshold of amyloid burden is reached, an increase in neural activity may follow. This increase may be due to an immunological response, or it may reflect compensatory activation to preserve function once neurodegeneration has begun.

The findings of the current study can be interpreted in line with the efficiency and compensatory activity described in Jagust and Mormino’s (2011) model: the negative relationship between the residual reserve index and FDG metabolism at less severe amyloid pathology may represent neural efficiency, and the positive relationship at higher levels of pathology may represent a compensatory response to amyloid burden. This proposed neural efficiency does not show a pronounced benefit on future executive function performance, as T3 performance was comparable at different levels of the residual reserve index; this is consistent with our previous work using ADNI data, which showed that the residual reserve index was not a meaningful predictor of future executive function in Alzheimer’s disease pathology-negative individuals (McKenzie et al., 2020). Nonetheless, these results indicate that a higher residual reserve index is associated with more efficient FDG metabolism.

At more severe levels of amyloid pathology, the current results show a positive relationship between FDG metabolism and the residual reserve index, which suggests that individuals with a higher residual reserve index are expected to have higher downstream executive function performance due to a compensatory upregulation of metabolic activity. We posit that these results are consistent with the concept of neural metabolic capacity, as they suggest that a higher residual reserve index benefits cognitive performance by allowing healthy neurons to increase their metabolic activity in the face of neurological insult. Taken together, the results of our study suggest that the variance in the residual reserve index is capturing variation in cognitive reserve (Barulli and Stern, 2013): at average and below levels of amyloid pathology, the residual reserve index manifests as variation in metabolic efficiency, and at high levels of amyloid pathology, the residual reserve index manifests as variation in metabolic capacity.

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

One salient limitation of our study is that, while we have interpreted our results under the assumption that the residual reserve index is modifying the effect of amyloid pathology on FDG metabolism, we cannot exclude the reverse possibility because amyloid pathology was measured at the same time point as the residual reserve index; some argue a moderator should be modeled as antecedent to the effects being modified (Kraemer et al., 2008). It was not possible for us to model the residual reserve index and amyloid pathology at different time points within the overall sequential mediation model due to insufficient data coverage. Future research that can compare the results of a model using the residual reserve index as the initial antecedent, to one in which amyloid pathology is the initial antecedent may be able to characterize their interaction with greater clarity, e.g., such a study may be able to test Jagust and Mormino’s (2011) hypothesized negative association between cognitive reserve and one’s degree of amyloid accumulation.

A strength of this study is that we included baseline measurements of AT(N) biomarkers in the memory performance decomposition model that created the residual reserve index, in contrast to prior iterations of the residual reserve index that accounted only for structural brain integrity (Reed et al., 2010; McKenzie et al., 2020). The AT(N) biomarkers, particularly the amyloid and tau biomarkers, are more sensitive to early neurodegenerative change than MRI indicators of structural brain integrity, and likely added meaningful information about baseline memory performance (Hohman et al., 2016). By adding biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease pathology and neurodegeneration as predictors in the decomposition model, we may also have accounted for some variance in memory performance that relates to brain maintenance (Cabeza et al., 2018). In other words, adding the AT(N) biomarkers to the decomposition model may have resulted in a more precise indicator of cognitive reserve. However, it also means that the residual reserve index created in our study is not directly comparable to the residual reserve index used in prior studies, as the interpretation of the residual reserve index depends on the predictors used in the decomposition (Ewers, 2020). This could mean that the residual reserve index used in the current study may have captured a unique facet of cognitive reserve compared to other versions of the residual reserve index. This idea could be examined in a replication of our study that compares the results using our residual reserve index vs. a different version of the residual reserve index, such as one defined using only indicators of structural brain integrity (per the original model by Reed et al., 2010).

Another strength is our use of a sequential mediation model, which allowed us to exclude the possibility of bidirectional relationships between biomarkers; this was especially important given some research suggests that metabolic activity influences the accumulation of amyloid pathology (Cohen et al., 2009; Jagust and Mormino, 2011). In addition, the current study revealed that the association between a CSF biomarker of amyloid pathology and downstream FDG metabolism, independent from plasma tau pathology, comprises a substantial proportion of the overall effect of amyloid pathology on future executive function performance; further, we demonstrated that the residual reserve index preferentially moderated this association rather than all other possible paths in our model of the amyloid cascade. Perhaps further understanding of cognitive resilience mechanisms can be gleaned by studying the mechanisms by which amyloid pathology affects FDG metabolism, and by identifying factors that modify the expression of these mechanisms.

