Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Artif. Intell., 14 June 2021
Sec. Language and Computation
This article is part of the Research Topic Computational Sociolinguistics View all 32 articles

Characterising Online News Comments: A Multi-Dimensional Cruise Through Online Registers

  • Discourse Processing Lab, Department of Linguistics, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada

News organisations often allow public comments at the bottom of their news stories. These comments constitute a fruitful source of data to investigate linguistic variation online; their characteristics, however, are rather understudied. This paper thus contributes to the description of online news comments and online language in English. In this spirit, we apply multi-dimensional analysis to a large dataset of online news comments and compare them to a corpus of online registers, thus placing online comments in the space of register variation online. We find that online news comments are involved-evaluative and informational at the same time, but mostly argumentative in nature, with such argumentation taking an informal shape. Our analyses lead us to conclude that online registers are a different mode of communication, neither spoken nor written, with individual variation across different types of online registers.

1 Introduction

We present a text-linguistic study of the characteristics of online news comments as compared to other online registers. In contrast to many other registers on the web, online news comments have so far not been thoroughly scrutinised. However, there has been a sense, among journalists (Woollaston, 2013; McGuire, 2015) and researchers alike (Godes and Mayzlin, 2004; Marcoccia, 2004; North, 2007), that online news comments are like conversation or dialogue. We have challenged this assumption, in a related article comparing online news comments to face-to-face conversation and other traditional registers: While online news comments were found to contain features of personal involvement typical of face-to-face conversation, they can best be described as a type of written, evaluative discourse (Ehret and Taboada, 2020). As a matter of fact, we argue that online news comments should be regarded as their own register, and that language on the web, in general, is quite different from either standard written or spoken language (Ehret and Taboada, 2020, 23–24). It is natural to describe new registers in terms of other, more familiar registers, which is perhaps what leads to the characterisation of online news comments as conversations. This label has also sometimes been applied to blogs, but has also been found inadequate, as Peterson (2011) has argued. In his analysis of blogs, Peterson found that, although blogs have an expressive potential, such potential is not realised in the same way as it is in conversation.

An ever-growing body of research analyses online language in general (e.g., Crystal, 2011; McCulloch, 2020), specific online registers, such as email (Frehner, 2008; McVeigh, 2020), blogs (Herring et al., 2004; Peterson, 2011), reviews (Taboada, 2011; Vásquez, 2014), Facebook (West, 2013; Farina, 2018), Twitter (Zappavigna, 2012; Clarke and Grieve, 2019), or online and social media language in general (Giltrow and Stein, 2009; Titak and Roberson, 2013; Page et al., 2014; Biber and Egbert, 2016; Berber Sardinha, 2018; Biber and Egbert, 2018). Little attention, however, has been paid to the linguistic characteristics of online news comments, a register now ubiquitous in our interactions with news, whether on the pages of newspapers or through platforms such as Twitter and Facebook.

Against this backdrop, the present paper explores the structural linguistic properties of online news comments in comparison with other online registers such as travel and opinion blogs, interactive discussions and news reports, or advice pieces, since our previous analysis involved a traditional written and spoken corpus. We will thus establish what—if not like spontaneous conversation—online news comments are like in the context of other online registers. The data for our analysis is drawn from the comments section of the Simon Fraser University Opinion and Comments Corpus (SOCC) on the one hand, and the Corpus of Online Registers of English (CORE) on the other. SOCC is the largest corpus of online comments publicly available, while CORE is to date the largest available corpus of registers on the web. Methodologically, we conduct a multi-dimensional analysis (Biber, 1988), considering a comprehensive set of well-established lexico-grammatical features, to describe online news comments along the emerging dimensions of variation in our dataset.

Our analysis shows that multi-dimensional analysis (MDA) is very well suited to capturing the variation found in some common online registers. By applying the part-of-speech tag frequency statistics and dimensionality reduction characteristic of MDA, we are able to place online news comments in a unique space as compared to other online registers. To be more precise, we find that there are three dimensions along which online news comments can be described in online variational space, with two of them being most prominent. The first dimension, which we labelled “Involved-evaluative” points to the involved nature (in the Biberian sense; Section 3) of online registers and online comments, with an involvement that includes evaluative meaning. We find, however, that the most characteristic dimension is “Informational-argumentative”, marked by information density (nominalisations, longer words) and argumentative features such as conjuncts. Finally, the third, minor dimension, “Narrative-descriptive vs. instructional” supports our analyses of the first two, showing an involved personal narrative mixed with instructional detail.

The paper is structured as follows: Section 2 describes the data source and methodology. In Section 3 the results of the MDA analysis are presented. Section 4 discusses online news comments in light of the results. Section 5 offers a brief summary and concluding remarks.

2 Material and Methods

2.1 Online News Comments and Other Online Registers

Our aim is to compare online comments to other, well-studied online registers. To that end, we use the Corpus of Online Registers of English (CORE), the largest, most diverse corpus of online language currently available (Biber et al., 2015; Egbert et al., 2015; Biber and Egbert, 2018). CORE was conceived as an attempt to classify various online registers. The data was obtained by sampling publicly-available documents and tagging them in a bottom-up process. About 50,000 web documents were labelled through crowd sourcing, resulting in six general (written) register types and several sub-registers. The general registers were provided by the researchers, but the sub-registers were crowd sourced and labelled by users according to guidelines (Biber et al., 2015). Registers were labelled according to their communicative purpose: to narrate events, describe or explain information, express opinion, persuade, explain instructions, or to express oneself through lyrics. Many of the sub-registers were deemed to be hybrid, because they include characteristics of more than one register or sub-register. CORE thus comprises, for instance, sub-registers (with main register in parentheses) such as personal blog (narrative), FAQ (description), review (opinion), description for sale1 (persuasive), recipe (instructional), or song lyrics (lyrical).

