Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 30 September 2020
Sec. Stroke

Characteristics of Non-linguistic Cognitive Impairment in Post-stroke Aphasia Patients

\nJingfan Yao,,&#x;Jingfan Yao1,2,3Xinxin Liu&#x;Xinxin Liu4Qi LiuQi Liu1Jinfang WangJinfang Wang5Na YeNa Ye1Xiao LuXiao Lu6Yishuang ZhaoYishuang Zhao6Hongyan ChenHongyan Chen7Zaizhu HanZaizhu Han8Miaoxin YuMiaoxin Yu1Yu WangYu Wang1Gaifen Liu,,
Gaifen Liu1,2,3*Yumei Zhang,
Yumei Zhang2,6*
  • 1Department of Neurology, Beijing Tiantan Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 2China National Clinical Research Center for Neurological Diseases, Beijing, China
  • 3Beijing Office for Cerebrovascular Disease Prevention and Control, Beijing, China
  • 4Department of Neurology, Tianjin First Central Hospital, Tianjin, China
  • 5Department of Neurology, General Hospital of the Yang Tze River Shipping, Wuhan Brain Hospital, Wuhan, China
  • 6Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Beijing Tiantan Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 7Department of Neuroradiology, Beijing Tiantan Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 8State Key Laboratory for Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Background: Non-linguistic cognitive training has been suggested to improve the communication skills of patients with post-stroke aphasia (PSA). However, the association between language and non-linguistic cognitive functions is not fully understood. In this study, we used the Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment (LOTCA) to evaluate the characteristics of non-linguistic cognitive impairments in Chinese PSA patients.

Methods: A total of 86 stroke patients were recruited in this study. Language and non-linguistic cognitive impairments were evaluated by the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) and LOTCA, respectively. The patients were divided into two groups (PSA and non-PSA), and the Chinese norm (the data came from 44 Chinese individuals without neurological disorders in a previous study) was used as the control group. The LOTCA scores were compared among the three groups. Patients in the PSA group were subdivided into the fluent aphasia group (FAG) and the non-FAG according to the Chinese aphasia fluency characteristic scale. The LOTCA scores were also compared between the PSA subdivisions. Potential confounders were adjusted in the analysis of covariance. Partial correlation analysis between the subscores of WAB and LOTCA was also performed.

Results: The total LOTCA scores in the PSA group (75.11 ± 17.08) were significantly lower compared with scores in the non-PSA (96.80 ± 7.75, P < 0.001) and the control group (97.65 ± 16.24, P < 0.001). The PSA group also had lower orientation, visual perception (VP), spatial perception (SP), visuomotor organization, thinking operation, and attention scores. The total LOTCA, orientation, VP, SP, and MP scores were lower in the non-FAG (69.24 ± 18.06, 8.62 ± 5.09, 12.76 ± 2.47, 7.48 ± 3.01, and 9.62 ± 2.25, respectively) compared with the FAG (80.36 ± 14.07, 12.14 ± 3.99, 14.09 ± 1.93, 9.68 ± 3.01, 10.55 ± 1.63, respectively, P's < 0.05). The aphasia quotient was positively correlated with the total score of LOTCA and scores of orientation, VP, SP, and MP (r = 0.710, 0.744, 0.565, 0.597, and 0.616; P < 0.001).

Conclusion: Compared with stroke patients without aphasia, patients with PSA often have more extensive and serious non-linguistic cognitive impairments. Patients with non-fluent aphasia often present with serious cognitive impairments than those with fluent aphasia, especially the impairments of orientation and SP. Non-linguistic cognitive impairments correlate with language impairments in aphasia.

Introduction

Aphasia occurs in about 30% of stroke patients and is characterized by impairments in oral fluency, comprehension, repetition, naming, reading, or writing (1). The processing of language is the core of cognition (2) and requires the participation of other non-linguistic cognitive functions (25). Moreover, the overlapping neural networks of language and non-linguistic cognition (6) suggested that the language and non-linguistic cognitive function of patients with post-stroke aphasia (PSA) (3) cannot be separated.

There is growing consensus that aphasia might be accompanied with deficits of other cognitive functions (2, 79), such as executive function (4, 6, 10), attention (6, 1113), visuospatial perception (14, 15), logical thinking (10), and memory (14, 16, 17), after a left hemispheric stroke. Non-linguistic cognitive impairments are associated with semantic cognitive deficits (18), comprehension difficulty (5), and other language impairments (4), which also play an important role in aphasia recovery and rehabilitation (1925).

