Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 16 March 2018
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Narrative Discourse in Young and Older Adults: Behavioral and NIRS Analyses

\r\nCharles-Olivier Martin,*Charles-Olivier Martin1,2*Stphanie Pontbriand-Drolet,Stéphanie Pontbriand-Drolet1,2Valrie Daoust,Valérie Daoust1,2Eric Yamga,Eric Yamga1,2Mahnoush Amiri,Mahnoush Amiri2,3Lilian C. HübnerLilian C. Hübner4Bernadette Ska,Bernadette Ska1,2
  • 1Faculty of Medicine, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 2Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3Génie Biomédical, École Polytechnique de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 4Departamento de Linguistica, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul (PUCRS), Porto Alegre, Brazil

Discourse comprehension is at the core of communication capabilities, making it an important component of elderly populations’ quality of life. The aim of this study is to evaluate changes in discourse comprehension and the underlying brain activity. Thirty-six participants read short stories and answered related probes in three conditions: micropropositions, macropropositions and situation models. Using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS), the variation in oxyhemoglobin (HbO2) and deoxyhemoglobin (HbR) concentrations was assessed throughout the task. The results revealed that the older adults performed with equivalent accuracy to the young ones at the macroproposition level of discourse comprehension, but were less accurate at the microproposition and situation model levels. Similar to what is described in the compensation-related utilization of neural circuits hypothesis (CRUNCH) model, older participants tended to have greater activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex while reading in all conditions. Although it did not enable them to perform similarly to younger participants in all conditions, this over-activation could be interpreted as a compensation mechanism.

Introduction

Comprehension is a basic component of communication. Being able to extract the gist of discourse, while reading a text or listening to a speech, and relate it to our own knowledge of the world is essential for appropriate behavior and interaction in society. Thus, understanding the changes in discourse comprehension capacity and in the underlying brain changes during aging is crucial to maintain the elderly population’s quality of life.

The aim of this study is to evaluate changes in discourse comprehension and the underlying brain activity. The article is structured in four sections reflecting the way the study was organized in order to reach this aim: (1) the model allowing the discourse analysis on which most of studies are based; (2) results of studies on discourse comprehension, neural substrate and aging; (3) models of cerebral plasticity in aging; (4) near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) technique used to evaluate the cerebral activity during the reading task.

The construction-integration model (Kintsch and Van Dijk, 1978; Kintsch, 1988; Lebreton, 2012) describes comprehension as a progressive construction, through many cycles, in which successive propositional networks are elaborated and integrated into the networks built in prior cycles to form a cohesive whole. This model lists four levels of representation in the comprehension process that interact back and forth through the cycles of comprehension. Those levels will be described below.

First, the surface form refers to the linguistic components of discourse such as vocabulary and grammar without regard to its contents (Ska and Duong, 2005; Chesneau et al., 2007b).

Second, the textbase corresponds to the propositions of a text joined together in a structured, cohesive network (Kintsch and Van Dijk, 1978). Two types of propositions and structures can be distinguished in the first model developed by Kintsch: (a) Micropropositions and microstructure contain all the propositions of the discourse in a hierarchical network (Kintsch and Van Dijk, 1978; Lebreton, 2012). Thus, the microstructure comprises all the information and details of the discourse. (b) Macropropositions and macrostructure are derived from the microstructure by deleting the unneeded propositions and generalizing some groups of propositions, forming the gist of the discourse (Kintsch and Van Dijk, 1978). The macrostructure reduces and organizes the information in the textbase without losing the significance of the discourse (Denhière and Baudet, 1992).

Third, in the updated construction-integration model, the situation model tends to overlap the macrostructure. The situation model is a mental representation of the discourse integrating the individual’s knowledge (Kintsch, 1988; Lebreton, 2012). Unlike the textbase, the situation model does not represent the text itself but the subjects and events described in it (Radvansky et al., 2001). A situation model is a situation containing events, objects and characters delimited in time and space. An event taking place in a different time or space would be considered part of a new situation model (Radvansky and Dijkstra, 2007). The situation model results from inferencing capacity, which is essential to comprehension, because it associates with the text the necessary information from the knowledge of an individual and integrates the various pieces of information in the discourse (Perrig and Kintsch, 1985). In our study, the gist of information portion of the mental representation is classified as the macrostructure, and the inferences needed to properly build the mental representation are classified as the situation model.

Finally, the superstructure constitutes a framework for the other levels corresponding to the type of discourse presented (e.g., narrative or procedural). This framework allows one to anticipate and orient information processing depending on the type of discourse (Ska and Duong, 2005). This level will not be considered further here, because all texts in this study are narratives. Thus, this study will focus only on the textbase and the situation model.

The processes needed to accomplish discourse comprehension involve several cognitive functions and brain regions. Studies have shown that inhibition (Kintsch, 1988) and working memory (Radvansky and Copeland, 2004; Chesneau et al., 2007a) are involved at the textbase level, while inhibition (Radvansky, 1999b), long-term working memory (Ericsson and Kintsch, 1995) and semantic memory play a role at the situation model level (Radvansky and Dijkstra, 2007). However, the role of working memory in discourse comprehension is controversial. Some studies come to the conclusion that working memory does not influence the discourse comprehension (e.g., Evans et al., 2015) while others argue the opposite (Noh and Stine-Morrow, 2009; Borella et al., 2011; Payne and Stine-Morrow, 2017). The methodology and tasks used in these studies may explain these divergent results.

Although discourse comprehension depends mainly on the left hemisphere, most of the relevant processes need the contribution of both hemispheres (Perfetti and Frishkoff, 2008). Story comprehension entails a network of frontal, temporal and cingulate areas, which also are association areas (Mar, 2004). The anterior temporal lobes and left middle temporal gyrus could be involved in the semantic integration and interpretation of text in discourse as they are only activated while listening or reading meaningful texts or sentences (Mazoyer et al., 1993; Ferstl et al., 2008; Perfetti and Frishkoff, 2008; Bowman and Dennis, 2015; Zhuang et al., 2016). The prefrontal cortex, especially the superior dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, may be involved in the integration of one’s knowledge or when inferences from earlier parts of the discourse are needed, thus contributing to the maintenance of global coherence of texts (Schmalhofer and Perfetti, 2007; Perfetti and Frishkoff, 2008; Miotto et al., 2014; Bowman and Dennis, 2015).

