Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 18 April 2017
Sec. Pathological Conditions
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00067

Event-Related Potentials Altered in Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder during Working Memory Tasks

Ying Liu1†‡ Mingtian Zhong2†‡ Chang Xi1 Xinhu Jin1 Xiongzhao Zhu1,3 Shuqiao Yao1,3 Jinyao Yi1,3*
  • 1Medical Psychological Center, Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 2Center for Studies of Psychological Application, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
  • 3Medical Psychological Institute, Central South University, Changsha, China

Whereas some studies have demonstrated impaired working memory (WM) among patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD), these findings have not been consistent. Furthermore, there is a lack of neurophysiological evidence about WM function in patients with BPD. The goal of this study was to examine WM function in patients with BPD by using event-related potentials (ERPs). An additional goal was to explore whether characteristics of BPD (i.e., impulsiveness and emotional instability) are associated with WM impairment. A modified version of the N-back task (0- and 2-back) was used to measure WM. ERPs were recorded in 22 BPD patients and 21 age-, handedness-, and sex-matched healthy controls (HCs) while they performed the WM task. The results revealed that there were no significant group differences for behavioral variables (reaction time and accuracy rate) or for latencies and amplitudes of P1 and N1 (all p > 0.05). BPD patients had lower P3 amplitudes and longer N2 latencies than HC, independent of WM load (low load: 0-back; high load: 2-back). Impulsiveness was not correlated with N2 latency or P3 amplitude, and no correlations were found between N2 latency or P3 amplitude and affect intensity scores in any WM load (all p > 0.05). In conclusion, the lower P3 amplitudes and longer N2 latencies in BPD patients suggested that they might have some dysfunction of neural activities in sub-processing in WM, while impulsiveness and negative affect might not have a close relationship with these deficits.

Introduction

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a serious mental disorder that is characterized by a pervasive pattern of instabilities in affect regulation, impulse control, interpersonal relationships, and self-image (van Zutphen et al., 2015; Chanen and Thompson, 2016). Clinical theoreticians and researchers have proposed that the symptoms and behaviors of BPD are associated, at least in part, with disruptions in basic neurocognitive processes, and those neurocognitive impairments may moderate development of BPD (Judd, 2005; Fertuck et al., 2006). Neurocognitive deficits associated with BPD include dysfunctions in attention, concentration, memory and executive functions, such as impulse control, planning and problem solving (Judd, 2005). These dysfunctions might be for a faulty allocation of processing resources (Bazanis et al., 2002), which would be exemplified by a deficit in the efficacy of the central executive component of human working memory (WM; Oberauer et al., 2003).

WM, which supports online maintenance and manipulation of information (Baddeley, 2003), consists of three subcomponents (central executive, phonological storage and spatial information), and provides attention control over other cognitive abilities. But only a few studies have considered WM in BPD, and the results have been inconsistent. For example, whereas some studies found a WM deficit in BPD populations (O’Leary, 2000; Stevens et al., 2004; Lazzaretti et al., 2012), others did not (Sprock et al., 2000; Judd, 2005; Gvirts et al., 2012). When normal populations engage in a WM task, the parietal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex are activated (Curtis, 2006; Fang et al., 2016; Ng et al., 2016). The function of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is abnormal in BPD patients (Lis et al., 2007; Rossi et al., 2015; Krause-Utz et al., 2016). Thus, WM deficits observed in BPD patients might implicate dysfunction in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or parietal cortex. However, there is insufficient direct neurophysiological evidence of WM impairment in BPD patients. Previous studies using accuracy rate and reaction time only generally explored whether WM deficits exist in BPD patients, without examining possible WM subcomponent deficits. Meanwhile, some of these studies didn’t eradicate the influence of medicine, comorbidity with other disorders, and could not conclude the real features of WM in BPD.