We chose CORE because of its focus on the public web, the readily available set of registers that one is likely to encounter online. An additional set of computer-mediated communication exists, including text messages (SMS, WhatsApp, Telegram, Signal, Direct Messages on Facebook or Twitter, etc.,), but that tends to be a one-to-one or small-group type of communication, not one to be publicly displayed the way online news comments are.

From this varied source of online materials, we select a large sample, excluding registers that are not unambiguously defined or not directly comparable to the online news comments we are interested in. In this vein, we exclude all hybrid registers, registers labelled as “other”, lyrical and fully narrative registers, i.e. short story, historical article, and narrative, as well as spoken material. The sample does include typical online registers such as personal blog, travel blog, or news report, which are also labelled as narrative in CORE. In general, the sampling criterion excluded registers that may appear outside of the internet (short stories), but included online-only registers (travel blog), even when they were both under the same macro-register (narrative). This sample of CORE amounts to 43.7 million words (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Overview of analysed registers, corpus source, and number of words.

The online news comments come from the comments section of the SFU Opinion and Comments Corpus (SOCC), a large dataset of comments posted on the website of the Canadian English-language newspaper The Globe and Mail (Kolhatkar et al., 2020). The corpus contains more than 660,000 comments, a rough total of 37 million words. In this paper we specifically analyse comment threads, sequentially posted comments with a seemingly conversation-like structure, rather than individual comments. The analysis is furthermore restricted to comment threads with a minimum of 700 words, to improve the robustness of the multi-dimensional analysis (cf. Ehret and Taboada, 2020, 6). The comment threads were then analysed as individual comments, for a total of 5,949 comments. This selection of the SOCC corpus contains 5.8 million words and 388,141 sentences (but note that sentence boundaries are imprecise due to the online and informal nature of the data).

We should point out that we analyse comment threads rather than individual comments. This is in part due to technical considerations, because multi-dimensional analysis requires texts of a certain length, with 400 words the most common minimum length in the literature (Biber, 1995). There are also methodological considerations, in that what we are studying is the nature of online comments, which are typically posted in sequential form and constitute a thread of ideas and contributions. The drawback of this method is that the communicative function of one comment may be different from the next comment. We treat the entire thread as a communicative event, just like spoken conversations which include more than one participant.

2.2 Multi-Dimensional Analysis

Multi-dimensional analysis (MDA), originally introduced by Biber (1988) to describe variation in written and spoken registers of English, is a multi-variate statistical technique and the classic tool in text-linguistic approaches to register variation. MDA employs exploratory factor analysis to determine the shared variation in a given dataset based on the co-occurrence frequencies of linguistic features. The extracted factors are then interpreted as dimensions of variation according to the functional-communicative properties of the most important linguistic features on each factor.

We conduct a multi-dimensional analysis of our dataset largely following the statistical recommendations outlined in Biber (1988, 71–93), which we have also employed and detailed in previous work (Ehret and Taboada, 2020, 7–11). This paper differs from our previous work in that it focuses specifically on online language. To be more precise about the methodology, we apply maximum likelihood factor analysis as available in the R stats package and utilise a promax factor rotation. All statistics, unless otherwise indicated, were performed in R (R Core Team, 2020). The scripts, all statistics described here and elsewhere in the paper, additional statistical material, and data are available on GitHub.2

The linguistic features analysed in this paper consist of 67 core grammatical features of English customarily utilised in MDA studies (Biber, 1988; Biber and Finegan, 1989; Pavalanathan et al., 2017; Clarke and Grieve, 2019). These features include, but are not limited to, modals, pronouns, subordination and coordination, tense and aspect markers, as well as some special verb classes (Biber, 1988, 221–245). The dataset was automatically annotated with part-of-speech tags for these features using the Multi-dimensional Analysis Tagger, version 1.3.2 (Nini, 2019), a replication of Biber’s (1988) original MDA tagger.3 The part-of-speech tags and corresponding features are listed in Supplementary Table S1 in the supplementary material. Subsequently, the occurrence frequencies of the 67 features were automatically retrieved, and normalised per 1,000 word tokens using a custom-made python script (available from our GitHub repository; see Data Availability Statement at the end of the paper). The features type-token-ratio (TTR) and average word length (AWL) were not normalised. Type-token ratio was calculated for the first 400 words in each text file, and average word length was calculated by dividing the number of orthographic characters by the number of tokens in each text file.

With an overall measure for sample adequacy of 0.77 and a p-value = 0 for Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity, our dataset is statistically suitable for conducting a factor analysis (Dziuban and Shirkey, 1974, 358–359). After inspecting the screeplot of eigenvalues in Figure 1, which shows a first break after the third factor before flattening out into a straight line, and the linguistic interpretability of the factors, we extract three factors for the final model (Supplementary Table S2). Traditionally, a factor is regarded as linguistically interpretable if it comprises at least five salient loadings. Following Biber (1988, 87), we consider loadings with a conservative cut-off ≥ |0.3| as statistically significant and hence salient. Note that Factor 3 is not fully linguistically interpretable according to these criteria, because it only comprises four salient loadings. However, it is included in the final model in order to avoid conflating factors, and to enhance the interpretability of the other factors in the model. Furthermore, for a tentative interpretation of Factor 3, we consider secondary features with loadings ≥ |0.2|. The total variance explained by the final model is about 20%.4

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Screeplot of eigenvalues for the first twelve factors. Eigenvalues were rounded to the second digit.

