Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 17 September 2025

Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Volume 17 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2025.1666449

This article is part of the Research TopicInteractions of Environment and Exercise on Geriatric HealthView all 4 articles

The effects of Nordic walking on cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis


Haobai Li&#x;&#x;Haobai Li1†‡Ke Zhu&#x;Ke Zhu1†Jianyu Gan*Jianyu Gan1*Ziyi WangZiyi Wang1Zhikun GaoZhikun Gao1Liangru LiuLiangru Liu2Xiaojie GuoXiaojie Guo3Jianfeng Niu*Jianfeng Niu1*
  • 1Sports Coaching College, Beijing Sport University, Beijing, China
  • 2School of Physical Education, Guangxi University, Guangxi, China
  • 3Sports Department, Nankai University, Tianjin, China

Objectives: Nordic walking (NW), as a specialized form of aerobic exercise, emerges as a promising strategy to improve the cognitive function in older population. However, the effectiveness of NW has yet to be definitively confirmed due to the variances in the study designs and observations. This systematic review and meta-analysis was thus conducted to examine the effect of NW interventions on cognitive function of older adults.

Methods: The search was conducted in August 2025 on Web of Science, PubMed, SPORT-Discus, Medline, the Cochrane Library, Scopus, and PsycINFO databases. Two reviewers independently reviewed the search results, extracted the data, and assessed the risk of bias and certainty of evidence. Meta-analyses and meta-regressions were performed to determine the overall effect size and the impact of potential moderators.

Results: Initial screening identified 336 records, and after full-text assessment, eight studies (from 2014 to 2024) comprising 327 participants (71.19 ± 5.44 yrs) were included. The effect size of NW on executive function was significant [Hedges’ g = 0.89, 95% CI (0.27, 1.50), p = 0.01], while the effects were non-significant for global function, memory function, attention, information processing, and perceptual ability (p > 0.05). Subgroup analysis indicated that the health conditions of participants and the types of control groups significantly moderated executive function. Specifically, NW showed significant improvements (i) in older adults with health conditions and (ii) compared with inactive control groups (p = 0.04). Meta-regression revealed a significant positive correlation between the total intervention time of NW and its effect size (p < 0.01).

Conclusion: This systematic review and meta-analysis demonstrates that NW interventions could improve executive function in older adults, especially those with health conditions.

Systematic review registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero, identifier CRD42025638467.

Introduction

Aging is associated with progressive physiological changes, including reduced neurogenesis, impaired synaptic plasticity, and decreased cerebral perfusion in older adults (Castellano et al., 2017; Poulose et al., 2017; Raichlen and Alexander, 2017; Gonzales et al., 2022). These changes collectively contribute to cognitive decline, particularly affecting memory, executive function, and processing speed (Bettio et al., 2017; Fan et al., 2017; Leeuwis et al., 2017; Huang et al., 2022). The resultant cognitive decline in the older population leads to profound adverse consequences, including a marked deterioration in quality of life, increased dependency on caregiving support, and a higher risk of neurodegenerative diseases, particularly Alzheimer’s disease (Li et al., 2017; Sáez De Asteasu et al., 2017). Given these impacts, developing effective interventions to maintain or improve cognitive function in older adults remains essential.

Current approaches against cognitive decline primarily encompass pharmacological interventions, cognitive training paradigms, and lifestyle modifications (Srikanth et al., 2020; Barnes et al., 2023; Antonenko et al., 2024; Faraziani and Eken, 2024). While these strategies demonstrate some efficacy, they are constrained by some limitations. Specifically, pharmacological interventions are frequently associated with adverse side effects and potential long-term complications, raising concerns about their safety profile and sustainability (Parnetti et al., 1997; Blackman et al., 2021; Van Dyck et al., 2023). Cognitive training approaches, though theoretically promising, often exhibit limited ecological validity and practical applicability, with questionable generalizability to real-world cognitive functions (Bahar-Fuchs et al., 2013; Butler et al., 2018). Lifestyle modifications face challenges in implementation and long-term adherence, particularly in elderly populations with varying health conditions and functional capacities (Knight et al., 2016; Barber et al., 2023). These limitations highlight the need for developing alternative intervention strategies that are not only efficacious but also characterized by enhanced safety, accessibility, and sustainability.

Recent literature increasingly corroborates that exercise is a particularly promising intervention strategy (Sanders et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2023; Hatami et al., 2025). Exercise has been shown to promote neuroplasticity and regulate inflammatory processes, thereby creating an optimal neurobiological environment for cognitive preservation and enhancement in older populations (Hortobágyi et al., 2022; Vints et al., 2024; Lavie et al., n.d.). Among various forms of exercise, walking is the preferred choice for most older adults to enhance their cognitive function due to its safety and low intensity, especially for those with limited physical function associated with aging or disease (Adderley et al., 2025; Ahmadpour et al., 2025; Cunha et al., 2025; Sandroff et al., 2025). Notably, recent research has shown that compared with standard walking, a form of walking known as Nordic walking (NW) is less physically demanding for older adults and may provide greater cognitive benefits (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Nemoto et al., 2021; Kettinen et al., 2023). NW distinguishes itself through the incorporation of two specially designed poles that facilitate active engagement of the upper body musculature, thereby resulting in more propulsion and energy expenditure (Schiffer et al., 2006). The arm-swinging motion involved in NW is beneficial for maintaining coordination of upper and lower limbs, which may generate similar cognitive effects to dual-task walking (Doi et al., 2014; Franzoni et al., 2018; Gomeñuka et al., 2019). Studies have demonstrated that NW induced significant improvements in executive function and memory of older adults with or without health conditions as compared to walking or blank control groups (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Guszkowska et al., 2022; Kettinen et al., 2023; Ploydang et al., 2023). However, despite these promising preliminary results, existing evidence remains inconsistent. Several studies reported that no significant changes in cognitive function were observed following NW intervention (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Haas et al., 2024). These inconsistencies may stem from variations in study design, such as intervention duration, intensity, or participant characteristics. Additionally, the lack of standardized protocols for NW and the heterogeneity in cognitive function assessment metrics further complicate the interpretation of study findings.