However, there is still challenge on the evaluation of non-linguistic cognition in aphasia. Because of the interaction between language and cognitive function, it is important to choose the cognitive tests that are independent of language skills. A behavioral study has found the significant correlation between scores of Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) and Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), and thought MMSE could be used as a rapid screening tool for cognitive function (except for visuospatial function) in PSA patients (26). Some previous studies have focused on domain-specific tests to evaluate different cognitive functions, such as studying the impact of executive impairment on semantic cognition (18), comprehension, and naming (27), investigating the relationship between attention and language performance (12, 28). Some other studies have combined different domain-specific tests (10, 29) or chosen the comprehensive cognitive scales, such as Aphasia Check List (30), Cognitive Linguistic Quick Test (9), and Oxford Cognitive Screen (31). In recent years, researchers have begun to focus on the relationship between the images and the benefits they provide for patients with aphasia, which is essential to advance therapeutic practices (32). Thus, visual-supporting materials may be the most important assessment tools for aphasia. The Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment (LOTCA) is one kind of comprehensive scale (33, 34), which is developed by the Loewenstein Rehabilitation Hospital and widely used to evaluate the cognitive function of various brain diseases. It has the superiority of avoiding the influence of language-related factors on test results because of its use of the pictures. The Chinese version of LOTCA has been revised and verified (35), but it still needs to be further verified via larger-scale Chinese PSA patients.

This study aims to evaluate the cognitive performance of PSA by LOTCA and to compare the cognitive performance between PSA and stroke without aphasia, furtherly to investigate the relationship between non-linguistic cognitive dysfunction and language impairment.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

A total of 86 stroke patients from Beijing Tiantan Hospital, Capital Medical University, were recruited between May 2017 and January 2019. WAB (36) was used to diagnose aphasia. The patients were divided into the PSA and the non-PSA (stroke patients without aphasia) groups. The inclusion criteria for the PSA group were as follows: (a) native Chinese speaker, (b) able to complete all neuropsychological assessments, (c) aphasia quotient (AQ) of WAB <93.8, (d) first stroke with lesions located in the left brain hemisphere, (e) right handedness, and (f) signed informed consent. The inclusion criteria for the non-PSA group were the same as those for the PSA group except for AQ ≥98.4. The exclusion criteria for both the PSA and the non-PSA groups were as follows: (a) AQ ranging from 93.8 to 98.4 (defined as diffuse brain injury or subcortical injury); (b) existing language and cognitive impairments before stroke or combined with other neurological diseases (assessed by medical history, examination, or psychological assessment); (c) recurrent stroke, cerebellar stroke, or brainstem stroke; (d) severe dysarthria; (e) history of mental illness or depression indicated by the score of Stroke Aphasic Depression Questionnaire in Hospital, 10th Version (SADQ-H10) (Appendix 1 in Supplementary Material) (37) >6; (f) severe internal medicine diseases, hearing or vision disorders; and (g) cerebral small vessel disease, which were detectable by image markers (white matter lesions (Fazekas score >0) (38), microbleeds, lacuna, and dilated perivascular spaces) via routine brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan [including T1-weighted image, T2-weighted image, fluid-attenuated inversion recovery, diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI), apparent diffusion coefficient, and susceptibility-weighted imaging (SWI)].

In this study, the norm of the LOTCA scores was used as the control group. The norm of the LOTCA scores was obtained from 44 Chinese individuals without neurological disorders, whose average age was 55.4 ± 23.7 years, and its reliability and validity of the LOTCA (Chinese version) were verified by Yan et al. (35).

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Beijing Tiantan Hospital, Capital Medical University (ethical approval no. KYSB2016-023). Signed written informed consent was given by all participants or their legal representative.

General Clinical Information

Age, gender, educational level, days after stroke onset, medical history, and the score on the National Institutes of Health Stoke Scale (NIHSS) (to assess neurological deficits of stroke) were extracted from medical records by trained research coordinators.

Information of Lesions

All patients underwent brain MRI or computed tomography (CT); two radiologists who were blinded to the neuropsychological assessment interpreted the images independently. Cerebral infarction was determined by DWI, and hemorrhagic lesions were determined by CT or SWI. Slicer 4.10.2 software was used to manually draw and extract lesions. Then MATLAB script was used to obtain the lesion volume (milliliter) based on the product of the number of voxels and the size of voxels in the mask.

