Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychiatry, 17 April 2024
Sec. Behavioral and Psychiatric Genetics

No gene by stressful life events interaction on individual differences in adults’ self-control

  • 1Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Max Planck Research Group Biosocial – Biology, Social Disparities, and Development, Berlin, Germany
  • 2Department of Biological Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 3Department of Interdisciplinary Social Science, Universiteit Utrecht, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 4Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam University Medical Centres, Amsterdam, Netherlands

Background: Difficulty with self-control, or the ability to alter impulses and behavior in a goal-directed way, predicts interpersonal conflict, lower socioeconomic attainments, and more adverse health outcomes. Etiological understanding, and intervention for low self-control is, therefore, a public health goal. A prominent developmental theory proposes that individuals with high genetic propensity for low self-control that are also exposed to stressful environments may be most at-risk of low levels of self-control. Here we examine if polygenic measures associated with behaviors marked by low self-control interact with stressful life events in predicting self-control.

Methods: Leveraging molecular data from a large population-based Dutch sample (N = 7,090, Mage = 41.2) to test for effects of genetics (i.e., polygenic scores for ADHD and aggression), stressful life events (e.g., traffic accident, violent assault, financial problems), and a gene-by-stress interaction on self-control (measured with the ASEBA Self-Control Scale).

Results: Both genetics (β =.03 -.04, p <.001) and stressful life events (β = .11 -.14, p <.001) were associated with individual differences in self-control. We find no evidence of a gene-by-stressful life events interaction on individual differences in adults’ self-control.

Conclusion: Our findings are consistent with the notion that genetic influences and stressful life events exert largely independent effects on adult self-control. However, the small effect sizes of polygenic scores increases the likelihood of null results. Genetically-informed longitudinal research in large samples can further inform the etiology of individual differences in self-control from early childhood into later adulthood and its downstream implications for public health.

Introduction

Difficulties with self-control, including the ability to delay gratification, control impulses, and regulate emotions, predicts interpersonal conflict, criminal involvement and more adverse health outcomes. For example, adults with lower self-control are more likely to experience workplace and interpersonal conflicts, display unhealthier lifestyles, experience mental health problems, show faster biological aging, and a shorter health span than individuals with higher self-control (14). Understanding the etiology, early detection, and intervention of low self-control in adulthood is, therefore, a public health goal.

Family studies suggest that individual differences in self-control are influenced by both genetic and environmental effects (5, 6). Stressful life events, such as exposure to violence, interpersonal conflict, and economic hardships, are commonly associated with reduced self-control both in the short-term and the long-term by draining psychological reserves needed for self-control and developmentally by chronic physiological insults or impacted learning (710).

A prominent developmental theory proposes that individuals with high genetic propensity for low self-control that are also exposed to stressful environments may be most at-risk of low levels of self-control. The diathesis-stress model, proposes that stress may activate or increase a vulnerability (a diathesis) that transforms the potential of vulnerability into the actuality of psychopathology (11, 12). This can also be operationalized as gene-by-environment interaction (G x E), where different genotypes induce different sensitivity to the environment (13, 14). Extending this theoretical framework to self-control, it is hypothesized that the genetic vulnerability for low self-control interacts with the experience of life stressors to elevate the risk of developing self-control problems (15, 16, see Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Hypothesized relationships between genetic propensity for lower self-control and stressful life events on individual differences in self-control. (A) reflects a significant effect of stressful life events on self-control, with individuals having experienced more life stressors showing lower self-control. there are no effects of genetic propensity on self-control. (B) indicates a significant effect of genetic propensity on self-control, with those with a higher polygenic score for aggression or ADHD showing lower self-control. there are no effects of stressful life events on self-control. (C) reflects a significant effect both of stressful life events and genetic propensity on self-control. Those who experienced more life stressors show lower self-control, and those who have higher polygenic scores for ADHD or aggression show lower self-control. This does not indicate an interaction effect, as the joint effects of genetic and environmental risk factors are not significantly greater than the sum of the separate effects. (D) displays one potential type of gene-environment interaction as posited by diathesis stress model, where those who have both experienced more stressful life events and have a higher polygenic score for ADHD or aggression show elevated self-control problems. Other gene-environment interactions are possible, e.g. buffering effect which are not illustrated here.