Finally, factor scores are automatically calculated for each text in the dataset. Factor scores indicate the position of each text on a given factor: the higher the absolute value of a factor score for a given text on a specific factor, the more typical is this text for the factor and the underlying linguistic dimension represented by the factor (Biber, 1988, 93). Additionally, factor scores also indicate on which pole of a factor a given text is to be positioned. Positive factor scores indicate that a given text weighs on the positive pole of a specific factor while negative scores indicate that a given text weighs on the negative pole of a specific factor. Consider, for instance, the text with the filename 19_N_personal_1747770_MAT.txt which belongs to the register personal blog. This text has a factor score of 2.36 on the first factor and a factor score of −0.85 on the second factor. On the basis of these factor scores, we can conclude that this text is more typical of Factor 1 than of Factor 2. Furthermore, the text contains many of the linguistic features which load high on the positive pole of Factor 1 and is marked by the absence of linguistic features which load high on Factor 2 (a detailed interpretation of the factors is given in Section 3).

In addition to factor scores, we calculate scores to position the individual registers as a whole on each factor. These scores are referred to as “mean factor scores” in this paper and are calculated as the arithmetic mean of the factor scores for all texts pertaining to a given register (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Mean factor scores per register. Positive values indicate that a register weighs on the positive pole of a factor; negative values indicate that a register weighs on the negative pole of a factor. All values were rounded to four decimal points.

3 Dimensions of Linguistic Variation Online

In this section, the extracted factors are interpreted as dimensions of variation. This means that each factor is linguistically interpreted based on the co-occurrence and complementary distribution of linguistic features and their shared functional-communicative properties (Biber, 1988, 91–92). Specifically, features with loading |≥ 0.3| are given the greatest importance in this interpretation, yet secondary features with less salient loadings are also considered. Features which load on multiple factors with the same polarity are primarily considered on the factor where they load highest. This interpretation is aided and confirmed by analysing the distribution of registers across the various dimensions. Table 3 provides a summary of the three factors (for a complete list of features and loadings, see the GitHub repository in footnote 2).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Overview of the three factors including features with loadings ≥ |0.3|. Positive loadings indicate co-occurrence of the features; negative loadings indicate complementary distribution.

The factors in our analysis and the emerging dimensions for this particular set of online registers vary from those that have been proposed for the CORE corpus by Biber and Egbert (2018). In their analysis, Biber and Egbert explore the entire CORE corpus, which, as we mention in Section 2.1, includes hybrid registers and spoken registers. Their first dimension, for instance, is thus “Oral-involved vs. literate”, which captures the differences between song lyrics, TV dialogue, and interactive discussions on the one hand, and written registers such as research articles and encyclopedia entries on the other. Our dataset is a different one and, consequently, the emerging dimensions capture variation of online registers that are closer in nature to online news comments.

Factor 1 comprises 15 positive and seven negative features with salient loadings ≥ |0.3| and is therefore the most clearly defined factor. On the positive pole of the factor, we find features which are typical of spontaneous, informal, and involved communication such as contractions, first and second person pronouns, analytic negation, the pronoun it, private verbs which express personal attitudes or emotions (e.g. believe, decide, know), and emphatics (Biber, 1988, 105–106). In addition, some of the most salient features are not only well known as characteristic of spontaneous spoken language (Biber, 1988, 228–229), but have also been recently identified as markers of evaluation and opinion in online news comments (Ehret and Taboada, 2020, 13): be as main verb, adverbs, and predicative adjectives. Together, these three features often occur in constructions which are typically used to convey evaluation (White, 2003; Hunston, 2011), such as in Example (1).

(1)  a. It’sbe main verb not idealpredicative adjective for my husband                  […] (personal blog, 19_N_personal _0000263_MAT.txt).

     b. This isbe main verb sometimesadverb hardpredicative adjective to                  conjure up when you have been woken numerous times in                  the night to feed. (advice, 10_O_advice_3360949_MAT.txt).

On the negative pole of Factor 1, we find features which are well known as characteristic of an informational and abstract style in English: average word length, nouns, nominalisations, attributive adjectives, and prepositions are all indicators of information density and lexical specificity and are common in scientific or academic writing (Biber, 1988, 104–105). All in all, Factor 1 strongly resembles the Dimension “Involved vs. informational production” identified in Biber (1988) with, one could argue, an evaluative slant. We therefore interpret Factor 1 as Dimension 1 “Involved-evaluative vs. informational” and we shorten it to “Involved-evaluative” in the rest of the paper. In work by Biber and colleagues, multiple registers across different languages have been shown to be distributed across two main axes, involved vs. informational. The involved dimension refers to language use that includes “affective, interactional, and generalized content”, as opposed to language with “high informational density and exact informational content” (Biber, 1988, p. 107).