In a similar vein, future research could build on our findings by examining changes in amyloid pathology and FDG metabolism over time, to quantify the degree of amyloid pathology needed to trigger compensatory increases in FDG metabolism predicted by the residual reserve index, and whether this threshold varies with the residual. In addition, our findings may be extended by replicating our analyses using individual FDG regions of interest, as this may reveal important information about possible cognitive resilience mechanisms; for example, FDG metabolism in prefrontal regions may be associated with cognitive reserve (e.g., Morbelli et al., 2013). Modeling dynamic change in the residual reserve index may also provide valuable information in future studies that aim to extend on our findings, as the rate of depletion of the residual may be an important predictor of future outcomes (Bettcher et al., 2019). A longitudinal mediation model would have the advantage of controlling for autoregressive effects in the predictors (Cain et al., 2018), which was not possible with our sequential mediation model.

Given that the residual reserve index contains variance in memory performance which is “unexplained,” another important step in extending upon our findings would be to examine associations between the residual reserve index and modifiable lifestyle factors purported to build cognitive reserve (e.g., engagement in cognitively stimulating activities over the lifespan). Factors strongly associated with the residual reserve index may then be tested within the AT(N) framework to identify potential mechanisms underlying cognitive resilience to Alzheimer’s disease pathology, and highlight important targets for interventions that aim to reduce the risk of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease.

Conclusion

The current study presents a novel investigation of the moderating effects of the residual reserve index on future executive function in the context of a sequential mediation model based on the modified amyloid cascade hypothesis (Jack et al., 2013). We found significant indirect pathways from CSF amyloid to executive function outcomes via tau pathology and FDG metabolism in series, and via FDG metabolism independent from tau, the latter moderated by the residual reserve index. Our results showed that the relationship between FDG metabolism and future executive function performance varies as a function of the interaction between antecedent amyloid pathology and the residual reserve index. At less severe levels of amyloid pathology, individuals with a higher residual reserve index are expected to show lower FDG metabolism, but a comparable future executive function performance, relative to those with a lower residual reserve index. When amyloid pathology is more severe, a higher residual reserve index benefits future executive function performance, via an association with higher FDG metabolism. We propose that these effects of the residual reserve index are consistent with the cognitive resilience conferred by cognitive reserve, insofar as cognitive reserve increases metabolic efficiency and the capacity of neurons to increase metabolism to compensate for neurodegeneration and maintain cognitive function in the face of greater amyloid burden.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: Data is available from ADNI upon request. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to https://adni.loni.usc.edu/data-samples/access-data/.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

CM was responsible for study design, data analysis and interpretation of results, and was the primary contributor in writing the manuscript. RB assisted with data analysis and interpretation of results and contributed feedback and revisions to the written manuscript. PB contributed to data preparation, as well as feedback and revisions to the written manuscript. MW and OS contributed feedback and revisions to the written manuscript. BG assisted with study design, data analysis and interpretation of results, and contributed feedback and revisions to the written manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health Grant U01 AG024904) and DOD ADNI (Department of Defense award number W81XWH-12-2-0012). ADNI was funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from the following: AbbVie, Alzheimer’s Association; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation; Araclon Biotech; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen; Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; CereSpir, Inc.; Cogstate; Eisai Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; EuroImmun; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; Fujirebio; GE Healthcare; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research and Development, LLC.; Johnson and Johnson Pharmaceutical Research and Development LLC.; Lumosity; Lundbeck; Merck and Co., Inc.; Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC.; NeuroRx Research; Neurotrack Technologies; Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Takeda Pharmaceutical Company; and Transition Therapeutics. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (www.fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer’s Therapeutic Research Institute at the University of Southern California. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern California.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2022.943823/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