Therefore, to highlight the recent study findings and explicitly and comprehensively examine the effects of NW on cognitive function in older adults and the potential contributors to such effects, we completed a systematic review and meta-analysis based upon up-to-date peer-reviewed publications. This work will ultimately provide critical knowledge to inform the appropriate intervention design in future research and rehabilitative practice for the maintenance of cognitive function in older populations.

Materials and methods

Study protocol

This systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted using Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., 2021) and registered with PROSPERO (Registration ID: CRD42025638467), an international prospective registry for systematic reviews.

Literature search

Two authors (HL and JG) independently searched Web of Science, PubMed, MEDLINE, SPORT-Discus, Cochrane Library, Scopus, and PsycINFO from inception to August 5, 2025; Studies were searched in the electronic databases using the following key terms combined by Boolean logic (“AND”, “OR”): (“Nordic walking” OR “Nordic pole walking” OR “pole walking”) AND (“Cognitive function” OR “cognition” OR “Cognitive performance”). A secondary search strategy was also used, which involved a manual search in the reference lists of eligible studies (i.e., citation tracking). Searches were limited to publications in English. Any disagreements arising during this process were resolved through discussion between the two authors (HL and JG), with additional input provided by a third author (JN). Detailed search strategies are provided in Supplementary Table 1.

Selection criteria

All included studies must be published articles. The inclusion criteria were carried out according to the PICOS principle: (1) Population: All the participants included in this study were at least 60 years old. None of them had used any drugs known to significantly affect cognitive function, or had discontinued such drugs for more than 4 weeks, or had indicated their actual medication status at baseline to ensure the comparability of drug exposure between the two groups; (2) Interventions: the interventions used were NW only or NW combined with other interventions. When NW was used in combination with other interventions, the control group was supposed to receive other interventions alone to ensure that the observed changes were caused by NW; (3) Comparisons: Each group is characterized as either active (e.g., interventions involving physical activities other than Nordic walking, NW) or inactive (e.g., non-intervention, or routine treatment for the diseases the subjects suffer from); (4) Outcomes: Outcome measures reflecting cognitive function were employed. (5) Study design: The study employed randomized controlled trials or randomized crossover trial designs. Articles with the following conditions will be excluded: (1) did not investigate cognitive function outcomes or provide specific data of outcome measures (e.g., reporting only p-values without means/SDs); (2) review papers, conference abstracts, and articles; (3) those with duplicate publications; (4) non-English publications.

Data extraction

The process of data extraction was conducted independently by two authors (HL and JG) according to the Cochrane Collaboration Handbook. The extracted information of the publications included: study (authors, year), participants (age, sex, physical condition), grouping and sample size, interventions (type, frequency, number of sessions, duration of each session, duration of intervention), auxiliary means (e.g., Nordic walking poles or Hiking pole, etc.), and outcome measures. Any outcome measures on which the two authors disagreed were discussed with the other two authors (JN and KZ) until a consensus was achieved. For each study, extract the mean and standard deviation (SD) of the post-intervention indicator results. For studies that do not report changes in results before and after, or those presenting results in the form of “Mean ± SE/SEM (Standard Error/Standard Error of the Mean)”, use the following formula for calculation (Borenstein et al., 2013):

SD ( pre ) = SE ( pre ) n = × ; SD ( post ) = SE ( post ) n
R = 0.4 / 0.5

When the full-text article data were presented only in a figure format, WebPlotDigitizer (Ankit Rohatgi, 2019, V.4.2; WebPlotDigitizer, Pacifica, CA, USA) was used to extract the data from the figures. In the absence of any relevant data, the first author or the corresponding author of the article would be contacted via email to obtain the required data.

Quality assessment

The quality of the included studies was independently evaluated by two authors (HL and JG) according to the principles of the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro). The PEDro scale, specifically designed to assess the methodological quality of randomized controlled trials in physical interventions, is highly suitable for evaluating the studies in this research (de Morton, 2009; Cashin and McAuley, 2020). This scale examines crucial elements such as randomization, blinding, and allocation concealment, which are essential for ensuring the internal validity of the studies included in this systematic review and meta-analysis (Cashin and McAuley, 2020).

Specifically, the PEDro scale consists of 11 items, to which we were required to respond with “no” or “yes”. For each “no” response, a value of 0 was assigned, and for each “yes” response, a value of 1 was assigned. The total score for each study ranged from 0 to 11. Since blinding (especially of participants and investigators) is difficult to implement in exercise intervention trials (Sherrington et al., 2010), the methodological quality classification of each article was adjusted, taking into account the eligibility criteria as previously described [sum scores: ≥6 (“high quality, low risk of bias”); scores: 4–5 (“acceptable quality, moderate risk of bias”); scores: ≤ 3 (“low quality, high risk of bias”)].

The quality of the evidence was also independently appraised by two authors (HL and JG) based on the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) criteria. The GRADE criteria characterize the evidence in terms of study limitations, imprecision, inconsistency, indirectness, and publication bias. In cases where the two authors disagreed on any score, a third author (JN or KZ) was consulted for discussion until a consensus was reached.

