Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 03 November 2022
Sec. Sleep and Circadian Rhythms
This article is part of the Research Topic Influence of Sleep and Recurrent Circadian Disruption on Cardiometabolic Health, Wellbeing, and Safety: From Shiftwork to Monday Mornings View all 8 articles

The circadian system modulates the cortisol awakening response in humans

  • 1Oregon Institute of Occupational Health Sciences, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 2Knight Cardiovascular Institute, School of Medicine, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 3School of Nursing, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 4OHSU-PSU School of Public Health, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 5Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 7VA Portland Health Care System, Portland, OR, United States

Background: In humans, circulating cortisol usually peaks 30–60 min after awakening from nocturnal sleep, this is commonly referred to as the cortisol awakening response (CAR). We examined the extent to which the CAR is influenced by the circadian system, independent of behaviors including sleep.

Materials and methods: We examined the CAR in 34 adults (20 female) using two complementary multiday in-laboratory circadian protocols performed in dim light, throughout which behavioral factors were uniformly distributed across the 24-hour circadian cycle. Protocol 1 consisted of 10 identical consecutive 5-hour 20-minute sleep/wake cycles, and protocol 2 consisted of 5 identical consecutive 18-hour sleep/wake cycles. Salivary melatonin was used as the circadian phase marker (0° = dim light melatonin onset). During each sleep/wake cycle, salivary cortisol was measured upon scheduled awakening and 50-minutes later, with the change in cortisol defined as the CAR. Cosinor analyses were used to detect any significant circadian rhythmicity in the CAR. In secondary analyses, we adjusted the models for time awake before lights on, total sleep time, percent of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, and percent of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep.

Results: Both protocols revealed a similar circadian rhythm in the CAR, with peaks occurring at a circadian phase corresponding to 3:40–3:45 a.m., with no detectable CAR during the circadian phases corresponding to the afternoon. In addition to the sinusoidal component of the circadian rhythm, total sleep time was also associated with the CAR for protocol 1. The percent of sleep spent in REM or NREM sleep were not associated with the CAR in either protocol.

Conclusion: Our results show that the CAR exhibits a robust circadian rhythm that persists even after adjusting for prior sleep. Presuming that the CAR optimizes physiological responses to the anticipated stressors related to awakening, these findings may have implications for shift workers who wake up at unusual circadian phases. A blunted CAR in shift workers upon awakening in the evening may result in diminished responses to stressors.

Introduction

Shortly after awakening from nocturnal sleep, initial cortisol levels increase by 50% or more, a phenomenon known as the cortisol awakening response (CAR) (Pruessner et al., 1997; Clow et al., 2004). This CAR and associated physiological activation are argued to help prepare the body for the anticipated stressors of the day after the nocturnal rest period (Schlotz et al., 2004; Fries et al., 2009; Law et al., 2013). These changes include preparation for altered posture, increased energy demands, and social interactions (Wetherell et al., 2015; Ogasawara et al., 2022). Although the CAR may be considered a physiological adaptation in healthy humans, the traditionally assessed CAR after nocturnal sleep coincides with the period of the day with the greatest risk for serious adverse cardiovascular events (Muller et al., 1985; Manfredini et al., 2005; Thosar et al., 2018a) and it is conceivable that the CAR could be involved in such events in vulnerable individuals. Several studies show that the CAR is associated with prior-day experiences and anticipation of stress for the day ahead (Adam et al., 2006; Fries et al., 2009), but the neurobiological mechanisms that generate the CAR have not been fully elucidated. While the CAR requires awakening from sleep (Vargas and Lopez-Duran, 2020) or the anticipation of awakening (Born et al., 1999), a CAR can be observed following daytime sleep, including naps with the magnitude of the CAR varying according to the timing of the sleep episode (Späth-Schwalbe et al., 1992; Griefahn and Robens, 2008; Devine and Wolf, 2016). These daily variations in the CAR in response to awakening from sleep suggest that CAR dynamics could be regulated by internal circadian mechanisms as well as responses to daily behaviors (awakening) or environmental cues (i.e., light).

In clinical populations, the CAR has been considered a marker for depression among other pathologies (Vrshek-Schallhorn et al., 2013). However, inconsistencies between studies (e.g., an attenuated vs. increased CAR in association with the level of depression (Dedovic and Ngiam, 2015)), prevent the use of the CAR as a standard diagnostic tool. In recent years consensus guidelines for CAR assessment have been developed as an effort to reduce the variabilities both between and within study measurements (Stalder et al., 2016). The consensus guidelines note that the time of awakening is likely to impact CAR levels and CAR measurements should be postponed if the participant recently experienced circadian disruption (e.g., shift work or travel across time zones). While, for example, the collection of CAR following consecutive morning and night shifts intimates that differences in the CAR when measured after night-sleep compared to day-sleep would be at least partly caused by measurements occurring at different circadian phases (Griefahn and Robens, 2010), this and similar studies are not designed to fully separate the contributions of the endogenous circadian system and behaviors such as sleep. Thus, it remains unclear to what extent the CAR is dependent on the endogenous circadian system. Based on these previous findings, we hypothesized that the endogenous circadian system modulates the CAR. Findings in agreement with our hypothesis would suggest that measurement of an individual’s internal circadian clock time with respect to the time of CAR assessment would further improve consistency of the measure. Finally, if the circadian system modulates the CAR, then the CAR is likely to be perturbed in shift work when people wake up at unusual circadian phases. This perturbation could lead to sub-optimal physiological responses to stressors in shift-workers, and this could conceivably be related to the increased risk of cardiovascular disease that accompanies shift work (Torquati et al., 2018). Therefore, understanding the circadian mechanisms that influence the CAR is potentially important for certain diagnoses, as well as the adverse health effects of night shift work.