Aging is known to have an impact on many cognitive functions, some of which, like working memory, are involved in discourse comprehension (Brébion, 2003; DeDe et al., 2004; Payne and Stine-Morrow, 2017). Among other things, elderly participants seem to rely more on the gist of the text and the situation model while performing comprehension tasks, while younger participants rely more on the textbase and microstructure (Radvansky et al., 2001; Ferstl, 2006; Radvansky and Dijkstra, 2007; Magliano et al., 2012; Davis et al., 2015; Zhuang et al., 2016). On the other hand, it seems that the characteristics of a text, especially its semantic load, highly influence the capacities of older adults to recall the microstructure and macrostructure. In fact, older adults have more problems recalling the microstructure of texts containing a lot of propositions (high semantic load) than texts with few propositions (low semantic load). Conversely, they have more problems recalling the macrostructure of texts with a low semantic load than texts with a high semantic load (Ferstl, 2006; Chesneau et al., 2007a). These results highlight the complex influence of working memory on the changes in discourse comprehension during aging, as it seems to be easier to handle and extract the essential information of a text when it has a high semantic load, even if each propositions become harder to recall in itself.

Several models have sought to explain the brain plasticity mechanisms that apply during aging in relation to cognitive decline. One model in particular is relevant to our study.

The compensation-related utilization of neural circuits hypothesis (CRUNCH) specifies that the over-activation of brain regions often observed in elderly participants is a compensation mechanism (Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008; Fu et al., 2017). This over-activation may entail activation of more regions than in younger people or greater activation of the region normally dedicated to the task. Sometime, an interhemispheric or an intrahemispheric reorganization seems to take place such as, respectively, in the HAROLD (Cabeza, 2002) and PASA models (Davis et al., 2008). Although the goal of those mechanisms is to maintain performance, they are not always sufficient (Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex often plays a compensatory role (Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008; Cappell et al., 2010).

Many studies investigated the influence of aging on discourse comprehension (e.g., Stine-Morrow et al., 2001, 2008; De Beni et al., 2003; Hannon and Daneman, 2009; McGinnis, 2009; Noh and Stine-Morrow, 2009; Wright et al., 2011; Mund et al., 2012). Other studies used cerebral imagery approaches to elicit neural contribution to language and semantic treatment (e.g., Cahana-Amitay and Albert, 2014; Shafto, 2015; Wang et al., 2015; Cohen et al., 2016). One previous study combined both and investigated cerebral plasticity in a discourse comprehension task. It described both an intrahemispheric and an interhemispheric reorganization in the elderly group (Scherer et al., 2012). Nonetheless, that study only collected data from the posterior part of the frontal lobes and a part of the temporal lobes and used a small sample size. Another study (Martin et al., 2017) compared high and low educated old participants in a task of discourse comprehension and did not find differences between the two groups on the behavioral and the cerebral activity. Thus, the global picture of cerebral plasticity in discourse comprehension is still incomplete.

NIRS technique uses near-infrared light to examine changes in the concentration of susceptible chromophores in this spectral window being oxyhemoglobin (HbO2) and deoxyhemoglobin (HbR; Jöbsis, 1977). Since those chromophores partially absorb near-infrared light, in the range of 650–950 nm, a change in their concentration will alter the intensity of light passing through the biological tissue (Strangman et al., 2002; Kocsis et al., 2006; Gratton and Fabiani, 2007). Brain activity induces a local hemodynamic response, which is characterized by an increase in HbO2 concentration coupled with a decrease in HbR concentration and is followed by a return to normal (Chance et al., 1993; Hoshi, 2007; Racz et al., 2017). This light-medium interaction allows NIRS to be used for assessing hemodynamic changes and thus estimating local brain activity as a result.

This technique is known to have a temporal resolution around 10 ms and a spatial resolution of 1–3 cm, which gives a better temporal resolution but poorer spatial resolution comparing to the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and better spatial resolution but poorer temporal resolution than electroencephalography (EEG; Strangman et al., 2002; Rodden and Stemmer, 2008). It’s totally non-invasive nature facilitates studies with babies, young children and elderly people (Strangman et al., 2002). For purposes of our study, NIRS is particularly useful because it allows one to create a more natural environment for reading, namely a sitting position without background noise, unlike fMRI; therefore, it is less disturbing for participants, especially elderly people (Hoshi, 2007).

The aim of this study was to use NIRS technique to evaluate the effect of aging on different levels of discourse comprehension and on the brain activity related to those levels. Another goal of this study was to set the bases concerning the evolution of discourse comprehension in normal and optimal aging with a view to future studies investigating differences within the aging population. Two main hypotheses were formulated. First, at the behavioral level, the older participants should be less accurate at the microproposition level than the younger participants, but equally (or almost equally) accurate at the macroproposition and situation model level. Second, consistent with the CRUNCH plasticity model discussed above, the older participants would show greater hemodynamic response in the prefrontal cortex. Differences between the three levels of discourse comprehension studied were not expected on the aspect of hemodynamic responses because of the simultaneous processing of those comprehension levels.

Materials and Methods

This project was approved by the Comité mixte d’éthique de la recherche du Regroupement Neuroimagerie Québec (Joint Committee on Research Ethics of the Quebec Neuroimaging group). All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants

Thirty-six adults participated in this study: 18 older adults, from 66 to 78 years old, and 18 younger adults, from 20 years to 27 years old. All had university degrees (15–21 years of education). All were right handed, as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), with a score of 80 or higher; had French as their mother tongue; and had normal or corrected vision, as assessed by the Near Vision Test Card (Snellen, 1862). Their vocabulary level was evaluated with the Mill Hill Vocabulary Scale (Deltour, 1993). Participants did not have dyslexia, diabetes, psychiatric or neurological impairments, or a history of alcoholism or drug addiction.

All older participants had a score of 26 or more on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine et al., 2005). All participants underwent a short neuropsychological testing to assess their cognitive functions, including working memory (Digit Span test; Wechsler, 1991), short-term and long-term memory (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; Rey, 1970), attention (Trail Making Test, parts A and B; Reitan and Wolfson, 1985), inhibitory control (Stroop Victoria Test; Stroop, 1935), mental flexibility (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, 64-card version; Nelson, 1976) and processing speed (Digit Symbol; Wechsler, 1991); see the Appendix Table 1 for details.