Neuropsychological Assessment

All patients underwent neuropsychological assessment, performed by a trained neurologist. The SADQ-H10 (Chinese version) (37) was used to exclude cognitive impairment caused by depression. The WAB was used to diagnose aphasia and to calculate the AQ and scores for spontaneous speech (information and fluency), comprehension, repetition, and naming. The scores for spontaneous speech ranged from 0 to 20. The scores for comprehension, repetition, and naming ranged from 0 to 10: AQ = (spontaneous speech + comprehension + repetition + naming) × 2, which ranged from 0 to 100. The fluency of language was judged by the Chinese aphasia fluency characteristic scale (39) (attached to Appendix 3 in Supplementary Material). This scale included parameters such as vocabulary, intonation, pronunciation, length of phrase, laborsome speech, press of speech, substantive words, grammar, and paraphasia. There were three options for each item with scores ranging from 1 to 3. The total scores ranged from a minimum of 9 to a maximum of 27. Fluent aphasia (from 21 to 27), intermediate aphasia (from 14 to 20), and non-fluent aphasia (from 9 to 13) were subsequently distinguished. Finally, the LOTCA (Chinese version) (Appendix 2 in Supplementary Material) (35) was used to evaluate the non-linguistic cognitive function of all participants. The LOTCA evaluated orientation, visuospatial perception, abstract thinking, organizational reasoning, executive function, and attention. All patients finished the following tasks with verbal and written instructions, pictures, and objects: (a) orientation included orientation to place and orientation to time; the scores ranged from 2 to 16; (b) visual perception (VP) included object identification, shape identification, overlapping figures, and object constancy; the total scores ranged from 4 to 16; and (c) spatial perception (SP) included directions on the client's body, spatial relations, and spatial relations in pictures; the total scores ranged from 3 to 12; (d) motor praxis (MP) included motor imitation, utilization of objects, and symbolic actions; the total scores ranged from 3 to 12; (e) visuomotor organization (VMO) checked the integration of perceptive movement and space; it included copying geometric forms, reproduction of two-dimensional models, pegboard construction, colored block designing, plain block designing, reproduction of a puzzle, and drawing of a clock; the total scores ranged from 7 to 28; (f) thinking operations (TOs) included pictorial classification, Riska unstructured object classification, Riska structured object classification, pictorial sequencing A and B, geometrical sequencing, and logical questions; the total scores ranged from 7 to 31; (g) attention: the scores ranged from 1 to 4; the total scores ranged from 26 to 115, and the score for attention was calculated separately. A lower score represented serious cognitive impairments.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences version 25.0, SAS version 9.4 statistical software (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC), and PRISM 8.0 software were used to analyze the data. Continuous variables with normal distribution are presented as mean (standard deviation [SD]) and compared using t-test. Non-normal distribution variables were presented as median (interquartile range) and compared using the non-parametric rank sum test. Categorical variables were presented as number and percentage and were compared using the χ2-test or Fisher exact test. The total score and subscores of LOTCA between the PSA, non-PSA groups, and Chinese norm were compared using single analysis of variance with group as a factor and followed up with post-hoc tests. When comparing the total score and subscores of LOTCA between the PSA and non-PSA groups, as well as between FAG and non-FAG groups, covariance analysis was used, and the lesion volume was taken as covariate. Partial correlation analyses were performed between WAB scores and LOTCA scores in the PSA group. A two-sided P < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Results

General Clinical Characteristics

A total of 86 stroke patients were enrolled in the study. There were 45 (52.3%) PSA patients and 41 (47.7%) non-PSA patients. The general clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1. No significant differences were found on age, gender, educational level, days after stroke onset, and NIHSS scores between the PSA and the non-PSA groups (P = 0.52, 0.42, 0.40, 0.78, and 0.99, respectively). The volume of lesion of non-PSA group was significantly lower than that of the PSA group (P < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. General clinical characteristics.

WAB and LOTCA Assessments of Subjects

The total LOTCA scores of the PSA group (75.11 ± 17.08) were significantly lower than the Chinese norm (97.65 ± 16.24; P < 0.001) (Table 2). The scores for orientation, VP, SP, MP, VMO, TO, and attention were all significantly lower in the PSA group than that in the Chinese norm (P < 0.017).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of LOTCA scores among PSA group, non-PSA group, and Chinese norm.

No statistical significance was found in the total LOTCA scores between the non-PSA group and the Chinese norm (P = 0.787). However, a significant reduction was observed in the VP and attention scores in the non-PSA group (P = 0.014 and P < 0.001, respectively).

The total LOTCA score in the PSA group was also significantly lower than that in the non-PSA group (96.80 ± 7.75; P < 0.001). Significant differences in orientation, VP, SP, VMO, TO, and attention scores were observed between the PSA and the non-PSA groups (P < 0.001) (Table 2). PSA patients have more severe and extensive non-linguistic cognitive impairments.

Considering the lesion volume as covariate, the analysis of covariance (Table 3) showed there is no significant effect of lesion volume on the difference of LOTCA scores (except for MP) between PSA group and non-PSA group (P > 0.05).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The effect of lesion volume on the difference of LOTCA scores between PSA group and non-PSA group.