42/Aβ40, ratio of CSF β-amyloid1–42 to β-amyloid1–40; ADNI-Mem, ADNI’s composite measure of episodic memory; ADNI-EF, ADNI’s composite measure of executive function; AT(N), the biological classification system wherein AD is classified according to the presence/severity of amyloid pathology (A), tau pathology (T), and neurodegeneration (N); HCV, bilateral hippocampal volume; p-tau181, plasma phosphorylated tau 181; WBV, whole brain volume; WMH, white matter hyperintensity volume; TIV, total intracranial volume.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/

References

Apostolova, I., Lange, C., Mäurer, A., Suppa, P., Spies, L., Grothe, M. J., et al. (2018). Hypermetabolism in the hippocampal formation of cognitively impaired patients indicates detrimental maladaptation. Neurobiol. Aging 65, 41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., and Vemuri, P. (2020). Improving the resistance and resilience framework for aging and dementia studies. Alzheimers. Res. Ther. 12, 1–4. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00609-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. O. (2010). Plausible values for latent variables using Mplus. Available online at: http://www.statmodel.com/papers.shtml (accessed October 31, 2021).

Google Scholar

Barulli, D., and Stern, Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: Emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 502–509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettcher, B. M., Gross, A. L., Gavett, B. E., Widaman, K. F., Fletcher, E., Dowling, N. M., et al. (2019). Dynamic change of cognitive reserve: Associations with changes in brain, cognition, and diagnosis. Neurobiol. Aging 83, 95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birkenbihl, C., Salimi, Y., Domingo-Fernándéz, D., Lovestone, S., Fröhlich, H., and Hofmann-Apitius, M. (2020). Evaluating the Alzheimer’s disease data landscape. Alzheimers Dement. Transl. Res. Clin. Interv. 6, 1–11. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blennow, K., and Zetterberg, H. (2018). Biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease: Current status and prospects for the future. J. Intern. Med. 284, 643–663. doi: 10.1111/joim.12816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bocancea, D. I., van Loenhoud, A. C., Groot, C., Barkhof, F., van der Flier, W. M., and Ossenkoppele, R. (2021). Measuring resilience and resistance in aging and Alzheimer disease using residual methods: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology 97, 474–488. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: Multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 44, 195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Head, D., Parker, J., Fotenos, A. F., Marcus, D., Morris, J. C., et al. (2004). A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: Reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage 23, 724–738. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Albert, M. S., Belleville, S., Craik, F. I. M., Duarte, A., Grady, C. L., et al. (2018). Maintenance, reserve and compensation: The cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 701–710. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cain, M. K., Zhang, Z., and Bergeman, C. S. (2018). Time and other considerations in mediation design. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 78, 952–972. doi: 10.1177/0013164417743003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, A. D., Price, J. C., Weissfeld, L. A., James, J., Rosario, B. L., Bi, W., et al. (2009). Basal cerebral metabolism may modulate the cognitive effects of Aβ in mild cognitive impairment: An example of brain reserve. J. Neurosci. 29, 14770–14778. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3669-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crane, P. K., Carle, A., Gibbons, L. E., Insel, P., Mackin, R. S., Gross, A., et al. (2012). Development and assessment of a composite score for memory in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Brain Imaging Behav. 6, 1–15. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9186-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ewers, M. (2020). Reserve in Alzheimer’s disease: Update on the concept, functional mechanisms and sex differences. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 33, 178–184. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ewers, M., Insel, P. S., Stern, Y., and Weiner, M. W., and Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative [ADNI] (2013). Cognitive reserve associated with FDG-PET in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology 80, 1194–1201. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828970c2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garibotto, V., Borroni, B., Kalbe, E., Herholz, K., Salmon, E., Holtoff, V., et al. (2008). Education and occupation as proxies for reserve in aMCI converters and AD FDG-PET evidence. Neurology 71, 1342–1349. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000327670.62378.c0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, A., and Rubin, D. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat. Sci. 7, 457–511.