Stimuli

The task is composed of 36 short stories and probe questions created by Scherer et al. (2012). The story-probe combinations were divided into three blocks evaluating the different components of the discourse comprehension model: micropropositions, macropropositions and situation model. The micropropositions block refers to the raw textual information, the macropropositions block refers to the extraction of the gist of texts via the macrorules (deletion, generalization and construction) and the situation model blocks refer to the capacities to inference a conclusion combining one’s knowledge and information from the text. Participants read a short story on a computer and, immediately afterward, they pressed a button to indicate whether the presented probe was true or false, according to the story they just had read. True and false answers were balanced in each condition and the order of block presentation was counterbalanced between participants. None of the stories contained metaphors, irony or indirect language. Short stories were chosen to decrease the influence of working memory on discourse comprehension by reducing the information load. All stories were built to be equivalent regarding number of words, syllables, letters, sentences and propositions. All stories were also equivalent regarding syntactic complexity. The timeline and task design are presented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Timeline and task design for each block of stimuli. Participants had 15 s to read the text. Then a probe was displayed for 5 s, as soon as the probe appeared, participants had 10 s to answer as fast and accurately as they could. When the probe disappeared, blank screen was displayed for 30 s (Nothing). Ten seconds before the next text presentation, a cross appeared to notify the participants of the upcoming text. This sequence was repeated 12 times.

Examples of three short narratives with their corresponding probes.

Microproposition Level

Text: Michaël est propriétaire d’une voiture verte depuis douze ans. Il l’aime bien et l’appelle Pierrette. La voiture de Michaël est très vieille et tombe en panne. Michaël est triste parce qu’il va devoir acheter une autre voiture.

Michaël has owned a green car for 12 years. He loves it and calls it Pierrette. Michaël’s car is very old and breaks down. Michaël is sad because he will need to buy another car.

Probe: La vieille voiture de Michaël est de couleur bleue. (F)

Michaël’s old car is blue. (F)

Macroproposition Level

Text: Joanne a l’habitude d’être malade durant ses voyages, mais aujourd’hui elle se sent bien. Soudain, la plus jeune de ses filles tombe. Heureusement, un autre touriste la ramène à bord en la tirant par son gilet de sauvetage.

Joanne usually gets sick when traveling, but today she feels well. Suddenly, her youngest daughter falls. Fortunately, another tourist pulls her back on board by grabbing her life jacket.

Probe: Joanne et sa fille sont des touristes en croisière sur un bateau. (V)

Joanne and her daughter are tourists traveling on a boat. (T)

Situation Model Level

Text: Marie ne parle pas couramment le français. Elle a lu une offre d’emploi de réceptionniste dans un hôtel de Montréal. Elle va à l’entrevue mais sa candidature n’est pas retenue. Marie décide de prendre des cours de français.

Marie does not speak French fluently. She has read a posting for a job as a receptionist in a Montreal hotel. She goes for the interview, but her application is not accepted. Marie decides to take French courses.

Probe: Marie étudiera le français parce qu’elle adore cette langue. (F)

Marie is going to study French because she loves the language. (F)

Procedure

During the task, participants were seated on a chair in front of a computer while wearing a NIRS helmet. Their index fingers were placed on two buttons, one with a green “V” (for vrai in French, meaning “true”) and one with a red “F” (for faux in French, meaning “false”). The locations of the buttons were counterbalanced among the participants. During the task, they were asked to read silently and to avoid speaking or moving. After receiving the instructions, the participants performed a practice task composed of three stories, each followed by a probe. Then, they performed the three blocks (duration of 780 s each) with a few minutes’ pause after each block. Participants had 15 s to read each text, after which they had 10 s maximum to read and answer the probe about the preceding text. While no instructions were given about reading speed, participants were asked to answer as quickly as possible when the probe appears on the screen. All participants were able to answer in the given time.

Data Acquisition

Behavioral data were recorded using E-Prime2 program1. Functional NIRS data were acquired using a TECHEN Continuous Wave systems model CW6. Ten sources and 28 detectors were located on each participant’s head, according to the language-related brain regions, for a total of 58 optical channels. Each source consisted of two lasers emitting light at 690 nm and 830 nm. After the acquisition, channels were grouped in 16 regions of interest (ROIs) evenly distributed on each brain hemisphere. The helmet was placed on each participant’s head based on the 10-20 standard (Jasper, 1958) as a reference. The helmet design and the position of sources, detectors, and ROIs are presented in Figure 2. To insure reliability, for each participant, nasion-inion distance was measured and the helmet was placed at 10% of that distance from the nasion. Other points of reference were measured to ensure good positioning of the helmet (see Figure 2). Anatomical MRI from a previous study (Amiri et al., 2014) with the same helmet allowed us to estimate the Brodmann’s Area covered by each ROI (see Table 1). NIRS acquisition was performed throughout each block of the task and trigger points were set at the beginning of each story and probes to dissociate those segments in the analyses.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Positions of sources (S), detectors (D) and regions of interest (ROI).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Estimation of the Brodmann’s area covered by each region of interest (ROI).

Data Analysis

NIRS data were processed, normalized and analyzed using HomER22—that is acronym for Hemodynamic optically measured Evoked Response—an open-source data analysis toolbox with GUI interface. HomER2 provides users with tools to estimate the concentration changes for HbO2 and HbR from the change in light intensity recovered by detectors with NIRS (Huppert et al., 2009). Channels S4-D12, S5-D10, S5-D13, S9-D26, S10-D24 and S10-D27 were recorded but excluded from the analysis because of a poor signal across most participants. Statistical analyses of the behavioral and NIRS data (mean amplitude of hemoglobin concentration’s variation) were performed using SPSS software3. Data distributions were normal. Thus, independent sample Student t-tests were used to compare the two groups at the behavioral level. As the two independent variables (level of discourse comprehension and age) studied here were not related, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVAs) with a Bonferroni correction were used to compare mean hemoglobin (Hb) changes between each condition within groups for both parts of the task. Independent sample Student t-tests were used to compare mean Hb changes in each ROI between the two groups.