The PSA group was further divided into the fluent aphasia group (FAG, n = 22) and the non-fluent aphasia group (non-FAG, n = 21) according to their oral fluency. AQ for FAG ranged from 13.7 to 92.6 with a mean of 73.99 (SD = 16.52), and AQ for non-FAG ranged from 1.5 to 87.6 with a mean of 42.29 (SD = 22.56). Two aphasia patients were judged as intermediate aphasia and thus were not included in the analysis comparing the clinical characteristics between the FAG and non-FAG groups. There was no statistically significant difference in the general clinical characteristics on gender, educational level, stroke type, days after onset, and lesion volume between the FAG and non-FAG groups (Table 4). However, statistically significant difference in the age and NIHSS score (P = 0.047 and P = 0.009, respectively) was found between these two groups (Table 4). Furthermore, the WAB scores were significantly lower in non-fluent aphasia patients compared to patients with fluent aphasia (P < 0.05). The total LOTCA score and scores of orientation, VP, SP, and MP in non-FAG patients were 69.24 ± 18.06, 8.62 ± 5.09, 12.76 ± 2.47, 7.48 ± 3.01, and 9.62 ± 2.25, respectively, which were significantly lower than that in FAG patients (80.36 ± 14.07, P = 0.012; 12.14 ± 3.99, P = 0.007; 14.09 ± 1.93, P = 0.048; 9.68 ± 3.01, P = 0.006; 10.55 ± 1.63, P = 0.033, respectively) (Figure 1).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparison of general characteristics and WAB or LOTCA scores between fluent aphasia and non-fluent aphasia patients.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The covariance analysis of LOTCA scores between FAG group and non-FAG group. FAG, fluent aphasia group; VP, visual perception; SP, spatial perception; MP, motor praxis; VMO, visuomotor organization; TO, thinking operations; LOTCA, Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment.

The analysis of covariance showed there is no significant effect of lesion volume on the difference of LOTCA scores (except for SP score, P = 0.044) between the FAG group and non-FAG group (P > 0.05) (Table 5). We also included the NIHSS score and age into the analysis of covariance and found that they had no significant effect on the difference in LOTCA score.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. The effect of lesion volume on the difference of LOTCA scores between FAG group and non-FAG group.

Relationship Between WAB Scores and LOTCA Scores of PSA Patients

To investigate the interaction of non-linguistic cognitive impairment and language function in PSA patients, the relationship between LOTCA scores and WAB scores was analyzed. Age, gender, days after stroke onset, educational years, lesion volume, and NIHSS scores were used as covariates for partial correlation analysis. The Bonferroni correction for P-value was performed according to the multiple comparisons with the adjusted P = 0.05/40 = 0.00125. The AQ positively correlated with the total LOTCA scores (r = 0.710, P < 0.001). This showed an indication that the severity of aphasia also positively correlated with the severity of non-linguistic cognitive impairment (Table 6). There were also positive correlations between AQ and the orientation, VP, SP, and MP scores, with the exception of VMO, TO, and attention. The scores for spontaneous speech positively correlated with the scores for orientation, SP, MP, and TO (r = 0.613, P < 0.001; r = 0.526, P = 0.001; r = 0.519, P = 0.001; and r = 0.480, P = 0.001, respectively). The scores for comprehension also positively correlated with the scores for orientation, SP, and MP (r = 0.597, P < 0.001; r = 0.664, P < 0.001; and r = 0.607, P < 0.001, respectively). The scores for repetition positively correlated with the scores for orientation, MP, and VMO (r = 0.745, P < 0.001; r = 0.589, P < 0.001; and r = 0.518, P = 0.001, respectively). The score for naming positively correlated with the scores for orientation and SP (r = 0.594, P < 0.001; r = 0.576, P < 0.001, respectively). Thus, almost all the subscores of LOTCA correlated with AQ with the exception of attention, which did not correlate with all the WAB subscores. These results showed the close relationship between language function and non-linguistic cognitive function in PSA patients.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Partial correlation analysis between LOTCA scores and WAB scores of the PSA group.

Discussion

The present study showed that PSA patients suffered from obvious non-linguistic cognitive impairment. In line with our study, Fonseca et al. (40) have analyzed 47 studies and found that 61.3% of the PSA patients (of a total of 1,710 patients) also had non-linguistic cognitive impairment. Compared to stroke patients without aphasia and healthy individuals, patients with PSA had severe and extensive impairments in multiple cognitive domains, such as orientation, visuospatial perception, VMO, thinking, and attention. This result is similar to that of previous studies (2, 7). This study also showed that non-linguistic cognitive impairment in PSA patients correlated with language impairment. The total LOTCA scores positively correlated with the subscores of WAB and AQ. The AQ scores also positively correlated with some subscores of LOTCA, thus indicating that the severity of the language impairments directly impacts on the severity of the non-linguistic cognition impairments. Other studies have also found that non-linguistic cognitive function could be severely impaired when the severity of aphasia increases (7, 41). Both neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies have provided some evidence on how language and non-linguistic cognition could be closely linked together. The poor performance of non-linguistic cognition in PSA patients can be attributed to the involvement of language in the tasks of attention, executive control, visuospatial perception, and logical thinking (2, 3, 6). Another reason could be due to the disturbance of cognitive networks caused by the impairment of the language network (6).