Google Scholar

Gibbons, L. E., Carle, A. C., Mackin, R. S., Harvey, D., Mukherjee, S., Insel, P., et al. (2012). A composite score for executive functioning, validated in Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) participants with baseline mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging Behav. 6, 517–527. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9176-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habeck, C., Razlighi, Q., Gazes, Y., Barulli, D., Steffener, J., and Stern, Y. (2016). Cognitive reserve and brain maintenance: Orthogonal concepts in theory and practice. Cereb. Cortex 27, 3962–3969. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampel, H., Lista, S., Neri, C., and Vergallo, A. (2019). Time for the systems-level integration of aging: Resilience enhancing strategies to prevent Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Neurobiol. 181:101662. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.101662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond baron and kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Commun. Monogr. 76, 408–420.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behav. Res. 50, 1–22. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.962683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hohman, T. J., McLaren, D. G., Mormino, E. C., Gifford, K. A., Libon, D. J., Jefferson, A. L., et al. (2016). Asymptomatic Alzheimer disease defining resilience. Neurology 87, 2443–2450. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R., Bennett, D. A., Blennow, K., Carrillo, M. C., Dunn, B., Haeberlein, S. B., et al. (2018). NIA-AA research framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 14, 535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R., Bernstein, M. A., Fox, N. C., Thompson, P., Alexander, G., Harvey, D., et al. (2008). The Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative (ADNI): MRI methods. J. Magn. Reson. imaging 27, 685–691. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R., Hampel, H., Universities, S., Cu, M., and Petersen, R. C. (2016). A new classification system for AD, independent of cognition A / T / N: An unbiased descriptive classification scheme for Alzheimer disease biomarkers. Neurology 87, 539–547. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R., Knopman, D. S., Jagust, W. J., Petersen, R. C., Weiner, M. W., Aisen, P. S., et al. (2013). Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: An updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 12, 207–216. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jagust, W. J., and Mormino, E. C. (2011). Lifespan brain activity, β-amyloid, and Alzheimer’s disease. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 520–526. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janelidze, S., Mattsson, N., Palmqvist, S., Smith, R., Beach, T. G., Serrano, G. E., et al. (2020). Plasma P-tau181 in Alzheimer’s disease: Relationship to other biomarkers, differential diagnosis, neuropathology and longitudinal progression to Alzheimer’s dementia. Nat. Med. 26, 379–386. doi: 10.1038/s41591-020-0755-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janelidze, S., Zetterberg, H., Mattsson, N., Palmqvist, S., Vanderstichele, H., Lindberg, O., et al. (2016). CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 and Aβ42/Aβ38 ratios: Better diagnostic markers of Alzheimer disease. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol. 3, 154–165. doi: 10.1002/acn3.274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. C., Christian, B. T., Okonkwo, O. C., Oh, J. M., Harding, S., Xu, G., et al. (2014). Amyloid burden and neural function in people at risk for Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 576–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.09.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadir, A., Almkvist, O., Forsberg, A., Wall, A., Engler, H., Långström, B., et al. (2012). Dynamic changes in PET amyloid and FDG imaging at different stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 198.e1–198.e14. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, J. H., Korecka, M., Figurski, M. J., Toledo, J. B., Blennow, K., Zetterberg, H., et al. (2015). The Alzheimer’s Disease neuroimaging initiative 2 biomarker core: A review of progress and plans. Alzheimers Dement. 11, 772–791. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karikari, T. K., Benedet, A. L., Ashton, N. J., Lantero Rodriguez, J., Snellman, A., Suárez-Calvet, M., et al. (2021). Diagnostic performance and prediction of clinical progression of plasma phospho-tau181 in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 429–442. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-00923-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemppainen, N. M., Aalto, S., Karrasch, M., Någren, K., Savisto, N., Oikonen, V., et al. (2008). Cognitive reserve hypothesis: Pittsburgh compound b and fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography in relation to education in mild Alzheimer ’ s Disease. Ann. Neurol. 63, 112–118. doi: 10.1002/ana.21212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemppainen, N. M., Joutsa, J., Johansson, J., Scheinin, N. M., Någren, K., Rokka, J., et al. (2015). Long-term interrelationship between brain metabolism and amyloid deposition in mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 48, 123–133. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korecka, M., Waligorska, T., Figurski, M., Toledo, J. B., Arnold, S. E., Grossman, M., et al. (2014). Qualification of a surrogate matrix-based absolute quantification method for amyloid-β42 in human cerebrospinal fluid using 2D UPLC-tandem mass spectrometry. J. Alzheimers. Dis. 41, 441–451. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraemer, H. C., Kiernan, M., Essex, M., and Kupfer, D. J. (2008). How and why criteria defining moderators and mediators differ between the baron & kenny and macarthur approaches. Heal. Psychol. 27, S101–S108. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2(Suppl.).S101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landau, S. M., Harvey, D., Madison, C. M., Reiman, E. M., Foster, N. L., Aisen, P. S., et al. (2010). Comparing predictors of conversion and decline in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 75, 230–238. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e8e8b8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewczuk, P., Lelental, N., Spitzer, P., Maler, J. M., and Kornhuber, J. (2014). Amyloid-β 42/40 cerebrospinal fluid concentration ratio in the diagnostics of Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of two novel assays. J. Alzheimers Dis. 43, 183–191. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewczuk, P., Matzen, A., Blennow, K., Parnetti, L., Molinuevo, J. L., Eusebi, P., et al. (2017). Cerebrospinal fluid Aβ42/40 corresponds better than Aβ42 to amyloid PET in Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 55, 813–822. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., et al. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the lancet commission. Lancet 396, 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGraw, K. O., and Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming Inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychol. Methods 1, 30–46. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.1.30