Thus far, attempts to test this hypothesis mostly pertained to candidate gene studies and yield inconclusive results (17, 18). The latest advances in genomics have enabled a more comprehensive understanding of complex behaviors going beyond candidate genes and acknowledging their polygenic nature (19, 20). This is, amongst others, achieved through the use of polygenic scores, which aggregate the combined effects of various genetic variants linked to a specific trait (21). However, few studies so far have used this polygenic approach when investigating G x E effects on self-control. Leveraging data from a large population-based adult sample (N=7,090), we test for effects of genetics (i.e., polygenic scores of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and aggression), stressful life events (e.g., traffic accident, violent assault, financial problems), and a gene-by-stress interaction on adult self-control (see Figure 1).

Methods

Participants

This study is based on data of participants of the Netherlands Twin Register (NTR). The NTR was initiated in 1987, and collects data on the health and wellbeing of twins and their family members across the lifespan (22). We used an adult subsample of the NTR, with data available on self-control, experienced stressful life events, and genotype (N=7,090, Mage = 41.2, SDage = 15.4, 18-90 years old, 66% female, all European Ancestry). All participants provided written informed consent, and the data collection was approved by the Central Ethics Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects of the VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam.

Measures

Self-control

We used the ASEBA Self-Control Scale (ASCS) to assess self-control (23). This self-reported questionnaire consists of 8 items assessing varying dimensions of self-control based on selected items of the ADHD and Aggression subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (24). Items include: “I fail to finish things that I should do”, “I can’t concentrate, can’t pay attention for long”, I break rules at work or elsewhere”, “I am impulsive or act without thinking”, “I am easily distracted”, “I am sullen, or irritable”, “I have sudden changes in moods or feelings”, “I can have a hot temper” (see 23 for a thorough discussion of item selection). Items are measured on a 3-point scale (1= Not true, 2= Somewhat or Sometimes True, 3= Very True or Often True). We created a sum score across these 8 items, so that higher scores reflect lower self-control. The scale shows good inter-rater and test-retest reliability (23), and has been applied across several other cohorts (25, 26).

Stressful life events

We used the Dutch Life Event Scale (“Schokverwerkings Inventarisatie Lijst”, 27) to assess the experience of stressful life events across the lifespan. This measure has been used earlier in the same data to assess the effect of life stressors on psychopathology within the Netherlands Twin Register (28, 29). It consisted of 11 items about stressful life events including having experienced traffic accident, violent assault, sexual assault, robbery, serious illness or injury of self or a significant other (e.g. partner, child, parent), death of a significant other, dismissal from work, financial problems, and relationship problems with a close partner. Response categories were 1= Not experienced, 2= Experienced Less than a year ago, 3= Experienced 1-5 years ago, 4= Experienced longer than 5 years ago. We considered both lifetime and past year stressful experiences respectively to understand the cumulative impact of stress and the acute influence of recent events on individual’s self-control problems. As such, we created two continuous scores; one sum score for stressful life events experienced in the previous year (SLE previous year) and one sum score for experience of stressful life events across the lifespan (SLE lifetime).

Polygenic scores

Genome Wide Association Studies (GWAS) use genetic data of large samples to detect the cumulative effects of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) on an outcome of interest. Using such summary statistics of GWAS allows to create an individual polygenic score for all genotyped participants, weighing the predictive value of each individual SNPs and generating an overall predictive polygenic score (21).