Our aim was to determine, by using the gold standard approach for assessing intrinsic circadian rhythms in humans, whether the endogenous circadian system modulates the magnitude of the CAR when awakenings occur at different circadian phases. We demonstrate the robustness of the circadian rhythm of the CAR using two circadian protocols, with results that can be generalized across sexes and ages based on the different populations studied in the two protocols.

Materials and methods

To test whether there is an endogenous circadian rhythm in the CAR, we completed a secondary analysis of data from two complementary “forced desynchrony” (FD) protocols (Figure 1) throughout which participants regularly provided saliva samples for the assessment of cortisol and melatonin, with the latter used as an internal circadian phase marker, as detailed below.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of study protocols for an individual with a habitual sleep time from midnight to 8:00 a.m. (clock time is provided on the x-axis). For protocol 1, (A) a baseline night of sleep at this habitual time is followed by a baseline day of wakefulness (11-hours). After baseline assessments, the circadian protocol is introduced which evenly distributes behaviors across all circadian phases with 10 identical consecutively recurring 5-hour 20-minute cycles (2-hour and 40-minute of scheduled wakefulness and 2-hour and 40-minute of a scheduled sleep opportunity) across 5 days. For protocol 2, (B) two nights of sleep at the participant’s habitual sleep time with 16-hour scheduled wakefulness in between, is followed by a circadian protocol with reoccurring 18-hour “days” (12-hour wake and 6-hour sleep cycles) across 7 days. Black boxes indicate scheduled sleep opportunities in darkness (<0.1 lux), gray bars indicate scheduled wakefulness in dim light (∼3 lux), and white boxes represent regular room light used when participants are admitted or discharged from the study.

Participants

We studied adults (characterized in Table 1) in two different FD protocols (n = 17 and n = 18 respectively); one female participated in both. Participants were healthy except for mild untreated hypertension (Wetherell et al., 2018) (n = 9; eight from protocol 1). Participants were taking no medications with the exception of contraceptives in some females. Studies were approved by the institutional review board for human subject protection at Oregon Health and Science University, and all participants provided written informed consent. These data were collected as part of two larger studies on the effects of the circadian system, sleep, and activity on cardiovascular function in humans, of which a number of other measurements taken in protocol 1 (Figure 1A) have been reported (Thosar et al., 2018b,2019a,b; Emens et al., 2020).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

Protocols

In order to ensure the stability of circadian rhythms prior to admission to the in-laboratory study, participants maintained a constant self-selected nightly 8-hour sleep schedule. This schedule was verified by actigraphy (wGT3X-BT, ActiGraph) with an accompanying written sleep diary and time-stamped voice messages at bedtime and upon-awakening for at least 1 week before entering the laboratory. Additionally, participants refrained from medications, including food supplements, caffeine, alcohol, and intense physical activity; a urine drug-screening test was performed to ensure adherence to pre-admission instructions. A pregnancy test was also administered in pre-menopausal women.

In the FD protocols (Figure 1), environmental factors were controlled and behaviors that can affect either cortisol or the circadian pacemaker (e.g., activity, posture, meals, sleep, room temperature, light) were evenly distributed across the circadian cycle, while all measurements, including saliva collections for cortisol, were made across the whole circadian cycle. To avoid resetting the phase of the circadian system, all laboratory protocols scheduled during wakefulness were performed in dim light (<3 lux) and lights were <0.1 lux during scheduled sleep opportunities (Zeitzer et al., 2000). Protocol 1 (Figure 1A) was a 5-day protocol with 10 identical consecutive 5-hour 20-minute “days” with 2-hour and 40-minutes of both scheduled wakefulness and sleep opportunity. Protocol 2 (Figure 1B) was a 7-day protocol with five identical consecutive 18-hour “days” with 12-hour scheduled wakefulness and a 6-hour sleep opportunity. In protocol 1, the at-home routine was followed by a baseline night and day in the laboratory (8-hour scheduled sleep, 11-hour scheduled wakefulness, Figure 1A). Protocol 2 provided an additional night for laboratory acclimation followed by a baseline day and night in the laboratory (8-hour scheduled sleep, 16-hour scheduled wakefulness, Figure 1B). Protocol 2 also provided 1 additional 12-hour wake period before the five reoccurring 18-hour “days.”

Measurements

Saliva was regularly collected using cotton swabs (Salivette®, Starstedt, Inc, Newton, NC, USA). Following saturation, the swabs were spun and the collected saliva samples were frozen at −80°C until analyses. Salivary melatonin was measured using a radioimmunoassay from Bühlmann Laboratories (Schönenbuch, Switzerland), employing the Kennaway G-280 anti-melatonin antibody. The inter-assay coefficient of variation (CV) was 11% and the mean reported intra-assay CV was 7.9%. Dim-light melatonin onset (DLMO) was used as the circadian phase marker for the assignment of circadian phases to the other variables including the CAR. From the time series of salivary melatonin concentrations for each day, we determined the DLMO as the linear interpolated time point when melatonin exceeded 3 pg/ml (Benloucif et al., 2008). In one participant whose salivary melatonin never dipped below 3 pg/ml, 4 pg/ml was used as the threshold. Circadian period was calculated from the slope of daily DLMO assessments captured across the FD protocol as previously described (Thosar et al., 2019a,b). For four participants, only one DLMO was detected as they withdrew from the study on day four or five, so circadian period could not be measured. In these participants, we used this study’s average circadian period (τ = 24.03 h) to prevent excluding their data from analysis.

