Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Health Serv., 04 February 2022
Sec. Mental Health Services
Volume 1 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/frhs.2021.831032

Changes in Trauma-Related Cognitions and Emotions After Eliciting Moral Elevation: Examining the Effects of Viewing Others' Virtuous Behavior on Veterans With PTSD

Adam P. McGuire1,2,3* Joanna G. Fagan3 Binh An N. Howard1,2 Annika I. Wurm3 Yvette Z. Szabo1,2,4
  • 1VISN 17 Center of Excellence for Research on Returning War Veterans, Waco, TX, United States
  • 2Central Texas Veterans Health Care System, Temple, TX, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology and Counseling, The University of Texas at Tyler, Tyler, TX, United States
  • 4Department of Health, Human Performance and Recreation, Baylor University, Waco, TX, United States

Moral elevation is described as feeling inspired after witnessing someone perform a virtuous act. Past work suggests the features of moral elevation may be contrary to PTSD, yet few studies have directly tested its impact on relevant symptoms. This experimental study assessed changes in trauma-related cognitions and emotions from after a trauma reminder task to after an elevation induction exercise. We hypothesized that higher elevation after the induction exercise would be associated with greater reductions in cognitions and emotions. Veterans with probable PTSD (N = 38) completed measures of trauma-related cognitions and emotions, once after a written trauma narrative exercise (T1) and again after watching two videos designed to elicit elevation (T2). Veterans also completed measures of state elevation after each video. Results suggest veterans experienced small, significant decreases in self-blame (d = 0.36) and negative beliefs about others (d = 0.46), and medium, significant decreases in guilt (d = 0.68), shame (d = 0.60), and negative beliefs about self (d = 0.69) between T1 and T2. As hypothesized, higher elevation was associated with significantly greater reductions in multiple outcomes above and beyond the effects of general positive affect. Specifically, there were medium effects for changes in shame (β = −0.42, SE = 0.17, p = 0.019, Δf2 = 0.25), negative view of others (β = −0.34, SE = 0.16, p = 0.044, Δf2 = 0.20), and a large effect for changes in negative view of self (β = −0.31, SE = 0.13, p = 0.019, Δf2 = 0.54). These findings suggest elevation may be well-suited to target trauma-related symptoms and future research should further examine its clinical utility.

Introduction

For United States veterans, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a common consequence following exposure to a traumatic event that is associated with distress, impaired functioning and increased healthcare utilization (13). Symptoms of PTSD can involve distressing memories, strong negative beliefs, avoidance behaviors, higher physiological reactivity, and feelings of anger, guilt, or shame (4). Compared to civilians, service members are disproportionately diagnosed with PTSD (5). Furthermore, veterans often experience severe symptomology and poor prognoses (6), highlighting the need to study and identify new avenues to enhance treatment. The purpose of this experimental study is to examine the role of moral elevation, a positive psychology construct, in the context of experiencing trauma-related cognitions and emotions among a sample of veterans with PTSD symptoms.

Moral Elevation

Moral elevation (hereafter, elevation) is a multi-faceted social and emotional state that typically arises after observing another person engage in virtuous behavior (7), such as an act of generosity, compassion, or perseverance. In response, people often report subjective experiences of feeling uplifted, moved, or touched, along with physiological reactions such as a lump in one's throat, piloerection (i.e., goosebumps), tears, or warmth in the chest. Subsequently, the observer may also feel motivated or inspired to imitate the virtuous behavior they witnessed (8, 9).

Previous studies have identified a wide range of benefits and correlates of elevation. For example, in non-clinical populations, elevation was found to be associated with volunteerism and community engagement (10). It has also been correlated with increased pro-social behavior (11) both in the context of considering one's own moral self-image [i.e., helping after affirming positive views of self; (12)] and for extrinsic, altruistic purposes [i.e., helping others without expectation of reward; (13)]. Additionally, elevation experiences are correlated with increased positive affect, compassionate motives, and affiliation with others (14). Notably, these studies also demonstrate the ability to induce elevation by asking participants to watch videos of real people demonstrating virtuous behavior—a common method in elevation research (8, 13, 14). Thus, previous research suggests elevation has the capacity to affect emotional states, as well as motivations and behaviors associated with social engagement by means of compassion, pro-sociality, or social connection more broadly. However, it should be noted that many of these studies used community or university samples and the effects of elevation on military and clinical populations are not well-understood.

Elevation and PTSD

Despite limited research on clinical populations, there is some initial support for the notion that elevation could be useful in mitigating psychological distress. Specifically, preliminary findings regarding elevation and trauma distress suggests elevation may be particularly well-suited to target the cognitive and emotional symptomatology of PTSD. Boosting moral elevation to target trauma distress warrants further investigation because it has been argued that elevation and its features are antithetical to PTSD symptoms (15). For example, military-related trauma often corresponds with exaggerated negative cognitions about the world (16), and sometimes moral injury—witnessing or perpetrating an act that violates deeply held morals or beliefs and results in significant psychological, behavioral, and spiritual consequences (17). In contrast, elevation is associated with positive appraisal of others after witnessing and perceiving virtuous acts. People suffering from PTSD may experience strong negative emotions or sometimes a numbing of positive emotions (18), whereas elevation is described as a strong positive emotional response that can also include involuntary physiological reactions (e.g., lump in throat, piloerection, etc.). Finally, individuals with PTSD often engage in strong avoidance and isolation behaviors, retreating from the world and those around them (19, 20), whereas elevation may elicit the desire to help and connect with others and behave compassionately.