The VP test examines visual recognition and visual reasoning about objects and shapes. The SP test examines whether patients have spatial agnosia. During the test, except for verbal instruction, we can avoid the impact of comprehension and naming deficits of aphasia on the visuospatial performance by matching the pictures with the target card. In our study, we found the VP score of the PSA group was lower than the non-PSA group and positively correlated with the AQ. Thus, the impairment of VP negatively impacts on the general language function. The present study also observed that VP correlated with repetition, and SP significantly correlated with spontaneous speech, comprehension, and naming. This results are consistent with other studies that confirmed the association between visuospatial dysfunctions and comprehensive or expressive difficulties (10, 15). Encoding of stored speech is one of the critical steps when performing comprehension and production tasks, as well as visuospatial tasks. However, some controversies still exist about how language stimulation and visual image stimulation are stored in the mental lexicon. Gainotti et al. (42) agreed with the hypothesis that speech encoding was separate from image encoding. A study also reported that speech encoding was used in the storage of language and visual information. It was deduced from these studies that one of the mechanisms of repetition was the transmission of information from the visuospatial storage to the language buffer (43). Therefore, the mechanism of language processing such as comprehension, repetition, and production may be similar to the mechanism of visuospatial function.

The VMO test is a result of integrating perceptual activities and movements. In the present study, we also observed that the VMO of PSA patients was significantly impaired, which was related to the impairment of repetition. Although VMO cannot systematically evaluate the executive function, the poor performance may reflect the executive dysfunction to some extent. Previous studies have found that executive dysfunction in aphasia patients can predict the language performance (23, 44, 45), which may be explained by the following deductions. As an important part of executive function, working memory is responsible for short-term storage and manipulation of information needed to perform cognitive tasks, which is also crucial to the complex language processing (43), and this may explain the repetition disorder. Thus, if the executive control system is damaged, the processing of the language cannot be completed. Some researchers have considered that executive dysfunction in PSA may be caused by the comprehension impairment (5). However, in the study, the performance of VMO was not found to be related to the comprehension score. Some studies have also shown that it could not be thoroughly explained by comprehension difficulty (27).

Apart from the VMO test, the MP test can also help determine whether the patient has apraxia using password imitation and physical operation. In our study, it was observed that MP significantly correlated with almost all the language functions with the exception of naming. Apraxia often co-occurs with aphasia, but whether they have common mechanisms or anatomical structures is still not clear (46). It is important to screen for apraxia in PSA patients and to investigate the association between apraxia and aphasia with the use of functional MRI and other neuropsychological methods.

The TO test examines the ability of thinking conversion, judgment, summarization, and reasoning about concepts by using picture-based reasoning and classification tasks. It has been previously reported that patients with aphasia have impairment in logical thinking (15, 47). In this study, the severity of TO positively correlated with the severity of auditory comprehension in patients. This finding suggests that language may play an important role in advanced cognitive activities, and we hypothesized that the impairments of critical language functions, such as comprehension, can affect complex cognitive functions in PSA patients. Logical thinking and reasoning involve many core functions, especially the working memory, which can provide cognitive flexibility for reasoning and solving problems (47). Another possible explanation is the damage to “inner language,” which means patients have difficulty both in comprehension and meaningful verbal output (47).

In the present study, patients presented with attention deficit problems, but no significant association was found between attention and language performance. Previous studies have shown that misdistribution and attention deficits in aphasia patients can lead to difficulty in the production and comprehension of language (12, 13, 48). The training of attention can significantly improve comprehensive performance in aphasia patients (9). Attention is a multidimensional cognitive system, including multiple modules such as vigilance, sustained attention, selective attention, attention switching, and distraction attention (12, 49), which is important for the input and output of language (50). Attention deficit may result in the lack or improper allocation of information resources (28). The potential reason for having negative results on attention in our study may be that the scoring method used for attention is simple and subjective. This also suggests that other attention tests are needed when assessing non-linguistic cognitive functions by LOTCA for PSA.