Salivary cortisol concentration, which reflects the concentration of biologically active unbound cortisol in the serum and is unaffected by variations in cortisol binding globulin (Vining et al., 1983), was determined by a high sensitivity ELISA from Salimetrics (State College, PA, USA). The reported analytical and functional sensitivity of the assay were <0.007 and 0.018 ug/dL, respectively. The mean in-house inter-assay CV was 5.1% and the mean reported intra-assay CV was 5%. We used the saliva samples collected upon-awakening and 50-minutes post-awakening to measure the CAR. Each participant in protocol 1 and protocol 2 provided 6–10 and 1–5 CAR assessments, respectively across the study protocol. Upon-awakening samples were collected while participants were still supine at 1 min after lights-on [∼3 lux] in protocol 1, and 7 min after lights on [∼3 lux] in protocol 2. Collection was later in protocol 2 to accommodate a blood pressure assessment first. The second collection 50-minutes post-awakening was collected while seated in protocol 1 and while supine in protocol 2. In between the two saliva collections, participants were provided the opportunity to use the bathroom and may have gently moved around the room. Additionally, in between the saliva measurements, we assessed flow-mediated dilation (Thosar et al., 2019a) and blood pressure in both protocols. During this time in protocol 1, we also measured mood using the Profile of Mood States and Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Emens et al., 2020), sleepiness using the Stanford Sleepiness Scale (Hoddes et al., 1973), appetite levels using a visual analogue scale, and a 3-minute psychomotor vigilance task (measurement for sustained attention). The aforementioned measures were introduced upon admission (i.e., before the first sleep opportunity) in order to acclimate participants to procedures and surveys. Saliva samples were collected before participants ate or brushed their teeth. Participants did not drink any water for at least 15 min prior to saliva collection.

Standard full polysomnography (except leg electromyogram) was acquired and scored according to American Academy of Sleep Medicine guidelines (Iber, 2007). Time awake prior to lights on, total sleep time, sleep efficiency, arousal index, and percent sleep time spent in NREM and REM were used in secondary analyses to examine the association of sleep (or time awake prior to lights on) and CAR.

Data analysis

Each datum was assigned a circadian phase (0–359°) based on an individual’s DLMO (0°) and estimated circadian period (one full circadian period = 360°). The CAR was calculated as the value of cortisol 50 min post-awakening minus the value immediately upon-awakening. The independent effects of the circadian cycle on cortisol concentration immediately upon-awakening, 50-minutes post-awakening, and CAR were assessed by using linear mixed model cosinor analyses with the fundamental component of the circadian cycle (∼24 h) as a fixed effect and participant (to account for repeated measurements within an individual) included as a random factor. To account for any non-sinusoidal circadian effects and any gradual changes across the protocol, the model was also run with additional fixed effects of the second harmonic (∼12 h) and a linear time-into-protocol component. However, as the latter were not significant and did not improve model fit (assessed using Akaike information criterion and Bayesian information criterion), neither factor was included in the final model. Only the data collected during the reoccurring FD “days” were used for these linear mixed models. The circadian models for the CAR of each protocol are described below by the phases at which their peaks occur and by their peak-to-trough rhythm amplitudes. Secondary analyses separately added age, sex, or hypertension to the aforementioned linear mixed models. Additionally, separate linear mixed models for each sleep parameter with participant as a random factor were first assessed for their contribution on CAR. In order to estimate possible non-linear relationships with CAR, both linear and quadratic terms for sleep parameters were included in our models. Significant parameters were then added step-wise to the circadian model. Given the non-normality of cortisol, CAR, and sleep data, the data were log transformed. The models run using raw units were similar to the models generated using data transformations, thus the raw units are used for easier interpretation. All statistical analyses were performed using STATA 16.0. The Wald Z test was used to evaluate significance of individual fixed effects, and Wald Chi-Square was used to evaluate significance of the overall model. The significance was accepted at p < 0.05. Descriptive statistics are expressed as mean ± standard error unless otherwise specified.

Results

Demographic details for each protocol are summarized in Table 1 and details for individual participants are provided in Supplementary Tables 1, 2. Both men and women in protocol 1 were significantly older and had a higher apnea-hypopnea index (AHI), measured during their baseline night of sleep in the laboratory, compared to participants in protocol 2. However, no participant exceeded an AHI in the range of mild sleep apnea (AASM Task Force, 1999), therefore AHI was not considered a co-variate in our analyses.

Salivary cortisol upon-awakening was similar in both protocols (averages: 0.17 μg/dL in protocol 1 and 0.18 μg/dL in protocol 2; non-significant difference). Salivary cortisol upon-awakening exhibited a robust significant circadian rhythm in both protocols, with levels rising across the circadian phases that correspond to the participants’ habitual sleep times, and peaking at the circadian phase corresponding to shortly after the participants’ habitual awakening time (6:36 a.m. for both protocols) (Figure 2). Cortisol concentrations 50-minutes post-awakening exhibited a similar circadian rhythm but with values on average increased by 0.13 μg/dL in protocol 1 and 0.06 μg/dL in protocol 2. When examining the rhythm of the CAR (difference between 50-minute post-awakening and immediately upon-awakening), the magnitude of the CAR also exhibited a significant circadian rhythm with a peak at 110° for protocol 1 (corresponding to an average time of ∼3:40 a.m. in this subgroup of participants) and a peak at 122° for protocol 2 (corresponding to an average time of ∼3:45 a.m. in the other subgroup of participants). The peak-to-trough amplitude in the CAR for protocol 1 was 0.28 μg/dL. The peak-to-trough amplitude for protocol 2 was less than half of that value (0.12 μg/dL). In both protocols, the CAR resulted in a cortisol increase of ≥50% when awakening occurred ∼3 h before the time of habitual awakening (circadian phase range = 110–122°). The magnitude of the CAR decreased across the morning with no change in cortisol levels from awakening when awakening occurred in the afternoon and evening (circadian phase range = 260–350°). Individual data (phase and amplitude for the CAR) are available in Supplementary Tables 1, 2. When examining the individual circadian rhythms, in protocol 1, 60% of participants had a significant rhythm (Supplementary Table 1); and in protocol 2, 71% of the detected rhythms were significant (Supplementary Table 2). There were no significant fixed effects nor impact on the circadian variation of the CAR when age, sex, or hypertension status were separately added to the model.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Circadian rhythm in the cortisol awakening response (CAR). The cortisol concentrations upon-awakening and 50 min post-awakening as a function of circadian time are plotted on the left column (A,C) and the CAR is plotted on the right (B,D). Data are expressed as absolute values (left y-axes) and as a percentage of the mean of each participant’s upon-awake values (right y-axes). Participants’ binned data (60°, 4-hour intervals, means±SEM, left y-axes) are depicted as black circles (upon-awake) and red triangles (50 min post-awakening). Binned data were averaged within an individual first, as an individual could contribute more than one point to each bin. The solid lines represent the cosinor model fits. The blue arrows in (A,C) highlight the peaks of CAR in the two protocols. The corresponding clock times (determined from the average times of the DLMO for these participants) are shown on the top x-axes. Gray bars also on the top x-axes indicate the participants’ average habitual sleep times (determined from the at home assessment).

