Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 19 June 2019
Sec. Neurorehabilitation

Effects of Exergaming on Attentional Deficits and Dual-Tasking in Parkinson's Disease

\nEva Schaeffer
Eva Schaeffer1*Jan-Hinrich BuschJan-Hinrich Busch2Benjamin RoebenBenjamin Roeben2Sascha OtterbeinSascha Otterbein2Pavel SaraykinPavel Saraykin2Edyta LeksEdyta Leks3Inga Liepelt-Scarfone,Inga Liepelt-Scarfone2,4Matthis Synofzik,Matthis Synofzik2,4Morad Elshehabi,,Morad Elshehabi1,2,4Walter Maetzler,,Walter Maetzler1,2,4Clint HansenClint Hansen1Sarah AndrisSarah Andris5Daniela Berg,,Daniela Berg1,2,4
  • 1Department of Neurology, Christian-Albrecht-University Kiel, Kiel, Germany
  • 2Department of Neurodegeneration, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
  • 3Department of Biomedical Magnetic Resonance, University of Tüebingen, Tüebingen, Germany
  • 4German Center of Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Tübingen, Germany
  • 5Mathematical Image Analysis Group, Faculty of Mathematics and Computer Science, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany

Introduction: Impairment of dual-tasking, as an attention-based primary cognitive dysfunction, is frequently observed in Parkinson's Disease (PD). The Training-PD study investigated the efficiency of exergaming, as a novel cognitive-motor training approach, to improve attention-based deficits and dual-tasking in PD when compared to healthy controls.

Methods: Eighteen PD patients and 17 matched healthy controls received a 6-week home-based training period of exergaming. Treatment effects were monitored using quantitative motor assessment of gait and cognitive testing as baseline and after 6 weeks of training.

Results: At baseline PD patients showed a significantly worse performance in several quantitative motor assessment parameters and in two items of cognitive testing. After 6 weeks of exergames training, the comparison of normal gait vs. dual-tasking in general showed an improvement of stride length in the PD group, without a gait-condition specific improvement. In the direct comparison of three different gait conditions (normal gait vs. dual-tasking calculating while walking vs. dual-tasking crossing while walking) PD patients showed a significant improvement of stride length under the dual-tasking calculating condition. This corresponded to a significant improvement in one parameter of the D2 attention test.

Conclusions: We conclude, that exergaming, as an easy to apply, safe technique, can improve deficits in cognitive-motor dual-tasking and attention in PD.

Introduction

Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disorder defined by its cardinal motor symptoms rigor, tremor, and bradykinesia. However, motor impairment in PD is not limited to these symptoms but includes more complex deficits of motor control and coordination. Additionally, a wide range of non-motor symptoms, including cognitive deficits, may occur, having a considerable effect on the patients' quality of life (1). Among cognitive dysfunctions, impairment of dual-tasking, as a primary cognitive deficit with a direct impact on motor and especially gait functions (25), is frequently observed in PD. Functional MRI studies revealed a close relation of altered attentional networks and dual-task performance (6, 7). Impaired dual-tasking can have a considerable influence on daily activities of PD patients and has been associated with an increased risk of falls (8). As there is a limited effect of pharmacological therapy or deep brain stimulation on dual-tasking (9), other strategies are sought to positively influence these deficits. Previous studies showed that both, attentional cognitive performance and dual-tasking are responsive to training interventions and have a reciprocal impact on each other (1012).

While specific non-pharmacological interventions like physiotherapy or occupational therapy are well-known and recommended for PD, new training techniques, which deploy different motivational incentives, are increasingly applied. Another promising approach is “exergaming,” a combination of physical exercise and gaming, where patients have to control a videogame with their movements using different forms of optical or tactile sensors. Exergaming has some considerable advantages, e.g., the possibility of home-based utilization. The direct feedback on task performance as well as the highly motivational and challenging character of the games implement important aspects of cognitive engagement (13). Moreover, the simultaneous training of cognitive and motor aspects in constantly changing virtual environments is particularly suited to address dual-tasking as required for the constantly changing situations of everyday life. Exergames have been seen to improve motor and cognitive functions in neurological diseases including ataxia and stroke (1416) and an increasing number of studies have shown promising results in PD (1722).