While there have been efforts to identify SNPs associated with self-control related traits, the summary results from GWASs of impulsivity and delay of gratification are not publicly available (30, 31). We therefore created polygenic scores based on traits that reflect low self-control behaviors, namely the summary results from GWASs of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD, 32) and aggression (33). ADHD taps into struggles with impulsivity and difficulty regulating one’s emotions and behavior (34, 35). Similarly, aggression taps into issues regulating impulses and emotions that ultimately lead to aggressive behaviors (35, 36). Both the PGI for attention problems and aggression problems and our measure of self-control indexes impulsive and emotional aspects of self-control reflected in questions on aggression and attention problems subscale (23, 32, 33).

Preprocessing of the genetic data is described in Supplementary Material 1. As the genotyped participants of the NTR were part of the ADHD and aggression GWA studies, we used summary statistics where the NTR participants were left out to avoid an overestimation of the effects. LD-pred (v0.9) was used to compute the polygenic scores in the target sample, accounting for linkage disequilibrium (LD) among SNPs by using the LD structure of a set of well-imputed variants in a selection of unrelated individuals in the NTR sample (37). Both selections were performed on the genotype data described above. The fraction of causal SNPs was set at 0.50 as this was previously shown to perform optimally in the NTR population (33). Setting it to infinity showed similar results.

Statistical analyses

Regression analyses were carried out with self-control as the dependent variable, using generalized estimation equations (GEE) in R clustering on family membership to adjust for dependency of the observations (version 4.4.2, 38, using package “geepack”, 39). We added age, age2, sex, 10 principal components, and array as covariates in the regression analyses. Scores were log-transformed to account for skew. We included 10 principal components of genetic similarity to ancestral reference groups to account for population stratification and/or cryptic relatedness. These 10 principle components refer to the linear combinations of genotypes of SNPs, which, when included, reduce the bias of confounding (40). In case of significant interaction effect, we would include interaction-terms of covariates x genes (e.g., age x polygenic score) and covariates x environment (e.g., age x stressful life event) to eliminate effects of interactions between covariates and the variables of interest (41). We conducted the analyses separately for having experienced stressful life events less than a year ago (SLE previous year) and having experiences stressful events across the life time (SLE lifetime). We also conducted the analyses separately for the polygenic score of ADHD and the polygenic score of aggression. We applied Bonferroni-type adjustment to correct for inflated Type 1 error due to multiple testing (taking alpha level of 0.05/8 tests = .006). The results section presents standardized beta estimates, their 95% confidence interval, and p-values of the main and interaction effects. Descriptives can be found in Supplementary Material 2.

Results

We first examined whether genetic influences were associated with individual differences in self-control by regressing self-control on polygenic score and covariates. We found that both the polygenic score for ADHD and aggression were associated with lower self-control, but effect sizes were very small (PGSADHD β =.03, SD=.01, 95% CI.02 -.06, p <.001; PGSaggression β = .04, SD=.01, 95% CI.02 -.07, p <.001). The PGI for ADHD and aggression were modestly correlated (r=.20, p<.001), which is in line with the notion that self-control is a broad construct influenced by a variety of underlying genetic factors (35).

Second, we examined whether stressful life events were associated with self-control by regressing self-control on life events and covariates. We found that having experienced more stressful life events (SLE) was associated with lower self-control (SLE last year: β = .11, SD=.01, 95% CI.09 -.15, p <.001; SLE lifetime: β = .14, SD=.01, 95% CI.11 -.16, p <.001).

Third, we examined whether there was a gene by stressful life events interaction on self-control by regressing self-control on polygenic score, stressful life events, polygenic score by stressful life events interaction, and covariates. We did not find evidence for a gene-environment interaction (see Table 1; Figure 2).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Coefficients of the interaction effects of stressful life events (SLE) and polygenic scores (PGS) associated with self-control.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Association between PGS for self-control problems and self-control. Stressful life events represent stressful life events experienced across the lifetime. For illustration purposes, participants were stratified into a high stress and low stress group by mean split of stressful life events (Mstressfull life events= 2.5). The lines for the two groups are approximately parallel and resemble those in Panel C in Figure 1, indicating there is a main effect of stressful life events and genetic propensity on self-control problems, but no significant interaction effect between the two.