Event-related potentials (ERPs) measure electrical brain activity that is time-locked to sensory or cognitive events. ERPs elucidate the time course of ongoing brain activity during WM tasks and reflect the spatiotemporal sequence of cortical information processing (Kayser et al., 2006). ERPs are superior to behavioral or other neuroimaging measures, the latter of which have poor temporal resolution, when seeking information about the cognitive processing stages that contribute to WM abnormalities in BPD. Among the ERP components, N2 and P3 amplitudes have been reliably associated with WM function (Kim et al., 2014; Stroux et al., 2016). As a negative component was typical elicited between 200 ms and 350 ms poststimulus, N2 reflects retrieval of memory representations and perceptual comparisons (Patel and Azzam, 2005; Folstein and Van Petten, 2008). N2 is assumed to be an index of interference control (Donkers and van Boxtel, 2004; Folstein and Van Petten, 2008) and conflict monitoring/resolution. P3, a positive component typically elicited between 300 ms and 600 ms poststimulus, is associated with the general processes of attention control and stimulus categorization/evaluation (Bledowski et al., 2004; Rueda et al., 2004; Neuhaus et al., 2010a,b; Dai and Feng, 2011; Rossi et al., 2015).

Some studies have investigated N2 and P3 in subjects with BPD. For example, Houston et al. (2004) reported that adolescents who had BPD characteristics exhibited decrements in P3 amplitude. Ruchsow et al. (2008) recently found that BPD patients showed reduced P3 amplitude in a Go/Nogo task. However, there have been no studies investigating the N2/P3 characteristics in WM tasks in BPD patients. Several studies have focused on the performances of BPD patients in tasks with variable complexity or cognitive load. Stevens et al. (2004) reported that WM was impaired in BPD subjects, but WM function did not worsen when cognitive load was increased. Lazzaretti et al. (2012) only observed WM deficit in BPD patients when WM demands were high.

In light of this background, we recruited BPD patients and used the N2 and P3 components of ERPs to investigate the WM mechanism of BPD patients while performing an N-back task, while the effects medicine and comorbidity were controlled. Early sensory processing is the foundation of more complex cognition, and it influences WM performance (Tek et al., 2002). To examine whether there are early sensory defects in BPD, we recorded P1 and N1, which might be related to the early sensory stages of information processing (Thomas et al., 2013). Besides, impulsiveness and emotional instability are two core characteristics of BPD (Berlin et al., 2005; Domes et al., 2006; Jacob et al., 2010), and the degree of instability of these characteristics may be correlated with cognition impairment (Ruchsow et al., 2008; Svaldi et al., 2012; Hagenhoff et al., 2013). Therefore, we also examined whether impulsiveness and emotional instability influence WM performance of BPD patients.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Two professional psychiatrists diagnosed patients with BPD using Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV (DSM-IV) criteria (Maffei et al., 1997). Subjects were excluded from this study if they had schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), delusional (paranoid) disorder, bipolar disorder, psychotic disorder, hypothyroidism, or seizure disorder, or any history of head injury, neurosurgery, or substance abuse. Patients with other Axis I/II disorders were also excluded. Patients were not taking any medication at the time of enrollment. The BPD group included 22 right-handed young subjects (14 males, 8 females; age: 22–27 years).

The control group comprised 21 age-, education- and handedness-matched healthy subjects (10 males, 11 females; age: 22–25 years). All subjects in the control group were interviewed by two professional psychiatrists to exclude DSM-IV criteria and other Axis I/II disorders. Criteria for inclusion in the control group were as follows: no current medical problems, no history of substance or alcohol abuse, and no history of psychiatric disorders among first-degree relatives.

All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of “ethics committee of Central South University”. The protocol was approved by the ethics committee of  Central South University. Every subject signed an informed consent form in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. In the case of BPD patients, this consent form was also signed by a well-informed relative.

Psychometric Instruments

Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D)

The CES-D was used to assess depressive levels of participants. This scale consists of 20 items, each of which is assigned a value from 1 to 4. Four items are positively worded and reverse-scored. The total score is computed by summing the 20 items, such that the range of scores is 20–80 (Natamba et al., 2014). The Chinese version of the CES-D has high internal consistency and construct validity (Xiao et al., 2016).

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale—11th Version (BIS-11)

The BIS-11 is one of the most widely used measures of impulsive personality traits. The 30 items are rated from 1 (rarely/never) to 4 (almost always/always). Items are summed to determine the overall impulsiveness score, with higher scores indicating greater impulsivity (Patton et al., 1995). The Chinese translation of the BIS-11 shows sufficient reliability and validity (Yao et al., 2007).

Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ)

The CTQ is a self-administered questionnaire that addresses childhood trauma in the following five areas: physical abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse, physical neglect and emotional neglect. The short form of this questionnaire includes 28 items that are scored on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never true) to 5 (very often true). There are five questions for each type of trauma, scored 5–25, with an additional three questions to assess minimization/denial (Lee et al., 2015). The Chinese version of the CTQ has good reliability and validity (Zhao et al., 2005).

Short Affect Intensity Scale (SAIS)

The SAIS has 20 items that are scored on a six-point Likert scale ranging from 1 to 6 (Geuens and De Pelsmacker, 2002). Three factors are analyzed with this scale: positive intensity (8 items), negative intensity (6 items), and serenity (6 items). There are no total scale scores. The score for each factor is a mean score of the items in that factor; consequently, the scores for individual factors range from 1 to 6. The Chinese version of the SAIS has good reliability and validity (Zhong et al., 2010).

Stimulation and Task Procedures

A modified version of the N-back task was used to measure WM (Blokland et al., 2008; Baller et al., 2013). Participants performed a 0-back task and a 2-back task. As shown in Figure 1, numbers (1–4) of the N-back task were in a fixed position in one of four large white circles. Circles were positioned at each of the corners of a diamond-shaped square on a gray background of the screen. Stimuli were projected by using E-prime 2.0. Using their right index or middle finger, participants pressed one of four buttons to match the target stimulus. In this study, the 0-back task required a simple button press in response to the number displayed, while the 2-back task required participants to press the key corresponding to the number presented in two trials before the current one, therefore, the 2-back task requires on-line monitoring, updating and manipulation of remembered information and is assumed to place great demands on a number of key processes within WM (Glahn et al., 2005; Owen et al., 2005). Differing from traditional N-back task which judged if present number was similar to the n-back one, our task demanded participants to pressed one of four buttons to match the n-back one, which meant that the participants just had 25% chance to guess right, which was more difficult than traditional task.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The paradigm of N-back task. For 0-back, the task required a simple button press in response to the number displayed. For 2-back, participants pressed the key corresponding to the number presented two trials before the current one.

There were 16 blocks and 8 blocks per condition. Each block consisted of 16 trials with a stimulus presentation time of 200 ms and interstimulus interval of 800 ms. There were 16-s rest intervals for subjects when they finished each block. The total experimental length was 8.2 min (492 s).

Electrophysiological Recording and Analysis

As participants completed the N-back task, continuous EEG signals were acquired with a 64 channel electroencephalographeic system (EEG Nuamps, NeuroScan, Inc., El Paso, TX, USA). Electrodes were placed by using a 10/20 extended QuikCap system (NeuroScan, Inc., El Paso, TX, USA). References were placed at a vertex by default and off-line re-referenced to averaged mastoids. Impedanc values were kept lower than 5 KΩ for all electrodes. Horizontal electrooculograms (EOGs) were recorded with electrodes placed on the bilateral external canthi. Vertical EOGs were recorded from electrodes placed above and below the left eye. EEG data were sampled at 1000 Hz and analyzed offline with a 30-Hz low pass filter. Trials with undesired eye movements and eye blink artifacts were removed from analysis by a semiautomatic and manual block rejection procedure.

The continuous EEG was subsequently segmented beginning at 200 ms before stimulus onset and lasting for 800 ms. The baseline for ERP analysis was 100 ms before appearance of the target stimuli. Individual segments were excluded if the absolute voltage of each channel exceeded 100 μV. In each subject, artifact-free trials were averaged for each task (0-back/2-back) to obtain the corresponding ERP waves. Subjects with less than 30 epochs for each task were excluded. All analyses were performed by using Scan 4.3 and Curry 7.0 (NeuroScan, Inc., El Paso, TX, USA).

Three electrode positions (frontal: Fz; central: Cz; parietal: Pz) were chosen for statistical analyses of N2 and P3. Occipital electrodes (O1, Oz and O2) were selected for P1 and N1 because these components are usually maximal at these electrodes. ERP waves were analyzed in terms of peak latency and baseline-to-peak amplitude, as determined by visual inspection. Latency ranges for potentials were designated as follows: 50–150 ms for P1, 140–200 ms for N1, 200–400 ms for N2, and 250–500 ms for P3.