This interpretation for Factor 1 dovetails with the distribution of registers on Dimension 1 (Figure 2). For instance, research and encyclopedia articles are located on the negative pole, while personal blogs and interactive discussions are representative of the positive pole of Dimension 1. Note that, in contrast to Biber’s original Dimension 1, the dimension presented in this paper does not represent the fundamental distinction between written and spoken language. Instead, all registers analysed in this paper are written, and Dimension 1 thus distinguishes between online written discourse which is involved and evaluative and online written discourse which is informational (and presumably constructed as objective).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Register distribution across the three factors/dimensions. Colour intensity indicates strength of mean factor scores. Red bars indicate negative values; blue bars indicate positive values.

In contrast to the first factor, Factor 2 is defined exclusively by positive features. The five salient positive features are nominalisations, average word length, that verb complement, conjuncts, and attributive adjectives. The co-occurrence of nominalisations, high average word length, conjuncts, and attributive adjectives are indicators of information density and information integration. Nominalisations can also be interpreted as conveying specialised or abstract information (Biber, 1988, 227) such as, for instance, in scientific discourse. Conjuncts (e.g. however, on the other hand) are also prominent markers of argumentation and coherence (Halliday and Hasan, 1976; van Eemeren et al., 2007; Tseronis, 2011; Kolhatkar and Taboada, 2017b) as exemplified in (2-a). The argumentative aspect of Factor 2 is stressed by the secondary non-salient feature suasive verbs (feature loading 0.296) which express varying degrees of persuasion such as propose, suggest, or allow, but also future intent and certain speech acts (e.g. ask) (see Quirk et al. (1985) for a full list). In combination with that verb complements, we interpret them as markers of argumentative discourse with the aim to promote ideas, make an argument, or persuade an audience, as in Example (2-b). A look at the distribution of registers confirms this interpretation. Research articles are the most representative register on this factor, followed by FAQ and comments. Factor 2 is thus dubbed Dimension 2 “Informational-argumentative”.

(2) a. These are issues of jurisdiction, howeverconjunct, not privacy. (comments, comments_28791923 _54_MAT).

b. He proposessuasive verb thatthat verb complement an individual might be genetically predetermined […] (research article, 31_IDE_res_0026415_MAT.txt).

Factor 3 counts only four features with loadings ≥ |0.3|, and can, strictly speaking, not be fully and reliably linguistically interpreted. The interpretation provided here is therefore a tentative one, but we believe it is useful, as it supports the interpretation of the first two factors. Past tense, third person pronouns, and public verbs load on the positive pole of Factor 3 and are clear indicators for a narrative style (Biber, 1988, 108). Furthermore, the non-salient feature that deletion with a loading of 0.264 suggests description or elaboration of information—although this feature is common in spontaneous production (Biber, 1988, 244). Representative registers on the positive pole of Factor 3 are description of a person, personal blog, and sports report. Such registers describe or narrate events, actions, or people in a spontaneous or informal fashion and thus correspond to the co-occurrence of the positive features described above.

There is only one salient negative feature on Factor 3: present tense. According to the literature, present tense usually occurs in spontaneous and involved discourse. To derive at a more dependable interpretation, we examine secondary, non-salient features with loadings | ≥ 2| which do not load higher with the same polarity on another factor. These features consist of second person pronouns (−0.26) and modals expressing possibility (−0.206). Together with present tense verbs, they can serve to convey instruction, direction, or advice as illustrated in Example 3. As a matter of fact, the most characteristic registers on the negative pole of Factor 3 are FAQ, description for sale, advice, and recipe. Factor 3 is thus tentatively labelled as Dimension 3 “Narrative-descriptive vs. instructional”.

(3) a. If the feta ispresent tense more salty than sharp, you2nd person pronoun maypossibility modal want to squeeze over a little lemon juice (recipe, 07_I_recipe_1478719_MAT.txt).

b. If you2nd person pronoun ’represent tense expecting some kind of fairy tale ending, you2nd person pronoun canpossibility modal forgetpresent tense about that right now. (description for sale, 16_IP_sale_0010352 _MAT.txt).

All together, these three factors paint a clear picture of the nature of online comments and online registers. We find an involved vs. informational divide, a result that has consistently been found in multi-dimensional analyses to be a feature of most registers, including cross-linguistically (Biber, 1995), and thus proposed as a universal of register variation (Biber, 2014). In our case, that first dimension is also imbued with evaluative meaning, conveyed by be as a main verb and predicative adjectives, which is why we have characterised that Factor as “Involved-evaluative”.

The “Informal argumentation” label for Factor 2 will be familiar to anyone who has spent any time online. One is likely to encounter vast amounts of argumentation, often involving a passionate defence of somebody’s choice of movie, book, video game, or other artistic productions and consumer products. Argumentation, of course, is often deployed to defend or attack political ideas, argue for and against the conspiracy theory du jour, or to praise and vilify public figures. The web is an opinionated space and comments on news even more so. This is what Tufekci (2008) has described as the expressive internet.

Finally, Factor 3 points to the helpful and friendly aspects of the internet, a place where we can encounter descriptions and personal narratives, together with extremely helpful advice on the most esoteric or mundane aspects of everyday life, the instrumental internet (Tufekci, 2008). We can personally attest to the usefulness of the internet’s collective wisdom when it comes to answering programming questions, solving plumbing issues, or fixing a bike. This factor combines that friendly aspect together with the construction of certain social personas (the helpful advice-giver, for instance).