Discussion

The diathesis-stress theory hypothesizes that peoples’ genetic propensity interacts with environmental stressors as a shaping factor for the development of behavioral problems (11, 12). Using data from a large adult population-based sample (N=7,090, Mage = 41.2, SDage = 15.4, 66% female), we investigated this hypothesis for self-control. We found that participants who had experienced more life stressors showed lower levels of self-control. We also found that participants with higher genetic propensity for self-control problems (using the polygenic score for ADHD and aggression, respectively), showed lower levels of self-control. However, we did not find a significant gene-by-environment (G x E) interaction effect: The joint effects of genetic and environmental risk factors were not significantly greater than the sum of the separate effects.

We confirm previous findings that individual differences in self-control are related to both genetic variation and stressful life events (6). We extend these findings by showing that polygenic scores based on large GWAS for ADHD and aggression are associated with low self-control. This suggest that these scores are beneficial for future research intended to explore or adjust for genetic influences on individual variations in self-control. Additionally, in line with earlier research (8, 10, 42), we find that having experienced life stressors in the past year or across the lifetime are both associated with lower levels of self-control in adulthood.

Our findings are consistent with the notion that genetic influences and stressful life events exert largely independent effects on adult self-control. While some studies do find significant G x E effects (43, 44), other studies fail to detect G x E moderation effects on self-control related traits (e.g. 4548). This and other research (e.g., 49) suggest that consistently identifying gene-environment (G x E) interactions for the prediction of complex traits, which are shaped by numerous genetic and environmental factors, is challenging. In the future, it is recommended to rethink how to select and measure relevant environmental measures (49) or to create more fine-tuned polygenic scores for example by identify genetic variants associated with variation in the outcome rather than the average level (49, 50) or to conduct genome-wide by environment interaction studies (GWEIS) on the genomic level to better capture gene-environment interactions (51).

The results of the current study should be interpreted with some limitations in mind. First, the small effect sizes of the polygenic scores increase the likelihood of the G x E null results. We utilized polygenic scores for ADHD and aggression, which do not completely represent the broader dimensions of self-controlling capacities (35, 52) and are likely to partly explain the low effect sizes as found in other studies (e.g., 4). One future recommendation would be to apply Genomic Structural Equation Modeling (53) to model multivariate genetic associations among self-control related phenotypes to generate a more encompassing polygenic score of self-control (e.g. also including GWAS results of impulsive personality, delay discounting, and executive functioning, 30, 31, 54).

Second, the timing of gene-environment interactions can be developmentally specific, and G x E interactions might have more noticeable or lasting impacts during early childhood compared to adulthood. Future research is recommended to replicate our findings in a pediatric sample.

Lastly, our life events measure only included stressful life events (e.g., theft, illness, financial strain etc.) and not positive life events (e.g., marriage, birth of a child) which can also impact people’s self-control levels (55, 56). We additionally weighted all stressful life events to be the same, although some stressful life events could be more strongly associated with low self-control than others. Future research taking more fine-grained approach to assessing life events could provide interesting avenues.

To conclude, this study sought to understand individual differences in self-control by examining the interplay between genetic factors and life stressors. The findings showed that while genetic scores for ADHD and aggression and life stress independently predicted self-control levels, their combined influence did not significantly exceed their individual effects, suggesting no gene-environment interaction. The results highlight the importance of both genetics and life stress in understanding self-control, and pointing to the need for further research to unravel their complex relationship.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: genetic data. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to https://tweelingenregister.vu.nl/.