Data Analysis

We used SPSS 17.0 to analyze all data. Demographic data of participants were analyzed by using a Chi-Square or t-test. Reaction time and accuracy data were analyzed by 2 (group: BPD vs. control) × 2 (WM load: 0-back vs. 2-back) repeated-measures ANOVA with WM load as a within-subject factor and group as a between-subject factor. ERP components (latency and amplitude) were analyzed by 2 (group: BPD vs. healthy control (HC)) × 2 (WM load: 0-back vs. 2-back) × 3 (electrodes: O1, Oz, O2/ Fz, Cz, Pz) repeated-measures ANOVA with WM load and electrodes as within-subject factors, and with group as a between-subject factor. Greenhouse–Geisser was used to correct compound symmetry violations in the ANOVAs. Correlations between psychological measures (impulsiveness and different affect intensity) and ERP components were calculated by Pearson’s correlation. Where appropriate, Cohen’s d and η2 were calculated as indices of effect size.

Results

Clinical Data

Clinical data are reported in Table 1. Higher scores for impulsiveness (t = 3.94, p < 0.001) and depression (t = 5.22, p < 0.001) were obtained for BPD patients compared to HCs. BPD patients had higher scores on CTQ subscales of emotional abuse (t = 2.20, p = 0.034), emotional neglect (t = 2.14, p = 0.040), and physical neglect (t = 3.68, p < 0.001), as well as on the SAIS subscale of negative intensity (t = 3.40, p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical features.

Behavioral Results

Mean reaction time and accuracy data for both groups under each condition are reported in Table 2. On mean reaction time, there were no significant group differences (F(1,41) = 0.001, p = 0.979), no significant effects of WM load (F(1,41) = 0.79, p = 0.380), and no significant Group × WM load interaction effects (F(1,41) = 0.22, p = 0.644). However, WM load had a significant effect on accuracy (F(1,41) = 312.38, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.88). Low WM load had a higher accuracy rate (97.7% ± 3.5%) than high WM load (60.9% ± 12.3%), but no significant group differences (F(1,41) = 0.21, p = 0.652) or significant main Group × WM load interactions (F(1,41) = 0.04, p = 0.848) were observed on accuracy.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Reaction time [ms] and performances [%] for N-back tasks.

ERP Components

Mean amplitudes and latencies of P1, N1, N2 and P3 for each WM load are shown for both groups in Table 3. P1 and N1 grand-average ERPs are shown in Figure 2. N2 and P3 grand-average ERPs are shown in Figure 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean ERP amplitude and latency for N-back tasks by group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Grand-average event-related potential (ERP) waveforms of P1, N1 for 0-back and 2-back in borderline personality disorder (BPD) and healthy control (HC) groups. The electrodes of O1, Oz and O2 were chosen to record P1 and N1. P1 and N1 changed under 0-back and 2-back in both groups.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Grand-average ERP waveforms of N2, P3 for 0-back and 2-back in BPD and HC groups. The electrodes of Fz, Cz and Pz were chosen to record N2 and P3. N2 and P3 changed under 0-back and 2-back in both groups.

P1 Amplitude

The main effect of WM load in P1 amplitude was significant (F(1,41) = 18.38, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.31). P1 amplitude was more positive under low WM load (4.16 ± 3.15 μV) compared to high WM load (2.67 ± 2.56 μV). The effect of electrodes was also significant (F(2,82) = 14.06, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.26), the amplitude of Oz was the minimum (2.61 ± 2.49 μV). The group difference in P1 amplitude, interactions of WM load × Electrodes, WM load × Group, Electrodes × Group, and WM load × Group × Electrodes were not significant (all F < 2.60, p > 0.08).

P1 Latency

The repeated-measures ANOVA on P1 latency showed significant main effect of electrodes (F(2,41) = 10.52, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.20) and interaction of WM load × Electrodes (F(2,41) = 8.58, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.17). Further analysis revealed that O2 had a shorter latency (102.20 ± 14.25 ms) compared to O1 (110.06 ± 16.47 ms, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.51) or Oz (108.87 ± 21.19 ms, p = 0.017, Cohen’s d = 0.37) under low WM load. Oz had a shorter latency (102.31 ± 20.22 ms) compared to O1 (110.40 ± 17.33 ms, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.43) or O2 (106.48 ± 16.34 ms, p = 0.016, Cohen’s d = 0.23) under high WM load. The group difference, main effect of WM load and remaining interactions were not significant (all F < 1.09, p > 0.336).