Statistical analysis

Meta-analysis was carried out using Stata/MP 17.0 (STATA Corp, College Station, TX, USA) and R 4.2.0 (R Core Team, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). Given the diverse measurement units of outcome measures across studies, such as time, score, number of stimuli, etc., a random-effects model was employed to calculate Hedges’ g and the 95% confidence interval (CI), which served as the indicator of the effect size for the difference in pre-post changes between the intervention and control groups (Fritz et al., 2012). The effect sizes were categorized as follows: trivial (Hedges’ g < 0.2), small (0.2 ≤ Hedges’ g < 0.5), moderate (0.5 ≤ Hedges’ g < 0.8), or large (Hedges’ g ≥ 0.8) (Cohen, 2013). Additionally, prediction intervals were computed to reflect heterogeneity in comparison with confidence intervals. The between-study variance was estimated using the restricted maximum likelihood estimator with Hartung-Knapp adjustment (Veroniki et al., 2016). Statistical heterogeneity was evaluated by means of the heterogeneity chi-squared (χ2) and I2 values. The degree of heterogeneity was interpreted in accordance with the guidelines of the Cochrane Collaboration: 0%–40% might not be of significance; 30%–60% may indicate moderate heterogeneity; 50%–90% may represent substantial heterogeneity; and 75%–100% implies considerable heterogeneity (Higgins, 2003). In the event of substantial or considerable heterogeneity (I2 > 50%), subgroup analyses were conducted to explore the impact of study characteristics (e.g., the physical condition of the participants). Additionally, we performed meta-regression to explore the dose-response relationship of NW on cognitive function. The intervention duration, session number, and total time (i.e., session number × session duration) were used as the effect moderators. Subsequently, sensitivity analyses were performed to assess the stability of the pooled estimates and to determine whether any study influenced the overall effect size (Veroniki et al., 2016). Moreover, publication bias was assessed through the generation of funnel plots and the conduct of Egger’s test. If significant asymmetry was detected, the Trim and Fill method was utilized to adjust for publication bias (Duval and Tweedie, 2000).

Results

Study selection

The results of the study selection process are summarized in a PRISMA flowchart (Figure 1). The study selection commenced with the identification of 336 records through database searches, which were distributed among Web of Science (n = 84), PubMed (n = 64), MEDLINE (n = 29), SPORT-Discus (n = 47), Cochrane Library (n = 46), Scopus (n = 61), and PsycINFO (n = 3). Additionally, two further studies were identified through citation searching. After removing 174 duplicate records, a total of 162 records were available for the screening phase. During the title and abstract screening stage, 143 records were excluded. Subsequently, a full-text eligibility assessment was conducted on 18 articles, and 10 studies were excluded. Eventually, 8 studies were included in the meta-analysis.

FIGURE 1
Flowchart depicting the identification and screening of studies via databases and citation searches. Initially 334 records were identified, with 174 duplicates excluded. After title and abstract screening, 143 records were excluded for reasons like irrelevant content. Seventeen articles underwent full-text assessment, resulting in eight being eligible. Additionally, two records were identified through citation searching, both undergoing screening but one was excluded due to missing data.

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the study selection process.

Characteristics of included studies

Participant characteristics

The eight studies included in the analysis were conducted across seven countries: Germany (n = 1), Poland (n = 1), Japan (n = 1), Thailand (n = 1), Brazil (n = 2), Italy (n = 1), and Finland (n = 1). The total number of participants across all studies was 327. Excluding one study that did not report age information (Ebersbach et al., 2014), the mean age of participants in the remaining seven studies was 70.92 years (Table 1). Six studies included both male and female participants (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Miyazaki et al., 2022; Angiolillo et al., 2023; Kettinen et al., 2023; Ploydang et al., 2023; Haas et al., 2024), one study exclusively involved female participants (Guszkowska et al., 2022), and one study did not report the gender distribution of participants (Ebersbach et al., 2014).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies (n = 8).

Participants in three studies were essential health (Guszkowska et al., 2022; Miyazaki et al., 2022; Kettinen et al., 2023), while the other five studies included participants with health conditions (Ebersbach et al., 2014; Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Angiolillo et al., 2023; Ploydang et al., 2023; Haas et al., 2024), including Parkinson’s disease (n = 3) (Ebersbach et al., 2014; Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Haas et al., 2024), Type 2 diabetes (n = 1) (Ploydang et al., 2023), and Alzheimer’s disease (n = 1) (Angiolillo et al., 2023).

Intervention characteristics

Of the eight studies incorporated in this review, seven were conducted using a randomized controlled trial design (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Nemoto et al., 2021; Guszkowska et al., 2022; Miyazaki et al., 2022; Angiolillo et al., 2023; Ploydang et al., 2023; Haas et al., 2024), with the remaining study adopting a randomized crossover controlled trial design (Kettinen et al., 2023). The characteristics of the intervention measures are detailed in Table 1. Among the included studies, five studies independently employed NW as the intervention measure (Ebersbach et al., 2014; Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Kettinen et al., 2023; Haas et al., 2024, 2024). One study combined NW with daily protein supplementation (Miyazaki et al., 2022), while another study utilized aquatic NW as the intervention (Ploydang et al., 2023). Additionally, one study implemented a multimodal intervention approach, integrating NW with reality orientation therapy, music therapy, physical exercise, proprioceptive training, and postural rehabilitation (Angiolillo et al., 2023).

Regarding the control groups, three studies employed non-active control conditions (Guszkowska et al., 2022; Miyazaki et al., 2022; Ploydang et al., 2023), which included maintaining usual daily activities (n = 2) (Guszkowska et al., 2022; Ploydang et al., 2023) and nutritional supplementation (daily intake of 8g protein supplements) (n = 1) (Miyazaki et al., 2022). The remaining five studies utilized active control conditions (Ebersbach et al., 2014; Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Angiolillo et al., 2023; Kettinen et al., 2023; Haas et al., 2024), which involved interventions such as domestic exercise (n = 1) (Ebersbach et al., 2014), deep-water exercise (n = 1) (Haas et al., 2024), regular walking (n = 2) (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Miyazaki et al., 2022), and a multimodal training program incorporating reality orientation therapy, physical exercise, and proprioceptive training (n = 1) (Angiolillo et al., 2023).

The duration of a single intervention session varied from 35 to 60 min. The frequencies of intervention included a single session (n = 1) (Kettinen et al., 2023), two sessions per week (n = 5) (Ebersbach et al., 2014; Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Guszkowska et al., 2022; Angiolillo et al., 2023; Haas et al., 2024), and three sessions per week (n = 2) (Miyazaki et al., 2022; Ploydang et al., 2023).

The overall intervention period ranged from 1 day to 24 weeks, and the total number of interventions across the studies was between 1 and 36. Specifically, the intervention protocols varied across the studies: one studies involved 16 sessions over 8 weeks (Ebersbach et al., 2014), one study included 18 sessions over 9 weeks (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020), one study implemented 24 sessions over 3 months (Guszkowska et al., 2022), one study featured 9–12 sessions over 4 weeks (Miyazaki et al., 2022), one study consisted of 48 sessions over 24 weeks (Angiolillo et al., 2023), one study implemented 24 sessions over 24 weeks (Haas et al., 2024), one study involved a single session conducted over 1 day (Kettinen et al., 2023), and one study provided 36 sessions over 12 weeks (Ploydang et al., 2023). Across all studies, participants received prior instruction on the knowledge and skills necessary for NW before commencing the interventions.