Ethics statement

Data collection complied with current APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct and was approved by the medical ethical review committee of the VU Medical Center Amsterdam (NTR25052007). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

YW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LR: Writing – review & editing. LL: Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. RP: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. JH: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. CF: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MB: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. MB is funded by a European Research Council Consolidator Grant (WELL-BEING 771057, M. Bartels) and by an NWO VICI grant (VI.C.211.054).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1388264/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Buyukcan-Tetik A, Finkenauer C, Siersema M, Vander Heyden K, Krabbendam L. Social relations model analyses of perceived self-control and trust in families. J Marriage Family. (2015) 77:209–23. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. De Ridder DT, Lensvelt-Mulders G, Finkenauer C, Stok FM, Baumeister RF. Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. (2012) 16(1):76–99.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

3. Vazsonyi AT, Mikuška J, Kelley EL. It’s time: A meta-analysis on the self-control-deviance link. J Criminal Justice. (2017) 48:48–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Willems YE, deSteiguer A, Tanksley PT, Vinnik L, Fraemke D, Okbay A, et al. Self-control is associated with health-relevant disparities in buccal DNA-methylation measures of biological aging in older adults. Clin Epigenet. (2024) 16(1):22. doi: 10.1186/s13148-024-01637-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Beaver KM, Connolly EJ, Schwartz JA, Al-Ghamdi MS, Kobeisy AN. Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change in levels of self-control. J Criminal Justice. (2013) 41:300–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2013.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Willems YE, Boesen N, Li J, Finkenauer C, Bartels M. The heritability of self-control: A meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 100:324–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Davies PT, Cummings EM. Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychol Bull. (1994) 116:387–411. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Finkenauer C, Buyukcan-Tetik A, Baumeister RF, Schoemaker K, Bartels M, Vohs KD. Out of control: Identifying the role of self-control strength in family violence. Curr Dir psychol Sci. (2015) 24:261–6. doi: 10.1177/0963721415570730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Ellis BJ, Bianchi J, Griskevicius V, Frankenhuis WE. Beyond risk and protective factors: an adaptation-based approach to resilience. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2017) 12:561–87. doi: 10.1177/1745691617693054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Choung Y, Pak T-Y. More than just a bad day? Traumatic life events and self-control in old age. PloS One. (2023) 18:e0266312. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Monroe SM, Simons AD. Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: Implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull. (1991) 110:406–25. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.110.3.406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull. (2009) 135:885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Kendler KS, Eaves LJ. Models for the joint effect of genotype and environment on liability to psychiatric illness. Am J Psychiatry. (1986) 143(3):279–89.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

14. Plomin R, DeFries JC, Loehlin JC. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psycho. Bull. (1977) 84(2):309.

Google Scholar

15. Kochanska G, Philibert RA, Barry RA. Interplay of genes and early mother-child relationship in the development of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. In J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2009) 50:1331–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02050.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Belsky J, Beaver KM. Cumulative-genetic plasticity, parenting and adolescent self-regulation. In J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2011) 52:619–26. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2011.52.issue-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Beaver KM, DeLisi M, Vaughn MG, Wright JP. The intersection of genes and neuropsychological deficits in the prediction of adolescent delinquency and low self-control. Int J Offender Ther Criminology. (2010) 54:22–42. doi: 10.1177/0306624X08325349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Boisvert D, Wells J, Armstrong TA, Lewis RH. Serotonin and self-control: A genetically moderated stress sensitization effect. J Criminal Justice. (2018) 56:98–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2017.07.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Duncan LE, Keller MC. A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. (2011) 168:1041–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. van de Weijer MP, Pelt DHM, de Vries LP, Baselmans BML, Bartels M. A re-evaluation of candidate gene studies for well-being in light of genome-wide evidence. J Happiness Stud. (2022) 23:3031–53. doi: 10.1007/s10902-022-00538-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Dudbridge F. Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PloS Genet. (2013) 9:e1003348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ligthart L, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Kevenaar ST, de Zeeuw E, van Bergen E, Bruins S, et al. The Netherlands twin register: longitudinal research based on twin and twin-family designs. Twin Res Hum Genet. (2019) 22:623–36. doi: 10.1017/thg.2019.93

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Willems YE, Dolan CV, van Beijsterveldt CEM, de Zeeuw EL, Boomsma DI, Bartels M, et al. Genetic and environmental influences on self-control: assessing self-control with the ASEBA self-control scale. Behav Genet. (2018) 48:135–46. doi: 10.1007/s10519-018-9887-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. (1991). Child behavior checklist. Burlington (vt). 7:371–92.