Using a multidisciplinary approach, the Training-PD study set out to evaluate the effects of exergaming and other cognitive and physical training forms on neuronal plasticity, motor and non-motor function in PD in a randomized, parallel group trial. We here present data from the Training-PD exergaming part of the study on attentional motor and cognitive functions after a 6-week training intervention.

Methods

Subjects

In total, 64 patients were recruited between 07/2015 and 12/2017 from the outpatient clinic of the department of Neurodegeneration at the University of Tuebingen. Inclusion criteria for PD patients were: (1) diagnosis of PD according to the UK brain bank criteria and (2) Hoehn and Yahr score ≤ 2.5 (in order to enable an unguarded training at home and avoid potential injuries resulting from postural instability). After study inclusion, PD patients were randomly assigned to one of three interventions: (I) PD physiotherapy, (II) PD braingames, and (III) PD exergames. To obtain comparable groups a stratified randomization protocol was used, including age, sex, and PD disease duration. Moreover, 20 healthy controls were recruited using public notices. The Control group (IV) received the same exergaming training and was matched with regard to sex and age.

Due to logistic reasons, all interventions were performed in parallel with a priori planned separate data analyses. We here present data on exergaming training from group III (PD exergames, n = 18) and group IV (Controls exergames, n = 17).

Exclusion criteria for all study participants comprised: (1) Presence of major depression (Beck Depression Inventory >18 points); (2) physical status or diseases (other than PD) affecting physical training; (3) signs of dementia (Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA <21); (4) Hoehn and Yahr ≥ 3 or other signs in neurological examination indicating a higher risk for falls, (5) contraindications for the performance of MRI (exploratory outcome of the study, data reported separately), and (6) planned change in medication or the usual training. Participants with mild cognitive impairment (MOCA 21-25) were not excluded from the study (23). The study protocol was approved by the local ethical committee. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Intervention

All study participants received a training protocol for 6 weeks with three 45-min- sessions per week. Implementation of the training was documented using a training diary kept by the participants. PD patients and healthy controls received the same training protocol, using a commercially available Microsoft Kinect system (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA). An electronic PubMed search to identify game characteristics best suitable for PD patients regarding design and content was performed. Based on this research games focusing on two main aspects were selected: (1) games suitable to train bradykinesia, hypokinesia, and dual-tasking and (2) games using a clear design, without overwhelming visual or optic input (19, 24). Three games were chosen from the game pack “Your shape: Fitness evolved:”

(I) “Virtual smash” (15 min per session): participants had to shatter virtual boxes in different distances with long and fast arm swings. Fast speed and correct arm coordination resulted in higher point scores.

(II) “Light race” (15 min per session): participants had to step on virtual enlightened fields with long and fast steps. Fast speed and correct leg coordination resulted in higher point scores.

(III) “Kardio boxing” (15 min per session): participants had to follow the instructions of a virtual trainer, showing a complex, rhythmic coordination training including both, arms and legs. Correct motion sequences resulted in higher point scores.

The intensity and level of difficulty of the games adapted automatically to the participant's performance level.

At baseline each participant was individually instructed on how to use the software in a personal introductory session. Proficiency criteria were (a) the correct navigation through the program and (b) the correct performance of the games. Afterwards the training was performed at home.

Assessments

All participants underwent quantitative motor assessment and cognitive testing before and after the training.

Quantitative Motor Assessment

Motor performance was quantified using the Mobility Lab® system for an objective measurement of standardized motor tasks using six body sensors (OPAL APDM, Inc., Portland, OR, United States), which had been validated earlier in elderly individuals and PD (25, 26). The system included six sensors, one at the chest, one at each wrist and ankle, and one lower back sensor. Data of four different motor tasks were used for the analyses.

Instrumented Timed-up-and-go (iTUG)

Participants were asked to stand up from a chair, walk 7 m in a habitual speed, turn around 180° at a specific mark, walk back, and sit down again. Total duration of the task, as well as duration and peak velocity of two specific motor aspects (sit-to-stand and turn-to-sit) were measured.

Instrumented Walk (iWalk) Normal Pace

Participants walked 20 m in a 3 m wide hallway in their self-selected speed.

iWalk Calculating

Participants were asked to walk the same route again, while solving a serial subtraction equation (minus 7) as a dual-tasking challenge. The standardized starting number for the serial subtraction was different for baseline and follow-up.

iWalk Crossing

Participants were asked to hold a clipboard in their non-dominant hand while walking the same distance and place crosses on a prepared document with their dominant hand.