To date, only a few studies have empirically examined the theorized benefits of elevation in the context of trauma-related distress. Tingey et al. (21) first demonstrated the relevance of elevation with a subclinical sample, finding that elevation was a predictor of post-traumatic growth and healthy adaptation following a college campus shooting. Expanding to a clinical population of veterans with PTSD, McGuire et al. (15) examined veterans who were enrolled in residential trauma treatment. In this pilot study, weekly measures of elevation in response to the acts of fellow group members was associated with greater weekly engagement in the group treatment and lower avoidance symptoms at post-treatment. Notably, results from the initial version of the current experimental study (Study 1) demonstrated that veterans who reported elevated levels of PTSD symptoms can indeed experience elevation in response to portrayals of virtuous behavior (22). In Study 1, veterans reported higher elevation after they watched videos intended to induce elevation compared to a control group that was presented with videos intended to induce general positive affect and amusement. Additionally, veterans in the elevation condition described other clinically-relevant outcomes in qualitative response including lessened trauma-related thoughts and emotions, feeling inspired, a desire to engage in pro-social behaviors, an urge to reach out to family members or connect with others, and a desire to be a better person. These early findings, combined with theoretical rationale, suggest that elevation might have the capacity to counteract trauma distress and PTSD symptoms; however, more research is needed to test this theory in a controlled setting with veteran participants who are suffering from significant PTSD symptoms.

Changes in Trauma Cognitions and Emotions

Two specific symptoms of PTSD that could be influenced by elevation are trauma-related cognitions and emotions. As previously noted, negative cognitions and emotions are prominent features of PTSD that can cause significant distress (2325). Trauma-related cognitions can include negative beliefs about the self [e.g., “There is something wrong with me as a person”; (16)], others [e.g., “People can't be trusted”; (26)], or the world in general [e.g., “Nowhere is safe”; (27)]. Two negative emotions that are prevalent among those with PTSD symptoms are guilt and shame. Guilt is considered a self-conscious moral emotion associated with feelings of remorse, and it can be a behavioral motivator to correct wrongs or return to a way of life more consistent with one's moral code (28). Shame is also a negative moral emotion but is distinct from guilt based on slightly different cognitions and outcomes; shame-based thoughts typically entail feelings of inadequacy, being damaged, or being flawed. Unlike guilt, shame is linked with acts of aggression or social isolation (17, 29).

In addition to the need for reducing distress, these targets are also well-suited for examining the within-person effects of elevation because research suggests they can change and are not stable over time. For example, a longitudinal study of Israeli combat veterans and ex-prisoners of war found that cognitions, specifically those pertaining to the benevolence and safety of the world, can worsen over time. Other experimental studies have replicated these findings with regard to worsening cognitions and emotions using script-driven imagery activities and other such trauma reminders (30, 31). Conversely, studies have also found that the severity of trauma-related cognitions and emotions can lessen over time in the context of experimental studies (32, 33) and during the course of treatment (34, 35). Given the capacity for cognitions and emotions to change—an innate goal of trauma-focused treatment for PTSD—the clinical utility of eliciting elevation could be demonstrated by assessing for within-person changes in trauma cognitions and emotions following an elevation induction exercise. This would expand our understanding of elevation's potential to impact survivors of trauma and people suffering from PTSD symptoms, while also adding to the literature by identifying a novel tool to alleviate cognitive and emotional distress in this population.

Current Study

The purpose of this study is to investigate the specific effects of eliciting elevation in veterans with significant PTSD symptoms on trauma-related cognitions and emotions following a trauma reminder—expanding on preliminary findings from the initial experiment (Study 1) that demonstrated veterans with PTSD symptoms have the capacity to experience elevation (22). Based on existing research, it was hypothesized that negative emotions (shame and guilt) and negative cognitions about the self, others, and self-blame would reduce over time following a trauma reminder task. We also hypothesized that higher levels of elevation following exposure to elevation-inducing videos would predict greater decreases in negative emotions and cognitions from post-trauma reminder to post-elevation induction.

Methods

Participants and Procedures

Recruitment methods included mailing out recruitment letters to veterans enrolled in Veterans Health Administration (VHA) care, in-person recruitment in VHA PTSD clinics, flyers posted in VHA clinics and community sites, and direct referrals from VHA clinicians. First, veterans were screened for potential eligibility using the Posttraumatic Checklist for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition [PCL-5; (36)]. Veterans with a total score of 33 or higher were viewed as experiencing significant symptoms of PTSD (37, 38) and therefore, deemed eligible to participate in the study. Other inclusion criteria included veteran status and at least 18 years of age. Exclusion criteria were active psychosis, active mania/hypomania, cognitive disorders (e.g., dementia, traumatic brain injury), and age of 90 years or older. Eligibility was assessed during a phone screen. Following the phone screen, eligible veterans scheduled an in-person appointment and provided written consent to participate in the study upon arrival to that appointment. All study procedures were approved by the local Institutional Review Board.

After consenting, veterans participated in an experimental session that included a series of repeated measures and study tasks. The schedule for session procedures is presented in Figure 1. First, participants completed baseline measures, then they completed a written trauma narrative describing their worst traumatic event in detail for 15 min. The purpose of this task was to elicit and measure thoughts and feelings associated with recalling a traumatic event. To ensure confidentiality of sensitive information and increase the likelihood that veterans would fully engage with the exercise, participants were notified that study staff would not collect or view their trauma narrative and participants were encouraged to destroy the written piece of paper at the end of the experimental session.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Procedures for the experimental session.

Immediately after the trauma narrative, veterans completed questionnaires that assessed the severity of trauma-related cognitions and emotions experienced at that moment (Time 1; T1). All participants were then presented with three video clips, followed by brief measures to assess state-level emotional responses to each video. The first video was a neutral stimulus that was not intended to elicit positive or negative emotions, which served as a manipulation check for elevation and positive affect responses.