Results

Behavioral Data

The sociodemographic characteristics of both groups are presented in Table 2. Younger and older groups differed only in their age, since no significant differences were observed in terms of education and vocabulary levels.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics of the groups.

Numbers of correct answers for each condition in the reading task are presented in Figure 3. The younger group produced significantly more correct answers in the microproposition and situation model conditions than the older group. No significant difference was found in the macroproposition condition between the two groups. The results about microproposition and macroproposition conditions are in accordance with studies revealing that elderly participants perform better on what the text is about than its structure and contain (Radvansky et al., 2001; Radvansky and Dijkstra, 2007). The result about the situation model is a little more surprising, but may be congruent with short texts not allowing to properly build a situation model. Figure 4 shows response times; it reveals that the younger group responded significantly faster than the older group to the probes in all conditions. This result is probably the consequence of processing speed declines in aging (Salthouse, 1993).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Accuracy in each condition (MIC, micropropositions; MAC, macropropositions; SIT, situation model). Significant level was set at *p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Response times (ms) in each condition (MIC, micropropositions; MAC, macropropositions; SIT, situation model). Significant level was set at *p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01.

NIRS Data

The grand-averaged waveforms of HbO2 and HbR concentrations during the reading part of the task are shown in Figure 5 for both groups in the three conditions.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Grand-averaged waveforms of oxyhemoglobin (HbO2) and deoxyhemoglobin (HbR) concentrations during the reading portion of the task (15 s). Variation in hemoglobin concentration (μM) as a function of time (s). ROI with significant differences are highlighted in gray. R, Right; L, Left.

No significant differences in HbO2 and HbR concentration variations were found between conditions in the within-group analysis (one-way ANOVA) during the reading portion of the task in either group. Those results were expected since brain activity while reading should stay constant and not be influenced by conditions. Student t-tests were performed for the between-group analysis. Significant differences between the two groups were found in ROI 1L and 2L: the older group showed a greater and more sustained increase in HbO2 in all three conditions while reading the texts. Significant differences were observed for region 1L, in the microproposition (p = 0.034) and macroproposition conditions (p = 0.028), with an almost significant difference in the situation model condition (p = 0.060). For region 2L, significant differences were seen in the microproposition (p = 0.021), macroproposition (p = 0.043) and situation model conditions (p = 0.015). These regions correspond to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the left frontopolar area and part of Broca’s area. This region is known to often play a role in compensation mechanisms (Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008).

The grand-averaged waveforms of HbO2 and HbR concentrations while participants answered the probe questions are presented in Figure 6 for both groups in the three conditions. No significant differences in HbO2 and HbR concentration variations were found between conditions in the intragroup analysis (one-way ANOVA) during the time allowed to answer the probe in either group. As well, no significant differences in HbO2 and HbR concentration variations were found between the two groups (Student t-test) while they were answering the probe questions.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Grand-averaged waveforms of HbO2 and HbR concentrations while participants answered the probes. Variation (10 s) in hemoglobin concentration (μM) as a function of time (s). R, Right; L, Left.

Discussion

Discourse is a highly elaborative linguistic ability (Ska et al., 2004). Consequently, it depends on several interrelated cognitive functions (Borella et al., 2011) and age related declines might be a consequence of deficits or modifications of a number of component processes rather than on specific processes (Hannon and Daneman, 2009). Moreover, the neural substrate is a complex network of frontal, temporal and cingulated areas (Mar, 2004).

Differences in Task Accuracy

In order to reduce the influence of working memory on the older group’s performance, short texts and short waiting periods between the texts and the probes were used. Nevertheless, the older group still performed less accurately than the younger group in the microproposition condition. The older adults managed to maintain equivalent accuracy to the younger group in the macroproposition (gist) condition. Similar results had been reported in the past in studies using longer texts and longer periods between reading and questions (Radvansky et al., 2001; Radvansky and Dijkstra, 2007; Davis et al., 2015). According to those studies, elderly participants perform better and rely more on what the text is about (macrostructure and situation model) than what it is (surface form and micropropositions). Our study tends to confirm those findings at least regarding the microstructure and macrostructure. The shorter texts in our study may explain the differences with those studies on the situation model condition, and this will be addressed in the following section.

Since the influence of working memory was minimized, the fact that the older adults still performed worse at the microstructural level may have been because, in general, they relied less on that component of comprehension. It is possible that older adults read more efficiently and set aside information they do not need more quickly, emphasizing the gist of the story instead of assimilating the whole text. By doing this, they keep a smaller propositional network active, which is easier to process through the different cycles of comprehension. This network is enough to build the situation model but does not allow them to retain all the details of a text. This is also in line with the better ability of younger participants to recall the verbatim of texts, especially when the time period allowed for reading is short, as they are able to correctly recall a higher percentage of propositions from a text than older participants (Morrow et al., 1992). On the other hand, an inhibitory deficit in older adults may explain a difficulty to reject false but probable information especially since their attention is oriented to semantic information neglecting details (Hannon and Daneman, 2009; Wright et al., 2011).

Another potential explanation arises from the fact that the younger group was mostly composed of individual who had stopped attending school less than 5 years before participating in the study. Students tend to try to retain most of the details of a text while preparing for exams and they may maintain that habit even afterward while reading for a test. This type of reading is unnecessarily tiring and resource-demanding, because retaining every detail is not useful for overall comprehension. Thus, this habit is likely to gradually disappear when an individual has not had to study for a long time. This interpretation may also explain why younger participants seem to rely more on the microstructural level even in studies with longer texts (Radvansky et al., 2001).

Because the macroproposition block covers the gist of the text and the generalization of propositions, and because it is harder to evaluate change in the spatiotemporal framework within short texts, the situation model block covers mainly the integration and inferences of knowledge aspects of the model. Integration refers to the necessity to combine two or more pieces of information, and inference refers to the need to derive implications from the information presented (Sommers et al., 2011). Although the construction of the situation model is known to be preserved in aging (Radvansky et al., 2001; Radvansky and Dijkstra, 2007), the older adults responded less accurately in this condition than the younger ones. This difference may have arisen because this study focuses more on the integration and inference of knowledge aspects. Those types of questions were reported to be harder to answer than informal questions (and inferences are harder than integration) in a study of listening comprehension (Sommers et al., 2011). However, Sommers et al. (2011) also reported that the rate of age-related decline does not differ for the three types of questions.

