Skip to main content

CORRECTION article

Front. Neurosci., 29 August 2017
Sec. Brain Imaging Methods

Corrigendum: Associations between Family Adversity and Brain Volume in Adolescence: Manual vs. Automated Brain Segmentation Yields Different Results

\r\nHannah Lyden*Hannah Lyden1*Sarah I. GimbelSarah I. Gimbel2Larissa Del PieroLarissa Del Piero1A. Bryna TsaiA. Bryna Tsai1Matthew E. SachsMatthew E. Sachs2Jonas T. Kaplan,Jonas T. Kaplan1,2Gayla MargolinGayla Margolin1Darby SaxbeDarby Saxbe1
  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

A corrigendum on
Associations between Family Adversity and Brain Volume in Adolescence: Manual vs. Automated Brain Segmentation Yields Different Results

by Lyden, H., Gimbel, S. I., Del Piero, L., Tsai, A. B., Sachs, M. E, Kaplan, J. T., et al. (2016). Front. Neurosci. 10:398. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00398

In the original article citations were accidentally omitted and sentences were written in error in the paragraphs titled: Anatomical definition of the hippocampus and Anatomical definition of the amygdala.

The paragraph titled Anatomical definition of the amygdala should read as follows (corrections italicized):

Separate left and right amygdala masks were hand-drawn onto each participant's T1-weighted image in the coronal plane according to tracing procedures described by Allen et al. (2005) and Clewett et al. (2014). As described by Clewett et al. (2014) the amygdala was traced in the medial temporal lobe. The anterior boundary was defined as the slice that is considered to be amygdala as viewed in all three orthogonal slices (see Figure 1). We defined the superior boundary of the amygdala as the CSF within the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle for more anterior slices. In more posterior slices, we used the visible gray-white matter boundary as the superior border. CSF defined the dorsomedial boundary. We defined the border between amygdala gray matter and parahippocampal white matter as the lateral boundary. In anterior coronal slices, we defined the inferior boundary by parahippocampal white matter and then extended the line dorsomedially until it connected with CSF. The inferior boundary was traced along the white matter strand of the alveus. Three different tracers (two graduate student tracers, one experienced postdoctoral tracer) traced bilateral amygdalae. An additional tracer was added to the amygdala tracing given that the amygdala is a smaller structure and more variability in measurement was expected. The average measure ICC was 0.80 with a 95% confidence interval of 0.62–0.89 [F(22, 110) = 4.76 2, p < 0.001] between all three tracers. A thresholded mask was then created including all voxels that were chosen in at least 2 out of the 3 tracings to address variability in measurement. This majority voting procedure for manual tracing has been shown to be effective in a number of contexts (Aljabar et al., 2009). Volume data was extracted, using FSL utilities, from all masks and entered into SPSS.

The paragraph titled Anatomical definition of the hippocampus should read as follows (corrections italicized):

The neuroanatomical criteria chosen for hippocampal delineation were taken from existing protocols (Narr et al., 2004; Nicolson et al., 2006). The hippocampi were traced in coronal brain slices from anterior to posterior, using fslview tools. All three (sagittal, coronal, and axial) planes were viewed simultaneously to facilitate the accurate identification of neuroanatomical boundaries. As described by Narr et al. (2004), we began the hippocampal tracing in each hemisphere at the indentation of the hippocampal sulcus, or the middle point of the hippocampus in the coronal plane. We used the alveus of the hippocampus as the superior boundary. As the inferior boundary, we used the white matter of the parahippocampal gyrus. As the lateral boundary, we used the inferior temporal horn of the lateral ventricle. Finally, as the medial boundary, we used the ambient cistern. We continued hippocampal tracing posteriorly until the gray matter resembled an oval mass that was medial to the atrium of the lateral ventricles. Bilateral hippocampi were traced three times, twice by a single graduate student tracer and once by an experienced postdoctoral tracer. The average measure intraclass coefficient (ICC) was 0.88 with a 95% confidence interval of 0.77–0.94 [F(22, 66) = 8.03, p < 0.001] between all three tracings. Subsequently, since only two tracers were used a thresholded mask was created using only the voxels that were chosen in all three tracings. Volume data was extracted, using FSL utilities, from all masks and entered into SPSS.

The authors apologize for this error and state that this does not change the scientific conclusions of the article in any way.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Aljabar, P., Heckemann, R. A., Hammers, A., Hajnal, J. V., and Rueckert, D. (2009). Multi-atlas based segmentation of brain images: atlas selection and its effect on accuracy. Neuroimage 46, 726–738. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, J. S., Bruss, J., Brown, C. K., and Damasio, H. (2005). Normal neuroanatomical variation due to age: the major lobes and a parcellation of the temporal region. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 1245–1260. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clewett, D., Bachmak, S., and Mather, M. (2014). Age-related reducted prefrontal-amygdala structural connectivity is associated with lower trait anxiety. Neuropsychology 28, 631–642. doi: 10.1037/neu0000060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narr, K. L., Thompson, P. M., Szesko, P., Robinson, D., Jang, S., Woods, R. P., et al. (2004). Regional specificity of hippocampal volume reductions in first episode schizophrenia. Neuroimage 21, 1563–1575. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolson, R., DeVito, T., Vidal, C. N., Sui, Y., Hiyashi, K. M., Drost, D. J., et al. (2006). Detection and mapping of hippocampal abnormalities in autism. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 22, 11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: amygdala, hippocampus, methodology, family aggression, early life stress, adolescence

Citation: Lyden H, Gimbel SI, Del Piero L, Tsai AB, Sachs ME, Kaplan JT, Margolin G and Saxbe D (2017) Corrigendum: Associations between Family Adversity and Brain Volume in Adolescence: Manual vs. Automated Brain Segmentation Yields Different Results. Front. Neurosci. 11:458. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00458

Received: 07 July 2017; Accepted: 28 July 2017;
Published: 29 August 2017.

Edited and reviewed by: Yaroslav O. Halchenko, Dartmouth College, United States

Copyright © 2017 Lyden, Gimbel, Del Piero, Tsai, Sachs, Kaplan, Margolin and Saxbe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hannah Lyden, lyden@usc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.