Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 21 February 2020
Sec. Neuropharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Pharmacology of BPSD (Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia) View all 31 articles

Large Sample Size Fallacy in Trials About Antipsychotics for Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in Dementia

Tessa A. HulshofTessa A. HulshofSytse U. ZuidemaSytse U. ZuidemaSarah I. M. JanusSarah I. M. JanusHendrika J. Luijendijk*Hendrika J. Luijendijk*
  • University Medical Center Groningen, Department of General Practice, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Background: A typical antipsychotics for neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia have been tested in much larger trials than the older conventional drugs. The advantage of larger sample sizes is that negative findings become less likely and the effect estimates more precise. However, as sample sizes increase, the trials also get more expensive and time consuming while exposing more patients to drugs with unknown safety profiles. Moreover, a large sample size might yield a statistically significant effect that is not necessarily clinically relevant.

Objective: To assess (1) the variation in sample size and sample size calculations of antipsychotic trials in dementia, (2) the size of reported treatment effects and related statistical significance, and (3) general study characteristics that might be related to sample size.

Study Design and Setting: We performed a meta-epidemiological study of randomized trials that tested antipsychotics for neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia. The trials compared conventional or atypical antipsychotics with placebo or another antipsychotic. Two reviewers independently extracted sample size, sample size calculations, reported treatment effects with p-values, and general study characteristics (drug type, trial duration, type of funding). We calculated a reference sample size of 83 and 433 per study group for the placebo-controlled and head-to-head trials respectively.

Results: We identified 33 placebo-controlled trials, and 18 head-to-head trials. Only 14 (42%) and 2 (11%), respectively, reported a sample size calculation. The average sample size per arm was 34 (range 6–179) in placebo-controlled trials testing conventional drugs, 107 (8–237) in such trials testing atypical drugs, and 104 (95–115) in such trials testing both drug types; it was 31 (10–88) in head-to-head trials. Thirteen out of 18 trials with sample sizes larger than required (72%) reported a statistically significant treatment effect, of which two (15%) were clinically relevant. None of the head-to-head trials reported a statistically significant treatment effect, even though some suggested non-inferiority. In placebo-controlled trials of atypical drugs, longer trial duration (>6 weeks) and commercial funding were associated with higher sample size.

Conclusion: Sample size calculations were poorly reported in antipsychotic trials for dementia. Placebo-controlled trials of atypical antipsychotics showed large sample size fallacy while head-to-head trials were massively underpowered.

Introduction

Over the years the sample sizes of antipsychotic trials in dementia have increased from as low as 18 in the 1960s to as high as 652 in the 1990s (Schneider et al., 2005; Cox Grad, 2009; Hulshof et al., 2015). The increase in sample sizes is generally viewed as a favorable development. Larger sample sizes provide more power to identify a treatment effect that is really present. In addition, the effect is estimated more precisely (smaller confidence intervals). Larger trials are also a natural consequence of head-to-head trials because the difference between two active drugs is generally expected to be small, and therefore, the required sample size needs to be relatively high.

However, larger sample sizes also make trials expensive and time consuming (Cox Grad, 2009). This can be barrier for non-commercial investigators to perform a trial. Moreover, it can be ethically questionable to ask more patients to participate, especially when the safety of the tested drug has not yet been established (Schipper and Weyzig, 2008). Another disadvantage of (very) large sample size is that a difference in outcomes between the groups will become (very) statistically significant, no matter how small or clinically meaningless it is (Sullivan and Feinn, 2012). If such results are nevertheless interpreted as clinically relevant, the ‘large sample size fallacy’ occurs (Lantz, 2013).

Sample size calculations for trials are based on four parameters if the response rate is the outcome. These are alpha, beta, the expected response rate in the active treatment groups, and the expected response rate in the comparison group (e.g. placebo) (Noordzij et al., 2010). Alpha is the probability of identifying a treatment effect that is not really present, which is usually set at 5%. Beta is the risk of not identifying a treatment effect that is really present, and is usually set at 20%. Sample size calculations for trials with continuous outcomes, such as the reduction of neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS), are based on alpha, beta, the expected (difference between) means in the active and comparison group, and the population variance around the mean. Furthermore, the expected number of participants dropping out should be taken into account when determining the final target sample size of a trial.

A different expected treatment effect might explain why the sample sizes of antipsychotic trials increased over time. Perhaps, atypical antipsychotics were expected to be less effective than conventional antipsychotics, even before it was shown in systematic reviews that they did not affect psychotic symptoms compared to placebo (Schneider et al., 2006; Smeets et al., 2018). Alternatively, drop-out could have increased because recent trials lasted longer and participants have become more assertive.

On the other hand, general study characteristics, which are not directly related to sample size calculation might have contributed to the increase in trial sample sizes over the years. Large sample size is generally considered a sign of high trial quality, and this increases the probability of publication and citation (Dickersin et al., 1992). In addition, pharmaceutical companies will have more resources to fund larger trials than non-commercial organizations. Therefore, the aim of this meta-epidemiological study was to assess (1) the variation in sample size and sample size calculations of antipsychotic trials in dementia, (2) the size of the reported treatment effects and related statistical significance, and (3) general study characteristics that might be related to sample size.

Methods

Search Strategy

Two reviewers (TAH, HJL) used a list of conventional and atypical antipsychotics from the websites of the World Health Organization, Food and Drug Administration, and Wikipedia to search the literature (US Food and Drug Administration, 2013; World Health Organization, 2013; Wikipedia, 2015). First, we first searched for studies in the electronic databases PubMed, CINAHL, EMBASE, and Cochrane library with the string ‘generic name of atypical/conventional antipsychotic’ and trial and dementia (see online supplement). We restricted the position of the drug name to title and abstract. Subsequently, we manually searched the references of published systematic reviews, which were identified with the same electronic databases. Titles and abstracts of potentially eligible studies were retrieved from PubMed. In addition, we sought trials in trial registration websites with the abovementioned search terms if possible; otherwise we used only the term dementia. These three searches were last re-run in June 2019. Finally, we had used the databases of the Dutch Medicines Evaluation Board and the FDA to find unpublished trials as part of a previous search performed in 2015 (Hulshof et al., 2019).