Moreover, McGinnis (2009) reported that older adults generate more inferences while reading and that inferential processing may be less sensitive to cognitive decline. This is consistent with the hypothesis that older adults rely more on their prior knowledge in reading comprehension to compensate for the age-related decline in resource allocation (Maury et al., 2010). In this case, to explain the unexpectedly poorer performance of the older group in the situation model condition in our study, it may be that very short texts are too difficult because they do not provide enough time and redundancy of propositions to properly build a situation model. In that context, older adults may not have enough material to build a good connection with their prior knowledge. Since the situation model was not consolidated, the older participants found it more difficult to produce the inferences they needed to answer the probes. Maybe they would even have benefited from more time between the reading of the texts and the presentation of the probes to consolidate what they had just read in a better situation model with the proper inferences. In fact, the decline of processing speed in aging is well documented and can interfere with working memory and executive functions during discourse comprehension (Borella et al., 2011).

The older adults responded significantly slower than the younger ones in all three conditions. This result was expected since many studies report that processing speed declines in aging (e.g., Salthouse, 1993, 1996; Rabbitt et al., 2007; Feld and Sommers, 2009). Building the microstructural network, extracting the macrostructure (gist) and updating the situation model require a lot of cycles of processing by different types of memory, as well as inhibition (Radvansky, 1999a; Chesneau et al., 2007a). Thus, a decline in processing speed in the older group may also explain the differences in accuracy. This assumption is especially likely at the microstructural level, where the network grows more and more as the person reads, making it more difficult to process. Since the macrostructure is an extract of the essential components of the microstructure, it is easier to process, with fewer propositions to carry from one cycle to the next, and therefore may be less affected by the decrease in processing speed observed in aging.

Constants and Changes in Hemodynamic Responses in Aging

The older and younger groups showed no significant differences in HbO2 and HbR concentration changes between the three conditions while reading and while answering the probe questions. Those results were expected since participants were not aware of the type of questions they would get after reading and answering involves retrieving information more than it depends on comprehension, which was done earlier in the process. Moreover, the levels of discourse comprehension are interconnected, simultaneous and self-directed while reading, making them nearly impossible to differentiate neuroanatomically.

No differences are observed in the temporal regions associated with discourse comprehension. In fact, while reading, both groups showed a substantial hemodynamic response in the left middle temporal gyrus (ROI 6L), which plays a role in the integration and interpretation of texts (Ferstl et al., 2008). Both groups also showed hemodynamic responses in the premotor and supplementary motor cortex (ROI 5, mainly in the left hemisphere). These regions are known to play an important role in reading aloud (Fiez and Petersen, 1998). While a person is reading aloud, some of the activation of the supplementary motor cortex is linked to the encoding of word form information, which may also be present during silent reading (Alario et al., 2006). The supplementary motor cortex is also activated when the retrieval of verbs from memory is necessary (Wise et al., 1991). The premotor cortex is also known to be activated while reading action words silently (Hauk and Pulvermüller, 2004). Hemodynamic responses were also present, in both groups, in the left fusiform gyrus (ROI 8L), which is associated with visual word recognition and is systematically activated during reading (McCandliss et al., 2003; Dehaene and Cohen, 2011).

No differences between groups were observed in the right hemisphere. The right hemisphere is mainly associated with prosodic and pragmatic processing (Ferré et al., 2011), which was avoided in this task. Thus, a task that emphasizes those aspects of discourse comprehension may reveal a greater difference between younger and older participants. Still, both groups demonstrated right frontal region activation during the reading part of the task, which could be linked to the integration of the propositional network built while reading (Robertson et al., 2000).

Significant differences between the younger and older groups were seen mainly in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, part of the left frontopolar area and part of Broca’s area (ROI 1L and 2L) while reading the texts. Older adults sustained greater and longer hemodynamic responses. This difference may be due to the longer time needed to read the text or a greater need for attentional resources. It also probably indicates an attentional effort in semantic processing (Zhuang et al., 2016). The prefrontal cortex as a whole is known to be a major association area, having a pivotal role in the synthesis of the multifaceted information generated by the brain (Kandel et al., 2000). Even in normal brain aging, functional connectivity has been shown to decrease leading to restructured network topology and associated behavioral alterations which impact the dynamic and structure of the global network of the brain (Ferreira and Busatto, 2013). Thus, the over-activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex may be the results of this restructuration occurring in aging. Furthermore, the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex had already been highlighted as a potential modulator of discourse comprehension. This region was observed to be more active in older participants who had good reading habits, but less active when these participants answered questions about their reading (Martin et al., 2012). This over-activation may reflect compensatory strategies in older adults, who possibly recruit more attentional resources while reading to maximize information encoding or make more efforts to properly integrate information. More studies on the subject would be necessary to confirm those results and investigated the potential modular role of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in discourse comprehension.

Despite differences in accuracy and in response times, the older and younger adults showed no significant differences in any brain regions while answering the probe questions. Nevertheless, the younger participants showed short hemodynamic responses in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex bilaterally while the older participants showed longer ones. This is certainly linked to the differences in response times discussed previously. Slower processes may generate more sustained hemodynamic responses, reflecting the time needed to perform the task.

Discourse Comprehension and Models of Aging

Given that discourse comprehension is a complex but common task, it is interesting to see whether some well-known aging models may shed light on our results.

The greater brain activity described in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the older adults is in line with the CRUNCH model which describes a role for that particular region in compensation mechanisms (Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008). Thus, the greater activation observed in our study may be a sign of a compensatory mechanism in the older group to achieve good comprehension while reading the texts. The CRUNCH model also states that over-activation is not always adequate to maintain performance in aging, which was the case in two out of the three conditions in this study.

Our study does not contradict an intrahemispheric reorganization like the one described in the PASA model (Davis et al., 2008) since there was greater activation in the anterior regions, but there was no decrease in activation in the posterior region. Since our study did not cover the whole posterior brain, it is not possible to rule out the existence of PASA phenomena in discourse comprehension.