Next, participants viewed two videos that were specifically intended to elicit elevation. One video described the heroic efforts of a man (self-identified as a veteran) who saved another man's life after he fell onto the subway tracks. The second video told a story about a father who competed in marathons and triathlons with his son who had paraplegia by carrying, pushing, and pulling his son through each race. The order of the videos was randomized across participants. Two videos were included with the intent to increase the dose of elevation experienced and increase the potential of impacting changes in trauma-related cognitions and emotions. The two elevation videos lasted ~7 min in total. In this study, there was no control condition and all participants were presented with the two elevation videos. This decision was based on two considerations. First, results from quantitative and qualitative analyses of data from Study 1 indicated the elevation videos elicited a distinct response from positive affect or general amusement that was consistent with the theory of elevation (22); thus, establishing that the same videos used in this study elicit the desired state elevation response within this population. Second, there are significant resource constraints to collecting data for an in-person, experimental study that requires a veteran sample with elevated PTSD symptom severity; thus, a within-person design without a control condition increased feasibility of completing data collection and increased power for the present set of analyses. Therefore, the present study can be considered a preliminary examination of the cognitive and emotional responses to elevation-inducing videos.

After watching both videos, participants were asked to complete a brief journal exercise describing their reactions to the video with prompts (see Supplementary Material). This exercise was added to encourage participants to reflect on what they witnessed in the elevation videos and to consider how they reacted or felt after viewing that content. After the journal exercise, they completed the same trauma-related cognition and emotion questionnaires for a second time (Time 2: T2). The two assessments of cognitions and emotions were administered ~50 min apart. Additionally, it should be noted that we also collected saliva samples at four timepoints for future analysis of biomarkers associated with elevation.1

Forty-six participants were enrolled, but five participants were excluded because they did not complete the experimental tasks. One participant was removed because study staff noted significant issues with inattention throughout the session (e.g., sleeping during videos), one participant was removed because of technical issues that prevented them from completing the second half of the session, and one participant was removed for having 87.5% of their data missing. All remaining participants had complete data except three veterans who had one item missing across three separate measures. The final sample included 38 veterans. Descriptive statistics for demographic characteristics and military history are reported in Table 1. The most commonly endorsed trauma was the sudden death of a friend/loved one (94.74%) and the most common worst trauma was related to combat (55.26%). The means and standard deviations for all measures used in subsequent analyses are reported in Table 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and military history of study sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean and standard deviation for study variables at all timepoints.

Measures

PTSD Symptoms

The 20-item PCL-5 (36) assessed the severity of PTSD symptoms over the past month. Items were rated on a Likert-style scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). Total scores are calculated by summing all items with higher scores indicating greater symptom severity. In this sample, person mean imputation was used to create total scores because one participant had one missing item. The PCL-5 has demonstrated adequate reliability and validity in previous studies that included veteran samples, with evidence that scores of 33 or higher indicate subclinical or clinical PTSD severity (37, 38). Internal consistency in the present sample was α = 0.83 [95% CI: 0.76, 0.91].

Trauma History

The Trauma Life Events Questionnaire [TLEQ; (39)] assessed history of trauma exposure by asking participants to indicate whether they experienced any of 24 different traumatic events. Examples of traumatic events assessed include natural disasters, motor vehicle accidents, warfare or combat, and sexual assault. Participants also indicated the worst traumatic event by selecting the trauma type that “causes the most distress.” This measure was included to characterize the sample.

Video Responses

State Elevation

Currently, there are no standardized measures for assessing state-level elevation. The Elevation Scale [ES; (40)] or a modified version of it is widely used, but this scale assesses trait-like tendencies to experience elevation rather than a momentary state response. To capture state elevation in this study (i.e., response to elevation stimuli), we combined items adapted from the ES with items from a daily elevation measure previously used with a clinical population (41). Instructions from the ES were altered by asking participants to rate the extent they experienced 12 items immediately after watching elevation video stimuli on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). Example items include “Somehow ‘lifted up’ or in touch with the better parts of myself,” “Tears in my eyes,” and “Motivated to live in a nobler or virtuous way” (see Supplementary Material for full measure). All items were summed to create a total score using person mean imputation because one participant had one missing item. Higher scores indicated greater elevation. Internal consistency in the present sample was α = 0.94 [0.92, 0.97] for state elevation following Video 1 and α = 0.95 [0.93, 0.97] for state elevation following Video 2.

Positive Affect

The Positive Affect and Negative Affect Schedule [PANAS; (42)] assessed positive affect (PA) in response to session tasks, including elevation videos. Five items were rated on a scale from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely). Example items include “alert” and “inspired.” Total scores were computed by summing all five items. Higher scores indicated greater positive affect. Internal consistency for state PA was α = 0.87 [0.81, 0.93] following Video 1 and α = 0.89 [0.85, 0.92] following Video 2.

Pre-post Measures

Shame and Guilt

The 10-item State Shame and Guilt Scale [SSGS; (43)] assessed severity of state-level shame and guilt at T1 and T2. Items were rated on a scale from 1 (I do not feel this way at all) to 5 (I feel this way very strongly) and summed to create total scores, with higher scores indicating more severe shame and guilt. Example items for shame include “I feel that I am a bad person” and “I feel worthless, powerless,” and example items for guilt include “I feel remorse, regret” and “I feel like apologizing, confessing.” Internal consistency for state shame was α = 0.88 [0.82, 0.94] at T1 and α = 0.90 [0.84, 0.95] at T2; state guilt was α = 0.86 [0.79, 0.94] at T1 and α = 0.90 [0.85, 0.95] at T2.