Study Selection

We screened the title and abstract of the hits. Full texts of potentially eligible published studies and online protocols for unpublished studies were retrieved. Two reviewers used the full texts to determine definitive eligibility (TAH, HJL). The selected trials had to have been randomized and double-blind. They should have tested the efficacy of antipsychotics on NPSs in persons diagnosed with Alzheimer or vascular dementia. The trial had to compare conventional or atypical antipsychotics with placebo or another antipsychotic (head-to-head trial). We excluded studies with multiple drugs in a single intervention arm, studies that were stopped early and thus did not reach the targeted sample size, and studies with a cross-over design as other than standard sample size calculations need to be applied for this design. There were no restrictions with respect to publication date, language, and duration of the study.

Data Extraction

Two reviewers (TAH or SIMJ and HJL) independently extracted the following general study characteristics besides the sample size from the included studies: placebo-controlled or head-to-head trial, type of dementia (Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, mixed, unspecified), type of NPS (agitation, psychosis, diverse), setting (nursing home, hospital, outpatient clinic), active drug tested (conventional, atypical, or both), trial duration, type of funding (not-for-profit or commercial), and whether a sample size calculation was reported.

If the sample size calculation was reported, we extracted the input for sample size calculations: alpha, beta, expected treatment effects in the comparison groups (response rate, or mean symptom reduction with population variance at endpoint), and the expected drop-out rates. For trials that had been published in an abstract or online trial registration only, this data-extraction was considered inapplicable.

In addition, we extracted the reported treatment effects and related statistical significance. The primary outcome of trials that test antipsychotics for NPS in dementia is most often the difference in response rate or difference in reduction of target symptoms between the treatment groups. We extracted both for each trial with the related p-value. For the response rate, we extracted the number of patients with a clinically relevant improvement as defined by the authors. For reduction in symptoms, we extracted the difference in mean change from baseline to endpoint as measured with a symptom scale, such as the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI) for agitation and Neuropsychiatric Inventory-Nursing Home (NPI-NH) for mixed symptoms. Initially, we also set out to extract standard deviations to calculate standardized mean differences, so that we could compare trial results. However, as many SDs turned out to be missing, we decided to extract the mean on the symptom scale at baseline as a reference instead (see data-analysis).

The primary source of extracted data was the published main results article. If that was not available, then conference abstracts or online published results were used. We received the individual patient data of two trials (Schneider et al., 2006; Paleacu et al., 2008), and additional meta-data of two others for use in another study (De Deyn et al., 1999; De Deyn et al., 2005; Hulshof et al., 2019).

Data Analyses

First, we described the variation in sample sizes for the different types of trials by plotting the mean number of participants per comparison group against the publication year of the trial. We present these data for the conventional and atypical placebo-controlled trials and head-to head trials separately.

To assess the adequacy of the reported sample sizes, we calculated reference sample sizes for trials with the response rate as outcome. For the placebo-controlled trials, we used an alpha of 0.05, beta of 0.20, a treatment response rate in the antipsychotic group of 55% and in the placebo group of 30%, and an expected drop-out of 30% (Brant, 2016). A treatment effect of 25% (NNT = 4) and drop-out rate of 30% is in line with previous literature and the reported response rates in antipsychotic trials in dementia (Schneider et al., 2006; Jeste et al., 2008; Drouillard et al., 2013). We used a conservative drop-out rate of 30% (it was 26% on average in the included trials), so that the reference sample size would not be an underestimation. The required sample size per study group was 58 without loss to drop-out, and 83 with loss.

For the head-to-head trials (no placebo group), we used a treatment effect of 55% for the drug of interest and 45% for the control antipsychotic drug, because a 10% difference seems the upper limit of no difference. The expected drop-out rate was set at 10%, which is in line with the average drop-out rate in the included head-to-head trials. The required sample size was 389 per group without loss, and 433 with loss. We used the ssi command in Stata version 15.0 to calculate the reference sample sizes (StataCorp, 2017).

To calculate reference sample sizes based on the outcome mean symptom reduction, the minimal clinically important difference (MCID) is required. However, the MCID is not known for most symptom scales used in this field (Shabbir and Sanders, 2014). The exception is the NPI, which was found to have an MCID of at least 8.0 (Howard et al., 2011; Zuidema et al., 2011). Nine of the included placebo-controlled trials in our study used this instrument, and we used the reported data to check our calculated reference sample size based on response rates. The reported mean reduction in symptoms was 19 (SD 14) for the placebo group (see Supplementary Table 1), and hence, assuming an MCID of 8.0, 27 (SD 16) for the antipsychotic group. We calculated a required sample size of 80 based on these data, and this finding confirms the reference sample size of 83 based on response rates. In addition, the MCID of 8.0 reflects an SMD of 0.500 given the SD of 16 reported in the included trials. This is in line with the lower limit for a visible (medium) treatment effect suggested by Cohen (2007).

The next step was to assess whether studies with larger sample size reported statistically significant treatment effects that were not clinically relevant (difference in response rate <25%; difference in symptom reduction < MCID or SMD <0.5), which would suggest the presence of large sample size fallacy. Treatment effects in terms of reported response rates can be compared between trials with varying sample sizes. However, it was not possible to use MCIDs or SMDs to compare reported reductions in symptoms across different symptoms scales. Therefore, we calculated the relative symptom reduction as the ratio of the difference in symptom reduction between the study groups relative to the baseline mean in the groups. This approach has been used before (Smith et al., 1974). Moreover, the MCID of 8.0 on the NPI and a mean baseline of 39 (see Supplementary Table 1) would translate into a relative symptom reduction of 21%. Hence, a relative symptom reduction of > = 20% seems appropriate.