Study outcomes

This meta-analysis included 23 effect sizes from 8 studies. These effect sizes encompassed multiple domains of cognitive ability, including global cognitive function (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Miyazaki et al., 2022; Angiolillo et al., 2023; Ploydang et al., 2023; Haas et al., 2024), memory function (Angiolillo et al., 2023), executive function (Miyazaki et al., 2022; Angiolillo et al., 2023; Kettinen et al., 2023; Ploydang et al., 2023), perceptual abilities (Guszkowska et al., 2022; Angiolillo et al., 2023), information processing speed (Guszkowska et al., 2022; Kettinen et al., 2023), and attention (Ebersbach et al., 2014; Guszkowska et al., 2022; Angiolillo et al., 2023). Specific cognitive tests administered and their corresponding domain classifications are detailed in Supplementary Table 3.

Effect of NW on cognitive function levels

The overall effect size for cognitive function was moderate and statistically significant [Hedges’ g = 0.56, 95% CI (0.29, 0.84), PI (−0.57, 1.70), p < 0.01, Figure 2], with substantial heterogeneity (I2 = 70.75%, p < 0.01). Meta-regression analysis indicated no significant association between effect size and the number of intervention sessions (b = 0.042, p = 0.62, Figure 3A), while it revealed a significant positive association between effect size and the total intervention duration (b = 0.0003, p = 0.049, Figure 3B).

FIGURE 2
Forest plot displaying meta-analysis results for cognitive domains: attention, executive function, global cognition, information processing, memory, and perceptual ability. Individual study effect sizes are shown as blue squares with confidence intervals. Summary effect sizes per category are depicted as red diamonds. The overall effect size is represented as a green diamond at the bottom. Heterogeneity statistics and weights are listed alongside study details.

Figure 2. The pooled effect size of NW on cognitive function levels.

FIGURE 3
Two scatter plots titled “Meta-Regression” show the standardized mean difference on the y-axis. The left plot has session number on the x-axis, ranging from 0 to 50, while the right plot has total time, ranging from 500 to 3000. Both plots feature a trend line with a shaded confidence interval, suggesting a slight positive correlation.

Figure 3. Dose-response curve. (A) Session number. (B) Total time.

For specific cognitive abilities, the effect size for executive function was large and significant [Hedges’ g = 0.89, 95% CI (0.27, 1.50), p = 0.01], with substantial heterogeneity (I2 = 73.04%, p < 0.01). The effect size for memory function was moderate but not statistically significant [Hedges’ g = 0.77, 95% CI (−0.86, 2.39), p = 0.11], with non-significant heterogeneity (I2 = 0.00%, p = 0.68). The effect size for global cognition was moderate but not statistically significant [Hedges’ g = 0.73, 95% CI (−0.08, 1.53), p = 0.07], with substantial heterogeneity (I2 = 80.46%, p < 0.01). The effect size for attention was small and not statistically significant [Hedges’ g = 0.20, 95% CI (−0.48, 0.88), p = 0.41], with moderate heterogeneity (I2 = 40.81%, p = 0.17). The effect size for information processing was trivial and not statistically significant [Hedges’ g = 0.18, 95% CI (−2.57, 2.93), p = 0.56], with non-significant heterogeneity (I2 = 7.50%, p = 0.30). The effect size for perceptual ability was trivial and not statistically significant [Hedges’ g = −0.07, 95% CI (−3.70, 3.56), p = 0.85], with non-significant heterogeneity (I2 = 20.61%, p = 0.26). The funnel plot (Figure 4) and Egger’s test (t = 2.47, p = 0.02) indicated a potential risk of publication bias, but the Trim and Fill method for sensitive analysis showed that the pooled effect size (Hedges’ g = 0.56, p < 0.01) was robust.

FIGURE 4
Funnel plot displaying the standardized mean difference on the x-axis and the standard error on the y-axis. Dots represent individual studies, with areas of significance shaded in gray. The plot helps identify publication bias, with two p-value thresholds, less than 0.05 and less than 0.01, indicated.

Figure 4. Funnel plot.

To examine characteristics that may contribute to the impact of Nordic walking on cognitive function, we conducted a subgroup analysis using a random effects model on studies that reported large effects involving cognitive function (Table 2). The analysis was based on the following study characteristics: participants’ health status (i.e., healthy population vs. diseased population) and control type (i.e., active vs. inactive).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Subgroup analysis results regarding the effects of executive function.

The subgroup analysis based on participants’ health status revealed a moderate and statistically significant effect size for the disease group [Hedges’ g = 0.70, 95% CI (0.04, 1.35), p = 0.04], while the healthy group showed a large but non-significant effect size [Hedges’ g = 1.06, 95% CI (−1.28, 3.40), p = 0.19]. The analysis by control type indicated that NW had a large but non-significant effect size compared to the active intervention control group [Hedges’ g = 1.06, 95% CI (−1.28, 3.40), p = 0.19]. In contrast, NW showed a moderate and statistically significant effect size when compared to the inactive control group [Hedges’ g = 0.56, 95% CI (0.20, 0.92), p < 0.01].