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenzie, C., Bucks, R. S., Weinborn, M., Bourgeat, P., Salvado, O., and Gavett, B. E. (2020). Cognitive reserve predicts future executive function decline in older adults with Alzheimer’s Disease pathology but not age-associated pathology. Neurobiol. Aging 88, 119–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morbelli, S., Perneczky, R., Drzezga, A., Frisoni, G. B., Caroli, A., van Berckel, B. N. M., et al. (2013). Metabolic networks underlying cognitive reserve in prodromal Alzheimer disease: A european Alzheimer disease consortium project. J. Nucl. Med. 54, 894–902. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.112.113928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mosconi, L., Mistur, R., Switalski, R., Tsui, W. H., Glodzik, L., Li, Y., et al. (2009). FDG-PET changes in brain glucose metabolism from normal cognition to pathologically verified Alzheimer’s disease. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 36, 811–822. doi: 10.1007/s00259-008-1039-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscoso, A., Grothe, M. J., Ashton, N. J., Karikari, T. K., Rodriguez, J. L., Snellman, A., et al. (2021). Time course of phosphorylated-tau181 in blood across the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum. Brain 144, 325–339. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mungas, D. M., Fletcher, E., Gavett, B. E., Widaman, K., Zahodne, L. B., Hohman, T. J., et al. (2021). Comparison of education and episodic memory as modifiers of brain atrophy effects on cognitive decline: Implications for measuring cognitive reserve. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 27, 401–411. doi: 10.1017/S1355617720001095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, B. O., and Asparouhov, T. (2011). Bayesian SEM: A more flexible representation of substantive theory. Psychol. Methods 17, 313–335. doi: 10.1037/a0026802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (2017). MPlus user’s guide eighth. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Nichols, E., Szoeke, C. E. I., Vollset, S. E., Abbasi, N., Abd-Allah, F., Abdela, J., et al. (2019). Global, regional, and national burden of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias, 1990–2016: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 18, 88–106. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30403-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordberg, A., Rinne, J. O., Kadir, A., and Lngström, B. (2010). The use of PET in Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 6, 78–87. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2009.217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., and Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 292–305. doi: 10.1016/J.TICS.2012.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, H., Habeck, C., Madison, C., and Jagust, W. (2014). Covarying alterations in a b deposition, glucose metabolism, and gray matter volume in cognitively normal elderly. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 297–308. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ossenkoppele, R., Madison, C., Oh, H., Wirth, M., Van Berckel, B. N. M., and Jagust, W. J. (2014). Is verbal episodic memory in elderly with amyloid deposits preserved through altered neuronal function? Cereb. Cortex 24, 2210–2218. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmqvist, S., Tideman, P., Cullen, N., Zetterberg, H., Blennow, K., Dage, J. L., et al. (2021). Prediction of future Alzheimer’s disease dementia using plasma phospho-tau combined with other accessible measures. Nat. Med. 27, 1034–1042. doi: 10.1038/s41591-021-01348-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perneczky, R., Drzezga, A., Diehl-Schmid, J., Schmid, G., Wohlschläger, A., Kars, S., et al. (2006). Schooling mediates brain reserve in Alzheimer’s disease: Findings of fluoro-deoxy-glucose-positron emission tomography. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 77, 1060–1063. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.094714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puente, A. N., Lindbergh, C. A., and Miller, L. S. (2015). The relationship between cognitive reserve and functional ability is mediated by executive functioning in older adults. Clin. Neuropsychol. 29, 67–81. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2015.1005676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Available online at: https://www.r-project.org (accessed August 18, 2021).