Finally, we analyzed the association between other general study characteristics and mean sample size per group. The characteristics were type of drug tested (category: conventional, atypical, or both), trial duration (< = 6 weeks, > 6 weeks), and type of funding (non-for-profit, commercial). We calculated mean sample sizes of comparison groups per category, and used the two-sample t-test to determine whether the means differed between the first (reference) category and other categories. The analyses were performed for the placebo-controlled and head-to-head trials separately. All analyses were carried out with Stata version 15.0 (StataCorp, 2017).

Results

Our search yielded 2,768 potentially relevant hits (Figure 1). We obtained the reports of 92 studies for full text review. We considered 57 studies eligible, but 6 had no useable data at the time of assessment. Hence, we used 51 studies in the current study (Hamilton and Bennet, 1962; Sugerman et al., 1964; Smith et al., 1974; Rada and Kellner, 1976; Rosen, 1979; Vergara et al., 1980; Götestam et al., 1981; Barnes et al., 1982; Petrie et al., 1982; Spagnolo et al., 1983; Morris and Rickels, 1984; Stotsky, 1984; Ather et al., 1986; Lovett et al., 1987; Carlyle et al., 1993; Finkel et al., 1995; Auer et al., 1996; Auchus and Cheryl Bissey-Black, 1997; Devanand et al., 1998; De Deyn et al., 1999; Katz et al., 1999; Allain et al., 2000; Street et al., 2000; Howanitz and Wisotzek, 2001; Herz et al., 2002; Pollock et al., 2002; Brodaty et al., 2003; Fontaine et al., 2003; De Deyn et al., 2004; Garerl et al., 2004; Mulsant et al., 2004; Sheng et al., 2004; Sun et al., 2004; Ballard et al., 2005; De Deyn et al., 2005; Deberdt et al., 2005; Verhey et al., 2006; Tariot et al., 2006; Mintzer et al., 2007; Rainer et al., 2007; Zhong et al., 2007; Paleacu et al., 2008; Streim et al., 2008; Teri et al., 2000). Online or other clinical trial reports of the following studies were used: NCT00287742, NCT01862640, NCT01922258, NCT02992132, ZIP-128-105, RIS-BEL-14, RIS-INT-83.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Flow diagram of literature search and study selection.

Table 1 shows the general study characteristics. Eleven trials compared conventional antipsychotics to placebo and 19 trials atypical antipsychotics to placebo. Six of the latter 19 trials tested multiple doses of one atypical drug, so they had more than one drug group (range 2–4). Three placebo-controlled trials tested both conventional and atypical antipsychotics. Eighteen trials compared an antipsychotic drug with another antipsychotic drug. The studies were performed in outpatients, nursing homes, or hospitals. The target symptom for treatment consisted of agitation, psychosis, or diverse NPSs.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Characteristics of randomized placebo-controlled and head-to-head trials of antipsychotics in patients with dementia.

Sample Size Variation and Calculations

Figure 2 shows the mean number of participants per comparison group in each trial against publication year. The symbols indicate the type of drug tested (conventional, atypical, or both) and type of study (placebo-controlled or head-to-head). In the conventional antipsychotic placebo-controlled studies, the mean number per group was 34 patients (range 6–179), while those comparing atypical antipsychotics to placebo included on average 107 patients per group (range 8–237). The three trials that included both conventional and atypical antipsychotics and compared these to placebo included 104 patients per group (range 95–115). Head-to-head trials included a mean number of 31 patients per group (range 10–88). The increase in sample size over time seems to be related to type of drug tested.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Scatter plot of sample sizes per arm over the years per treatment group.

We calculated a reference sample size of 83 patients per group for the placebo-controlled trials and 433 patients for the head-to-head trials, as explained above. The group sample size was lower than the reference sample size in 10 placebo-controlled trials of conventional antipsychotics (small sample size) and higher in one such trial (large sample size), whereas 5 of the 19 atypical antipsychotic trials and none of the 3 trials including both conventional and atypical antipsychotics had small sample sizes. At least four of the five atypical underpowered antipsychotic trials were investigator initiated, although one was performed with commercially acquired funds. All head-to-head trials had a small sample size that was lower than the reference sample size of 433.

Sixteen of 47 articles (excluding 2 abstracts and 2 reports on online trial registers) reported a sample size calculation (34%), which was often called a power analysis (Table 1). Fourteen were placebo-controlled trials and two head-to-head trials (Table 2). Table 2 shows, which input for these sample size calculations was reported. There were only three studies that reported sufficient information (Ballard et al., 2005; Mintzer et al., 2006; Schneider et al., 2006). Two studies reported an alpha that differed from 5% (2.5% and 7%). Eight studies reported a beta that differed from 20% and it varied between 1% and 15%. Except for the alpha of 2.5%, this input will yield higher sample sizes. Expected drop-out rates were reported in seven studies and varied between 10% and 30%.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Input for sample size calculations*.

There were seven placebo-controlled trials that postulated an expected treatment effect in terms of symptom reduction, four of which reflected a relative symptom reduction below 20%. The expected differences in relation to baseline means (relative symptom reduction) were: 10% (Ballard et al., 2005); 11% (Tariot et al., 2006); 12% (Brodaty et al., 2003); 14% (Street et al, 2000); 20% (Mintzer et al., 2006); 31% (De Deyn et al., 2004); 31% (Ballard et al., 2018). For a head-to-head trial, the expected relative risk reduction was 16% (Verhey et al., 2006).