Additionally, we examined the independent contribution of sleep (or lack thereof) on CAR. Descriptive statistics for sleep across the FD in each protocol are provided in Table 1. Sleep parameters were only available for 12 of the 17 participants of protocol 1. However, a sensitivity analysis with only these 12 participants revealed that their unadjusted CAR model was identical to the CAR model with all 17 participants. As noted in the methods, the effects of each sleep parameter on the CAR were fit using both linear and quadratic terms. The quadratic term was not significant when estimating the log transformed data and was therefore removed from the models herein. Separate mixed models (Table 2 and Figures 3, 4) for the linear relationship between the CAR and sleep parameters demonstrated that each additional minute in total sleep time was associated with an increased CAR of 0.002 ± 0.0005 μg/dL (p < 0.001) in protocol 1; total sleep time during each sleep opportunity was in the range of 1–155 min out of a 160 min opportunity. In protocol 2, where sleep was in the range of 52–350 min out of a 360 min opportunity, total sleep time was not significantly associated with the CAR (p = 0.657). Similarly, in protocol 1, each percent increase in sleep efficiency was associated with a 0.003 ± 0.0008 μg/dL (p < 0.001) increase in the CAR, and, in contrast, an increase in the arousal index was associated with a decrease in the CAR −0.004±0.0016 μg/dL (p = 0.011). However, in protocol 2, sleep efficiency and arousal index were not associated with the CAR. The percent of sleep spent in NREM and REM were not associated with CAR in either protocol. Finally, the amount of time awake before lights-on was negatively associated with CAR in protocol 1 but not protocol 2 (Protocol 1: -0.002 ± 0.0007 μg/dL per min awake, p < 0.01, range of time awake 0–115 min; Protocol 2: −0.0004 per min awake, p = 0.115, range 0–286 min). When either total sleep time or sleep efficiency were added into the circadian CAR model for protocol 1, the circadian component remained significant but the peak was shifted earlier to 85° (∼2:00 a.m.), and the peak-to-trough amplitude was reduced to 0.17 μg/dL (p = 0.002). Additionally, total sleep time and sleep efficiency remained a significant independent component, with each additional minute of sleep being associated with an increase in the CAR by 0.001 ± 0.0006 μg/dL (p = 0.015) and each percent of sleep efficiency was associated with an increase in the CAR by 0.002 ± 0.001 μg/dL (p = 0.016). When time awake prior to lights on was added to the circadian CAR model, the rhythm remained significant (p = 0.001) with a peak at 95° (∼2:40 a.m.) and a peak-to-trough amplitude of 0.19 μg/dL; time awake remained significant in the model and was associated with a decrease in CAR by 0.001 ± 0.0007 μg/dL (p = 0.035). Arousal index was no longer significant when added to the circadian model.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Univariable and multivariable mixed models for the circadian and sleep effects on the cortisol awakening response (CAR) in two distinct protocols.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Associations between measured sleep parameters during the 10 forced desynchrony sleep opportunities of protocol 1 and the cortisol awakening response (CAR). Participants’ binned data (binned into 5–6 intervals for a given sleep parameter, means±SEM) are depicted as black squares. Binned data were averaged within an individual first, as an individual could contribute more than one point to each bin. The solid lines represent significant linear regressions as tested using mixed models (see Table 2). A quadratic term was also estimated but not significant.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Associations between measured sleep parameters during the five forced desynchrony sleep opportunities of protocol 2 and the cortisol awakening response (CAR). Participants’ binned data (binned into 4–5 intervals for a given sleep parameter, means±SEM) are depicted as black circles. Binned data were averaged within an individual first, as an individual could contribute more than one point to each bin. Linear (see Table 2) and quadratic models were not significant.

Discussion

We found that the endogenous circadian system modulates the magnitude of the CAR with the largest CAR following awakenings at circadian phases that occurred ∼3 h prior to the participants’ habitual awakening times. This finding is robust and generalizable as it was confirmed using two independent circadian protocols with largely distinct populations.