Sensitivity analysis

Of the eight included studies, one study was with a randomized crossover controlled design. To evaluate the impact of this study on the results, we performed a sensitivity analysis by excluding it. The overall effect size remained significant after excluding it [Hedges’ g = 0.49, 95% CI (0.23, 0.75), p < 0.01], and was consistent with the previous overall effect size [Hedges’ g = 0.56, 95% CI (0.29, 0.84), p < 0.01]. It was noted that the heterogeneity between studies assessing executive function decreased significantly after excluding this study (before exclusion: I2 = 73.04%, p ≤ 0.01; after exclusion: I2 = 8.42%, p = 0.27), suggesting the difference in the duration of the intervention contributed to the inter study heterogeneity. However, the effect size for executive function also remained significant [Hedges’ g = 0.56, 95% CI (0.06, 1.06), p = 0.04]. These results confirm the robustness of our findings. Furthermore, by eliminating one effect size at a time, we explore the robustness of the pooled results across different cognitive classifications. The sensitivity analysis showed that the results in terms of attention, executive function, information processing, memory and perception were relatively robust (Supplementary Figure 1). In terms of global cognition alone, after excluding a study where the control group engaged in deep-water exercise, the effect size of the meta-analysis showed a significant change. Before exclusion [Hedges’ g = 0.73, 95% CI (−0.08, 1.53), p = 0.07], after exclusion [Hedges’ g = 0.94, 95% CI (0.23, 1.66), p = 0.02]. The above results indicate that the differences in the control conditions might be the reason for the relatively sensitive global cognitive effect. Therefore, these results should be treated with caution.

Risk of bias and GRADE

The methodological assessment details of each included study are presented in Table 3. The overall quality rating of all included studies is high. The overall mean score is 7.13 ± 1.25; high-quality studies account for 100% (8/8). The level of certainty of the evidence was downgraded by one level due to limitations in study bias (Supplementary Table 2).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Quality assessment of included studies (n = 8).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this study represents the first systematic review and meta-analysis to comprehensively evaluate the effects of NW on cognitive function in older adults. The findings highlight the potential of NW as a non-pharmacological intervention to mitigate age-related cognitive decline with moderate quality of evidence. Eight studies were included, and the overall score was assessed as “high quality”, indicating no risk of bias. The primary results suggest that NW significantly enhances the executive function of older adults but does not substantially improve global function, memory function, attention, information processing, and perceptual ability. Meta-regression analysis revealed a positive correlation between the total duration of the intervention and the cognitive benefits. Subgroup analysis indicated that the effect of NW might be more pronounced in the diseased populations as compared to essential healthy individuals. The knowledge from this work suggests that NW should be carefully considered in future studies for the rehabilitation plans of older adults with health conditions.

NW showed no significant effects on cognitive domains except executive function, consistent with prior studies of standard walking (Chen et al., 2020a; Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Tomoto et al., 2021; Lin et al., 2023). The limited cognitive benefits from walking interventions may largely reflect insufficient aerobic intensity, a well-established determinant of exercise-induced neuroplasticity (Mavros et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2020b; Thomas et al., 2020). Although NW increases energy expenditure compared to conventional walking, its intensity probably remains below the threshold required for broad cognitive adaptations, particularly in hippocampal-dependent memory or temporoparietal-mediated perceptual processing (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020; Nemoto et al., 2021; Angiolillo et al., 2023; Kettinen et al., 2023; Baker et al., 2025). The neurocognitive value of NW appears to stem primarily from its motor complexity rather than absolute intensity. Unlike standard walking, NW requires synchronized upper and lower limb activation through pole propulsion, recruiting supplementary motor areas, premotor cortices, and frontoparietal networks (Schiffer et al., 2006; Erickson et al., 2013; Niemann et al., 2014). These regions are integral to planning, decision-making, and inhibitory control (Liu-Ambrose et al., 2012; Erickson et al., 2013; Voelcker-Rehage and Niemann, 2013; Niemann et al., 2014; Müller et al., 2017). Such enhanced cortical activation may improve synaptic density and white matter integrity, supported by elevated BDNF levels in NW interventions (Gmiat et al., 2018). Additionally, NW’s rhythmic bilateral coordination may stimulate cerebellar-thalamocortical circuits critical for executive function, optimizing neural efficiency for task-switching and error monitoring (Passos-Monteiro et al., 2020). Consequently, NW acts as a dual-task modality that enhances prefrontal efficiency through mechanisms distinct from pure aerobic stimulation. Our meta-regression indicated longer interventions were associated with greater cognitive benefits, suggesting executive improvements emerge relatively quickly with NW, while other domains may require extended exposure to achieve intensity thresholds through cumulative neurotrophic effects (Jasim et al., 2024; Baker et al., 2025).

Subgroup analyses indicated greater cognitive benefits from NW in diseased populations (e.g., Alzheimer’s patients) compared to essential healthy older adults. This difference may reflect compensatory neuroplasticity in individuals with health conditions, where baseline cognitive impairment increases sensitivity to exercise-induced neurotrophic and vascular changes (Kirk-Sanchez and McGough, 2013; Ploydang et al., 2023; Kadiyala et al., 2024; Popescu et al., 2024). For example, the capacity of NW to improve cerebral perfusion through upper limb engagement may counter hypoperfusion in Alzheimer’s pathology, particularly benefiting clinical groups (Sanders et al., 2020; Angiolillo et al., 2023). Healthy older adults may require higher-intensity or cognitively enhanced NW protocols to exceed their neurocognitive reserve thresholds (Zhang et al., 2023). Additionally, NW demonstrated comparable efficacy to active controls (e.g., standard walking) but outperformed inactive controls. This suggests that while the biomechanical advantages of NW improve adherence and physical outcomes like reduced perceived exertion (Angiolillo et al., 2023; Haas et al., 2024), its cognitive benefits may overlap with other exercise modalities. Nevertheless, NW’s scalability, low injury risk, and dual-task potential make it a practical option for older adults with mobility limitations. However, all studies using active controls involved essential healthy participants, making it unclear whether outcomes were influenced primarily by health status or control group type. Further research is required to clarify the relevant conclusions.

Limitations

The results of this work still need to be taken with caution. First, the number of included studies was relatively limited (n = 8), which may potentially affect the statistical power of our meta-analysis and meta-regression results. Additionally, due to the limited number of studies, subgroup analyses to characterize the influences of protocol settings of NW (e.g., appropriate number of sessions, the frequency of intervention, and Intervention intensity) cannot be completed. Furthermore, interpretations regarding underlying mechanisms remain to be validated in future studies due to the small sample size and the limited number of existing studies. Lastly, although sensitivity analyses showed robust results, heterogeneity among the studies may have a potential influence on the interpretation of outcome measures. Nevertheless, the knowledge obtained from this work will help inform the appropriate design of intervention protocols of NW.