Reported Treatment Effects In Relation To Sample Size

Table 3 presents the reported treatment effects in order of sample size per study group. A positive difference in response rate and negative difference in symptom reduction means that the investigated drug performed better than the control group. Six trials did not report what the effect of treatment on the primary outcome was: four studies were old, published between 1974–1983, but two were relatively new, published after 2000 (Smith et al., 1974; Rada and Kellner, 1976; Vergara et al., 1980; Spagnolo et al., 1983; Herz et al., 2002; Mulsant et al., 2004). Five placebo-controlled studies reported only p-values without effect sizes in the abstract (Katz et al., 1999; Brodaty et al., 2003; Deberdt et al., 2005; Mintzer et al., 2006; Zhong et al., 2007).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Results of randomized trials in order of group sample size.

Thirteen of 18 overpowered trials (72%) versus seven of 15 underpowered placebo-controlled trials (47%) yielded a statistically significant difference between the study groups in either response rate or symptom reduction. Two of 13 (15%) and four of seven (57%) of these treatment effects respectively were clinically relevant (difference in response rate > = 25%, or relative symptom reduction > = 20%). The statistically significant response rates were 10–22% and reported by studies with large sample sizes. The two studies with a difference in response rate of > = 25%, which is the difference deemed clinically relevant (Cohen, 2007), were underpowered and did not report a statistically significant result. In addition, large sample size trials reported statistically significant relative symptom reductions between 10% and 23%, and small sample size trials reported statistically significant relative symptom reductions varying between 17% and 55%.

Many placebo-controlled trials had more than one intervention group, adding up to a total of 54 individual comparisons. Thirteen of the 33 overpowered comparisons (39%) from 18 trials yielded a statistically significant treatment effect on either response rate or symptom reduction, versus seven of the 21 underpowered comparisons (33%) from 15 trials.

Five of 18 head-to-head trials reported a difference in response rate of 10%, the lower limit that we set for non-inferiority in our reference sample size calculation, and four a relative symptom reduction of 10%. Yet, none of these results were statistically significant.

The reported treatment effect was lower than the expected treatment effect in the 14 studies that presented an expected treatment effect in a sample size calculation, except in two studies (Street et al., 2000; Brodaty et al., 2003). The reported drop-out rates varied between 6% and 37% (not shown), which was higher than the expected drop-out rate in most studies.

Study Characteristics and Sample Size

Table 4 shows the mean sample size per comparison group by type of drug tested, trial duration, and type of funding. The mean sample size per study group was statistically significantly higher in placebo-controlled trials that tested an atypical antipsychotic drug (107.0) or both a conventional and an atypical drug (103.8) in comparison to placebo-controlled trials of conventional antipsychotics (34.4; p < .05). The mean sample size per study group was also statistically significantly higher in trials that lasted more than 6 weeks (109.2) compared to less than 6 weeks (28.9; p < .001), and that were commercially (100.3) versus non-commercially (18.1; p < .001) funded. Head-to-head-trials that tested atypical drugs only had a significantly larger mean sample size (46.3) than trials that tested conventional drugs (22.3; p < .05). Trial duration and commercial funding did not seem to be related to the sample size of head-to-head trials.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Mean sample size by study characteristic.

Discussion

We assessed the presence of large sample size fallacy in 51 antipsychotic trials in dementia. Most placebo-controlled trials of conventional antipsychotics had small sample size, i.e. smaller than the calculated reference sample size, but most trials of atypical antipsychotics had large sample sizes. All head-to-head trials had very small sample sizes. Only one third of trials reported a sample size calculation. Thirteen of 18 trials with large sample sizes (72%) reported a statistically significant treatment effect, of which two (15%) were clinically relevant. In contrast, seven of 15 placebo-controlled trials with small sample sizes (47%) yielded a statistically significant treatment effect, and four were clinically relevant (57%). None of the head-to-head trials reported a statistically significant treatment effect, even though some suggested non-inferiority.

Large Sample Size Fallacy

Sample sizes need to be large enough to guarantee a minimum level of discriminative power to detect a real treatment effect. Moreover, precision of an estimate increases with sample size. Studies based on small sample size may yield a non-statistically significant but clinically relevant treatment effect. On the other hand, studies based on large sample size—larger than necessary—may yield statistically significant but clinically insignificant treatment effects (Roggla and Fortunat, 2004; Chan et al., 2008). Large sample size fallacy occurs when such results are interpreted as relevant for medical practice (Lantz, 2013; Lin et al., 2013). Nevertheless, pharmaceutical companies and academic scholars benefit from statistically significant treatment results being interpreted as clinically relevant (Dickersin et al., 1992). The emphasis on statistical significance was confirmed by six trials in our review that did not report effect sizes, and five trials that reported just p-values in the abstract.

The sample sizes of trials testing atypical antipsychotics versus placebo, whether or not simultaneously with a conventional antipsychotic, were generally larger than necessary. These trials were commercially funded by the manufacturer of the atypical antipsychotic drugs. Only investigator-initiated trials were too small. The majority of large trials reported a statistically significant treatment effect, despite lack of clinical relevance, which confirms the presence of large sample size fallacy. The mean sample size was also higher when the study lasted longer than 6 weeks and was commercially funded, but this might be explained by the fact that placebo-controlled trials of atypical antipsychotics were generally longer and often industry-initiated. The chance of statistically significant findings was further enhanced by the use of multiple comparisons per study and multiple measurement scales per outcome in a number of the larger trials.

Many placebo-controlled trials of conventional antipsychotics had small sample sizes. Most were relatively old (published before 1990) and seemed to be investigator-initiated. Some of these trials reported clinically relevant results, but most were not statistically significant. That small placebo-controlled trials yielded statistically significant and clinically relevant effects relatively often might reflect publication bias.