The initiation of the CAR requires the transition from sleep to a conscious state (Wilhelm et al., 2007; Vargas and Lopez-Duran, 2020). Although the CAR is most frequently assessed following nighttime sleep when cortisol levels are on the rise, the CAR is generally considered a distinct response from the natural basal circadian levels of cortisol (Wilhelm et al., 2007). The sudden rise in cortisol in response to awakening may reflect the rapid switch of cortical and sub-cortical brain regions during the transition to awakening (Clow et al., 2010). This switch is associated with an increase in adrenal sensitivity to adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH). However, adrenal sensitivity can also be driven by the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the hypothalamus (Buijs et al., 2003). Research in rodents suggests that this sensitivity is mediated by direct sympathetic innervation of the adrenal gland by the splanchnic nerve (Buijs et al., 1997; Ulrich-Lai et al., 2006; Bornstein et al., 2008). In addition to this extra-pituitary regulation of cortisol secretion, the SCN regulates basal levels of cortisol via input into the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus which activates ACTH in the pituitary via corticotropin releasing hormone (Buijs et al., 2003; Dickmeis, 2009). With respect to the CAR, it has been conceptualized that extra-pituitary neural pathways such as the splanchnic nerve are essential both for the fine-tuning of ACTH sensitivity immediately prior to awakening from nocturnal sleep and for the increased sensitivity post-awakening in response to light during this period (Buijs et al., 2003). A handful of human studies support the light sensitivity hypothesis by demonstrating a positive association between the CAR magnitude and light exposure (Scheer and Buijs, 1999; Thorn et al., 2004; Petrowski et al., 2019). While light may impact magnitude of the CAR, the CAR does persist in the absence of light (Scheer and Buijs, 1999).

Our findings are consistent with previous reports that an earlier morning awakening is associated with a higher CAR (Edwards et al., 2001; Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2003; Federenko et al., 2004; Devine and Wolf, 2016). Among these studies, Kudielka and Kirschbaum (Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2003) concluded that an increase in cortisol earlier in the morning was incongruent with the aforementioned premise that cortisol levels increase in response to light (as cortisol levels ought to be increased later in the morning when light is brighter) (Leproult et al., 2001). Thus, the authors noted that the CAR is not simply a response to light. Robust cortisol responses to awakening were also present in our study where light levels upon-awakening were dim. Thus, while light levels may contribute to the magnitude of the CAR, we demonstrate that the endogenous circadian system also robustly modulates the CAR and is responsible for the larger CAR at earlier than normal awakening times.

The association between total sleep time and efficiency in the 5-hour 20-minute “day”, with a 2-hour 40-minute sleep opportunity, but not the 18-hour “day” with a 6-hour sleep opportunity, highlights that prior sleep, or potentially sleep pressure (in accordance with the two process model of sleep regulation (Borbely et al., 2016)) which is shaped by sleep/wake ratios, can affect the CAR. Outside the laboratory, the typical sleep/wake ratio is approximately 1:2 (i.e., 8 h asleep, 16 h awake, per 24-hour day). Due to concerns that sleep onset could be difficult at certain circadian phases in protocol 1 with brief sleep opportunities (2-hour 40 min), we increased the sleep/wake ratio such that sleep opportunity encompassed 50% of the time in the protocol (as opposed to 33.3% sleep opportunity in protocol 2, and when at home). Thus, in protocol 1, some participants may have had reduced sleep pressure and this may have contributed to the observation that some participants achieved little sleep at times. Interestingly, in protocol 1, total sleep time and efficiency were associated with CAR levels. In protocol 2, where sleep opportunities were longer (6-hours) and participants slept at least 50 min, neither total sleep time nor efficiency were associated with the CAR. Nonetheless, after adjusting for total sleep time or efficiency in the circadian models, the circadian rhythm of the CAR was nearly identical in the two protocols.

As with our two protocols, total sleep time and quality have inconsistently been associated with the magnitude of the CAR (Eek et al., 2012; Elder et al., 2014; Castro-Diehl et al., 2015), once more potentially a reflection of prior sleep pressure or the timing of this measure. From our findings, we cannot definitively deduce the minimum amount of prior sleep needed for a CAR. However, Devine and Wolf assessed the CAR following morning (8:30 am) and afternoon (∼1:30 pm) naps and found that a 90-minute nap at either time induced a CAR (Devine and Wolf, 2016). A 50-minute afternoon nap, on the other hand, did not induce a CAR. The authors did not examine a 50-minute morning nap. Federenko et al. similarly showed that an hour-long nap during the early evening did not induce a CAR (Federenko et al., 2004), but again, this nap duration was not compared to an earlier morning timepoint. Additional research is required to determine if the CAR is dependent on total sleep time or efficiency (or the amount of prior wakefulness) or if the lack of a CAR during the afternoon is driven by the circadian system. In line with the latter hypothesis, our circadian model developed with both protocols shows that the CAR is not induced during the afternoon, or that the measure 50-minutes following awakening may even be lower than the level of baseline at that circadian time.

Our findings have important implications for the assessment of the CAR as a diagnostic tool. As noted above, the CAR has been extensively studied as a potential biomarker for physical and psychological health outcomes such as chronic stress, depression, and hypertension (Chida and Steptoe, 2009; O’Connor et al., 2009; Juster et al., 2011; Lovell et al., 2011; Saban et al., 2012; Duan et al., 2013). However, CARs have also been noted to be inconsistent not only between people, but within an individual (Almeida et al., 2009; Stalder et al., 2016). In their review, Stalder et al. (2016) outline methods and a number of factors to consider in an effort to minimize variability between CAR measures. Based on our findings, the CAR can drastically change based on the phase of the endogenous circadian cycle at which measures are collected. This circadian rhythm was not significantly altered when we adjusted our models for age and sex, two trait co-variates that have been identified to impact the CAR but with mixed findings (Wust et al., 2000; Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2003). Thus, to more accurately use the CAR as a biomarker it may be necessary to account for an individual’s internal circadian phase using a validated method, as recently reviewed by Reid (2019). In our participants, who habitually slept at night (prior to in-laboratory assessment), we found that the largest CAR resulted when awakening occurred at the circadian phase that corresponded to ∼3 h prior to the time of habitual wakening, whereas there was virtually no CAR when awakening occurred in the afternoon nor in the evening. It therefore seems highly likely that night shift workers who sleep during the daytime, and whose internal circadian rhythms are not completely reset to synchronize with their altered sleep/wake timing (Saksvik et al., 2011) will have a reduced CAR following daytime sleep, which could result in sluggish, diminished, and therefore potentially sub-optimal physiological respronses to stressors. It would be useful to test these dynamics in shift workers in the field as there could be many aspects of physiology, mood and clinical syndromes that are affected by such a hypothesized blunted CAR.