Negative Trauma-Related Cognitions

The 36-item Posttraumatic Cognition Inventory [PTCI; (44)] assessed strength of negative cognitions and beliefs associated with an index trauma—in this case, the index trauma referred to the trauma described in the written narrative exercise. Each item is rated from 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree). Administration for this study was modified by instructing veterans to rate the extent to which they endorsed each item “at this moment.” The PTCI was administered twice: at T1 and T2. Scores on each item are summed to create subscale scores for negative views of the self (e.g., “I am inadequate”), the world or others (e.g., “People can't be trusted”), and self-blame (e.g., “The event happened because of the way I acted”), with higher scores indicating stronger beliefs. Person mean imputation was used for calculating subscale scores given that one participant had one missing item. Previous studies demonstrated reliability and validity for all three subscales (45). This study also found adequate reliability for each subscale: PTCI negative view of self was α = 0.95 [0.93, 0.98] at T1 and α = 0.96 [0.94, 0.98] at T2; PTCI negative view of others was α = 0.80 [0.70, 0.90] at T1 and α = 0.88 [0.82, 0.94] at T2; PTCI self-blame was α = 0.87 [0.80, 0.94] at T1 and α = 0.86 [0.79, 0.94] at T2.

Data Analytic Plan

All data management and analyses were conducted with R (46). First, because the amount of missingness is limited to three items across the final sample and appears to be completely random (distinct participants and measures), missing data was handled with person mean imputation for the three measures that include one missing item each. Next, to test our hypothesis that emotions and cognitions reduced from T1 to T2, we used the base stats package to calculate paired samples t-tests and the rstatix package (47) to estimate standardized mean difference effect sizes with 95% confidence intervals (CI). We interpreted d > 0.20, d > 0.50, and d > 0.80 as small, medium, and large effects (48).

To test our second hypothesis that higher elevation experienced after videos would be associated with greater reductions in cognition and emotion, we used a series of multiple linear regression models for five outcomes of interest: guilt, shame, negative views of self, self-blame, and negative views of others. Specifically, we fit a baseline model and a theoretical model for each outcome in which the T2 score of that cognition or emotion was set as the dependent variable. The baseline model included the T1 score of the same target variable such that remaining variance explained in the model could be attributed to residual change in that variable. We also added the average PA score across both videos as a covariate in the baseline model to account for the potential variance explained by PA (i.e., generic, positive emotional response). The theoretical model added the average elevation score across both videos as a predictor in addition to the T1 score and average PA score as covariates. Thus, effects for average elevation as a predictor would demonstrate the extent to which elevation responses explained unique variance in the observed residual change or decrease for each outcome above and beyond the effects of PA. Regression coefficients were reported in the standardized metric (β).

We also examined R2 to understand the percent of variance accounted for in the model and inspected f2 to assess effect size. To further examine the hypothesis that higher elevation is linked with greater reductions in trauma-related cognitions and emotions, we calculated changes in effect sizes (Δf2) between the baseline model and the theoretical model after average state elevation was added as a predictor. We interpreted changes in f2 > 0.02, f2 > 0.15, and f2 > 0.35 as small, medium, and large effects (48) attributable to the addition of state elevation. We also calculated 95% CI for change in f2 using online calculators (49).

Results

Changes in Cognitions and Emotions

Consistent with hypotheses, participants reported medium significant decreases in state guilt and state shame from the moment they completed the trauma narrative exercise (T1) to after viewing both videos and completing the reflection exercise (T2; see Table 3). Regarding PTCs, consistent with hypotheses, participants reported medium significant decreases in negative beliefs about self from pre- to post-video, and small significant decreases in self-blame and negative beliefs about others.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparing cognitions and emotions between post-trauma narrative and post-elevation induction.

The Impact of Elevation on Changes in Cognitions and Emotions

As hypothesized, higher elevation following the two elevation-inducing videos and reflection exercise was a significant predictor of residual change or decrease in state shame (β = −0.40, SE = 0.19, p = 0.038) from before the elevation videos, above and beyond the effects of positive affect experienced in response to the videos. Furthermore, adding elevation as a predictor of change resulted in a medium-sized contribution to the model for state shame (Δf2 = 0.21), with the theoretical model explaining 42% of the variance in shame at T2. However, contrary to hypotheses, higher elevation was not a significant predictor of residual change for guilt (β = −0.24, SE = 0.16, p = 00.139). The addition of state elevation resulted in a medium-sized contribution to the guilt model (Δf2 = 0.18), but the 95% CI included zero (−0.03, 0.52). Since state elevation was not a significant predictor in the model by itself, the medium-sized effect should be interpreted with caution. See Table 4 for a summary of models.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Model comparison for baseline and theoretical models (IV = state elevation average; COV = T1 score, PA Ave; DV = T2 score).

As hypothesized, after accounting for positive affect experienced in response to videos, higher elevation was also a significant predictor of residual change or decrease in negative beliefs about self (β = −0.33, SE = 0.14, p = 0.022) and negative beliefs about others (β = −0.38, SE = 0.17, p = 0.036). The addition of elevation resulted in a large-sized contribution to the model for negative beliefs about self (Δf2 = 0.54), with the theoretical model explaining 72% of the variance explained in negative views of self at T2. Furthermore, adding elevation as a predictor of change in negative beliefs about others resulted in a medium-sized contribution to the model (Δf2 = 0.23) with the theoretical model explaining 48% of the variance in negative views of others at T2. However, contrary to hypothesis, higher elevation was not a significant predictor of self-blame (β = 0.02, SE = 0.11, p = 0.833). The addition of state elevation did not contribute significant changes in the effect size of the self-blame model (Δf2 = 0.00).

Discussion

Existing literature supports the notion that veterans with PTSD can experience elevation and that doing so may elicit distinct responses from other positive emotions. The purpose of the present study was to further evaluate the role of elevation on trauma-related cognitions and emotions among a sample of veterans with probable PTSD. It was hypothesized that negative emotions (shame and guilt) and negative cognitions about the self, others, and self-blame would reduce from T1 to T2. We also hypothesized that higher levels of elevation reported following exposure to elevation-inducing videos would predict greater decreases in negative emotions and cognitions from post-trauma narrative to post-elevation induction.