Head-to-head trials had sample sizes that were (much) smaller than required, and these studies yielded non-statistically significant results that sometimes suggested a substantial effect. Even if we had set the limit for non-inferiority at 15%, the required sample would have been a lot higher than the sample sizes of the included studies were (346 without loss, and 385 with loss). It is unclear why these trials were so clearly underpowered. Perhaps, industry has little to gain from properly testing their own product against that of competitors. Non-commercial funds might not be interested in a trial with at least 2 × 433 patients to show that the tested drugs are non-inferior, even if patients might be quite willing to participate in a study that ensure treatment with an active drug.

Sample Size Requirements

It is generally agreed that a trial protocol and report should report a sample size calculation (CONSORT Group, 2010). Nevertheless, only a third of trials in our review reported a sample size calculation and just three were complete. Although some trials can be considered old, most were published in the 1990s or later when it had become common to report trial methods in detail. Sample size calculations are often not (completely) reported in randomized trials in other fields of research was well (Chan et al., 2008; Charles et al., 2009). One review found that articles about newer randomized controlled trials included sample size calculations more often, and showed positive results more often (76%) than older studies (55%) (Latif et al., 2011).

Some studies in our review reported a lower alpha (2.5%) or beta (5%) than is usual in sample size calculations (5% and 20% respectively). In addition, the MCID proposed in the sample size calculations seemed rather small: difference in response rates <25% in 3/6 trials, and in relative risk reduction of <20% in 4/7 trials. The lower the alpha, beta, and MCID, the higher the calculated sample size will be and hence the power to detect a statistically significant but not clinically relevant treatment effect. Moreover, even if the expected difference is equal to the MCID, a proportion of the patients will not have a clinically relevant effect on the individual level. On the other hand, the expected drop-out rate in the sample size calculations was mostly lower than the (mean) reported drop-out, and this would have led to a spuriously smaller calculated sample size. Real drop-out might have been high because trial duration was long on average. Most trials lasted more than a month, even though in clinical practice, antipsychotics usually show an effect within 2 weeks, four at the most. It has been estimated that up to 64% of trials with continuous outcomes are underpowered or overpowered because of imprecise input (Tavernier and Giraudeau, 2015).

Strengths and Limitations

To our knowledge determinants of sample size in trials testing antipsychotics for NPSs in dementia have not been studied previously. Our study showed that sample size calculations in the reports of these trials were missing on a large scale as was the correct interpretation of effect size. A limitation of our study is its focus on antipsychotic trials in dementia, which might be perceived as a small field of research. In addition, the interpretation of our results is limited by the possible presence of multiple testing. Many trials used multiple comparisons of either different drugs, different dosages, multiple outcomes, and sometimes multiple measurement instruments per outcome. Such multiple testing might reinforce the large sample size fallacy.

With our study, we do not want to suggest that large sample sizes should be avoided. It is important for clinical practice that study results are precise. Moreover, large sample sizes are very useful for identification of adverse effects. Small trials should not be avoided either, as long as they are published irrespective of results and available for pooling in meta-analyses.

The implication of our study is that researchers need to be encouraged to report and consider effect sizes in line with p-values to avoid the large sample size fallacy. Journals should probably mention this in their author instructions.

Conclusion

Placebo-controlled trials that tested atypical antipsychotics showed large sample size fallacy. Placebo-controlled trials of conventional antipsychotics and head-to-head trials had insufficient power to detect a real difference between the treatment groups. Sample size calculations in antipsychotic trials for dementia need to be reported adequately.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

TH, SJ, and HL extracted the data. TH and HL searched and selected the trials, performed the data analysis, and drafted the manuscript. SJ and SZ critically reviewed the manuscript and suggested revisions. HL designed the study.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphar.2019.01701/full#supplementary-material

References

Allain, H., Dautzenberg, P. H. J., Maurer, K., Schuck, S., Bonhomme, D., Gerard, D. (2000). Double blind study of tiapride versus haloperidol and placebo in agitation and aggressiveness in elderly patients with cognitive impairment. Psychopharmacol. (Berl). 148, 361–366. doi: 10.1007/s002130050064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ather, S. A., Shaw, S. H., Stoker, M. J. (1986). A comparison of chlormethiazole and thioridazine in agitated confusional states of the elderly. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 73, 81–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1986.tb10541.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auchus, A. P., Cheryl Bissey-Black, R. N. C. (1997). CLINICAL pilot study of haloperidol, fluoxetine, and placebo for agitation in alzheimer’s disease. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 9, 591–593. doi: 10.1176/jnp.9.4.591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auer, S. R., Monteiro, I. M., Reisberg, B. (1996). Behavioral symptoms in dementia: community-based research. in. Int. Psychogeriatr. 8(Suppl. 3), 363–366. doi: 10.1017/S1041610297003633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballard, C., Margallo-Lana, M., Juszczak, E., Douglas, S., Swann, A., Thomas, A., et al. (2005). Quetiapine and rivastigmine and cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease: randomised double blind placebo controlled trial. Br. Med. J. 330, 874–877. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38369.459988.8F

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballard, C., Banister, C., Khan, Z., Cummings, J., Demos, G., Coate, B., et al. (2018). Evaluation of the safety, tolerability, and efficacy of pimavanserin versus placebo in patients with Alzheimer’s disease psychosis: a phase 2, randomised, placebo-controlled, double-blind study. Lancet Neurol. 17, 213–222. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30039-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, R., Veith, R., Okimoto, J., Raskind, M., Gumbrecht, G. (1982). Efficacy of antipsychotic medications in behaviorally disturbed dementia patients. Am. J. Psychiatry. 139 (9), 1170–1174. doi: 10.1176/ajp.139.9.1170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brant, R. (2016). Inference for proportions: comparing two independent samples.