N1 Amplitude

The ANOVA conducted on N1 amplitude revealed significant main effects of WM load (F(1,41) = 27.03, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.40), electrodes (F(2,82) = 12.18, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.23). The interaction of WM load × Electrodes was also significant (F(2,82) = 5.47, p = 0.011, η2 = 0.12), post hoc analysis showed that the N1 amplitude was smallest in O1 under both low (−2.58 ± 3.47 μV) and high (−4.19 ± 3.39 μV) WM load. The group difference and the remaining interactions were not significant (all F < 2.01, p > 0.152).

N1 Latency

Electrodes exhibited a main effect on N1 latency (F(2,82) = 4.66, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.10). The N1 latency of Oz (149.26 ± 23.31 ms) was shorter than the N1 latency of O1 (156.39 ± 20.41 ms, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.33) or O2 (154.86 ± 17.36 ms, p = 0.040, Cohen’s d = 0.27), but there was no significant difference between O1 and O2 (p = 0.572). None of the group difference, main effect of WM load and remaining interactions were significant (all F < 1.80, p > 0.188).

N2 Amplitude

There were significant main effects of WM load (F(1,41) = 30.54, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.43) and electrodes (F(2,82) = 10.47, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.20). The interaction of WM load × Electrodes (F(2,82) = 12.12, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.23) was also significant, N2 amplitude was smaller for Pz (−3.35 ± 4.35 μV) than for Fz (−5.83 ± 3.60 μV, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.62) or Cz (−5.85 ± 4.38 μV, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.57) under low load, while the amplitude of Fz (−3.13 ± 2.77 μV) was larger than that of Pz (−1.78 ± 3.34 μV, p = 0.010, Cohen’s d = 0.44) or Cz (−2.22 ± 3.09 μV, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.31) under high WM load. The group difference and other interactions were not significant (all F < 2.02, p > 0.151).

N2 Latency

A significant group difference was found in N2 latency (F(1,41) = 4.33, p = 0.044, η2 = 0.10). BPD patients had longer latencies (257.34 ± 20.03 ms) than HC (244.66 ± 19.91 ms). The main effect of electrodes (F(2,82) = 23.97, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.37) and the Group × Electrode interaction (F(2,82) = 3.95, p = 0.034, η2 = 0.09) were also significant, indicating that BPD patients had a longer latency (249.64 ± 35.28 ms) than HC (226.21 ± 33.47 ms, p = 0.011) in the Pz electrode only. The main effect of WM load and other interactions were not significant (all F < 3.63, p > 0.064).

P3 Amplitude

Significant group difference was found in P3 amplitude (F(1,41) = 5.62, p = 0.023, η2 = 0.12), BPD patients had a smaller P3 amplitude (5.03 ± 2.61 μV) compared to controls (7.24 ± 3.46 μV). WM load (F(1,41) = 66.46, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.62), electrodes (F(2,82) = 41.71, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.50) and WM load × Electrodes interaction (F(2,82) = 20.68, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.33) all had significant effects. Post hoc analysis showed that P3 amplitude was significant larger for Pz (10.92 ± 5.18 μV) compared to Fz (5.73 ± 4.24 μV, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.10) or Cz (8.99 ± 5.36 μV, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.37) under low WM load, the P3 amplitude for Fz (2.85 ± 3.19 μV) was smaller than for Cz (3.96 ± 3.12 μV, p = 0.003, Cohen’s d = 0.35) or Pz (4.34 ± 3.04 μV, p = 0.0311, Cohen’s d = 0.48) under high WM load. The remaining interactions were not significant (all F < 1.36, p > 0.258).

P3 Latency

Electrodes significantly affected P3 latency (F(2,82) = 6.98, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.15). Fz had a longer latency (372.37 ± 27.46 ms) than Cz (365.03 ± 22.76 ms, p = 0.030, Cohen’s d = 0.29) or Pz (357.85 ± 24.51 ms, p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.56). Furthermore, Cz had a longer latency than Pz (p = 0.033). There was no significant group difference or main effect of WM load, as well as no significant interactions for WM load × Group, Electrodes × Group, WM load × Electrodes, or WM load × Electrodes × Group (all F < 3.02, p > 0.09).