Changes in Cognitions and Emotions

Consistent with hypotheses, there were significant differences in levels of negative emotions (i.e., shame and guilt) and negative cognitions about the self, others, and self-blame between T1 to T2; these reduced from after a written trauma narrative exercise to after watching and journaling about elevating videos. Pre-post mean difference scores ranged in magnitude from small to medium, with the largest difference found in participants' negative views about the self, followed by guilt, shame, and negative beliefs about others. Self-blame had the smallest effect relative to other changes, which could suggest that guilt, shame, and the other types of trauma cognitions may be particularly amenable to change following exposure to elevation stimuli, whereas self-blame may be less flexible or more challenging to modify. One possible explanation for the smaller effect is potential heterogenous experiences of self-blame. For example, Janoff-Bulman (50) proposed two types of self-blame attributions: behavioral and characterological self-blame. Behavioral self-blame reflects survivors' belief that their own behavior led to the traumatic event. Characterological self-blame focuses on the survivor's personality or character as the cause of event, rather than their behavior. Perhaps it is difficult to modify specific self-blame attributions, which could be made even more rigid depending on the type of traumatic event (e.g., behavioral self-blame for engaging in combat). More research is needed to understand how variations of self-blame and the context of traumatic events influences changes in beliefs about self-blame, particularly to the extent it informs existing and future treatment application.

Effects of Elevation on Changes in Cognitions and Emotions

Subsequent analyses were used to expand on these findings by assessing the extent to which reported levels of elevation predicted changes in each cognition and emotion. As hypothesized, higher state elevation experienced after viewing inspirational videos and completing a reflection exercise was a significant predictor of reductions in shame, negative views of self, and negative views of others. Regarding significant findings, there was a medium effect on changes in shame and negative views of others, which should be interpreted with caution given that the lower bounds of the 95% confidence intervals were slightly below zero (shame LLI = −0.02; neg view of others LLI = −0.01). On the other hand, we found a large effect for negative views of the self. Notably, these findings reflect the unique association with elevation experienced during the videos above and beyond the effects of general PA. Although we did not directly test mechanisms for these associations, there are several potential explanations based on the existing literature for these outcome variables.

Shame is an affective state that includes a focus and concern about the self; negative experiences are a reflection of the bad self and the self is consequently scrutinized and negatively evaluated (51). Given that elevation is theorized to shift attention outside the self (52), it may impact levels of shame by redirecting one's focus from negative self-perceptions to the uncommon acts of goodness or moral beauty of others. Shame is characterized by heightened self-focus, whereas elevation is an other-praising emotion that draws attention toward the good in others (8, 14).

Because elevation involves positive appraisals of others' actions by definition, it follows that there would be a significant effect on changes in negative cognitions about the world. Specifically, negative cognitions about the world involve beliefs that people cannot be trusted and that the world is a dangerous place (44), whereas experiences of elevation are associated with connectedness, openness, and loving feelings toward other people [e.g., (40)]. Perhaps elevation provides an opportunity to collect evidence that might contradict strong negative beliefs about others, while also providing exposure to experiencing genuine compassion toward strangers or humanity in general. These findings are consistent with another study that examined daily elevation in a clinical population of people with depression and anxiety, which suggested higher levels of elevation were linked to stronger beliefs that people should look out for one another, perceived closeness to others, and urges to support and help people (41).

Although elevation is an other-praising emotion, it was also associated with a large magnitude change in negative views of the self. Notably, this was a stronger effect relative to medium changes in negative views of others. One potential explanation for this finding is that elevation might increase awareness of one's capacity for good or moral characteristics in general. Monin (53) proposed that observing exceptional moral behavior may trigger an upward social comparison, which might remind people of their own values and elicit evidence or recognition that one is capable of acting in line with those values [e.g., being a good, moral person; (54)]. A study by Schnall and Roper (12) also found that self-affirmation (before an elevating film clip) was associated with participants' identifying positive qualities within themselves (e.g., helping others) and accessing evidence of their own ability to do good. Lastly, previous studies have found that elevation is associated with a strong desire to become a better person [e.g., (55)]; perhaps experiencing the motivation for self-improvement alone has some kind of positive impact on views of the self. Now that the present study provides initial evidence that elevation is associated with immediate cognitive and emotional changes, future research is needed to expand on these findings and decipher the specific mechanisms that facilitate the change process, and the extent to which these immediate changes have any kind of long-term, positive impact on trauma recovery.

Contrary to hypotheses, state elevation was not a significant predictor of changes in guilt or self-blame. There are several possible reasons this association was not observed in the present study. First, regarding self-blame, it could be related to the smaller reduction in scores from T1 to T2, which may indicate there was not enough variance or change in reported self-blame for state elevation to be a meaningful predictor above and beyond the effects of PA. Alternatively, guilt and self-blame might be conceptually different than the other outcomes in ways that might be differentially influenced by elevation. For example, guilt and self-blame share some conceptual features insofar as they could be more tied to specific facts surrounding certain events/trauma that might be more descriptive of the traumatic incident itself (56) and less flexible to interpretation (e.g., I did not fight-off my attacker). Alternatively, shame and negative cognitions of the self/world could be described as more generalized and somewhat based on interpretations of the traumatic event or one's actions/inaction; therefore, those generalized beliefs or interpretations may be more amenable to change than guilt or self-blame. For example, it seems possible that elevation may not impact guilt or self-blame if a veteran believes they had some degree of responsibility for an event (e.g., I had a role in a friend's death during a combat situation), but one can reasonably assume it is possible to reduce the amount of shame that veteran experiences as a result of that same event. Further research is needed to identify mechanisms exclusive to changes in guilt and self-blame, and to determine whether features of these symptoms are in less likely to be modified following experiences of elevation.