Google Scholar

Brodaty, H., Ames, D., Snowdon, J., Woodward, M., Kirwan, J., Clarnette, R., et al. (2003). A randomized placebo-controlled trial of risperidone for the treatment of aggression, agitation, and psychosis of dementia. J. Clin. Psychiatry 64, 134–143. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v64n0205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlyle, W., Ancill, R. J., Sheldon, L. (1993). Aggression in the demented patient: a double-blind study of loxapine versus haloperidol. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 8, 103–108. doi: 10.1097/00004850-199300820-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, A., Hrobjartsson, A., Jorgensen, K., Gotzsche, P., Altman, D. (2008). Discrepancies in sample size calculations and data analyses reported in randomised trials: comparison of publications with protocols. BMJ 337, 1–8. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a2299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, W. C., Lam, L. C., Choy, C. N., Leung, V. P., Li, S. W., Chiu, H. F. (2001). A double-blind randomised comparison of risperidone and haloperidol in the treatment of behavioural and psychological symptoms in Chinese dementia patients. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 16 (12), 1156–62. doi: 10.1002/gps.504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charles, P., Giraudeau, B., Dechartres, A., Baron, G., Ravaud, P. (2009). Reporting of sample size calculation in randomised controlled trials: review. BMJ 338, b1732. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b1732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (2007). A power primer. Tutor. Quant. Methods Psychol. 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CONSORT Group. (2010). Consolidating Standards of Reporting Trials [cited 2020 Jan 25]. Available from: http://www.consort-statement.org

Google Scholar

Cox Grad, S. (2009). Trial design (New Jersey: John Wiley. Hoboken).

Google Scholar

De Deyn, P. P., Rabheru, K., Rasmussen, A., Bocksberger, J. P., Dautzenberg, P. L. J., Eriksson, S., et al. (1999). A randomized trial of risperidone, placebo, and haloperidol for behavioral symptoms of dementia. Neurology 53, 946–946. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.5.946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Deyn, P. P., Carrasco, M. M., Deberdt, W., Jeandel, C., Hay, D. P., Feldman, P. D., et al. (2004). Olanzapine versus placebo in the treatment of psychosis with or without associated behavioral disturbances in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 19, 115–126. doi: 10.1002/gps.1032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Deyn, P., Jeste, D. V., Swanink, R., Kostic, D., Breder, C. (2005). Aripiprazole for the treatment of psychosis in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 25, 463–467. doi: 10.1097/01.jcp.0000178415.22309.8f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deberdt, W. G., Feldman, P. D., Young, C. A., Hay, D. P., Lehman, D. L., Degenhardt, E. K., et al. (2005). Comparison of olanzapine and risperidone in the treatment of psychosis and associated behavioral disturbances in patients with dementia. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 13, 722–730. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.8.722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devanand, D. P., Marder, K., Michaels, K. S., Sackeim, H. A., Bell, K., Sullivan, M. A., et al. (1998). A Randomized, placebo-controlled dose-comparison trial of haloperidol for psychosis and disruptive behaviors in Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Psychiatry 155, 11. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.11.1512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickersin, K., Min, Y. I., Meinert, C. L. (1992). Factors influencing publication of research results: follow-up of applications submitted to two institutional review boards. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 267, 374–378. doi: 10.1001/jama.1992.03480030052036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drouillard, N., Mithani, A., Chan, P. (2013). Therapeutic approaches in the management of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia in the elderly. BC Med. J. 55, 90–95.

Google Scholar

Finkel, S. I., Lyons, J. S., Anderson, R. L., Sherrell, K., Davis, J., Cohen-Mansfield, J., et al. (1995). A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of thiothixene in agitated, demented nursing home patients’. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 10, 129–136. doi: 10.1002/gps.930100208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontaine, C. S., Koch, K., Martin-Cook, K., Svetlik, D., Weiner, M. F., Hynan, L. S. (2003). A double-blind comparison of olanzapine versus risperidone in the acute treatment of dementia-related behavioral disturbances in extended care facilities. J. Clin. Psychiatry 64, 726–730. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v64n0617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Götestam, K. G., Ljunghall, S., Olsson, B. (1981). A double-blind comparison of the effects of haloperidol and cis (Z)-clopenthixol in senile dementia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 64, 46–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1981.tb06213.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garerl, P., Cotroneo, A., Lacava, R., Seminara, G., Marigliano, N., Loiacono, A., et al. (2004). Comparison of the efficacy of new and conventional antipsychotic drugs in the treatment of behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD). Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. Suppl. 9, 207–215. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2004.04.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutzmann, H., Kühl, K., Kanowksi, S., Kahn-Bolukl (1997). Measuring the Efficacy of Psychopharmacological Treatment of Psychomotors Restlessness in Dementia: Clinical Evaluation of Tiapride. Pharmacopsychiatry 30 (1), 6–11 doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, L., Bennet, J. (1962). The use of trifluoperazine in geriatric patients with chronic organic brain syndrome. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 10 (17), 596–601. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1962.tb00266.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herz, L., Volicer, L., Frankenburg, F., Colon, S., Kittur, S. (2002). A 6 week, double blind comparison of olanzapine, risperidone, and placebo for behavioural disturbance in Alzheimers disease. in The International College of Geriatric Psychoneuropharmacology. J. Clin. Psychiatry 63 (11), 1065.

Google Scholar

Howanitz, E., Wisotzek, I. (2001). Olanzapine versus placebo in the treatment of behavioural disturbances associated with vascular dementia. Poster presented at the 14th Annual Meeting of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry(San Francisco), 23–6.