4 Online News Comments Compared to Other Online Registers

After having interpreted the various factors as dimensions of variation, we will now turn to discussing the position of online news comments on the three dimensions relative to other online registers. Figure 3 provides a two-dimensional view of the analysed registers along the major dimensions: Dimension 1 “Involved-evaluative” on the x-axis and Dimension 2 “Informational-argumentative” on the y-axis. For strength and direction of mean factor scores and register distribution, see also Figure 2.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Two-dimensional view of register distribution along the most prominent dimensions. Note that negative values on Dimension 1 represent the informational pole while positive values on Dimension 1 represent the involved-evaluative pole.

On Dimension 1, online news comments (mean factor score 0.129) are positioned on the positive pole, i.e., they are mainly characterised by the joint occurrence of involved and to some extent evaluative features such as contractions, first and second person pronouns, adverbs, predicative adjectives, and be as main verb. However, in comparison to other online registers, online news comments exhibit comparatively few of these features. Registers such as interactive discussion, personal blog, advice, and recipe, for instance, are much more involved in nature than online comments. Thus, while online comments are positioned on the positive pole of Dimension 1, they also contain a fair amount of informational-abstract features such as average word length, nouns and nominalisations, prepositions and attributive adjectives—this can also be seen from their location on Dimension 2 (see below). The registers most closely positioned or similar to online news comments on the positive pole of Dimension 1 are review (mean factor score 0.168) and opinion blog (mean factor score 0.062). On the negative pole of Dimension 1, the most similar registers to the comments are FAQ (mean factor score −0.417) and informational blog (mean factor score −0.423). Titak and Roberson (2013) also found that reader comments were on the personal narrative pole, closer to e-mail and blogs, rather than on the informational pole.

Dimension 2 “Informational-argumentative” is the most characterising dimension for online news comments in this analysis: with a mean factor score of 0.613, they are one of the most representative registers on Dimension 2. They are clearly marked by the co-occurrence of nominalisations, a high average word length, conjuncts, that verb complements, and suasive verbs. As already mentioned in the previous section, all of these features contribute to creating informational and argumentative discourse. The other registers which are most representative of Dimension 2 are research articles (mean factor score 1.404) and FAQ (mean factor score 0.485)—both highly information-focused registers with an argumentative structure. The registers closest, and therefore most similar, to online news comments on this dimension are FAQ and informational blog (mean factor score 0.296), both marked by an informational-argumentative style, even though to a lesser extent than online news comments.

In regard to Dimension 3 (we remind the reader that the interpretation of this dimension is not conclusive) online news comments are rather instructional than narrative-descriptive. That said, their mean factor score on Dimension 3 is close to zero, which means that neither the features on the negative pole nor the features on the positive pole of this dimension are highly characteristic of online news comments. Typical instructional registers in this dataset are FAQ, description for sale, advice, and recipe. These registers are marked by a large amount of present tense forms and, to a lesser extent, second person pronouns and possibility modals. Registers representative of the narrative-descriptive pole are description of a person, personal blog, and sports report, which are marked by the co-occurrence of past tense verbs, third person pronouns, and public verbs. The registers most similar to online news comments (mean factor score −0.127) are research articles (mean factor score −0.045) and opinion blog (mean factor score −0.135) on the negative pole of Dimension 3, while the closest registers on the positive pole are religious sermon (mean factor score 0.044) and encyclopedia article (mean factor score 0.201).

According to their location on the three dimensions of variation, online news comments can best be characterised as instances of informational-argumentative discourse with a slight involved-evaluative slant. Anyone with experience reading online news comments will find this description apt: They tend to range from the preachy to the encyclopedic, with heavy argumentation. This characterisation is certainly intuitive if we consider the situational context in which online news comments are produced. Online news comments invite users to communicate their opinion on current news issues and can therefore contain involved and evaluative features (as indicated by their position on Dimension 1). However, online news comments are not subject to on-line production constraints and can be revised before posting, so that information can be integrated and commenters can make precise lexical choices to make their arguments (as indicated by their position on Dimension 2). This description is also in line with our other recent analyses. Ehret and Taboada (2020) compared online news comments to traditional written and spoken registers and found that they are strongly evaluative in nature, combining argumentative, informational, and some involved features (Ehret and Taboada, 2020, 23), while Cavasso and Taboada (2021) observe their overwhelmingly negative nature, with personal affective opinion (I hate the candidate) eschewed in favour of more detached evaluation (The candidate is incompetent; The candidate’s policies are bad). As illustrated in (4), online news comments can thus range from involved-evaluative to involved-argumentative and informational-argumentative. In our analysis of exclusively written online registers, however, online news comments are not as prominently evaluative as other online registers and their evaluative nature did not emerge as a separate dimension of variation.

(4) a. I1st person pronoun‘mcontraction veryamplifier flattered that my writing isbe main verb soemphatic powerfulpredicative adjective it scares you2nd person pronoun. (comments, comments_33450158_18 _MAT.txt).

b. I1st person pronoun agreepublic verb thatthat verb complement more controlled peer reviewed research still needs to be done but let’scontraction not run around sayingpublic verb thatthat verb complement there is 0 scientific evidence. (comments, comments_7018634_53_MAT.txt)

c. Howeverconjunct, the SCC quiteadverb oftenadverb throws back legislationnominalisation to the governmentnominalisation to redraft or abolish. (comments, comments_24630480_7_MAT.txt)

A large body of literature has explored the abusive and toxic nature of much online content and news comments in particular (McGuire, 2015; Gardiner et al., 2016; Muddiman and Stroud, 2017; Wolfgang, 2018; Juarez Miro, 2020). We found some toxicity in the comments in our corpus (Kolhatkar and Taboada, 2017a; Gautam and Taboada, 2019; Kolhatkar et al., 2020), but a relatively small amount, likely because the newspaper uses both automatic and human moderation to filter out the worst abuse.