It is also impossible to draw any conclusions concerning an interhemispheric reorganization like the one described in the HAROLD model (Cabeza, 2002). In fact, both older and younger adults showed signs of bilateralism while executing this comprehension task. This is not surprising, since discourse comprehension is a complex task involving many cognitive processes.

The present study does not replicate the previous finding of an interhemispheric and intrahemispheric reorganization in older adults reported by a study using a similar task by Scherer et al. (2012). That study used a helmet device covering only the posterior part of the frontal lobes and a part of the temporal lobes in a small sample of participants. In our study, even if a greater activation was observed in the anterior regions, no decrease in activation in the posterior regions was found. Furthermore, in our samples, both older and younger adults showed signs of bilateralism while executing the task which reflects the complexity of the cognitive processes involved.

Future Considerations and Limitations

Number of texts and probes was one of the main limitations caused by neuroimaging techniques which required a relatively long cool-down between each trial. The new data presented here could help resolve this limitation. Indeed, the absence of difference between conditions on the hemoglobin variation level will prompt to develop a task with less texts, but more probes to answer.

This study compared two groups with university-level education. Older adults with a high education level are known to be generally less affected by cognitive decline. Less homogeneous groups could reveal more differences and lead to a better understanding of discourse comprehension in the elderly population. Varying the length of the texts and the level of difficulty could also provide interesting data to help gain a better understanding of older adults’ capacity for inferencing and building a situation model.

Another limit was that a task with short texts was used to help to find a balance between the needed parameters for the discourse comprehension’s capabilities and the many restraints of neuroimaging and NIRS technique. Short texts were also chosen to decrease the influence of working memory on discourse comprehension by reducing the information load. Even if the task helped to highlight differences at the microstructure and situation model levels between young and older participants, it was probably a little bit too easy for the younger participants given that most of them got a really high accuracy. Thereby, this task would not be suited for a study with only a heterogeneous group of young participants as it would not permit to differentiate strength and shortcomings between them. Long texts and short texts also seem to be different in terms of capacities to recall information, since older adults show more difficulties to recall situation model information than expected in short texts. Our comprehension of this phenomenon would benefit from further investigation.

Discourse comprehension, and its changes during aging, is a vitally important subject due to its role in preserving quality of life. Future studies should look at these changes in a more realistic task such as reading the newspaper or engaging in conversation.

Conclusion

This study demonstrated that while older adults find it harder to recall the microstructure (details) or the situation model (inference) of a short text, they still manage to properly recall the macrostructure (gist of discourse). In accordance with the CRUNCH model, older adults show significantly greater activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex while reading texts in all conditions, although the increased activation was not sufficient to maintain the same accuracy as younger participants in two of the conditions. Thus, this greater activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex while reading is possibly link to a compensatory mechanism by the older adults. However, more studies are necessary to conclude on that point.

Author Contributions

C-OM is the main author of this article and he was responsible of the protocol, data collection and analysis and mansucript’s redaction. SP-D contributed to data analysis. VD and EY contributed to recruitment and data collection. MA was the ressource person regarding NIRS technique and NIRS data treatment. LCH developped a prior version of the task used in this study. BS supervised this project as a thesis director. All authors approved the final version of this manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR; award no. 8827) awarded to Y. Joanette, B. Ska and A.I. Ansaldo (2014-2017). A prior version of this study is presented in the thesis of the main author (Martin, 2015).

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.pstnet.com/eprime.cfm
  2. ^ http://www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/PMI/resources/homer2/home.htm
  3. ^ http://www-01.ibm.com/software/analytics/spss/

References

Alario, F. X., Chainay, H., Lehericy, S., and Cohen, L. (2006). The role of the supplementary motor area (SMA) in word production. Brain Res. 1076, 129–143. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amiri, M., Pouliot, P., Bonnery, C., Leclerc, P. O., Desjardins, M., Lesage, F., et al. (2014). An exploration of the effect of hemodynamic changes due to normal aging on the fNIRS response to semantic processing of words. Front. Neurol. 5:249. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borella, E., Ghisletta, P., and de Ribaupierre, A. (2011). Age differences in text processing: the role of working memory, inhibition, and processing speed. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 66, 311–320. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, C. R., and Dennis, N. A. (2015). Age differences in the neural correlates of novelty processing: the effects of item-relatedness. Brain Res. 1612, 2–15. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brébion, G. (2003). Working memory, language comprehension, and aging: four experiments to understand the deficit. Exp. Aging Res. 29, 269–301. doi: 10.1080/03610730303725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol. Aging 17, 85–100. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahana-Amitay, D., and Albert, M. L. (2014). Brain and language: evidence for neural multifunctionality. Behav. Neurol. 2014:260381. doi: 10.1155/2014/260381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappell, K. A., Gmeindl, L., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2010). Age differences in prefrontal recruitment during verbal working memory maintenance depend on memory load. Cortex 46, 462–473. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chance, B., Zhuang, Z., Unah, C., Alter, C., and Lipton, L. (1993). Cognition-activated low-frequency modulation of light absorption in human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 90, 3770–3774. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.8.3770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chesneau, S., Jbabdi, S., Champagne-Lavau, M., Giroux, F., and Ska, B. (2007a). Text comprehension, cognitive resources and aging. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 5, 47–64.

PubMed Abstract

Chesneau, S., Roy, M.-C., and Ska, B. (2007b). Evaluation of text comprehension based on a theoretical model. Can. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. Audiol. 31, 83–93.

Cohen, M. S., Rissman, J., Suthana, N. A., Castel, A. D., and Knowlton, B. J. (2016). Effects of aging on value-directed modulation of semantic network activity during verbal learning. Neuroimage 125, 1046–1062. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, D. K., Alea, N., and Bluck, S. (2015). The difference between right and wrong: accuracy of older and younger adults’ story recall. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 12, 10861–10885. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120910861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., and Cabeza, R. (2008). Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1201–1209. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Beni, R., Palladino, P., Borella, E., and Lo Presti, S. (2003). Reading comprehension and aging: does an age-related difference necessarily mean impairment? Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 15, 67–76. doi: 10.1007/bf03324482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeDe, G., Caplan, D., Kemtes, K., and Waters, G. (2004). The relationship between age, verbal working memory and language comprehension. Psychol. Aging 19, 601–616. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., and Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deltour, J. J. (1993). Échelle de Vocabulaire de Mill Hill de J. C. Raven. Adaptation Française et Normes Européennes du Mill Hill et du Standard Progressive Matrices de Raven (PM38) Mill-Hill Vocabulary Scale of J. C. Raven. French aDaptation and European Norms. Braine-le-Château, Belgium: Éditions L’application des techniques modernes.