Google Scholar

Howard, R., Phillips, P., Johnson, T., O’Brien, J., Sheehan, B., Lindesay, J., et al. (2011). Determining the minimum clinically important differences for outcomes in the DOMINO trial. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 26, 812–7. doi: 10.1002/gps.2607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hulshof, T., Zuidema, S., Ostelo, R., Luijendijk, H. (2015). The mortality risk of conventional antipsychotics in elderly patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. J. Am. Med. Dir Assoc. 16, 817–824. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2015.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hulshof, T. A., Zuidema, S. U., van Meer, P. J. K., Gispen-de Wied, C. C., Luijendijk, H. J. (2019). Baseline imbalances and clinical outcomes of atypical antipsychotics in dementia: a meta-epidemiological study of randomized trials. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 28, 1–10. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeste, D., Blazer, D., Casey, D., Meeks, T., Salzman, C., Schneider, L., et al. (2008). ACNP white paper: update on use of antipsychotic drugs in elderly persons with dementia. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 957–970. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, I. R., Jeste, V., Mintzer, J. E., Clyde, C., Napolitano, J., Brecher, M. (1999). Comparison of risperidone and placebo for psychosis and behavioral disturbances associated with dementia: a randomized, double-blind trial. J. Clin. Psychiatry 60 (2), 107–115. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v60n0207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lantz, B. (2013). The large sample size fallacy. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 27, 487–492. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2012.01052.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latif, L., Eduardo, J., Amadera, D., Pimentel, D., Pimentel, T., Fregni, F. (2011). Sample size calculation in physical medicine and rehabilitation: a systematic review of reporting, characteristics, and results in randomized controlled trials. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 92, 306–315. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, M., Henry C. Lucas, J., Galit, Shmueli (2013). Research commentary—too big to fail: large samples and the p-value problem. Inf. Syst. Res. 24, 906–917. doi: 10.1287/isre.2013.0480

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovett, W. C., Stokes, D. K., Taylor, L. B., Young, M. L., Free, S. M., Phelan, D. G. (1987). Management of behavioral symptoms in disturbed elderly patients: comparison of trifluoperazine and haloperidol. J. Clin. Psychiatry 48, 234–236.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mintzer, J., Greenspan, A., Caers, I., Hove, I., Kushner, S., Weiner, M., et al. (2006). Risperidone in the treatment of psychosis of alzheimer disease: results from a prospective clinical trial. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry, 14 (3), 280–291. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000194643.63245.8c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mintzer, J. E., Tune, L. E., Breder, C. D., Swanink, R., Marcus, R. N., Mcquade, R. D., et al. (2007). Aripiprazole for the treatment of psychoses in institutionalized patients with alzheimer dementia: a multicenter, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled assessment of three fixed doses. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 15 (11), 918–31. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181557b47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, R., Rickels, K. (1984). Loxapine in geriatric patients with chronic brain syndrome. Curr. Ther. Res. 35, 519–521.

Google Scholar

Mulsant, B. H., Pollock, B. G., Gharabawi, G. M., Bossie, C. A., Mao, L., Greenspan, A. J., et al. (2004). Correlates of anticholinergic activity in patients with dementia and psychosis treated with risperidone or olanzapine. J. Clin. Psychiatry 65, 1708–1714. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v65n1217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noordzij, M., Tripepi, G., Dekker, F. W., Zoccali, C., Tanck, M. W., Jager, K. J. (2010). Sample size calculations: Basic principles and common pitfalls. Nephrol. Dial. Transplant. 25, 1388–1393. doi: 10.1093/ndt/gfp732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otsuka Pharmaceutical (2017a). http://www.clinicaltrialsregister.eu. (EudraCT Number 2013-000504-41).

Google Scholar

Otsuka Pharmaceutical (2017b). http://www.clinicaltrialsregister.eu. (EudraCT Number 2013-000503-17).

Google Scholar

Paleacu, D., Barak, Y., Mirecky, I., Mazeh, D. (2008). Quetiapine treatmdnt for behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia in alzheimer’s disease patients: a 6-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 23, 393–400. doi: 10.1002/gps.1892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrie, W. M., Ban, T. A., Berney, S., Fujimori, M., Guy, W., Ragheb, M., et al. (1982). Loxapine in psychogeriatrics: a placebo- and standard-controlled clinical investigation. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2, 122–126. doi: 10.1097/00004714-198204000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollock, B. G., Mulsant, B. H., Rosen, J., Sweet, R. A., Mazumdar, S., Bharucha, A., et al. (2002). Article comparison of citalopram, perphenazine, and placebo for the acute treatment of psychosis and behavioral disturbances in hospitalized, demented patients. Am J Psychiatry. 159, 460–5. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rada, R. T., Kellner, R. (1976). Thiothixene in the treatment of geriatric patients with chronic organic brain syndrome. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc 24, 105–107. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1976.tb04280.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rainer, M., Haushofer, M., Pfolz, H., Struhal, C., Wick, W. (2007). Quetiapine versus risperidone in elderly patients with behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia: efficacy, safety and cognitive function. Eur. Psychiatry 22, 395–403. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roggla, G., Fortunat, S. (2004). Are cancer trials frequently overpowered? Br. Med. J. 328, 1463. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38118.685289.55

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, H. J. (1979). Double-blind comparison of haloperidol and thioridazine in geriatric outpatients. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 40 (1), 24–31.

Google Scholar

Satterlee, W. G, Reams, S. G, Burns, P. R, Hamilton, S, Tran, PV, Tollefson, G. D. (1995). A clinical update on olanzapine treatment in Schizophrenia and in elderly Alzheimers's disease patients. Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 534.