Stride length and velocity, cadence (steps per minute), cycle time (time used for one complete gait cycle), and arm swing (velocity and range of motion, RoM) were calculated from the latter three tasks by the software's validated algorithms.

Cognitive Testing

All participants received the same cognitive testing including the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA), the D2 Attention test (27), the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT, German version) (28), and the Regensburger Word Fluency test (RWT) (29).

The MOCA total score was used for a global overview of the participant's cognitive status. Two parameters of the D2 Test were used to assess the ability of the participant to concentrate on a certain task (crossing out the latter “d”): KL = amount of correctly crossed symbols minus amount of omissions and F% = percentage of mistakes in relation to edited signs. Immediate, short and long delay free recall of the CVLT were used to evaluate episodic memory. Four subtests of the RWT measured word fluency and executive function. Percentage ranks of the D2 and RWT were corrected for gender, age and education.

To rule out learning effects parallel-test versions for the CVLT (30) and RWT (31) were used for baseline and follow-up.

Statistics

Preprocessing of the Mobility Lab® data was performed using Matlab (Version R2016b, The Mathworks Inc., 1984). SPSS 24.0 (SPSS Inc., IBM, USA) was used for statistical analyses. Statistical distribution was tested using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov-test.

For cross-sectional group comparisons at baseline and follow-up, the Mann-Whitney-U-test was used for non-normally distributed variables and student's t-test for normally distributed variables. To identify parameters of the Mobility Lab® assessment indicating disease-specific deficits, in a first, exploratory analyses a group comparison of PD patients vs. healthy controls was performed at baseline. Parameters identified to be significantly different (p < 0.05) where used for further longitudinal analyses. Parameters of cross-sectional analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons as the main outcome purpose of the study was the evaluation of longitudinal intervention effects.

Response-to-intervention analyses were performed with a two-way repeated measures ANOVA (time × group). To compare the different gait challenges of the iWalk task, the factor “gait condition” was added (time × group × gait condition). In a first analyses normal gait was compared to gait under dual-tasking conditions in general (iWalk: normal pace vs. iWalk calculating + iWalk crossing subsumed), with a planned nested analyses of the two dual-tasking conditions. In a second analyses all different gait conditions were compared directly (iWalk: normal pace vs. iWalk calculating vs. iWalk crossing). Non-normally distributed data were log-transformed.

Correlations were performed using the Pearson's correlation coefficient (one-tailed).

A p < 0.05 was accepted as statistically significant. For better readability, data are presented with mean (standard deviation).

Results

Group Characteristics

Two PD patients and two healthy controls reported an incomplete training or major deviations from the training protocol and were thus excluded from the analyses. Only minor aberrations of the training protocol (e.g., missing of one training session) were accepted. The total exercise time of both groups was comparable (p = 0.42). Mean age was 58.6 (9.9) for the PD and 57.8 (11.4) for the Control group. 55.6% of the PD group and 52.9% of the healthy control group were male. MOCA total score at baseline was comparable between PD patients (Mean: 27.3) and Controls (Mean: 27.2) (minimal score: 22). Median disease duration of PD patients was 4 years (1–20) and the mean levodopa-equivalent dosage was 340 mg per day (100–925). The median total score of part III of the Movement Disorder Society Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS) was 28 (7–38). Seventeen PD patients were rated Hoehn & Yahr stage 2, one patient was rated Hoehn & Yahr stage 2.5.

Quantitative Motor Assessment

Between-Group Comparisons at Baseline and at Follow-Up

At baseline 13 parameters of the Mobility Lab® were significantly different between PD patients and Controls and were therefore used for further group comparisons. PD patients were significantly slower in the following parameters of the iTUG test: total duration, Sit-to-stand peak velocity, Turn-to-sit duration, and peak velocity. Moreover, they showed significant lower values in the following parameters: stride length and stride velocity in all iWalk tasks, peak arm swing velocity in iWalk normal pace, and dual-tasking calculating and arm swing range of motion in iWalk dual-tasking calculating. No significant differences were observed in cadence and cycle time of all iWalk assessments and for the duration from sitting to standing during the iTUG task. At follow-up two parameters showed no significant group difference between PD and Controls anymore: stride length and range of motion under dual-tasking calculating conditions (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Quantitative motor assessment—between-group comparisons at baseline and at follow-up.