This study has many strengths including the validation of our findings using two different circadian protocols with largely distinct study populations. Moreover, we used polysomnography to assess and statistically adjust for sleep parameters. To that extent, as we recognize the importance of collecting samples soon after awakening, we were able to adjust our models for the time awake before lights on and found that it had only a modest effect compared to the effects of the intrinsic clock. We were also able to control behaviors including the amount of physical activity between the two saliva collections. Our second saliva collection time point, ∼50 min post-awakening, is slightly later than the more commonly assessed 30–45 min post-awakening (Pruessner et al., 1997; Clow et al., 2004). However, cortisol concentrations do not greatly vary between 30–60 min (Pruessner et al., 1997). Thus, our time points should provide a good estimate of the CAR. There are also limitations to this study. For example, while we estimated possible linear and quadratic relationships between the CAR and sleep there are many physiological processes that could lead to other linear relationships that we did not explore. To this extent, additional nuances in sleep such as slow wave activity, a marker for homeostatic sleep drive, as detected by the electroencephalographic power spectrum, were not analyzed in this study but should be considered in future research (Achermann et al., 1993). The present study clearly demonstrates that the CAR is regulated by the circadian system. Future studies should additionally assess ACTH to determine if the magnitude of the CAR is dissociated from ACTH.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by OHSU Institutional Review Board (IRB). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

NB, SS, ST, and MB designed the initial studies. NB and SR contributed to the analysis of the data. NB drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to monitored, or managed data collection, and approved the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by NIH grants R01 HL125893, F32 HL131308, KL2 TR002370, K01 HL146992, R56 HL156948, K01 HL151745, R35 HL155681, R01 HL163232, R01 NS102962, and UL1TR000128, and the Oregon Institute of Occupational Health Sciences at Oregon Health and Science University via funds from the Division of Consumer and Business Services of the State of Oregon (ORS 656.630).

Acknowledgments

We express their thanks to the research participants for their time, and to the Oregon Clinical and Translational Research Institute for nursing support, processing samples, and advertising our studies.

Conflict of interest

Author AM is a consultant for Pure Somni.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.995452/full#supplementary-material

References

AASM Task Force (1999). Sleep-related breathing disorders in adults: Recommendations for syndrome definition and measurement techniques in clinical research. the report of an American Academy of sleep medicine task force. Sleep 22, 667–689. doi: 10.1093/sleep/22.5.667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Achermann, P., Dijk, D. J., Brunner, D. P., and Borbely, A. A. (1993). A model of human sleep homeostasis based on EEG slow-wave activity: Quantitative comparison of data and simulations. Brain Res. Bull. 31, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(93)90016-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adam, E. K., Hawkley, L. C., Kudielka, B. M., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 17058–17063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almeida, D. M., Piazza, J. R., and Stawski, R. S. (2009). Interindividual differences and intraindividual variability in the cortisol awakening response: An examination of age and gender. Psychol. Aging 24:819. doi: 10.1037/a0017910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benloucif, S., Burgess, H. J., Klerman, E. B., Lewy, A. J., Middleton, B., Murphy, P. J., et al. (2008). Measuring melatonin in humans. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 4, 66–69. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.27083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borbely, A. A., Daan, S., Wirz-Justice, A., and Deboer, T. (2016). The two-process model of sleep regulation: A reappraisal. J. Sleep Res. 25, 131–143. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Born, J., Hansen, K., Marshall, L., Molle, M., and Fehm, H. L. (1999). Timing the end of nocturnal sleep. Nature 397, 29–30. doi: 10.1038/16166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, S. R., Engeland, W. C., Ehrhart-Bornstein, M., and Herman, J. P. (2008). Dissociation of ACTH and glucocorticoids. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 19, 175–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2008.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buijs, R. M., van Eden, C. G., Goncharuk, V. D., and Kalsbeek, A. (2003). The biological clock tunes the organs of the body: Timing by hormones and the autonomic nervous system. J. Endocrinol. 177, 17–26. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1770017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buijs, R. M., Wortel, J., Van Heerikhuize, J. J., and Kalsbeek, A. (1997). Novel environment induced inhibition of corticosterone secretion: Physiological evidence for a suprachiasmatic nucleus mediated neuronal hypothalamo-adrenal cortex pathway. Brain Res. 758, 229–236. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00234-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castro-Diehl, C., Diez Roux, A. V., Redline, S., Seeman, T., Shrager, S. E., and Shea, S. (2015). Association of sleep duration and quality with alterations in the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenocortical axis: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA). J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 100, 3149–3158. doi: 10.1210/jc.2015-1198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chida, Y., and Steptoe, A. (2009). Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol. Psychol. 80, 265–278. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clow, A., Hucklebridge, F., Stalder, T., Evans, P., and Thorn, L. (2010). The cortisol awakening response: More than a measure of HPA axis function. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clow, A., Thorn, L., Evans, P., and Hucklebridge, F. (2004). The awakening cortisol response: Methodological issues and significance. Stress 7, 29–37. doi: 10.1080/10253890410001667205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dedovic, K., and Ngiam, J. (2015). The cortisol awakening response and major depression: Examining the evidence. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 11, 1181–1189. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S62289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine, J. K., and Wolf, J. M. (2016). Determinants of cortisol awakening responses to naps and nighttime sleep. Psychoneuroendocrinology 63, 128–134. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickmeis, T. (2009). Glucocorticoids and the circadian clock. J. Endocrinol. 200, 3–22. doi: 10.1677/JOE-08-0415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duan, H., Yuan, Y., Zhang, L., Qin, S., Zhang, K., Buchanan, T. W., et al. (2013). Chronic stress exposure decreases the cortisol awakening response in healthy young men. Stress 16, 630–637. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2013.840579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, S., Evans, P., Hucklebridge, F., and Clow, A. (2001). Association between time of awakening and diurnal cortisol secretory activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 613–622. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00015-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eek, F., Karlson, B., Garde, A. H., Hansen, ÅM., and Ørbæk, P. (2012). Cortisol, sleep, and recovery–Some gender differences but no straight associations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elder, G. J., Wetherell, M. A., Barclay, N. L., and Ellis, J. G. (2014). The cortisol awakening response–applications and implications for sleep medicine. Sleep Med. Rev. 18, 215–224. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2013.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emens, J. S., Berman, A. M., Thosar, S. S., Butler, M. P., Roberts, S. A., Clemons, N. A., et al. (2020). Circadian rhythm in negative affect: Implications for mood disorders. Psychiatry Res. 293:113337. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Federenko, I., Wust, S., Hellhammer, D. H., Dechoux, R., Kumsta, R., and Kirschbaum, C. (2004). Free cortisol awakening responses are influenced by awakening time. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 174–184. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00021-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, E., Dettenborn, L., and Kirschbaum, C. (2009). The Cortisol Awakening Response (CAR): Facts and future directions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 72, 67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griefahn, B., and Robens, S. (2008). The cortisol awakening response: A pilot study on the effects of shift work, morningness and sleep duration. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 981–988. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griefahn, B., and Robens, S. (2010). The normalization of the cortisol awakening response and of the cortisol shift profile across consecutive night shifts–an experimental study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1501–1509. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoddes, E., Zarcone, V., Smythe, H., Phillips, R., and Dement, W. C. (1973). Quantification of sleepiness: A new approach. Psychophysiology 10, 431–436. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1973.tb00801.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iber, C. (2007). The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: Rules. Terminology and Technical Specification. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine

Google Scholar

Juster, R. P., Sindi, S., Marin, M. F., Perna, A., Hashemi, A., Pruessner, J. C., et al. (2011). A clinical allostatic load index is associated with burnout symptoms and hypocortisolemic profiles in healthy workers. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 797–805. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kudielka, B., and Kirschbaum, C. (2003). Awakening cortisol responses are influenced by health status and awakening time but not by menstrual cycle phase. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 35–47. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00008-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, R., Hucklebridge, F., Thorn, L., Evans, P., and Clow, A. (2013). State variation in the cortisol awakening response. Stress 16, 483–492. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2013.817552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leproult, R., Colecchia, E. F., L’Hermite-Baleriaux, M., and Van Cauter, E. (2001). Transition from dim to bright light in the morning induces an immediate elevation of cortisol levels. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 86, 151–157. doi: 10.1210/jc.86.1.151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovell, B., Moss, M., and Wetherell, M. A. (2011). Perceived stress, common health complaints and diurnal patterns of cortisol secretion in young, otherwise healthy individuals. Horm. Behav. 60, 301–305. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manfredini, R., Boari, B., Smolensky, M. H., Salmi, R., La Cecilia, O., Maria Malagoni, A., et al. (2005). Circadian variation in stroke onset: Identical temporal pattern in ischemic and hemorrhagic events. Chronobiol. Int. 22, 417–453. doi: 10.1081/CBI-200062927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, J. E., Stone, P. H., Turi, Z. G., Rutherford, J. D., Czeisler, C. A., Parker, C., et al. (1985). Circadian variation in the frequency of onset of acute myocardial infarction. N. Engl. J. Med. 313, 1315–1322. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198511213132103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, D. B., Hendrickx, H., Dadd, T., Elliman, T. D., Willis, T. A., Talbot, D., et al. (2009). Cortisol awakening rise in middle-aged women in relation to psychological stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1486–1494. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogasawara, Y., Kadooka, S., Tsuchiya, H., and Sugo, T. (2022). High cortisol awakening response measured on day following high-intensity exercise. J. Phys. Fit. Sports Med. 11, 59–66. doi: 10.7600/jpfsm.11.59