Potential Treatment Implications

These findings have potential implications for the clinical utility of elevation and treatment for trauma distress. First, it should be noted that the long-term effects of inducing elevation are unknown and beyond the scope of this study. These findings primarily support an initial proof of concept that elevation may play a role in immediate or momentary shifts in relevant cognitions and emotions, above and beyond the effects of experiencing general PA. Accordingly, in the short-term, elevation might be useful as a novel tool to regulate emotions and buffer against strong negative beliefs in a given moment that veterans with PTSD encounter in their daily life. For example, it might be helpful in response to distress associated with trauma reminders, similar to this study in which elevation induction occurred immediately after a trauma-cue reminder. The extent to which momentary shifts via elevation promote long-term changes in symptoms and behaviors is unknown; thus, future research is needed to better understand the clinical utility of these immediate elevation reactions.

If additional empirical support is found, one example of how experiencing elevation could have long-term implications is by promoting increased trait-like sensitivity to experiencing elevation through repeated elevation inductions, as indicated in past work with civilian, non-clinical populations [e.g., (14)]. The potential clinical utility of a greater predisposition toward experiencing elevation could include a greater awareness of the goodness in others and increased likelihood of feeling motivated to engage in behaviors consistent with trauma recovery (e.g., connect with others, pro-sociality). If elevation is to be used as a therapeutic tool, then more research is required to better understand the additive and long-term effects of experiencing momentary states of elevation, determine whether elevation via exposure to others' virtuous behavior leads to observable mechanisms as theorized (e.g., increased social engagement), and assess whether there are identifiable mechanisms that directly mediate decreased PTSD symptoms and increased well-being. Furthermore, implementation research is also needed to examine whether elevation induction and related activities can be used as a brief skill or exercise, a standalone intervention, or perhaps a supplement to other treatments for PTSD.

Limitations

The findings of the present study should be considered in the light of study limitations. First, we used a small sample size that was primarily male and White. Future studies should aim to expand our understanding of this phenomenon with larger and more diverse samples. Related, because the authors are unaware of other studies examining elevation and trauma-related symptoms in an experimental study, it is unclear if these findings are specific to veterans or if they generalize to the civilian population, and subsequently a wider-range of trauma types. Given that there were no standardized measures for assessing state elevation when this study was conducted, the elevation measure we used is limited and further psychometric analyses are needed. Without developing and testing a new measure, we aimed to address this limitation by utilizing an elevation measure that demonstrated adequate reliability in past studies (40), is consistent with the established theoretical underpinnings of elevation, and demonstrated high internal consistency in the present study. Next, the present study examined change in several outcomes following elevation stimuli alone without a control condition. This decision was based on resource constraints and previous findings that elevation responses were distinct following the elevation videos compared to amusement videos, which suggests the same video stimuli used in this study would elicit the desired emotional response. However, the lack of a control group in the present study that examined changes in different outcomes precludes causal inference. Lastly, there are additional, unaccounted features of the study design that could have contributed to changes in pre-post scores. For example, because the journal exercise (i.e., reflecting on the elevation videos) was completed before the administration of T2 measures, participating in the journal exercise could have influenced the self-reported reduction of cognitions and emotions above and beyond the effects of state elevation following the videos—the primary predictor of change in our analyses. Improvements in cognition and emotion scores could have also been influenced by other features like generalized distraction from thinking about the traumatic event, return to baseline after activating trauma-related distress (i.e., regression to the mean), or unintended demand characteristics of the procedure. For example, high levels of social desirability could also influence reported changes as well as state elevation responses; thus, further research is needed to fully assess potential confounding variables with an appropriate sample size. Overall, future studies may benefit by expanding to include a control condition and aim to differentiate the effects of various components of engagement with virtuous behavior to determine specific mechanisms that might drive changes in trauma-related symptoms.

Conclusion

Findings from this study showed significant changes in negative emotions and cognitions about the self, others, and self-blame following a written trauma narrative and elevation-inducing videos. Greater experiences of elevation after watching elevation-inducing videos were associated with shame, negative cognitions about the self, and negative cognitions about others. According to results, exposure to elevation videos may shift trauma-related cognitions and emotions among a sample of veterans with probable PTSD. Future studies should continue to explore this positive emotion and expand our understanding of the clinical utility of elevation.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this article is not readily available because given the nature of this research, participants of this study did not agree for their data to be shared publicly. Requests to access the data should be directed to adam.mcguire@va.gov.

Ethics Statement

This study involved human participants and was reviewed and approved by Central Texas Veterans Health Care System Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AM, BH, and YS collected data. AM analyzed the data with statistical consultation from YS. AM, JF, BH, AW, and YS drafted the manuscript. BH, JF, and AW provided material support. BH provided administrative and technical support. All authors provided critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content and read, discussed, and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

AM was supported by a Small Projects in Rehabilitation Research Award I21-RX003035 and YS was supported by Career Development Award IK1-RX003122 from the United States (U.S.) Department of Veterans Affairs, Rehabilitation Research and Development Service.

Author Disclaimer

The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the official policy or position of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

This material is the result of work with resources and the use of facilities at the VISN 17 Center of Excellence for Research on Returning War Veterans and the Central Texas Veterans Health Care System.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/frhs.2021.831032/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^Participants completed additional measures that were added for secondary analyses and not reported here. A list of all measures and the administration schedule can be found in the Supplementary Material.