Response-to-Intervention Analyses

Both groups required significantly less time for the iTUG test after 6 weeks of exergaming (p = 0.005), without significant group differences. In the comparison of stride length under normal vs. dual-tasking conditions (subsumed calculating and crossing), a significant group effect was seen in favor of the PD group. No significant differences were seen for the time × gait condition and time × group × gait condition analysis. Therefore the additional nested analysis of the dual-tasking conditions crossing vs. calculating was not applicable. When comparing all three gait conditions directly a significant improvement of stride length with a significant interaction of time × group × gait condition was seen in favor of the PD group under dual-tasking calculating conditions (Table 2). Peak arm swing velocity improved in both groups (p = 0.024) without significant group or gait condition effects. No significant improvement was seen for stride velocity and range of motion of the arm.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Quantitative motor assessment—response-to-intervention analyses in PD vs. controls.

Cognition

Between-Group Comparisons at Baseline

At baseline, PD patients scored significantly worse than healthy controls in one item of the D2 Attention test (KL = amount of correctly crossed symbols minus amount of omissions) and in one item of the RWT (category change lexical) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cognition—Response-to-intervention analyses in PD vs. Controls.

Response-to-Intervention Analyses

Both, PD patients and Controls showed a significant increase of semantic word fluency and an improved cognitive performance in the D2 test of attention (KL) after the intervention. Improvement of the D2 test was significantly more pronounced in PD patients than in Controls (Table 3).

Correlations

The improvement of stride length under cognitive-motor dual-tasking conditions (iWalk: dual-tasking—calculating, V2-V1) correlated significantly with the improvement of the D2 test of attention (KL, V2-V1) (p-level 0.05).

Discussion

Our results show a significant improvement of deficits in motor and cognitive performance of attention-based tasks after 6 weeks of exergaming training in PD patients.

Quantitative motor assessment revealed a significant improvement on a single parameter level, i.e., stride length in PD patients under dual-tasking conditions (walking while calculating) after exergames training when compared to healthy controls. This is of particular interest, as a dysregulation in stride length has been identified as a key element for gait impairment in PD patients (32), correlating with disease progression (33). The targeted improvement of this deficit is therefore of high clinical relevance for PD patients. Interestingly, our results could not reveal an improvement of the “motor-motor” dual-tasking condition (crossing while walking), which was also reflected by a non-significant result when comparing normal gait vs. gait under dual-tasking conditions in general (with the calculating and crossing task subsumed). These results suggest that exergaming does not improve dual-tasking in general in PD patients, but only certain aspects, which is in line with previous studies (34, 35). We therefore hypothesize that the effect observed on calculating, but not crossing while walking, might reflect a specific improvement of cognitive-motor but not motor-motor dual-tasking.

Additionally, these results correspond well with the cognitive performance. PD patients showed a significantly worse performance than healthy controls in concentration capacity as measured by the D2 test at BL, in line with previous studies, identifying attentional deficits in PD as an important component of cognitive impairment in all stages of PD (36, 37). However, a significant, group-dependent improvement of concentration capacity in PD patients was observed after exergames. Taken together, the corresponding improvement in concentration capacity and a primary cognitive dual-task might reflect a targeted improvement of attention-based tasks with high everyday relevance.

Besides the improvement of attention-based tasks, one iTUG variable measuring motor performance (total duration) improved in both groups, without significant group differences. We conclude that this observed improvement reflects a learning of the motion sequence, independently from pre-existing functional deficits.