Conclusion

This study suggests that NW has promise to enhance the executive function in older adults with health conditions. Future RCTs with rigorous designs are needed to help obtain more definitive conclusions on the effects of NW on cognitive function in older adults.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in this article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

HL: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KZ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JG: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. ZW: Conceptualization, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. ZG: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. LL: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. XG: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. JN: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2025.1666449/full#supplementary-material

References

Adderley, J., Ciccarelli, S., and Ferraro, F. V. (2025). Do exercise interventions improve functional mobility and balance in Alzheimer’s patients? A systematic review. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness Online ahead of print. doi: 10.23736/S0022-4707.25.16503-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmadpour, M., Tatar, S., and Luigi Bragazzi, N. (2025). Aging, exercise, and injury: Integrative approaches for maintaining mobility and preventing falls. Int. J. Sport Stud. Health 8, 71–84. doi: 10.61838/kman.intjssh.8.2.9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Angiolillo, A., Leccese, D., Ciccotelli, S., Di Cesare, G., D’Elia, K., Aurisano, N., et al. (2023). Effects of nordic walking in alzheimer’s disease: A single-blind randomized controlled clinical trial. Heliyon 9:e15865. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e15865

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonenko, D., Fromm, A. E., Thams, F., Kuzmina, A., Backhaus, M., Knochenhauer, E., et al. (2024). Cognitive training and brain stimulation in patients with cognitive impairment: A randomized controlled trial. Alz. Res. Therapy 16:6. doi: 10.1186/s13195-024-01381-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahar-Fuchs, A., Clare, L., and Woods, B. (2013). Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2013:CD003260. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003260.pub2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, L. D., Espeland, M. A., Whitmer, R. A., Snyder, H. M., Leng, X., Lovato, L., et al. (2025). Structured vs self-guided multidomain lifestyle interventions for global cognitive function: The US POINTER randomized clinical trial. JAMA 334, 681–691. doi: 10.1001/jama.2025.12923

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, T. M., Kabisch, S., Pfeiffer, A. F. H., and Weickert, M. O. (2023). The effects of the mediterranean diet on health and gut microbiota. Nutrients 15:2150. doi: 10.3390/nu15092150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, L. L., Dhana, K., Liu, X., Carey, V. J., Ventrelle, J., Johnson, K., et al. (2023). Trial of the MIND diet for prevention of cognitive decline in older persons. N. Engl. J. Med. 389, 602–611. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2302368

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettio, L. E. B., Rajendran, L., and Gil-Mohapel, J. (2017). The effects of aging in the hippocampus and cognitive decline. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 79, 66–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackman, J., Swirski, M., Clynes, J., Harding, S., Leng, Y., and Coulthard, E. (2021). Pharmacological and non-pharmacological interventions to enhance sleep in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review. J. Sleep Res. 30:e13229. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13229

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., and Rothstein, H. R. (2013). Introduction to meta-analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Butler, M., McCreedy, E., Nelson, V. A., Desai, P., Ratner, E., Fink, H. A., et al. (2018). Does cognitive training prevent cognitive decline?: A systematic review. Ann. Intern. Med. 168, 63–68. doi: 10.7326/M17-1531

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cashin, A. G., and McAuley, J. H. (2020). Clinimetrics: Physiotherapy evidence database (PEDro) scale. J. Physiother. 66:59. doi: 10.1016/j.jphys.2019.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellano, J. M., Mosher, K. I., Abbey, R. J., McBride, A. A., James, M. L., Berdnik, D., et al. (2017). Human umbilical cord plasma proteins revitalize hippocampal function in aged mice. Nature 544, 488–492. doi: 10.1038/nature22067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F.-T., Etnier, J. L., Chan, K.-H., Chiu, P.-K., Hung, T.-M., and Chang, Y.-K. (2020). Effects of exercise training interventions on executive function in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 50, 1451–1467. doi: 10.1007/s40279-020-01292-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd Edn. Hoboken, NJ: Taylor and Francis.

Google Scholar

Cunha, P. M., Silva, G. O., Zou, L., Oliveira, M. D., Kanegusuku, H., Yu, Q., et al. (2025). Comparison of the acute effects of breaking up prolonged sitting time with single-task or a dual-task walking on cognitive function and cerebral blood flow in older adults: A randomized crossover trial. Geroscience Online ahead of print. doi: 10.1007/s11357-025-01817-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Morton, N. A. (2009). The PEDro scale is a valid measure of the methodological quality of clinical trials: A demographic study. Aust. J. Physiother. 55, 129–133. doi: 10.1016/s0004-9514(09)70043-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doi, T., Shimada, H., Makizako, H., Tsutsumimoto, K., Uemura, K., Anan, Y., et al. (2014). Cognitive function and gait speed under normal and dual-task walking among older adults with mild cognitive impairment. BMC Neurol. 14:67. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-14-67

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duval, S., and Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 56, 455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebersbach, G., Ebersbach, A., Gandor, F., Wegner, B., Wissel, J., and Kupsch, A. (2014). Impact of physical exercise on reaction time in patients with parkinson’s disease-data from the Berlin BIG study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 95, 996–999. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.10.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Gildengers, A. G., and Butters, M. A. (2013). Physical activity and brain plasticity in late adulthood. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 15, 99–108. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.1/kerickson

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, X., Wheatley, E. G., and Villeda, S. A. (2017). Mechanisms of hippocampal aging and the potential for rejuvenation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 251–272. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031357

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraziani, F., and Eken, Ö (2024). Enhancing cognitive abilities and delaying cognitive decline in the elderly through exercise-based health management systems. Int. J. Sport Stud. Health 7, 13–22. doi: 10.61838/kman.intjssh.7.2.2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzoni, L., Monteiro, E., Oliveira, H., Da Rosa, R., Costa, R., Rieder, C., et al. (2018). A 9-week nordic and free walking improve postural balance in Parkinson’s disease. Sports Med. Int. Open 02, E28–E34. doi: 10.1055/s-0043-124757