References

1. Calhoun PS, Bosworth HB, Grambow SC, Dudley TK, Beckham JC. Medical service utilization by veterans seeking help for posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2002) 159:2081–6. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Chan D, Cheadle AD, Reiber G, Unützer J, Chaney EF. Health care utilization and its costs for depressed veterans with and without comorbid PTSD symptoms. Psychiatric Serv. (2009) 60:1612–7. doi: 10.1176/ps.2009.60.12.1612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lang KP, Veazey-Morris K, Berlin KS, Andrasik F. Factors affecting health care utilization in OEF/OIF veterans: the impact of PTSD and pain. Milit Med. (2016) 181:50–5. doi: 10.7205/MILMED-D-14-00444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5 diagnostic classification. In: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). (2013). p. 10. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.x00DiagnosticClassification

CrossRef Full Text

5. Hoge CW, Castro CA, Messer SC, McGurk D, Cotting DI, Koffman RL. Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. N Engl J Med. (2004) 351:13–22. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa040603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Trivedi RB, Post EP, Sun H, Pomerantz A, Saxon AJ, Piette JD, et al. Prevalence, comorbidity, and prognosis of mental health among US veterans. Am J Public Health. (2015) 105:2564–9. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Haidt J. Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In: Keyes CLM, Haidt J, editors. Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well-Lived. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (2003). p. 275–89.

Google Scholar

8. Algoe SB, Haidt J. Witnessing excellence in action: the ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. J Pos Psychol. (2009) 4:105–27. doi: 10.1080/17439760802650519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Silvers JA, Haidt J. Moral elevation can induce nursing. Emotion. (2008) 8:291–5. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Cox KS. Elevation predicts domain-specific volunteerism 3 months later. J Pos Psychol. (2010) 5:333–41. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2010.507468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Schnall S, Roper J, Fessler DMT. Elevation leads to altruistic behavior. Psychol Sci. (2010) 21:315–20. doi: 10.1177/0956797609359882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Schnall S, Roper J. Elevation puts moral values into action. Soc Psychol Person Sci. (2012) 3:373–8. doi: 10.1177/1948550611423595

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Van de Vyver J, Abrams D. Testing the prosocial effectiveness of the prototypical moral emotions: elevation increases benevolent behaviors and outrage increases justice behaviors. J Exp Social Psychol. (2015) 58:23–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Erickson TM, McGuire AP, Scarsella GM, Crouch TA, Lewis JA, Eisenlohr AP, et al. Viral videos and virtue: moral elevation inductions shift affect and interpersonal goals in daily life. J Pos Psychol. (2018) 13:643–54. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2017.1365163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. McGuire AP, Nosen E, Lyons JA. Benefits of moral elevation in veterans with PTSD and moral injury: a proposed theoretical framework and pilot study. Milit Behav Health. (2019) 7:315–26. doi: 10.1080/21635781.2018.1540316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Horwitz AG, Held P, Klassen BJ, Karnik NS, Pollack MH, Zalta AK. Posttraumatic cognitions and suicidal ideation among veterans receiving PTSD treatment. Cognit Ther Res. (2018) 42:711–9. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9925-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Litz BT, Stein N, Delaney E, Lebowitz L, Nash WP, Silva C, et al. Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: a preliminary model and intervention strategy. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:695–706. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Able ML, Benedek DM. Severity and symptom trajectory in combat-related PTSD: a review of the literature. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2019) 21:58. doi: 10.1007/s11920-019-1042-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Badour CL, Blonigen DM, Boden MT, Feldner MT, Bonn-Miller MO. A longitudinal test of the bi-directional relations between avoidance coping and PTSD severity during and after PTSD treatment. Behav Res Ther. (2012) 50:610–6. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Sherman MD, Gress Smith JL, Straits-Troster K, Larsen JL, Gewirtz A. Veterans' perceptions of the impact of PTSD on their parenting and children. Psychol Serv. (2016) 13:401–10. doi: 10.1037/ser0000101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Tingey JL, McGuire AP, Stebbins OL, Erickson TM. Moral elevation and compassionate goals predict posttraumatic growth in the context of a college shooting. J Pos Psychol. (2019) 14:261–70. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2017.1402077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. McGuire AP, Mignogna J. Inducing moral elevation in veterans with PTSD: An experimental investigation using a mixed methods approach. J Exp Psychopathol. (2021) 12:204380872098241. doi: 10.1177/2043808720982415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Dunmore E, Clark DM, Ehlers A. Cognitive factors involved in the onset and maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after physical or sexual assault. Behav Res Ther. (1999) 37:809–29. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00181-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hayes JP, VanElzakker MB, Shin LM. Emotion and cognition interactions in PTSD: a review of neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Front Integr Neurosci. (2012) 6:89. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Holmes EA, Grey N, Young KAD. Intrusive images and “hotspots” of trauma memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: an exploratory investigation of emotions and cognitive themes. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2005) 36:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Wells SY, Morland LA, Torres EM, Kloezeman K, Mackintosh MA, et al. The development of a brief version of the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI-9). Assessment. (2019) 26:193–208. doi: 10.1177/1073191116685401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Brown LA, Belli GM, Asnaani A, Foa EB. A review of the role of negative cognitions about oneself, others, and the world in the treatment of PTSD. Cognit Ther Res. (2019) 43:143–73. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9938-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lee DA, Scragg P, Turner S. The role of shame and guilt in traumatic events: a clinical model of shame-based and guilt-based PTSD. Br. J Med Psychol. (2001) 74:451–66. doi: 10.1348/000711201161109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Crocker LD, Haller M, Norman SB, Angkaw AC. Shame versus trauma-related guilt as mediators of the relationship between PTSD symptoms and aggression among returning veterans. Psychol Trauma. (2016) 8:520–7. doi: 10.1037/tra0000151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Norte CE, Souza GGL, Vilete L, Marques-Portella C, Coutinho ESF, Figueira I, et al. They know their trauma by heart: an assessment of psychophysiological failure to recover in PTSD. J Affect Disord. (2013) 150:136–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Raudales AM, Short NA, Schmidt NB. Emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between trauma type and PTSD symptoms in a diverse trauma-exposed clinical sample. Pers Ind Diff. (2019) 139:28–33. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.10.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hagenaars MA, Arntz A. Reduced intrusion development after post-trauma imagery rescripting; an experimental study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2012) 43:808–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Woud ML, Cwik JC, Blackwell SE, Kleim B, Holmes EA, Adolph D, et al. Does napping enhance the effects of cognitive bias modification-appraisal training? An experimental study. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0192837. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Macdonald A, Pukay-Martin ND, Wagner AC, Fredman SJ, Monson CM. Cognitive–behavioral conjoint therapy for PTSD improves various PTSD symptoms and trauma-related cognitions: Results from a randomized controlled trial. J Family Psychol. (2016) 30:157–62. doi: 10.1037/fam0000177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Peck KR, Coffey SF, McGuire AP, Voluse AC, Connolly KM. A cognitive processing therapy-based treatment program for veterans diagnosed with co-occurring posttraumatic stress disorder and substance use disorder: the relationship between trauma-related cognitions and outcomes of a 6-week treatment program. J Anxiety Disord. (2018) 59:34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2018.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Keane TM, Palmieri PA, Marx BP, Schnurr PP. The PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). (2013). National Center for PTSD. Available online at: www.Ptsd.va.Gov (accessed May 08, 2021).