Our previous analyses compared online news comments to other traditional registers (Ehret and Taboada, 2020), showing that they are not conversational at all. Here, we explore online registers in general and find that the nature of online registers is quite different from traditional written and spoken registers, and that comments are unique in the space of online registers. On the one hand, online registers are substantially more evaluative than traditional written registers—hence, online news comments do not emerge as strongly evaluative in this analysis. Although the fundamental distinction between involved and informational discourse (Biber, 1988) is still present in online registers, the scale of this continuum differs from analyses of purely traditional registers. On the other hand, online registers—and therefore also the emerging dimensions presented in this paper—seem not as clearly delineated as traditional registers in that they tend to combine features customarily associated with several (traditional) registers, and/or written and spoken language (Biber et al., 2015; Egbert et al., 2015). They are involved, like spoken language, but informational and argumentative like many written registers.

Our results contribute to the growing body of evidence that online registers are a different form of communication, and not a hybrid mode somewhere between speech and writing. Studies of Twitter (Clarke and Grieve, 2019), Reddit (Liimatta, 2019), and other online platforms (Hardy and Friginal, 2012; Titak and Roberson, 2013; Pavalanathan et al., 2017; Berber Sardinha, 2018), point to a new type of communication, including individual variation within the various platforms and communication channels. For instance, Liimatta (2019) found the now-familiar informational style in Reddit posts, but also, similar to the present analysis, an instructional focus. Berber Sardinha (2018) discovered two different types of stance (evidentiality and affect) in a study of a mix of online registers. Titak and Roberson (2013) placed reader comments in a personal narrative space (with orientation to the past) and also found that they tend to be involved. Hardy and Friginal (2012) found, like us and most other MDA studies, an informational vs. involved dimension in their analysis of blogs. Unlike the present paper, and due to the personal and narrative style of blogs, they additionally found addressee focus and narrative style dimensions. This makes perfect sense, as each platform and communication medium serves different communicative purposes, has different affordances, and is built around different communities of practice. Thus, the online space can be best described as a “continuous space of register variation” (Biber and Egbert, 2018, 196).

We should point out, before concluding, that our study is firmly language-dependent. The two corpora analysed are in English and it is quite possible that other languages may differ in the dimensions exhibited by different types of online registers. Biber (1995) shows that the main dimensions are constant across languages, especially the first dimension repeatedly found in multi-dimensional analyses (involved vs. informational). That result applies, however, to traditional written and spoken registers and may not be as robust in the online context.

5 Conclusions

This paper presented an analysis of online news comments in the context of other online registers. In particular, we conducted an MDA analysis to explore the linguistic features of online news comments compared to an extensive set of common online registers such as personal blog, advice pieces, or reviews.

Describing the position of online news comments along the three emerging dimensions, “Involved-evaluative”, “Informational-argumentative”, and “Narrative-descriptive vs. instructional”, our results corroborate previous research on online news comments. A recent publication established online news comments as a separate register strongly different from other traditional written and spoken registers and described them as argumentative and evaluative instances of discourse (Ehret and Taboada, 2020). Although in the present analysis online news comments also turned out to combine an argumentative-informational style with some involved-evaluative characteristics, we found that online news comments are by far not as involved and evaluative as other online registers.

The analysis presented here thus further refines the previous description of online news comments and allows two general conclusions: First, online registers are not as clearly defined as traditional registers, because they combine features typically found in spoken and informal language with features typical of writing and formal language as well as feature combinations from multiple registers. Second, online registers tend to be more involved and evaluative than traditional registers. Although some online registers have consistently been shown to be involved (e.g. personal blog, advice) vs. other, more informational registers (e.g. research articles, informational blog), it is the involved plus evaluative makeup of online registers which marks them as distinct from other (traditional) registers. This unique combination of evaluative or opinionated features with informational, narrative, and descriptive styles has been previously noted and contributes to the hybrid nature of online registers (Biber and Egbert, 2016; Biber et al., 2015, for hybridisation of online registers see also; Santini, 2007).

These two general characteristics, their unique mix of spoken and written features combined with the involved-evaluative characteristics, suggest online registers are a different mode of communication, neither spoken nor written, and certainly not somewhere in the middle.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://github.com/sfu-discourse-lab/SOCC. Code for the study: https://github.com/sfu-discourse-lab/MDA-OnlineRegisters.

Author Contributions

Following the CRediT system.5KE: conceptualization, statistical analysis, investigation, methodology, software, validation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. MT: conceptualization, data curation, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, project administration, resources, validation, writing—review and editing.

Funding

The first author gratefully acknowledges funding from the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation through a Feodor Lynen Postdoctoral Research Fellowship. This research was also supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (Insight Grant No. 435-2014-0171 to M. Taboada) and by NVIDIA Corporation, with the donation of a Titan Xp GPU.