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., and Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. J. Exp. Psychol. General 141, 2–18. doi: 10.1037/a0024338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gmiat, A., Jaworska, J., Micielska, K., Kortas, J., Prusik, K., Lipowski, M., et al. (2018). Improvement of cognitive functions in response to a regular Nordic walking training in elderly women - A change dependent on the training experience. Exp. Gerontol. 104, 105–112. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2018.02.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomeñuka, N. A., Oliveira, H. B., Silva, E. S., Costa, R. R., Kanitz, A. C., Liedtke, G. V., et al. (2019). Effects of Nordic walking training on quality of life, balance and functional mobility in elderly: A randomized clinical trial. PLoS One 14:e0211472. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211472

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzales, M. M., Garbarino, V. R., Pollet, E., Palavicini, J. P., Kellogg, D. L., Kraig, E., et al. (2022). Biological aging processes underlying cognitive decline and neurodegenerative disease. J. Clin. Invest. 132:e158453. doi: 10.1172/JCI158453

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guszkowska, M., Piotrowska, J., Les, A., and Rutkowska, I. (2022). Nordic walking combined with simple cognitive exercises improves older women ability to select visual stimuli proportionally to the increase in physical fitness. Acta Kinesiol. 16, 93–98. doi: 10.51371/issn.1840-2976.2022.16.1.12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haas, A., Delabary, M., Passos-Monteiro, E., Wolffenbuttel, M., Donida, R., Casal, M., et al. (2024). The effects of Brazilian dance, deep-water exercise and nordic walking, pre-and post-12 weeks, on functional-motor and non-motor symptoms in trained PwPD. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 118:105285. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2023.105285

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatami, F., Atani, S. A., Caron, V., and Yousefi, S. (2025). Exploring the effects of physical activity levels and sleep quality on cognitive failure in elderly: A cross-sectional study. Int. J. Sports Stud. Health 8, 1–15. doi: 10.61838/kman.intjssh.8.4.6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hortobágyi, T., Vetrovsky, T., Balbim, G. M., Sorte Silva, N. C. B., Manca, A., Deriu, F., et al. (2022). The impact of aerobic and resistance training intensity on markers of neuroplasticity in health and disease. Ageing Res. Rev. 80:101698. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2022.101698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, X., Zhao, X., Li, B., Cai, Y., Zhang, S., Wan, Q., et al. (2022). Comparative efficacy of various exercise interventions on cognitive function in patients with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. J. Sport Health Sci. 11, 212–223. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2021.05.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasim, S. A., Singh, M., Al-Dhalimy, A. M. B., Zwamel, A. H., Jawad, I. A., and Shalaby, N. S. (2024). The impact of chronic exercise on cognitive function: An overview of reviews. Iran J. Psychiatry 19, 463–472. doi: 10.18502/ijps.v19i4.16561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadiyala, S., Bhamidipati, P., and Malla, R. R. (2024). Neuroplasticity: Pathophysiology and role in major depressive disorder. Crit. Rev. Oncog. 29, 19–32. doi: 10.1615/CritRevOncog.2024051197

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kettinen, J., Tikkanen, H., Hiltunen, M., Murray, A., Horn, N., Taylor, W., et al. (2023). Cognitive and biomarker responses in healthy older adults to a 18-hole golf round and different walking types: A randomised cross-over study. BMJ Open Sport Exercise Med. 9:e001629. doi: 10.1136/bmjsem-2023-001629

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirk-Sanchez, N., and McGough, E. (2013). Physical exercise and cognitive performance in the elderly: Current perspectives. Clin. Interv. Aging 9, 51–62. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S39506

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, A., Bryan, J., and Murphy, K. (2016). Is the Mediterranean diet a feasible approach to preserving cognitive function and reducing risk of dementia for older adults in Western countries? New insights and future directions. Ageing Res. Rev. 25, 85–101. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.10.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavie, C. J., Arena, R., Swift, D. L., Johannsen, N. M., Sui, X., Lee, D., et al. (n.d.). Exercise and the Cardiovascular System.

Google Scholar

Leeuwis, A. E., Benedictus, M. R., Kuijer, J. P. A., Binnewijzend, M. A. A., Hooghiemstra, A. M., Verfaillie, S. C. J., et al. (2017). Lower cerebral blood flow is associated with impairment in multiple cognitive domains in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 13, 531–540. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.08.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, B.-Y., Wang, Y., Tang, H., and Chen, S.-D. (2017). The role of cognitive activity in cognition protection: From Bedside to Bench. Transl. Neurodegener. 6:7. doi: 10.1186/s40035-017-0078-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J.-C., Chen, I.-H., and Cheng, F.-Y. (2023). Review articles (Meta-Analyses) effects of walking on cognitive function in individuals with mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. 23:500. doi: 10.1186/s12877-023-04235-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu-Ambrose, T., Nagamatsu, L. S., Voss, M. W., Khan, K. M., and Handy, T. C. (2012). Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: A 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1690–1698. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.05.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mavros, Y., Gates, N., Wilson, G. C., Jain, N., Meiklejohn, J., Brodaty, H., et al. (2017). Mediation of cognitive function improvements by strength gains after resistance training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: Outcomes of the study of mental and resistance training. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 65, 550–559. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14542

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyazaki, A., Okuyama, T., Mori, H., Sato, K., Kumamoto, K., and Hiyama, A. (2022). Effects of two short-term aerobic exercises on cognitive function in healthy older adults during COVID-19 confinement in Japan: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:6202. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19106202