Denhière, G., and Baudet, S. (1992). Lecture, Compréhension de Texte et Science Cognitive. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Ericsson, K. A., and Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychol. Rev. 102, 211–245. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, S. E., Caplan, D., Ostrowski, A., Michaud, J., Guarino, A., and Waters, G. (2015). Working memory and the revision of syntactic and discourse ambiguities. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 69, 136–155. doi: 10.1037/cep0000037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feld, J. E., and Sommers, M. S. (2009). Lipreading, processing speed, and working memory in younger and older adults. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52, 1555–1565. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0137)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferré, P., Ska, B., Lajoie, C., Bleau, A., and Joanette, Y. (2011). Clinical focus on prosodic, discursive and pragmatic treatment for right hemisphere damaged adults: what’s right? Rehabil. Res. Pract. 2011:131820. doi: 10.1155/2011/131820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, L. K., and Busatto, G. F. (2013). Resting-state functional connectivity in normal brain aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 384–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferstl, E. C. (2006). Text comprehension in middle aged adults: is there anything wrong? Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 13, 62–85. doi: 10.1080/13825580490904237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2008). The extended language network: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 581–593. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiez, J. A., and Petersen, S. E. (1998). Neuroimaging studies of word reading. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 914–921. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fu, L., Maes, J. H., Kessels, R. P., and Daselaar, S. M. (2017). To boost or to CRUNCH? Effect of effortful encoding on episodic memory in older adults is dependent on executive functioning. PLoS One 12:E2871. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0174217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratton, G., and Fabiani, M. (2007). “Optical imaging of brain function,” in Neuroergonomics: The Brain at Work, eds R. Parasuraman and M. Rizzo (Oxford and Toronto: Oxford University Press), 65–81.

Google Scholar

Hannon, B., and Daneman, M. (2009). Age-related changes in reading comprehension: an individual differences perspective. Exp. Aging Res. 35, 432–456. doi: 10.1080/03610730903175808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauk, O., and Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto-central cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 21, 191–201. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoshi, Y. (2007). Functional near-infrared spectroscopy: current status and future prospects. J. Biomed. Opt. 12:062106. doi: 10.1117/1.2804911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., and Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: a review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Appl. Opt. 48, D280–D298. doi: 10.1364/ao.48.00d280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasper, H. (1958). Progress and problems in brain research. J. Mt. Sinai Hosp. N Y 25, 244–253.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jöbsis, F. F. (1977). Noninvasive, infrared monitoring of cerebral and myocardial oxygen sufficiency and circulatory parameters. Science 198, 1264–1267. doi: 10.1126/science.929199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., and Jessel, T. M. (2000). Principles of Neural Science. 4th Edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction-integration model. Psychol. Rev. 95, 163–182. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.95.2.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kintsch, W., and Van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychol. Rev. 85, 363–394. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.85.5.363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kocsis, L., Herman, P., and Eke, A. (2006). The modified Beer-Lambert law revisited. Phys. Med. Biol. 51, N91–N98. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/51/5/n02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebreton, O. (2012). Adaptation du modèle de la construction-intégration de kintsch à la compréhension des énoncés et à la résolution des problèmes arithmétiques complexes. Thèse de Doctorat, Psychologie, Université de la Réunion, (Français), 469. Available online at: https://tel.archives-ouvertes.fr/tel-00716841

Magliano, J., Kopp, K., McNerney, M. W., Radvansky, G. A., and Zacks, J. M. (2012). Aging and perceived event structure as a function of modality. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 19, 264–282. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2011.633159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mar, R. A. (2004). The neuropsycholgy of narrative: story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neurpsychologia 42, 1414–1434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C.-O. (2015). Influence du vieillissement et de la scolarité sur la compréhension du discours: apport de l’imagerie optique. Ph. D. sous la Supervision de Bernadette Ska, Université de Montréal, (Montréal, QC). Available online at: http://hdl.handle.net/1866/15987

Google Scholar

Martin, C.-O., Desrochers, M., Demers, C., Scherer, L. C., and Ska, B. (2012). Influence of reading habits on cerebral plasticity for discourse comprehension in aging. Ilha Desterro 63, 101–127. doi: 10.5007/2175-8026.2012n63p101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C.-O., Pontbriand-Drolet, S., Daoust, V., Yamga, E., Amiri, M., Hubner, L. C., et al. (2017). High education seems not related to discourse comprehension in aging. J. Negat. Results Biomed. 2017, 1–19. Available online at: http://www.negative-results.org/index.php/2017/05/09/article-test/

Maury, P., Besse, F., and Martin, S. (2010). Age differences in outdated information processing during news reports reading. Exp. Aging Res. 36, 371–392. doi: 10.1080/0361073x.2010.511962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazoyer, B. M., Tzourio, N., Frak, V., Syrota, A., Murayama, N., Levrier, O., et al. (1993). The cortical representation of speech. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 467–479. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., and Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 293–299. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00134-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGinnis, D. (2009). Text comprehension products and processes in young, young-old, and old-old adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64, 202–211. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miotto, E. C., Balardin, J. B., Savage, C. R., Martin Mda, G., Batistuzzo, M. C., Amaro Junior, E., et al. (2014). Brain regions supporting verbal memory improvement in healthy older subjects. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 72, 663–670. doi: 10.1590/0004-282x20140120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrow, D. G., Leirer, V. O., and Altieri, P. A. (1992). Aging, expertise, and narrative processing. Psychol. Aging 7, 376–388. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mund, I., Bell, R., and Buchner, A. (2012). Aging and interference in story recall. Exp. Aging Res. 38, 20–41. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2012.636724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, H. E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 12, 313–324. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(76)80035-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noh, S. R., and Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2009). Age differences in tracking characters during narrative comprehension. Mem. Cogn. 37, 769–778. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.6.769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. R., and Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2017). The effects of home-based cognitive training on verbal working memory and language comprehension in older adulthood. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:256. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perfetti, C. A., and Frishkoff, G. A. (2008). “The neural bases of text and discourse processing,” in Handbook of the Neuroscience of Language, eds B. Stemmer and H. A. Whitaker (London: Academic Press), 165–174.