The funder was not involved in the study design, collection,. analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The authors declare that this study received funding from NVIDIA Corporation. The funder was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to the members of the Discourse Processing Lab at Simon Fraser University, and especially to Laurens Bosman for help with data manipulation and feature extraction. Thank you to Jesse Egbert for sharing the CORE data. We are also grateful for feedback from the audience at the 4th Conference of the American Pragmatics Association in Albany (November 2019).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/frai.2021.643770/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1

‘Description for sale’ was originally labelled in CORE as ‘Description with intent to sell’. We have shortened the label.

2

https://github.com/sfu-discourse-lab/MDA-OnlineRegisters

3

The tagger is based on and requires the Stanford part-of-speech tagger (Toutanova et al., 2003).

4

This would be considerably low if our primary interest was in variable reduction. However, the focus here is on the interpretability of the factors and the description of online news comments.

5

https://casrai.org/credit/

References

Berber Sardinha, T. (2018). Dimensions of Variation across Internet Registers. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 23, 125–157. doi:10.1075/ijcl.15026.ber

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D. (1995). Dimensions of Register Variation: A Cross-Linguistic Comparison. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Biber, D., Egbert, J., and Davies, M. (2015). Exploring the Composition of the Searchable Web: a Corpus-Based Taxonomy of Web Registers. Corpora 10, 11–45. doi:10.3366/cor.2015.0065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D., and Egbert, J. (2016). Register Variation on the Searchable Web. J. English Linguistics 44, 95–137. doi:10.1177/0075424216628955

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D., and Egbert, J. (2018). Register Variation Online. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Biber, D., and Finegan, E. (1989). Styles of Stance in English: Lexical and Grammatical Marking of Evidentiality and Affect. Text 9, 93–124. doi:10.1515/text.1.1989.9.1.93

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D. (2014). Using Multi-Dimensional Analysis to Explore Cross-Linguistic Universals of Register Variation. LiC 14, 7–34. doi:10.1075/lic.14.1.02bib

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D. (1988). Variation across Speech and Writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Cavasso, L., and Taboada, M. (2021). A Corpus Analysis of Online News Comments Using the Appraisal Framework. J. Corpora Discourse Stud. 4, 1–38. doi:10.18573/jcads.61

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, I., and Grieve, J. (2019). Stylistic Variation on the Donald Trump Twitter Account: A Linguistic Analysis of Tweets Posted between 2009 and 2018. PLoS ONE 14, e0222062. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0222062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crystal, D. (2011). Internet Linguistics: A Student Guide. New York: Routledge.

Dziuban, C. D., and Shirkey, E. C. (1974). When Is a Correlation Matrix Appropriate for Factor Analysis? Some Decision Rules. Psychol. Bull. 81, 358–361. doi:10.1037/h0036316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egbert, J., Biber, D., and Davies, M. (2015). Developing a Bottom-Up, User-Based Method of Web Register Classification. J. Assn. Inf. Sci. Tec. 66, 1817–1831. doi:10.1002/asi.23308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehret, K., and Taboada, M. (2020). Are Online News Comments like Face-To-Face Conversation? Register Studies 2, 1–36. doi:10.1075/rs.19012.ehr

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farina, M. (2018). Facebook and Conversation Analysis: The Structure and Organization of Comment Threads. London: Bloomsbury Publishing.

Frehner, C. (2008). Email, SMS, MMS: The Linguistic Creativity of Asynchronous Discourse in the New media age. Bern: Peter Lang.

Gardiner, B., Mansfield, M., Anderson, I., Holder, J., Louter, D., and Ulmanu, M. (2016). The Guardian. The Dark Side of Guardian Comments. April 12, 2016. https://www.theguardian.com/technology/2016/apr/12/the-dark-side-of-guardian-comments

Gautam, V., and Taboada, M. (2019). Hey, Tyee Commenters! Scholars Studied You. Here’s What They Found. Tyee. November 6, 2019. https://thetyee.ca/Culture/2019/11/06/Tyee-Commenters-Assessed/

Giltrow, J., and Stein, D. (2009). Genres in the Internet: Issues in the Theory of Genre. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Godes, D., and Mayzlin, D. (2004). Using Online Conversations to Study Word-Of-Mouth Communication. Marketing Sci. 23, 545–560. doi:10.1287/mksc.1040.0071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliday, M. A., and Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.

Hardy, J. A., and Friginal, E. (2012). Filipino and American Online Communication and Linguistic Variation. World Englishes 31, 143–161. doi:10.1111/j.1467-971x.2011.01728.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herring, S., Scheidt, L. A., Bonus, S., and Wright, E. (2004). Bridging the gap: A Genre Analysis of Weblogs. In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences. Hawaii, 11.

Google Scholar

Hunston, S. (2011). Corpus Approaches to Evaluation: Phraseology and Evaluative Language. New York: Routledge.

Juarez Miro, C. (2020). The Comment gap: Affective Publics and Gatekeeping in the New York Times’ Comment Sections. Journalism 1464884920933754.

Google Scholar

Kolhatkar, V., and Taboada, M. (2017a). Constructive Language in News Comments. In Proceedings of the First Workshop on Abusive Language Online. Vancouver, 11–17.

Google Scholar

Kolhatkar, V., and Taboada, M. (2017b). Using the New York Times Picks to Identify Constructive Comments. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Natural Language Processing and Journalism, Conference on Empirical Methods in NLP. Copenhaguen, 100–105.