Several limitations of the study have to be addressed, including first the limited number of participants. Second, it must be underlined that the two conditions walking while calculating and walking while crossing are both subtypes of dual-tasking. Therefore the analyses of normal gait vs. dual-tasking gait in general (with a nested factor calculating vs. crossing) would have been the preferred statistical method, but showed no significant differences in our study. However, previous studies showed that the complexity of the simultaneously performed task has a major impact on walking performance (38). Similarly, our data suggest that a mixed cognitive-motor dual-tasking challenge might require attentional capacity in a different way than a “motor-motor” dual-task. We therefore conclude that it is justified to analyze these gait conditions separately. Additionally, the use of home-based training documented by patient diaries lacks a strict external control of the performed training intensity, which may result in shortening of the exact training protocol in this study. However, the current study placed emphasis on feasibility for the patients and future exergame systems are likely to record information on training intensity as well. Finally, it must be discussed whether the improvement of motor and cognitive performance was due to re-test learning effects at follow-up. However, the correlation of an improvement in both attentional motor and cognitive tasks specifically in the PD group, suggests rather a specific intervention-driven and disease-dependent effect. Moreover, the randomization of two versions for the dual-tasking calculating task reduces the risk of simple learning effects. Moreover, the study did not include a PD control group without a training intervention, therefore placebo effects of the intervention cannot be ruled out.

Taken together, results of the Training-PD study indicate that specific aspects of dual-tasking, as a complex interaction of motor and cognitive function, can be improved by exergaming. These findings go in line with previous studies showing an improvement of gait parameters or cognitive function after specific motor-cognitive dual-tasking training (3944).

Especially considering the very limited effect of pharmacological treatment (45), the high correlation with an increased risk of falls and impaired quality of life (4649), the possibility to improve attentional deficits through exercise is of high clinical relevance for PD patients. Exergaming, combining feedback mechanisms, motivation and simultaneous motor-cognitive activation, might be particularly well-suited to address attentional deficits. It can therefore be considered a suitable alternative or add-on to the gold-standard physiotherapy to improve dual-tasking in PD (50, 51). Our results should be confirmed in future studies that assess long-term-effects of exergaming and may also investigate underlying (patho)mechanisms of different dual-tasking conditions.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the ethical committee of the Eberhard-Karls-University of Tuebingen, with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the ethical committee of the Eberhard-Karls-University of Tuebingen.

Author Contributions

ES, IL-S, MS, WM, and DB were responsible for conception, design, and organization of the study. ES, J-HB, BR, SO, PS, EL, IL-S, ME, and SA were responsible for execution of the study. ES, ME, and CH performed the statistical analysis. ES wrote the manuscript. All authors have contributed significantly and carefully reviewed this manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the AKF (Applied Clinical Research) program of the Faculty of Medicine, the Eberhard-Kals-University of Tübingen (AKF 342-1-0). ES received intramural research funding for a research rotation position from the Christian-Albrechts-University of Kiel, providing time to work on data analyses of this study (F358973). Both faculties had no role with regard to study design, the collection, analyses and interpretation of data, writing of the report or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

ES reports speaker's honoraria from Bayer Vital GmbH and Novartis outside the submitted work. BR has received financial support for research training at the University of Oxford within the CENTRE-PD project funded by the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement No 692320) and reports personal fees from Actelion Pharmaceuticals Ltd., outside the submitted work. IL-S reports grants from AKF Grant (Institutional), during the conduct of the study; grants from Johnson & Johnson, grants from European Commission, H2020-TWINN-2015, grants from “Parkinson National Center of Excellence in Research (NCER-PD),” grants from Michael J. Fox Foundation, outside the submitted work. MS reports grants and personal fees from Actelion Pharmaceuticals, outside the submitted work. WM reports grants and personal fees from Lundbeck, grants from Neuroalliance, grants from Janssen, grants from UCB, personal fees from Rölke, personal fees from Abbvie, outside the submitted work; in addition, WM has a patent for the assessment of dyskinesias licensed to German patent office, 102015220741.2. DB reports grants from Janssen Pharmaceutica, grants from Michael J. Fox Foundation, grants from Damp foundation, grants from German Parkinson's Disease Association (dPV), grants from BMWi, grants from BMBF, grants from Parkinson Fonds Deutschland GmbH, grants and speaker's honoraria from and consultancies for UCB Pharma GmbH, grants and speaker's honoraria from TEVA Pharma GmbH, grants from and consultancies for Novartis Pharma GmbH, grants and speaker's honoraria from and consultancies for Lundbeck speaker's honoraria from and consultancies for BIAL, speaker's honoraria from and consultancies for Biogen, honoraria from Bayer, outside the submitted work.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Bettina Faust, Sarah-Christina Weber, Franziska Weber, Sofie Kämereit, and Hanna Kagerer for their major support. Moreover, we thank all participants for their great commitment.