37. Bovin MJ, Marx BP, Weathers FW, Gallagher MW, Rodriguez P, Schnurr PP, et al. Psychometric properties of the PTSD checklist for diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders–fifth edition (PCL-5) in veterans. Psychol. Assess. (2016) 28:1379–91. doi: 10.1037/pas0000254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Wortmann JH, Jordan AH, Weathers FW, Resick PA, Dondanville KA, Hall-Clark B, et al. Psychometric analysis of the PTSD Checklist-5 (PCL-5) among treatment-seeking military service members. Psychol Assess. (2016) 28:1392–403. doi: 10.1037/pas0000260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kubany ES, Leisen MB, Kaplan AS, Watson SB, Haynes SN, Owens JA, et al. Development and preliminary validation of a brief broad-spectrum measure of trauma exposure: the Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire. Psychol Assess. (2000) 12:210–24. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Landis SK, Sherman MF, Piedmont RL, Kirkhart MW, Rapp EM, Bike DH. The relation between elevation and self-reported prosocial behavior: Incremental validity over the Five-Factor Model of Personality. J Pos Psychol. (2009) 4:71–84. doi: 10.1080/17439760802399208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Erickson TM, Abelson JL. Even the downhearted may be uplifted: Moral elevation in the daily life of clinically depressed and anxious adults. J Social Clin Psychol. (2012) 31:707–28. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2012.31.7.707

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Thompson ER. Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). J Cross-Cult Psychol. (2007) 38:227–42. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Marschall D, Sanftner J, Tangney JP. The State Shame and Guilt Scale. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University (1994).

44. Foa E, Ehler A, Clark D, Tolin D, Orsillo S. The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): development and validation. Psychol Assess. (1999) 11:303–14. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.11.3.303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Sexton MB, Davis MT, Bennett DC, Morris DH, Rauch SAM. A psychometric evaluation of the posttraumatic cognitions inventory with veterans seeking treatment following military trauma exposure. J Affect Disord. (2018) 226:232–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. R Core Team. R: A Language Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2021). Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed May 08, 2021).

Google Scholar

47. Kassambara A. rstatix: Pipe-Friendly Framework for Basic Statistical Tests. (2021). Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/rstatix (accessed May 08, 2021).

48. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Science. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Academic Press Inc. (1988).

Google Scholar

49. Soper D. (2006) Free Statistics Calculators version 4.0. Available online at: https://www.danielsoper.com/statcalc/defalut.aspx (accessed May 08, 2021).

50. Janoff-Bulman R. Characterological versus behavioral self-blame: Inquiries into depression and rape. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1979) 37:1798–809. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Tangney JP, Wagner P, Gramzow R. Proneness to shame, proneness to guilt, and psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol. (1992) 101:469–78. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.101.3.469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Keltner D, Haidt J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, aesthetic emotion. Cognit Emot. (2003) 17:297–314. doi: 10.1080/02699930302297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Monin B. Holier than me: threatening social comparison in the moral domain. In: Kuyper H, editor. Special Issue on Social Comparison. Grenoble: Presses Universitaires deGrenoble (2007).

Google Scholar

54. Sherman DK, Cohen GL. The psychology of self-defense: self-affirmation theory. In: Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 38. San Diego, CF: Academic Press (2006). p. 183–242. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38004-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Siegel JT, Thomson AL, Navarro MA. Experimentally distinguishing elevation from gratitude: oh, the morality. J Pos Psychol. (2014) 9:414–27. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.910825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Startup M, Makgekgenene L, Webster R. The role of self-blame for trauma as assessed by the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): a self-protective cognition? Behav Res Ther. (2007) 45:395–403. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: experimental design, veterans, guilt, shame, post-traumatic cognitions, positive emotion

Citation: McGuire AP, Fagan JG, Howard BAN, Wurm AI and Szabo YZ (2022) Changes in Trauma-Related Cognitions and Emotions After Eliciting Moral Elevation: Examining the Effects of Viewing Others' Virtuous Behavior on Veterans With PTSD. Front. Health Serv. 1:831032. doi: 10.3389/frhs.2021.831032

Received: 07 December 2021; Accepted: 30 December 2021;
Published: 04 February 2022.

Edited by:

Shay-Lee Bolton, University of Manitoba, Canada

Reviewed by:

Rachel A. Hoopsick, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, United States
Sadie Larsen, Milwaukee VA Medical Center, United States

Copyright © 2022 McGuire, Fagan, Howard, Wurm and Szabo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Adam P. McGuire, adam.mcguire@va.gov

Download