Google Scholar

Schipper, I., Weyzig, F. (2008). SOMO briefing paper on ethics in clinical trials: examples of unethical trials. Somo Brief. Pap. ethics clin9cal Res. 2008, 1–16. doi: 10.1002/micr ethics clin9cal Res.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, L., Dagerman, K., Insel, P. (2005). Risk of death with atypical antipsychotic drug treatment for dementia. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 294, 1934–1943. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.15.1934

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, L., Dagerman, K., Insel, P. (2006). Efficacy and adverse effects of atypical antipsychotics for dementia: meta-analysis of randomized, placebo-controlled trials. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 14, 191–210. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000200589.01396.6d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shabbir, S. H., Sanders, A. E. (2014). Clinical significance in dementia research: a review of the literature. Am. J. Alzheimers. Dis. Other Demen. 29, 492–497. doi: 10.1177/1533317514522539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheng, S., Gao, Z., Chen, M., Zhang, M., Liu, J. (2004). Risperidone vs haloperidol in treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia: a randomized, double blind trial. Chin. J. New Drugs Clin. Rem. 23 (6), 359–62.

Google Scholar

Smeets, C. H. W., Zuidema, S. U., Hulshof, T. A., Smalbrugge, M., Gerritsen, D. L., Koopmans, R. T. C. M., et al. (2018). Efficacy of antipsychotics in dementia depended on the definition of patients and outcomes: a meta-epidemiological study. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 101, 17–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, G. R., Taylor, C. W., Linkous, P. (1974). Haloperidol versus thioridazine for the treatment of psychogeriatric patients: a double-blind clinical trial. Psychosomatics 15, 134–138. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(74)71262-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spagnolo, C., Dall Asta, D., Iannnuccelli, M., Cucinotta, D., Passeri, M. (1983). A controlled double blind trial comparing etoperidone with thioridazine in the management of severe senile dementia. Drugs Exptl. Clin. Res. 4, 873.

Google Scholar

StataCorp (2017). Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. (College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC)

Google Scholar

Stotsky, B. (1984). Multicenter study comparing thioridazine with diazepam and placebo in elderly, nonpsychotic patients with emotional and behavioral disorders. Clin. Ther. 6, 546–559.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Street, J. S., Clark, W. S, Gannon, K. S, Cummings, J. L, Bymaster, F. P, Tamura, R. N, et al (2000). Olanzapine treatment of psychotic and behavioral symptoms in patients with alzheimer disease in nursing care facilities. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 968–976. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.10.968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Streim, J. E., Porsteinsson, A. P., Breder, C. D., Swanink, R., Marcus, R., Mcquade, R., et al. (2008). From the Section on Geriatric Psychiatry (Philadelphia Veteran Affairs Medical Center).

Google Scholar

Sugerman, A., Williams, B., Adlerstein, A. (1964). Haloperidol in the psychiatric disorders of old age. Am. J. Psychiatry 120, 1190–1192. doi: 10.1176/ajp.120.12.1190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, G., Feinn, R. (2012). Using effect size—or why the p value is not enough. J. Grad. Med. Educ. 4 (3), 279–282. doi: 10.4300/JGME-D-12-00156.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, X., Gao, Z., Feng, F. (2004). A randomized double-blind trial of haloperidol and risperidone for behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. Chin. J. Psychiatry 37, 156–159.

Google Scholar

Tariot, P. N., Schneider, L., Katz, I. R., Mintzer, J. E. (2006). Quetiapine treatment of psychosis associated with dementia: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 14, 767–776. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000196628.12010.35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tavernier, E., Giraudeau, B. (2015). Sample size calculation: inaccurate A priori assumptions for nuisance parameters can greatly affect the power of a randomized controlled trial. PloS One 10, 8–15. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teri, L., Logsdon, R. G., Peskind, E., Raskind, M., Weiner, M. F., Tractenberg, R. E., et al. (2000). Treatment of agitation in AD: a randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Neurology 55, 1271. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.9.1271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

US Food and Drug Administration (2013). Atypical Antipsychotic Drugs Information. 1.

Google Scholar

Vergara, L., Amin, M., Ban, T. (1980). Systematic clinical studies with clomacran III. a standard controlled clinical trial in geropsychiatric organic brain syndrome patients. Curr. Ther. Res. 27, 116–118.

Google Scholar

Verhey, F. R. J., Verkaaik, M., Lousberg, R. (2006). Olanzapine versus haloperidol in the treatment of agitation in elderly patients with dementia: results of a randomized controlled double-blind trial. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 21, 1–8. doi: 10.1159/000089136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wikipedia (2015). Typical antipsychotic. Wikipedia, Free Encycl., 1.

Google Scholar

World Health Organization. (2013). WHO Collaborating Centre for Drug Statistics Methodology. 1.

Google Scholar

Zhong, K., Tariot, P., Mintzer, J., Minkwitz, M., Devine, N. (2007). Quetiapine to treat agitation in dementia: a randomized, double-blind,placebo-controlled study. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 4, 81–93. doi: 10.2174/156720507779939805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuidema, S. U., Buursema, A. L., Gerritsen, M. G. J. M., Oosterwal, K. C., Smits, M. M. M., Koopmans, R. T. C. M., et al. (2011). Assessing neuropsychiatric symptoms in nursing home patients with dementia: reliability and reliable changeindex of the neuropsychiatric inventory and the Cohen-Mansfield agitation inventory. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 26, 127–134. doi: 10.1002/gps.2499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sample size, power, antipsychotics, dementia, placebo-controlled trials, head-to-head trials, meta-epidemiological study

Citation: Hulshof TA, Zuidema SU, Janus SIM and Luijendijk HJ (2020) Large Sample Size Fallacy in Trials About Antipsychotics for Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in Dementia. Front. Pharmacol. 10:1701. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.01701

Received: 23 August 2019; Accepted: 31 December 2019;
Published: 21 February 2020.

Edited by:

Bjorn Johansson, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Björn Lantz, Chalmers University of Technology, Sweden
Roger Clarnette, University of Western Australia, Australia

Copyright © 2020 Hulshof, Zuidema, Janus and Luijendijk. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hendrika J. Luijendijk, h.j.luijendijk@umcg.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.