Correlations among N2 Latency, P3 Amplitude and Psychological Measures

Pearson’s correlation analyses showed that N2 latency was not significantly correlated with P3 amplitude in the 0-back task (r = −0.05, p = 0.731) or 2-back task (r = −0.25, p = 0.100).

Pearson’s correlation analyses showed that impulsiveness, as detected by the BIS, was not correlated with N2 latency under any WM load. There were no significant correlations between N2 latency and affect intensity scores, or between P3 amplitude and impulsiveness or affect intensity scores (all p > 0.05).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first investigation of WM in BPD by ERPs. Our behavioral results showed no differences between BPD patients and HCs in N-back task. Although there were no significant group differences in accuracy or mean reaction time, the BPD group showed lower P3 amplitude and longer N2 latency results compared to the control group. As expected, we found that the accuracy and the P1, N2 and P3 amplitudes decreased as WM load increased. Nevertheless, the reduced P3 amplitude and longer N2 latency in the BPD group were independent of WM load. Meanwhile, in this study, neither impulsiveness nor negative affect was the main factor lead to the deficits of WM in BPD.

Despite the lack of general agreement about its functional role, N2 has been correlated with ease of visual information encoding (Nittono et al., 2007). N2 latency has been used as a physiological marker of the timing of access to different properties of a stimulus (Folstein and Van Petten, 2008). The finding that N2 latency was longer in BPD patients than in HCs suggests that stimulus analysis and evaluation during information encoding in WM might be slower in BPD patients compared to healthy individuals.

BPD patients showed lower P3 amplitudes than HCs. P3 amplitude in the WM task is related to the allocation of attention necessary for WM functioning. Kok (2001) reported a relationship between P3 amplitude and attentional resource allocation. Linden (2005) found P3 to be related to brain regions involved in attention, such as the parietal lobe, temporo-parietal junction, lateral prefrontal areas, and cingulate gyrus. Studies about ADHD (Kim et al., 2014; Stroux et al., 2016), which is often comorbid with BPD (Speranza et al., 2011), concluded that a diminished P3 amplitude could be interpreted as an inefficient allocation of attention in WM. These observations suggest that there is abnormal neural activity on allocating attention resources in BPD patients.

The obvious WM load effect in our study proved the effectiveness of the N-back task. However, the reduced P3 amplitude and longer N2 latency results were independent of WM load, consistent with a previous report that WM is impaired in BPD subjects regardless of WM load (Stevens et al., 2004). The N2 latency WM load-independence might occur because the speed of processing for a 0-back task is not different from that of a 2-back task. For P3 amplitude, consistent with other studies (Posner et al., 2002; Stevens et al., 2004; Lazzaretti et al., 2012), we found no correlation between P3 amplitude and affect intensity or impulsiveness suggested that the diminished P3 amplitude was not modulated by affect intensity and impulsiveness. Previous studies found that the information processing stages are not parallel, and those impairments in one stage may affect other stages (Di Russo et al., 2010; Portella et al., 2012). We did not observe any correlation between N2 latency and P3 amplitude, which suggests that the attenuated P3 amplitude might not be related to the compensation of the longer N2 latency. Based on these findings, we speculate that there was a general dysfunctional attention allocation in WM rather than specific problems due to increased demand on the WM load in BPD patients.

Hagenhoff et al. (2013) reported that perceptual processing and response selection are unaffected in BPD. Similarly to that report, our findings suggested that there were no group differences in P1 and N1. Both P1 and N1 are associated with early, rapid processing of stimuli, encoding, attentional focus and discrimination. These results suggest that BPD patients might not have deficits in the early phase of WM.

No differences of behavioral results were found between BPD group and HC group, which were inconsistent with our ERPs results. These inconsistent findings between behavioral data and imaging data were also found in many previous imaging studies (Karayanidis et al., 2000; Ruchsow et al., 2008; Myatchin et al., 2009), which suggested that EPRs results might be more sensitive, efficient and convinced than behavioral data.