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, P., Rehfeld, K., Schmicker, M., Hökelmann, A., Dordevic, M., Lessmann, V., et al. (2017). Evolution of neuroplasticity in response to physical activity in old age: The case for dancing. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:56. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nemoto, Y., Sakurai, R., Ogawa, S., Maruo, K., and Fujiwara, Y. (2021). Effects of an unsupervised Nordic walking intervention on cognitive and physical function among older women engaging in volunteer activity. J. Exerc. Sci. Fitness 19, 209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.jesf.2021.06.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemann, C., Godde, B., Staudinger, U. M., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2014). Exercise-induced changes in basal ganglia volume and cognition in older adults. Neuroscience 281, 147–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.09.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parnetti, L., Senin, U., and Mecocci, P. (1997). Cognitive enhancement therapy for Alzheimer’s disease. The way forward. Drugs 53, 752–768. doi: 10.2165/00003495-199753050-00003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Passos-Monteiro, E., Schuch, F., Franzoni, L., Carvalho, A., Gomeñuka, N., Becker, M., et al. (2020). Nordic walking and free walking improve the quality of life, cognitive function, and depressive symptoms in individuals with parkinson’s disease: A randomized clinical trial. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 5:82. doi: 10.3390/jfmk5040082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ploydang, T., Khovidhunkit, W., Tanaka, H., and Suksom, D. (2023). Nordic walking in water on cerebrovascular reactivity and cognitive function in elderly patients with type 2 diabetes. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 55, 1803–1811. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000003216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Popescu, B. O., Batzu, L., Ruiz, P. J. G., Tulbã, D., Moro, E., and Santens, P. (2024). Neuroplasticity in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neural Transm. 131, 1329–1339. doi: 10.1007/s00702-024-02813-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulose, S. M., Miller, M. G., Scott, T., and Shukitt-Hale, B. (2017). Nutritional factors affecting adult neurogenesis and cognitive function. Adv. Nutr. 8, 804–811. doi: 10.3945/an.117.016261

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichlen, D. A., and Alexander, G. E. (2017). Adaptive capacity: An evolutionary neuroscience model linking exercise, cognition, and brain health. Trends Neurosci. 40, 408–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sáez De Asteasu, M. L., Martínez-Velilla, N., Zambom-Ferraresi, F., and Casas-Herrero, Á, andIzquierdo, M. (2017). Role of physical exercise on cognitive function in healthy older adults: A systematic review of randomized clinical trials. Ageing Res. Rev. 37, 117–134. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2017.05.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, L. M. J., Hortobágyi, T., Karssemeijer, E. G. A., Van Der Zee, E. A., Scherder, E. J. A., and Van Heuvelen, M. J. G. (2020). Effects of low- and high-intensity physical exercise on physical and cognitive function in older persons with dementia: A randomized controlled trial. Alz. Res. Therapy 12:28. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00597-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandroff, B. M., Motl, R. W., Wylie, G. R., Wells, G. E., Wender, C. L. A., Picone, M. A., et al. (2025). Aerobic walking exercise training boosts thalamic connectivity in MS patients with cognitive processing speed impairment. Brain Cogn. 189:106349. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2025.106349

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiffer, T., Knicker, A., Hoffman, U., Harwig, B., Hollmann, W., and Strüder, H. K. (2006). Physiological responses to nordic walking, walking and jogging. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 98, 56–61. doi: 10.1007/s00421-006-0242-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherrington, C., Moseley, A. M., Herbert, R. D., Elkins, M. R., and Maher, C. G. (2010). Ten years of evidence to guide physiotherapy interventions: Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro). Br. J. Sports Med. 44, 836–837. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2009.066357

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Srikanth, V., Sinclair, A. J., Hill-Briggs, F., Moran, C., and Biessels, G. J. (2020). Type 2 diabetes and cognitive dysfunction—towards effective management of both comorbidities. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 8, 535–545. doi: 10.1016/S2213-8587(20)30118-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, B. P., Tarumi, T., Sheng, M., Tseng, B., Womack, K. B., Cullum, C. M., et al. (2020). Brain perfusion change in patients with mild cognitive impairment after 12 months of aerobic exercise training. JAD 75, 617–631. doi: 10.3233/JAD-190977

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomoto, T., Tarumi, T., Chen, J. N., Hynan, L. S., Cullum, C. M., and Zhang, R. (2021). One-year aerobic exercise altered cerebral vasomotor reactivity in mild cognitive impairment. J. Appl. Physiol. 131, 119–130. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00158.2021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dyck, C. H., Swanson, C. J., Aisen, P., Bateman, R. J., Chen, C., Gee, M., et al. (2023). Lecanemab in early Alzheimer’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 388, 9–21. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2212948

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Veroniki, A. A., Jackson, D., Viechtbauer, W., Bender, R., Bowden, J., Knapp, G., et al. (2016). Methods to estimate the between-study variance and its uncertainty in meta-analysis. Res. Synthesis Methods 7, 55–79. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1164

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vints, W. A. J., Šeikinaitë, J., Gökçe, E., Kušleikienë, S., Šarkinaite, M., Valatkeviciene, K., et al. (2024). Resistance exercise effects on hippocampus subfield volumes and biomarkers of neuroplasticity and neuroinflammation in older adults with low and high risk of mild cognitive impairment: A randomized controlled trial. GeroScience 46, 3971–3991. doi: 10.1007/s11357-024-01110-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Voelcker-Rehage, C., and Niemann, C. (2013). Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2268–2295. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, M., Jia, J., Yang, Y., Zhang, L., and Wang, X. (2023). Effects of exercise interventions on cognitive functions in healthy populations: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 92:102116. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2023.102116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Nordic walking, exercise, cognitive function, older adults, meta-analysis

Citation: Li H, Zhu K, Gan J, Wang Z, Gao Z, Liu L, Guo X and Niu J (2025) The effects of Nordic walking on cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 17:1666449. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2025.1666449

Received: 17 July 2025; Accepted: 03 September 2025;
Published: 17 September 2025.

Edited by:

Yih-Kuen Jan, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, United States

Reviewed by:

Alexandra Wolf, RIKEN Center for Advanced Intelligence Project (AIP), Japan
Morteza Taheri, University of Tehran, Iran
Hamza Küçük, Ondokuz May ı s University, Türkiye

Copyright © 2025 Li, Zhu, Gan, Wang, Gao, Liu, Guo and Niu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jianfeng Niu, Nzg3NDUxMDg3QHFxLmNvbQ==; Jianyu Gan, MjAxOTAxMDI1MEBic3UuZWR1LmNu

These authors have contributed equally to this work and shared the first authorship

ORCID: Haobai Li, orcid.org/0009-0009-2559-1453

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.