References

1. Barone P, Antonini A, Colosimo C, Marconi R, Morgante L, Avarello TP, et al. The PRIAMO study: a multicenter assessment of nonmotor symptoms and their impact on quality of life in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2009) 24:1641–9. doi: 10.1002/mds.22643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Desloovere K, Vandenberghe W, Kerckhofs E, Nieuwboer A. Freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease: the impact of dual-tasking and turning. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2563–70. doi: 10.1002/mds.23327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Strouwen C, Molenaar EALM, Keus SHJ, Münks L, Heremans E, Vandenberghe W, et al. Are factors related to dual-task performance in people with Parkinson's disease dependent on the type of dual tasdk? Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2016) 23:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Springer S, Giladi N, Peretz C, Yogev G, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual-tasking effects on gait variability: the role of aging, falls, and executive function. Mov Disord. (2006) 21:950–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.20848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Castiello U, Bennett KM. The bilateral reach-to-grasp movement of Parkinson's disease subjects. Brain. (1997) 120:593–604. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.4.593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Wu T, Hallett M. Neural correlates of dual task performance in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2008) 79:760–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.126599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Wu T, Liu J, Zhang H, Hallett M, Zheng Z, Chan P. Attention to automatic movements in Parkinson's disease: modified automatic mode in the striatum. Cereb Cortex. (2015) 25:3330–42. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Heinzel S, Maechtel M, Hasmann SE, Hobert MA, Heger T, Berg D, et al. Motor dual-tasking deficits predict falls in Parkinson's disease: a prospective study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2016) 26:73–7. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Seri-Fainshtat E, Israel Z, Weiss A, Hausdorff JM. Impact of sub-thalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation on dual tasking gait in Parkinson's disease. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2013) 10:38. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-10-38

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Tanaka K, Quadros AC, Santos RF, Stella F, Gobbi LTB, Gobbi S. Benefits of physical exercise on executive functions in older people with Parkinson's disease. Brain Cogn. (2009) 69:435–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Cruise KE, Bucks RS, Loftus AM, Newton RU, Pegoraro R, Thomas MG. Exercise and Parkinson's: benefits for cognition and quality of life. Acta Neurol Scand. (2011) 123:13–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2010.01338.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Caligiore D, Mustile M, Fineschi A, Romano L, Piras F, Assogna F, et al. Action observation with dual task for improving cognitive abilities in Parkinson's Disease: a pilot study. Front Syst Neurosci. (2019) 13:7 doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Petzinger GM, Fisher BE, McEwen S, Beeler JA, Walsh JP, Jakowec MW. Exercise-enhanced neuroplasticity targeting motor and cognitive circuitry in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol. (2013) 12:716–26. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70123-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Saposnik G, Teasell R, Mamdani M, Hall J, McIlroy W, Cheung D, et al. Effectiveness of virtual reality using Wii gaming technology in stroke rehabilitation: a pilot randomized clinical trial and proof of principle. Stroke. (2010) 41:1477–84. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.584979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Schatton C, Synofzik M, Fleszar Z, Giese MA, Schöls L, Ilg W. Individualized exergame training improves postural control in advanced degenerative spinocerebellar ataxia: a rater-blinded, intra-individually controlled trial. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2017) 39:80–4. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ilg W, Schatton C, Schicks J, Giese MA, Schöls L, Synofzik M. Video game-based coordinative training improves ataxia in children with degenerative ataxia. Neurology. (2012) 79:2056–60. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182749e67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Esculier J-F, Vaudrin J, Bériault P, Gagnon K, Tremblay LE. Home-based balance training programme using Wii Fit with balance board for Parkinsons's disease: a pilot study. J Rehabil Med. (2012) 44:144–50. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Pompeu JE, Mendes FADS, Silva KG, Lobo AM, Oliveira Tde P, Zomignani AP, et al. Effect of Nintendo WiiTM-based motor and cognitive training on activities of daily living in patients with Parkinson's disease: a randomised clinical trial. Physiotherapy. (2012) 98:196–204. doi: 10.1016/j.physio.2012.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Mendes FA, dos S, Pompeu JE, Lobo AM, da Silva KG, Oliveira Tde P, Zomignani AP, et al. Motor learning, retention and transfer after virtual-reality-based training in Parkinson's disease – effect of motor and cognitive demands of games: a longitudinal, controlled clinical study. Physiotherapy. (2012) 98:217–23. doi: 10.1016/j.physio.2012.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ribas CG, Alves da Silva L, Corrêa MR, Teive HG, Valderramas S. Effectiveness of exergaming in improving functional balance, fatigue and quality of life in Parkinson's disease: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2017) 38:13–8. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Shih M-C, Wang R-Y, Cheng S-J, Yang Y-R. Effects of a balance-based exergaming intervention using the Kinect sensor on posture stability in individuals with Parkinson's disease: a single-blinded randomized controlled trial. J Neuro Eng Rehabil. (2016) 13:78. doi: 10.1186/s12984-016-0185-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ferraz DD, Trippo KV, Duarte GP, Neto MG, Bernardes Santos KO, Filho JO. The effects of functional training, bicycle exercise, and exergaming on walking capacity of elderly patients with Parkinson disease: a pilot randomized controlled single-blinded trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2018) 99:826–33. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2017.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Dalrymple-Alford JC, MacAskill MR, Nakas CT, Livingston L, Graham C, Crucian GP, et al. The MoCA: well-suited screen for cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology. (2010) 75:1717–25. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc29c9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Assad O, Hermann R, Lilla D, Mellies B, Meyer R, Shevach L, et al. Motion-based games for Parkinson's disease patients. In: Entertainment Computing – ICEC 2011 Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Berlin: Springer. (2011). p. 47–58.