Upon further analysis, we also found that the total LOTCA scores and the orientation, VP, SP, and MP scores in non-FAG were significantly lower than that in the FAG group. This indicates that patients with non-fluent aphasia have concurrent serious cognitive impairments compared with fluent aphasia patients. This finding is in accordance with the study of Ekaterina et al. (51), in which they evaluated the cognition of stroke patients with fluent aphasia and non-fluent aphasia and found that both groups had non-linguistic cognitive impairment; however, the impaired cognitive domains were more extensive in non-fluent aphasia patients. Another study has also reported that non-fluent aphasia patients were more susceptible to cognitive impairment than fluent aphasia patients (40). This phenomenon may be caused by the difference of lesion location. The stroke lesion of patients with fluent aphasia is often located in the back of brain, and the lesion of patients with non-fluent aphasia is often located in the front of brain (40). The location and size of lesion are important factors of aphasia. In the study, we calculated the lesion volume by the routine MRI/CT scan and found the differences in LOTCA scores between fluent and non-fluent aphasia were independent of lesion volume. Non-fluent aphasia patients had higher NIHSS scores and were more likely to have anterior brain lesions, close to the motor cortex. We supposed the hemiplegia might be one of the reasons for different performance between fluent and non-fluent aphasia, but the patients have been trained to use their contralateral limb to finish tasks. Up to now, there are still not enough studies to provide evidence for the mechanism by which speech fluency and non-linguistic cognition are associated. More attention should be focused on the non-linguistic cognitive assessment of non-fluent aphasia patients in clinical practice.

There are some limitations to the present study. First, the LOTCA is timesaving and easy to master; however, it focuses on the evaluation of perception, visual motor organization, and TOs and ignores the detailed examination of memory, attention, and executive function. The scoring method of attention is subjective, and the severity of cognitive impairment cannot be classified. Therefore, it is necessary to add other domain-specific cognitive tests to assess these cognitive domains and the severity of cognitive impairment. Second, this study focused on the behavioral changes of PSA patients, and functional and structural MRI should be used to explore the mechanism of language and cognitive impairments in future studies. Third, the present study is cross-sectional; hence, we were unable to determine the causal relationship between language and non-linguistic cognitive impairments, as well as to learn about the influence of non-linguistic cognitive training. Longitudinal studies are needed to observe the dynamic changes in these factors to obtain more accurate conclusions. Fourth, the limited sample size of this study might have led to a certain degree of bias in the results.

Conclusion

In summary, PSA patients have more extensive and serious non-linguistic cognitive impairments compared with stroke patients without aphasia. Patients with non-fluent aphasia often present with serious cognitive impairments than those with fluent aphasia. Non-linguistic cognitive impairments correlate with language impairments in aphasia. These findings need to be validated in large-scale, longitudinal studies.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committees of Beijing Tiantan Hospital, Capital Medical University, China (ethical approval number: KYSB2016-023). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