Google Scholar

25. Zondervan-Zwijnenburg MAJ, Richards JS, Kevenaar ST, Becht AI, Hoijtink HJA, Oldehinkel AJ, et al. Robust longitudinal multi-cohort results: The development of self-control during adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2020) 45:100817. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kevenaar ST, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI, van Bergen E. Self-control and grit are associated with school performance mainly because of shared genetic effects. JCPP Adv. (2023) 3:e12159. doi: 10.1002/jcv2.12159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. van der Ploeg E, Mooren TTM, Kleber RJ, van der Velden PG, Brom D. Construct validation of the dutch version of the impact of event scale. Psychol Assess. (2004) 16:16–26. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Middeldorp CM, de Geus EJC, Willemsen G, Hottenga J-J, Slagboom PE, Boomsma DI. The serotonin transporter gene length polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and life events: no evidence for an interaction effect on neuroticism and anxious depressive symptoms. Twin Res Hum Genet. (2010) 13:544–9. doi: 10.1375/twin.13.6.544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Peyrot WJ, Middeldorp CM, Jansen R, Smit JH, Geus de, Eco. JC, et al. Strong effects of environmental factors on prevalence and course of major depressive disorder are not moderated by 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms in a large Dutch sample. J Affect Disord. (2013) 146:91–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.08.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Sanchez-Roige S, Fontanillas P, Elson SL, 23andMe Research Team, Pandit A, Schmidt EM, et al. Genome-wide association study of delay discounting in 23,217 adult research participants of European ancestry. Nat Neurosci. (2018) 21:16–8. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0032-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Sanchez-Roige S, Jennings MV, Thorpe HHA, Mallari JE, van der Werf LC, Bianchi SB, et al. CADM2 is implicated in impulsive personality and numerous other traits by genome- and phenome-wide association studies in humans and mice. Trans Psychiatry. (2023) 13:167. doi: 10.1038/s41398-023-02453-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Demontis D, Walters GB, Athanasiadis G, Walters R, Therrien K, Nielsen TT, et al. Genome-wide analyses of ADHD identify 27 risk loci, refine the genetic architecture and implicate several cognitive domains. Nat Genet. (2023) 55(2):198–208. doi: 10.1038/s41588-022-01285-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Ip HF, van der Laan CM, Krapohl EML, Brikell I, Sánchez-Mora C, Nolte IM, et al. Genetic association study of childhood aggression across raters, instruments, and age. Trans Psychiatry. (2021) 11(1):413–20. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01480-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. (1997) 121:65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Nigg JT. Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disciplines. (2017) 58:361–83. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Denson TF, DeWall CN, Finkel EJ. Self-control and aggression. Curr Dir psychol Sci. (2012) 21:20–5. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Vilhjálmsson BJ, Yang J, Finucane HK, Gusev A, Lindström S, Ripke S, et al. Modeling linkage disequilibrium increases accuracy of polygenic risk scores. Am J Hum Genet. (2015) 97:576–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2015.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, PBC. (2020). Available at: http://www.rstudio.com/.

Google Scholar

39. Højsgaard S, Halekoh U, Yan J. The R package geepack for generalized estimating equations. J Stat Software. (2006) 15:1–11. doi: 10.18637/jss.v015.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Abdellaoui A, Hottenga J-J, de Knijff P, Nivard MG, Xiao X, Scheet P, et al. Population structure, migration, and diversifying selection in the Netherlands. Eur J Hum Genet. (2013) 21:1277–85. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2013.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Keller MC. Gene × Environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: the problem and the (Simple) solution. Biol Psychiatry. (2014) 75:18–24. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2013.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Duckworth AL, Kim B, Tsukayama E. Life stress impairs self-control in early adolescence. Front Psychol. (2012) 3:608. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Musci RJ, Bettencourt AF, Sisto D, Maher B, Masyn K, Ialongo NS. Violence exposure in an urban city: A GxE interaction with aggressive and impulsive behaviors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2019) 60:72–81. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Plomin R, Gidziela A, Malanchini M, von Stumm S. Gene–environment interaction using polygenic scores: Do polygenic scores for psychopathology moderate predictions from environmental risk to behavior problems? Dev Psychopathol. (2022) 34:1816–26. doi: 10.1017/S0954579422000931