Google Scholar

Kolhatkar, V., Wu, H., Cavasso, L., Francis, E., Shukla, K., and Taboada, M. (2020). The SFU Opinion and Comments Corpus: A Corpus for the Analysis of Online News Comments. Corpus Pragmatics 4, 155–190. doi:10.1007/s41701-019-00065-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liimatta, A. (2019). Exploring Register Variation on Reddit. Register Studies 1, 269–295. doi:10.1075/rs.18005.lii

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcoccia, M. (2004). On-line Polylogues: Conversation Structure and Participation Framework in Internet Newsgroups. J. Pragmatics 36, 115–145. doi:10.1016/s0378-2166(03)00038-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCulloch, G. (2020). Because Internet: Understanding the New Rules of Language. New York: Riverhead Books.

McGuire, J. (2015). Uncivil Dialogue: Commenting and Stories about Indigenous People. CBC News, November 30, 2015. https://www.cbc.ca/newsblogs/community/editorsblog/2015/11/uncivil-dialogue-commenting-and-stories-about-indigenous-people.html

McVeigh, J. (2020). Thanks for Subscribing! A Genre Analysis of Email Marketing. Language@ Internet 18, urn:nbn:de:0009–7–51765

Google Scholar

Muddiman, A., and Stroud, N. J. (2017). News Values, Cognitive Biases, and Partisan Incivility in Comment Sections. J. Commun. 67, 586–609. doi:10.1111/jcom.12312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nini, A. (2019). “The Multi-Dimensional Analysis Tagger,” Multi-Dimensional Analysis: Research Methods and Current Issues. Editors T. Berber Sardinha, and M. Pinto Veirano (London: Bloomsbury), 67–94.

Google Scholar

North, S. (2007). ‘The Voices, the Voices': Creativity in Online Conversation. Appl. Linguistics 28, 538–555. doi:10.1093/applin/amm042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, R. E., Barton, D., Unger, J. W., and Zappavigna, M. (2014). Researching Language and Social Media: A Student Guide. New York: Routledge.

Pavalanathan, U., Fitzpatrick, J., Kiesling, S., and Eisenstein, J. (2017). Proceedings of the 55th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics. Vancouver, 884–895. A Multidimensional Lexicon for Interpersonal Stancetaking.

Google Scholar

Peterson, E. E. (2011). How Conversational Are Weblogs? Language@ Internet 8urn:nbn:de:0009–7–31201.

Google Scholar

Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., and Svartvik, J. (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman.

R Core Team (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Santini, M. (2007). Characterizing Genres of Web Pages: Genre Hybridism and Individualization. In Proceedings of the 40th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (HICSS-40). Hawaii: IEEE, 71.

Google Scholar

Taboada, M. (2011). Stages in an Online Review Genre. Text and Talk 31, 247–269. doi:10.1515/text.2011.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Titak, A., and Roberson, A. (2013). Dimensions of Web Registers: An Exploratory Multi-Dimensional Comparison. Corpora 8, 235–260. doi:10.3366/cor.2013.0042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toutanova, K., Klein, D., Manning, C., and Singer, Y. (2003). Feature-rich Part-Of-Speech Tagging with a Cyclic Dependency Network. Proceedings of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies. Edmonton, 252–259.

Google Scholar

Tseronis, A. (2011). From Connectives to Argumentative Markers: A Quest for Markers of Argumentative Moves and of Related Aspects of Argumentative Discourse. Argumentation 25, 427–447. doi:10.1007/s10503-011-9215-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tufekci, Z. (2008). Grooming, Gossip, Facebook and MySpace. Inf. Commun. Soc. 11 (4), 544–564. doi:10.1080/13691180801999050

CrossRef Full Text

van Eemeren, F. H., Houtlosser, P., and Henkemans, A. F. S. (2007). Argumentative Indicators in Discourse: A Pragma-Dialectical Study. Berlin: Springer.

Vásquez, C. (2014). The Discourse Of Online Consumer Reviews. London: Bloomsbury.

West, L. E. (2013). Facebook Sharing: A Sociolinguistic Analysis of Computer-Mediated Storytelling. Discourse, Context & Media 2, 1–13. doi:10.1016/j.dcm.2012.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, L. (2003). Second Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Wolfgang, J. D. (2018). Cleaning up the “Fetid Swamp”. Digital Journalism 6, 21–40. doi:10.1080/21670811.2017.1343090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woollaston, V. (2013). Online Conversations Are Damaging How We Speak to Each Other in Real Life: Author Claims People Could Soon ’forget’ How to Handle Social Situations. Daily Mail, September 30, 2013.

Google Scholar

Zappavigna, M. (2012). Discourse of Twitter and Social media: How We Use Language to Create Affiliation on the Web. London: Bloomsbury.

Keywords: register variation, online news comments, online language, multi-dimensional analysis, corpus linguistics

Citation: Ehret K and Taboada M (2021) Characterising Online News Comments: A Multi-Dimensional Cruise Through Online Registers. Front. Artif. Intell. 4:643770. doi: 10.3389/frai.2021.643770

Received: 18 December 2020; Accepted: 21 May 2021;
Published: 14 June 2021.

Edited by:

Jack Grieve, University of Birmingham, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Samira Shaikh, University of North Carolina at Charlotte, United States
Heike Wiese, Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany
Isobelle Clarke, Lancaster University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Ehret and Taboada. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katharina Ehret, a2VocmV0QHNmdS5jYQ==; Maite Taboada, bXRhYm9hZGFAc2Z1LmNh

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.