YZhan and GL conceived the study, participated in its design and coordination. JY and XL performed the research and drafted the manuscript. QL, NY, XL, and YZhao evaluated the language and cognitive function. YW, JW, and HC interpreted the lesion information, and extracted the lesion volume. JY and MY performed the statistical analyses. ZH participated in the evaluation of cognition and helped revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number: 81972144), National Key Research and Development Program of the Ministry of Science and Technology of the People's Republic of China (grant number: 2018YFC2002300, 2018YFC2002302, and 2017YFC1307702), and Beijing Municipal Administration of Hospitals Clinical Medicine Development of Special Funding Support (grant number: ZYLX201836).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2020.01038/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Flowers HL, Skoretz SA, Silver FL, Rochon E, Fang J, Flamand-Roze C, et al. Poststroke aphasia frequency, recovery, and outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2016) 97:2188–201. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2016.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Ardila A, Rubio-Bruno S. Aphasia from the inside: the cognitive world of the aphasic patient. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. (2018) 25:434–40. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2017.1323753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Peach RK. Cognitive approaches to aphasia treatment: application of the cognition of language to aphasia intervention. Semin Speech Lang. (2017) 38:3–4. doi: 10.1055/s-0036-1597259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Gonçalves APB, Mello C, Pereira AH, Ferré P, Fonseca RP, Joanette Y. Executive functions assessment in patients with language impairment a systematic review. Dement Neuropsychol. (2018) 12:272–83. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-030008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Caplan D, Michaud J, Hufford R. Short-term memory, working memory, and syntactic comprehension in aphasia. Cogn Neuropsychol. (2013) 30:77–109. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2013.803958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Schumacher R, Halai AD, Lambon Ralph MA. Assessing and mapping language, attention and executive multidimensional deficits in stroke aphasia. Brain. (2019) 142:3202–16. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bonini MV, Radanovic M. Cognitive deficits in post-stroke aphasia. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. (2015) 73:840–7. doi: 10.1590/0004-282X20150133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Renjen PN, Gauba C, Chaudhari D. Cognitive impairment after stroke. Cureus. (2015) 7:e335. doi: 10.7759/cureus.335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Helm-Estabrooks N. Cognition and aphasia: a discussion and a study. J Commun Disord. (2002) 35:171–86. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9924(02)00063-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. El Hachioui H, Visch-Brink EG, Lingsma HF, van de Sandt-Koenderman MW, Dippel DW, Koudstaal PJ, et al. Nonlinguistic cognitive impairment in poststroke aphasia: a prospective study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2014) 28:273–81. doi: 10.1177/1545968313508467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Villard S, Kiran S. Between-session intra-individual variability in sustained, selective, and integrational non-linguistic attention in aphasia. Neuropsychologia. (2015) 66:204–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Murray LL. Attention and other cognitive deficits in aphasia: presence and relation to language and communication measures. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. (2012) 21:51–64. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0067)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hula WD, McNeil MR. Models of attention and dual-task performance as explanatory constructs in aphasia. Semin Speech Lang. (2008) 29:169–87. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1082882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Marinelli CV, Spaccavento S, Craca A, Marangolo P, Angelelli P. Different cognitive profiles of patients with severe aphasia. Behav Neurol. (2017) 2017:3875954. doi: 10.1155/2017/3875954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Seniów J, Litwin M, Leśniak M. The relationship between non-linguistic cognitive deficits and language recovery in patients with aphasia. J Neurol Sci. (2009) 283:91–4. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.02.315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Murray L, Salis C, Martin N, Dralle J. The use of standardised short-term and working memory tests in aphasia research: a systematic review. Neuropsychol Rehabil. (2018) 28:309–51. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2016.1174718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Salis C, Kelly H, Code C. Assessment and treatment of short-term and working memory impairments in stroke aphasia: a practical tutorial. Int J Lang Commun Disord. (2015) 50:721–36. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Thompson HE, Almaghyuli A, Noonan KA, Barak O, Lambon Ralph MA, Jefferies E. The contribution of executive control to semantic cognition: convergent evidence from semantic aphasia and executive dysfunction. J Neuropsychol. (2018) 12:312–40. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Brownsett SL, Warren JE, Geranmayeh F, Woodhead Z, Leech R, Wise RJ. Cognitive control and its impact on recovery from aphasic stroke. Brain. (2014) 137:242–54. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Zhou Q, Lu X, Zhang Y, Sun Z, Li J, Zhu Z. Telerehabilitation combined speech-language and cognitive training effectively promoted recovery in aphasia patients. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:2312. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lambon Ralph MA, Snell C, Fillingham JK, Conroy P, Sage K. Predicting the outcome of anomia therapy for people with aphasia post CVA: both language and cognitive status are key predictors. Neuropsychol Rehabil. (2010) 20:289–305. doi: 10.1080/09602010903237875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Simic T, Rochon E, Greco E, Martino R. Baseline executive control ability and its relationship to language therapy improvements in post-stroke aphasia: a systematic review. Neuropsychol Rehabil. (2019) 29:395–439. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2017.1307768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Gilmore N, Meier EL, Johnson JP, Kiran S. Nonlinguistic cognitive factors predict treatment-induced recovery in chronic poststroke aphasia. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2019) 100:1251–8. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2018.12.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Lee B, Pyun SB. Characteristics of cognitive impairment in patients with post-stroke aphasia. Ann Rehabil Med. (2014) 38:759–65. doi: 10.5535/arm.2014.38.6.759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Zakariás L, Keresztes A, Marton K, Wartenburger I. Positive effects of a computerised working memory and executive function training on sentence comprehension in aphasia. Neuropsychol Rehabil. (2018) 28:369–86. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2016.1159579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kang EK, Jeong HS, Moon ER, Lee JY, Lee KJ. Cognitive and language function in aphasic patients assessed with the Korean version of mini-mental status examination. Ann Rehabil Med. (2016) 40:152–61. doi: 10.5535/arm.2016.40.1.152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wall KJ, Cumming TB, Copland DA. Determining the association between language and cognitive tests in poststroke aphasia. Front Neurol. (2017) 8:149. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Pompon RH, McNeil MR, Spencer KA, Kendall DL. Intentional and reactive inhibition during spoken-word stroop task performance in people with aphasia. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2015) 58:767–80. doi: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Butts AM, Machulda MM, Duffy JR, Strand EA, Whitwell JL, Josephs KA. Neuropsychological profiles differ among the three variants of primary progressive aphasia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2015) 21:429–35. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Kalbe E, Reinhold N, Brand M, Markowitsch HJ, Kessler J. A new test battery to assess aphasic disturbances and associated cognitive dysfunctions – German normative data on the aphasia check list. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2005) 27:779–94. doi: 10.1080/13803390490918273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Demeyere N, Riddoch MJ, Slavkova ED, Bickerton WL, Humphreys GW. The Oxford Cognitive Screen (OCS): validation of a stroke-specific short cognitive screening tool. Psychol Assess. (2015) 27:883–94. doi: 10.1037/pas0000082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Brown J, Thiessen A. Using images with individuals with aphasia: current research and clinical trends. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. (2018) 27:504–15. doi: 10.1044/2017_AJSLP-16-0190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Katz N, Itzkovich M, Averbuch S, Elazar B. Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment (LOTCA) battery for brain-injured patients: reliability and validity. Am J Occup Ther. (1989) 43:184–92. doi: 10.5014/ajot.43.3.184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Jang Y, Chern JS, Lin KC. Validity of the Loewenstein occupational therapy cognitive assessment in people with intellectual disabilities. Am J Occup Ther. (2009) 63:414–22. doi: 10.5014/ajot.63.4.414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Yan T, Ma C, Guo Y, Huang L. Validity and reliability of the simplified Chinese version of Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment. Chin J Phys Med Rehabil. (2004) 26: 81–4. doi: 10.3760/j:issn:0254-1424.2004.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Kertesz A. Western Aphasia Battery-Revised. San Antonio, TX: Pearson (2007). doi: 10.1037/t15168-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Liu Y, Zhang Y, Zhang Y, Liu X, Zhang Y. Chinese adaptation and application evaluation of stroke aphasic depression questionnaire-hospital version. Chin J Stroke. (2018) 13:928–33. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1673-5765.2018.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman RA. MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer's dementia and normal aging. AJR Am J Roentgenol. (1987) 149:351–6. doi: 10.2214/ajr.149.2.351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Gao SR. Aphasia (2nd Version). Beijing: Peking University Medical Press (2006). p. 568–9.