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Allegrini AG, Karhunen V, Coleman JRI, Selzam S, Rimfeld K, Stumm S, et al. Multivariable G-E interplay in the prediction of educational achievement. PloS Genet. (2020) 16:e1009153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1009153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. He Q, Li JJ. A gene-environment interaction study of polygenic scores and maltreatment on childhood ADHD. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. (2022) 50:309–19. doi: 10.1007/s10802-021-00873-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kandaswamy R, Allegrini A, Nancarrow AF, Cave SN, Plomin R, von Stumm S. Predicting alcohol use from genome-wide polygenic scores, environmental factors, and their interactions in young adulthood. Psychosomatic Med. (2022) 84:244. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000001005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. von Stumm S, Kandaswamy R, Maxwell J. Gene-environment interplay in early life cognitive development. Intelligence. (2023) 98:101748. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2023.101748

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Domingue BW, Trejo S, Armstrong-Carter E, Tucker-Drob EM. Interactions between polygenic scores and environments: methodological and conceptual challenges. Sociological Sci. (2020) 7:465–86. doi: 10.15195/v7.a19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Young AI, Wauthier FL, Donnelly P. Identifying loci affecting trait variability and detecting interactions in genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet. (2018) 50(11):1608–14. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0225-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Wang H, Zhang F, Zeng J, Wu Y, Kemper KE, Xue A, et al. Genotype-by-environment interactions inferred from genetic effects on phenotypic variability in the UK Biobank. Sci Adv. (2019) 5:eaaw3538. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aaw3538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Duckworth AL, Kern ML. A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. J Res Pers. (2011) 45:259–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Grotzinger AD, Rhemtulla M, de Vlaming R, Ritchie SJ, Mallard TT, Hill WD, et al. Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nat Hum Behav. (2019) 3(5):513–25. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0566-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hatoum AS, Morrison CL, Mitchell EC, Lam M, Benca-Bachman CE, Reineberg AE, et al. Genome-wide association study shows that executive functioning is influenced by GABAergic processes and is a neurocognitive genetic correlate of psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. (2023) 93:59–70. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.06.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. van Scheppingen MA, Denissen JJA, Bleidorn W. Stability and change in self-control during the transition to parenthood. Eur J Pers. (2018) 32:690–704. doi: 10.1002/per.2172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Pronk TM, Buyukcan-Tetik A, Iliás MMAH, Finkenauer C. Marriage as a training ground: Examining change in self-control and forgiveness over the first 4 years of marriage. J Soc Pers Relat. (2019) 36:109–30. doi: 10.1177/0265407517721065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-control, genetics, stress, gene-environment interaction, polygenic scores

Citation: Willems YE, Raffington L, Ligthart L, Pool R, Hottenga JJ, Finkenauer C and Bartels M (2024) No gene by stressful life events interaction on individual differences in adults’ self-control. Front. Psychiatry 15:1388264. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1388264

Received: 19 February 2024; Accepted: 03 April 2024;
Published: 17 April 2024.

Edited by:

Soo H. Rhee, University of Colorado Boulder, United States

Reviewed by:

Gianna Rea-Sandin, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, United States
Daniel Gustavson, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, United States

Copyright © 2024 Willems, Raffington, Ligthart, Pool, Hottenga, Finkenauer and Bartels. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yayouk Eva Willems, willems@mpib-berlin.mpg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.