Google Scholar

Reed, B. R., Mungas, D. M., Farias, S. T., Harvey, D., Beckett, L., Widaman, K., et al. (2010). Measuring cognitive reserve based on the decomposition of episodic memory variance. Brain 133, 2196–2209. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revelle, W. (2021). psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/package=psych (accessed September 27, 2021).

Google Scholar

Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie [The clinical psychological examination]. Paris: Presses Universitaries De France.

Google Scholar

Rigdon, E. E., Becker, J. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2019). Factor indeterminacy as metrological uncertainty: Implications for advancing psychological measurement. Multivariate Behav. Res. 54, 429–443. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2018.1535420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RStudio Team (2021). RStudio: Integrated development for R. Available online at: http://www.rstudio.com/ (accessed August 18, 2021).

Google Scholar

Schafer, J. L. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Google Scholar

Scheltens, P., De Strooper, B., Kivipelto, M., Holstege, H., Chételat, G., Teunissen, C. E., et al. (2021). Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 397, 1577–1590. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)32205-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P. E., and Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol. Bull. 86, 420–428.

Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2021). How can cognitive reserve promote cognitive and neurobehavioral health? Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 36, 1291–1295. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acab049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Albert, M. S., Barnes, C. A., Cabeza, R., Pascual-Leone, A., and Rapp, P. R. (2022). Framework for terms used in research of reserve and resilience. Available online at: https://reserveandresilience.com/framework/ (accessed June 25, 2022).

Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Alexander, G. E., Prohovnik, I., and Mayeux, R. (1992). Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 32, 371–375. doi: 10.1002/ana.410320311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés-Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., et al. (2020). Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement. 16, 1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stride, C. B., Gardner, S. E., Catley, N., and Thomas, F. (2015). Mplus code for mediation, moderation and moderated mediation models. Available online at: http://www.offbeat.group.shef.ac.uk/FIO/mplusmedmod.htm (accessed October 13, 2021).

Google Scholar

van Bokhoven, P., de Wilde, A., Vermunt, L., Leferink, P. S., Heetveld, S., Cummings, J., et al. (2021). The Alzheimer’s disease drug development landscape. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 13, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/s13195-021-00927-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III: Administration and scoring manual: Wechsler adult intelligence scale. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Yi, D., Lee, D. Y., Sohn, B. K., Choe, Y. M., Seo, E. H., Byun, M. S., et al. (2014). Beta-amyloid associated differential effects of APOE ε4 on brain metabolism in cognitively normal elderly. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 22, 961–970. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2013.12.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zahodne, L. B., Manly, J. J., Brickman, A. M., Siedlecki, K. L., Decarli, C., and Stern, Y. (2013). Quantifying cognitive reserve in older adults by decomposing episodic memory variance: Replication and extension. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 19, 854–862. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713000738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive reserve, Alzheimer’s disease, amyloid cascade hypothesis, executive function, AT(N) classification system, cognitive resilience, FDG metabolism

Citation: McKenzie C, Bucks RS, Weinborn M, Bourgeat P, Salvado O and Gavett BE (2022) Residual reserve index modifies the effect of amyloid pathology on fluorodeoxyglucose metabolism: Implications for efficiency and capacity in cognitive reserve. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:943823. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.943823

Received: 14 May 2022; Accepted: 25 July 2022;
Published: 12 August 2022.

Edited by:

Jee-Young Lee, Seoul Metropolitan Government - Seoul National University Boramae Medical Center, South Korea

Reviewed by:

Merle Hoenig, Helmholtz Association of German Research Centres (HZ), Germany
Han Soo Yoo, Yonsei University, South Korea

Copyright © 2022 McKenzie, Bucks, Weinborn, Bourgeat, Salvado and Gavett. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cathryn McKenzie, cathryn.mckenzie@uwa.edu.au

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.usc.edu). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at: http://adni.loni.usc.edu/wpcontent/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf

Download