Google Scholar

25. Dewey DC, Miocinovic S, Bernstein I, Khemani P, Dewey RB, Querry R, et al. Automated gait and balance parameters diagnose and correlate with severity in Parkinson disease. J Neurol Sci. (2014) 345:131–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2014.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Mancini M, Horak FB. Potential of APDM mobility lab for the monitoring of the progression of Parkinson's disease. Expert Rev Med Devices. (2016) 13:455–62. doi: 10.1586/17434440.2016.1153421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bates ME, Lemay EP. The d2 test of attention: construct validity and extensions in scoring techniques. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2004) 10:392–400. doi: 10.1017/S135561770410307X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. Manual for the California Verbal 438 Learning Test - Second Edition (CVLT–II). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2000). p. 439.

Google Scholar

29. Aschenbrenner S, Tucha O, Lange KW. Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test (RWT). Göttingen: Hogrefe Verlag (2000).

Google Scholar

30. Delis DC, Massman PJ, Kaplan E, Mckee R, Kramer JH, Gettman D. Alternate form of the california verbal learning test: development and reliability. Clin Neuropsychol. (1991) 5:154–62. doi: 10.1080/13854049108403299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Wagner S, Maurer J, Pape V, Mitschke T, Helmreich I, Tadić A. Entwicklung von drei parallelversionen des kategorienwechsels des regensburger wortflüssigkeitstests. Z Neuropsychol. (2015) 26:27–34. doi: 10.1024/1016-264X/a000131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Morris ME, Iansek R, Matyas TA, Summers JJ. The pathogenesis of gait hypokinesia in Parkinson's disease. Brain. (1994) 117:1169–81. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.5.1169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bayle N, Patel AS, Crisan D, Guo LJ, Hutin E, Weisz DJ, et al. Contribution of step length to increase walking and turning speed as a marker of Parkinson's disease progression. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0152469. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Galletly R, Brauer SG. Does the type of concurrent task affect preferred and cued gait in people with Parkinson's disease? Aust J Physiother. (2005) 51:175–80. doi: 10.1016/S0004-9514(05)70024-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Rochester L, Hetherington V, Jones D, Nieuwboer A, Willems A-M, Kwakkel G, et al. Attending to the task: interference effects of functional tasks on walking in Parkinson's disease and the roles of cognition, depression, fatigue, and balance. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2004) 85:1578–85. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Riekkinen M, Kejonen K, Jäkälä P, Soininen H, Riekkinen P. Reduction of noradrenaline impairs attention and dopamine depletion slows responses in Parkinson's disease. Eur J Neurosci. 10:1429–35. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00145.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Ballard CG, Aarsland D, McKeith I, O'Brien J, Gray A, Cormack F, et al. Fluctuations in attention: PD dementia vs DLB with parkinsonism. Neurology. (2002) 59:1714–20. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000036908.39696.FD