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrowski, K., Schmalbach, B., Niedling, M., and Stalder, T. (2019). The effects of post-awakening light exposure on the cortisol awakening response in healthy male individuals. Psychoneuroendocrinology 108, 28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruessner, J. C., Wolf, O. T., Hellhammer, D. H., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., von Auer, K., Jobst, S., et al. (1997). Free cortisol levels after awakening: A reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Sci. 61, 2539–2549. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3205(97)01008-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, K. J. (2019). Assessment of Circadian Rhythms. Neurol. Clin. 37, 505–526. doi: 10.1016/j.ncl.2019.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saban, K. L., Mathews, H. L., Bryant, F. B., O’Brien, T. E., and Janusek, L. W. (2012). Depressive symptoms and diurnal salivary cortisol patterns among female caregivers of stroke survivors. Biol. Res. Nurs. 14, 396–404. doi: 10.1177/1099800412439458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saksvik, I. B., Bjorvatn, B., Hetland, H., Sandal, G. M., and Pallesen, S. (2011). Individual differences in tolerance to shift work–a systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 15, 221–235. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2010.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheer, F. A., and Buijs, R. M. (1999). Light affects morning salivary cortisol in humans. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 84, 3395–3398. doi: 10.1210/jcem.84.9.6102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlotz, W., Hellhammer, J., Schulz, P., and Stone, A. A. (2004). Perceived work overload and chronic worrying predict weekend-weekday differences in the cortisol awakening response. Psychosom. Med. 66, 207–214. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000116715.78238.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Späth-Schwalbe, E., Schöller, T., Kern, W., Fehm, H., and Born, J. (1992). Nocturnal adrenocorticotropin and cortisol secretion depends on sleep duration and decreases in association with spontaneous awakening in the morning. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 75, 1431–1435. doi: 10.1210/jcem.75.6.1334495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stalder, T., Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Adam, E. K., Pruessner, J. C., Wust, S., et al. (2016). Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: Expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology 63, 414–432. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorn, L., Hucklebridge, F., Esgate, A., Evans, P., and Clow, A. (2004). The effect of dawn simulation on the cortisol response to awakening in healthy participants. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 925–930. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thosar, S. S., Berman, A. M., Herzig, M. X., McHill, A. W., Bowles, N. P., Swanson, C. M., et al. (2019a). Circadian rhythm of vascular function in midlife adults. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 39, 1203–1211. doi: 10.1161/ATVBAHA.119.312682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thosar, S. S., Rueda, J. F., Berman, A. M., Lasarev, M. R., Herzig, M. X., Clemons, N. A., et al. (2019b). Separate and interacting effects of the endogenous circadian system and behaviors on plasma aldosterone in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 316, R157–R164. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00314.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thosar, S. S., Butler, M. P., and Shea, S. A. (2018a). Role of the circadian system in cardiovascular disease. J. Clin. Invest. 128, 2157–2167. doi: 10.1172/JCI80590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thosar, S. S., Herzig, M. X., Roberts, S. A., Berman, A. M., Clemons, N. A., McHill, A. W., et al. (2018b). Lowest perceived exertion in the late morning due to effects of the endogenous circadian system. Br. J. Sports Med. 52, 1011–1012. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2018-099148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torquati, L., Mielke, G. I., Brown, W. J., and Kolbe-Alexander, T. (2018). Shift work and the risk of cardiovascular disease. A systematic review and meta-analysis including dose–response relationship. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 44, 229–238. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.3700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., Arnhold, M. M., and Engeland, W. C. (2006). Adrenal splanchnic innervation contributes to the diurnal rhythm of plasma corticosterone in rats by modulating adrenal sensitivity to ACTH. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 290, R1128–R1135. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00042.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas, I., and Lopez-Duran, N. (2020). The cortisol awakening response after sleep deprivation: Is the cortisol awakening response a “response” to awakening or a circadian process? J. Health Psychol. 25, 900–912. doi: 10.1177/1359105317738323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vining, R. F., McGinley, R. A., and Symons, R. G. (1983). Hormones in saliva: Mode of entry and consequent implications for clinical interpretation. Clin. Chem. 29, 1752–1756. doi: 10.1093/clinchem/29.10.1752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vrshek-Schallhorn, S., Doane, L., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R., Craske, M., and Adam, E. (2013). The cortisol awakening response predicts major depression: Predictive stability over a 4-year follow-up and effect of depression history. Psychol. Med. 43, 483–493. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherell, M. A., Lovell, B., and Smith, M. A. (2015). The effects of an anticipated challenge on diurnal cortisol secretion. Stress 18, 42–48. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2014.993967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whelton, P. K., Carey, R. M., Aronow, W. S., Casey, D. E., Collins, K. J., Dennison Himmelfarb, C., et al. (2018). 2017 ACC/AHA/AAPA/ABC/ACPM/AGS/APhA/ASH/ASPC/NMA/PCNA guideline for the prevention, detection, evaluation, and management of high blood pressure in adults: a report of the American College of Cardiology/American Heart Association Task Force on Clinical Practice Guidelines. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 71:e127–e248.

Google Scholar

Wilhelm, I., Born, J., Kudielka, B. M., Schlotz, W., and Wüst, S. (2007). Is the cortisol awakening rise a response to awakening? Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 358–366. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wust, S., Wolf, J., Hellhammer, D. H., Federenko, I., Schommer, N., and Kirschbaum, C. (2000). The cortisol awakening response-normal values and confounds. Noise Health 2:79.

Google Scholar

Zeitzer, J. M., Dijk, D. J., Kronauer, R. E., Brown, E. N., and Czeisler, C. A. (2000). Sensitivity of the human circadian pacemaker to nocturnal light: Melatonin phase resetting and suppression. J. Physiol. 526, 695–702. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.00695.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: HPA-axis, chronic stress, time of day, ACTH, sleep, circadian, cortisol, awakening

Citation: Bowles NP, Thosar SS, Butler MP, Clemons NA, Robinson LD, Ordaz OH, Herzig MX, McHill AW, Rice SPM, Emens J and Shea SA (2022) The circadian system modulates the cortisol awakening response in humans. Front. Neurosci. 16:995452. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.995452

Received: 26 July 2022; Accepted: 10 October 2022;
Published: 03 November 2022.

Edited by:

Alessandro Silvani, University of Bologna, Italy

Reviewed by:

Shadab Rahman, Harvard Medical School, United States
Hongxia Duan, Radboud University, Netherlands
Corrado Garbazza, University Psychiatric Clinic Basel, Switzerland

Copyright © 2022 Bowles, Thosar, Butler, Clemons, Robinson, Ordaz, Herzig, McHill, Rice, Emens and Shea. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nicole P. Bowles, bowlesn@ohsu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.