Google Scholar

Perrig, W., and Kintsch, W. (1985). Propositional and situational representations of a text. J. Mem. Lang. 24, 503–518. doi: 10.1016/0749-596x(85)90042-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabbitt, P., Mogapi, O., Scott, M., Thacker, N., Lowe, C., Horan, M., et al. (2007). Effects of global atrophy, white matter lesions, and cerebral blood flow on age-related changes in speed, memory, intelligence, vocabulary, and frontal function. Neuropsychology 21, 684–695. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racz, F. S., Mukli, P., Nagy, Z., and Eke, A. (2017). Increased prefrontal cortex connectivity during cognitive challenge assessed by fNIRS imaging. Biomed. Opt. Express 8, 3842–3855. doi: 10.1364/BOE.8.003842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radvansky, G. A. (1999a). Aging, memory and comprehension. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 8, 49–53. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radvansky, G. A. (1999b). Memory retrieval and suppression: the inhibition of situation models. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 128, 563–579. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.4.563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radvansky, G. A., and Copeland, D. E. (2004). Working memory span and situation model processing. Am. J. Psychol. 117, 191–213. doi: 10.2307/4149022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radvansky, G. A., Curiel, J. M., Zwaan, R. A., and Copeland, D. E. (2001). Situation models and aging. Psychol. Aging 16, 145–160. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.1.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radvansky, G. A., and Dijkstra, K. (2007). Aging and situation model processing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 1027–1042. doi: 10.3758/bf03193088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M., and Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 177–182. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rey, A. (1970). L’examen Clinique en Psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Google Scholar

Robertson, D. A., Gernsbacher, M. A., Guidotti, S. J., Robertson, R. R., Irwin, W., Mock, B. J., et al. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of the cognitive process of mapping during discourse comprehension. Psychol. Sci. 11, 255–260. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodden, F. A., and Stemmer, B. (2008). “A brief introduction to common neuroimaging techniques,” in Handbook of the Neuroscience of Language, eds B. Stemmer and H. A. Whitaker (London: Academic Press), 57–67.

Salthouse, T. A. (1993). Speed and knowledge as determinants of adult age differences in verbal tasks. J. Gerontol. 48, P29–P36. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.1.p29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol. Rev. 103, 403–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, L. C., Fonseca, R. P., Giroux, F., Senhadji, N., Marcotte, K., Tomitch, L. M., et al. (2012). Neurofunctional (re)organization underlying narrative discourse processing in aging: evidence from fNIRS. Brain Lang. 121, 174–184. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmalhofer, F., and Perfetti, C. A. (2007). “Neural and behavioral indicators of integration processes across sentence boundaries,” in Higher Level Language Processes in the Brain: Inference and Comprehension Processes, eds F. Schmalhofer and C. A. Perfetti (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 161–188.

Google Scholar

Shafto, M. A. (2015). Proofreading in young and older adults: the effect of error category and comprehension difficulty. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 12, 14445–14460. doi: 10.3390/ijerph121114445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ska, B., and Duong, A. (2005). Communication, discourse and dementia. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 3, 125–133.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ska, B., Duong, A., and Joanette, Y. (2004). “Discourse impairment,” in The MIT Encyclopedia of Communication Disorders, ed. R. D. Kent (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 302–304.

Snellen, H. (1862). Probebuchstaben zur Bestimmung der Sehschärfe Sample Letters for the Determination of Visual Acuity. Utrecht: Van de Weijer.

Sommers, M. S., Hale, S., Myerson, J., Rose, N., Tye-Murray, N., and Spehar, B. (2011). Listening comprehension across the adult lifespan. Ear Hear. 32, 775–781. doi: 10.1097/aud.0b013e3182234cf6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Milinder, L., Pullara, O., and Herman, B. (2001). Patterns of resource allocation are reliable among younger and older readers. Psychol. Aging 16, 69–84. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.1.69

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Soederberg Miller, L. M., Gagne, D. D., and Hertzog, C. (2008). Self-regulated reading in adulthood. Psychol. Aging 23, 131–153. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strangman, G., Boas, D. A., and Sutton, J. P. (2002). Non-invasive neuroimaging using near-infrared light. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 679–693. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01550-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference on serial verbal reaction. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, W. C., Dew, I. T., and Cabeza, R. (2015). Age-related differences in medial temporal lobe involvement during conceptual fluency. Brain Res. 1612, 48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1991). Échelle Clinique de Mémoire de Wechsler—Révisée. Montreal, QC: Centre de Psychologie appliquée.

Wise, R., Hadar, U., Howard, D., and Patterson, K. (1991). Language activation studies with positron emission tomography. Ciba Found. Symp. 163, 218–228; discussion 228–234.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wright, H. H., Capilouto, G. J., Srinivasan, C., and Fergadiotis, G. (2011). Story processing ability in cognitively healthy younger and older adults. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 54, 900–917. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0253)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhuang, J., Johnson, M. A., Madden, D. J., Burke, D. M., and Diaz, M. T. (2016). Age-related differences in resolving semantic and phonological competition during receptive language tasks. Neuropsychologia 93, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Estimation of the Brodmann’s area covered by each region of interest (ROI).

Keywords: aging, discourse comprehension, cerebral plasticity, language, NIRS

Citation: Martin C-O, Pontbriand-Drolet S, Daoust V, Yamga E, Amiri M, Hübner LC and Ska B (2018) Narrative Discourse in Young and Older Adults: Behavioral and NIRS Analyses. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:69. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00069

Received: 10 June 2017; Accepted: 01 March 2018;
Published: 16 March 2018.

Edited by:

Aurel Popa-Wagner, Department of Neurology, University Hospital Essen, Germany

Reviewed by:

Mark Leikin, University of Haifa, Israel
Andras Eke, Semmelweis University, Hungary

Copyright © 2018 Martin, Pontbriand-Drolet, Daoust, Yamga, Amiri, Hübner and Ska. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Charles-Olivier Martin, charles-olivier.martin@umontreal.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.