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Wild LB, Lima DB, Balardin JB, Rizzi L, Giacobbo BL, Oliveira HB, et al. Characterization of cognitive and motor performance during dual-tasking in healthy older adults and patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol. (2013) 260:580–9. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6683-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Brauer SG, Morris ME. Can people with Parkinson's disease improve dual tasking when walking? Gait Posture. (2010) 31:229–33. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Yogev-Seligmann G, Giladi N, Brozgol M, Hausdorff JM. A training program to improve gait while dual tasking in patients with Parkinson's disease: a pilot study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2012) 93:176–81. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Beck EN, Intzandt BN, Almeida QJ. Can dual task walking improve in Parkinson's disease after external focus of attention exercise? A single blind randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2018) 32:18–33. doi: 10.1177/1545968317746782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Geroin C, Nonnekes J, de Vries NM, Strouwen C, Smania N, Tinazzi M, et al. Does dual-task training improve spatiotemporal gait parameters in Parkinson's disease? Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2018) 55:86–91. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2018.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Strouwen C, Molenaar EALM, Münks L, Keus SHJ, Bloem BR, Rochester L, et al. Dual tasking in Parkinson's disease: should we train hazardous behavior? Expert Rev Neurother. (2015) 15:1031–9. doi: 10.1586/14737175.2015.1077116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Fritz NE, Cheek FM, Nichols-Larsen DS. Motor-cognitive dual-task training in persons with neurologic disorders: a systematic review. J Neurol Phys Ther. (2015) 39:142–53. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Elshehabi M, Maier KS, Hasmann SE, Nussbaum S, Herbst H, Heger T, et al. Limited effect of dopaminergic medication on straight walking and turning in early-to-moderate Parkinson's disease during single and dual tasking. Front Aging Neurosci. (2016) 8:4. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Jacobs JV, Nutt JG, Carlson-Kuhta P, Allen R, Horak FB. Dual tasking during postural stepping responses increases falls but not freezing in people with Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2014) 20:779–81. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bekkers EMJ, Dockx K, Devan S, Van Rossom S, Verschueren SMP, Bloem BR, et al. The impact of dual-tasking on postural stability in people with Parkinson's disease with and without freezing of gait. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2018) 32:166–74. doi: 10.1177/1545968318761121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Allcock LM, Rowan EN, Steen IN, Wesnes K, Kenny RA, Burn DJ. Impaired attention predicts falling in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2009) 15:110–5. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2008.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Lawson RA, Yarnall AJ, Duncan GW, Breen DP, Khoo TK, Williams-Gray CH, et al. Cognitive decline and quality of life in incident Parkinson's disease: the role of attention. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2016) 27:47–53. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Killane I, Fearon C, Newman L, McDonnell C, Waechter SM, Sons K, et al. Dual motor-cognitive virtual reality training impacts dual-task performance in freezing of gait. IEEE J Biomed Health Inform. (2015) 19:1855–61. doi: 10.1109/JBHI.2015.2479625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Mirelman A, Maidan I, Herman T, Deutsch JE, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Virtual reality for gait training: can it induce motor learning to enhance complex walking and reduce fall risk in patients with Parkinson's disease? J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2011) 66A:234–40. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson's disease, exergaming, dual-tasking, cognition, quantitative motor assessment, attention

Citation: Schaeffer E, Busch J-H, Roeben B, Otterbein S, Saraykin P, Leks E, Liepelt-Scarfone I, Synofzik M, Elshehabi M, Maetzler W, Hansen C, Andris S and Berg D (2019) Effects of Exergaming on Attentional Deficits and Dual-Tasking in Parkinson's Disease. Front. Neurol. 10:646. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00646

Received: 25 February 2019; Accepted: 31 May 2019;
Published: 19 June 2019.

Edited by:

Nicola Smania, University of Verona, Italy

Reviewed by:

Marialuisa Gandolfi, University of Verona, Italy
Federica Piras, Fondazione Santa Lucia (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2019 Schaeffer, Busch, Roeben, Otterbein, Saraykin, Leks, Liepelt-Scarfone, Synofzik, Elshehabi, Maetzler, Hansen, Andris and Berg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eva Schaeffer, eva.schaeffer@uksh.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.