Google Scholar

40. Fonseca J, Raposo A, Martins IP. Cognitive functioning in chronic post-stroke aphasia. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. (2019) 26:355–64. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2018.1429442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Lang CJ, Quitz A. Verbal and nonverbal memory impairment in aphasia. J Neurol. (2012) 259:1655–61. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6394-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gainotti G, Cappa A, Perri R, Silveri MC. Disorders of verbal and pictorial memory in right and left brain-damaged patients. Int J Neurosci. (1994) 78:9–20. doi: 10.3109/00207459408986041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Baddeley A. Working memory and language: an overview. J Commun Disord. (2003) 36:189–208. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol. (2012) 63:1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fridriksson J, Nettles C, Davis M, Morrow L, Montgomery A. Functional communication and executive function in aphasia. Clin Linguist Phon. (2006) 20:401–10. doi: 10.1080/02699200500075781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Goldenberg G, Randerath J. Shared neural substrates of apraxia and aphasia. Neuropsychologia. (2015) 75:40–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Baldo JV, Paulraj SR, Curran BC, Dronkers NF. Impaired reasoning and problem-solving in individuals with language impairment due to aphasia or language delay. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:1523. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Kemper S, Schmalzried R, Herman R, Mohankumar D. The effects of varying task priorities on language production by young and older adults. Exp Aging Res. (2011) 37:198–219. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2011.554513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Murray LL. Attention deficits in aphasia: presence, nature, assessment, and treatment. Semin Speech Lang. (2002) 23:107–16. doi: 10.1055/s-2002-24987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Goldstein LB, Bertels C, Davis JN. Interrater reliability of the NIH stroke scale. Arch Neurol. (1989) 46:660–2. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1989.00520420080026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kuzmina E. Weekes BS. Role of cognitive control in language deficits in different types of aphasia. Aphasiology. (2016) 31:765–92. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2016.1263383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: post-stroke aphasia, non-linguistic cognitive impairment, LOTCA, fluent aphasia, non-fluent aphasia

Citation: Yao J, Liu X, Liu Q, Wang J, Ye N, Lu X, Zhao Y, Chen H, Han Z, Yu M, Wang Y, Liu G and Zhang Y (2020) Characteristics of Non-linguistic Cognitive Impairment in Post-stroke Aphasia Patients. Front. Neurol. 11:1038. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.01038

Received: 30 October 2019; Accepted: 10 August 2020;
Published: 30 September 2020.

Edited by:

Swathi Kiran, Boston University, United States

Reviewed by:

Evy Visch-Brink, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Netherlands
Isabel P. Martins, University of Lisbon, Portugal

Copyright © 2020 Yao, Liu, Liu, Wang, Ye, Lu, Zhao, Chen, Han, Yu, Wang, Liu and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gaifen Liu, liugaifen1997@163.com; Yumei Zhang, zhangyumei95@aliyun.com

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.