The results of lower P3 amplitudes and longer N2 latencies in BPD patients revealed that BPD patients might have some abnormal brain activities in sub-processing of WM, especially in the process of allocating attention resources and the speed of stimulus analysis and evaluation during information encoding, furthermore, these abnormalities were independent of WM load. Although these detriments might not always be observed in behavioral performance, these findings could also provide some theoretical supports for the dysfunction of WM or cognition in patients with BPD. Even no significant correlations were found between ERPs indexes (N2 latency and P3 amplitude) and impulsiveness or negative affect intensity, we still could not conclude that the abnormalities of ERPs during WM tasks are independent of the symptoms of BPD, just as Fertuck et al. (2006) concluded that cognitive impairment is a key moderator in the development of BPD, influences the formation of insecure disorganized attachment and dissociation and interferes with cognitive development in the interpersonal arena. In the future, more attention should be paid to the relationship between impairment of WM and symptoms of BPD, and the WM and other cognition function should be considered in the diagnosis and the development of BPD. Fertuck et al. (2006) has found that BPD patients with higher executive control and higher performance on visual memory tasks were more likely to finish treatment, in this study, we also found that BPD patients had some dysfunctions of neural activity when finishing WM tasks, which suggested that BPD patients might be hard to allocate more attention resources effectively on the process of treatment, therefore, WM training and other cognitive training might have some advantages in improving the effectiveness of treatment in BPD.

This study has several limitations. First, although there is high temporal resolution, the poor spatial resolution of ERPs makes it difficult to identify which brain regions are related to the impairment of WM in BPD patients. Second, no patients had taken medicine in this cross-sectional study. Therefore, it is unclear whether medicine or psychological treatments would reduce impairment of WM. Further studies tracking the performance of BPD patients after taking medicine would help us to identify which impairment of WM are related to symptoms of BPD. Third, previous research suggests there are sex differences in BPD (Hoertel et al., 2014), whereas we did not analyze differences of WM between males and females with BPD. Therefore, sex differences of WM in BPD should be considered in the future. Finally, although no correlation was found between negative emotion and BPD performance in WM, an emotional N-back task should be used to explore the relationship between impaired WM and emotional dysfunction directly, since instability in affect regulation is a core symptom in BPD patients.

Author Contributions

YL was responsible for analyzing data and writing the manuscript. MZ designed the experiment and wrote the manuscript with YL. CX, XJ, XZ and SY were responsible for data collection. JY was responsible for designing the study and revising the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all research members for their help, and all the subjects for their participation. This study was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant nos. 81370034 and 81000590).

References

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baller, E. B., Wei, S.-M., Kohn, P. D., Rubinow, D. R., Alarcón, G., Schmidt, P. J., et al. (2013). Abnormalities of dorsolateral prefrontal function in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder: a multimodal neuroimaging study. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 305–314. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12030385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bazanis, E., Rogers, R. D., Dowson, J. H., Taylor, P., Meux, C., Staley, C., et al. (2002). Neurocognitive deficits in decision-making and planning of patients with DSM-III-R borderline personality disorder. Psychol. Med. 32, 1395–1405. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlin, H. A., Rolls, E. T., and Iversen, S. D. (2005). Borderline personality disorder, impulsivity, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 2360–2373. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bledowski, C., Prvulovic, D., Goebel, R., Zanella, F. E., and Linden, D. E. J. (2004). Attentional systems in target and distractor processing: a combined ERP and fMRI study. Neuroimage 22, 530–540. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blokland, G. A. M., McMahon, K. L., Hoffman, J., Zhu, G., Meredith, M., Martin, N. G., et al. (2008). Quantifying the heritability of task-related brain activation and performance during the N-back working memory task: a twin fMRI study. Biol. Psychol. 79, 70–79. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chanen, A. M., and Thompson, K. N. (2016). Prescribing and borderline personality disorder. Aust. Prescr. 39, 49–53. doi: 10.18773/austprescr.2016.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, C. E. (2006). Prefrontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory. Neuroscience 139, 173–180. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, Q., and Feng, Z. (2011). Deficient interference inhibition for negative stimuli in depression: an event-related potential study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 52–61. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Russo, F., Bultrini, A., Brunelli, S., Delussu, A. S., Polidori, L., Taddei, F., et al. (2010). Benefits of sports participation for executive function in disabled athletes. J. Neurotrauma 27, 2309–2319. doi: 10.1089/neu.2010.1501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domes, G., Winter, B., Schnell, K., Vohs, K., Fast, K., and Herpertz, S. C. (2006). The influence of emotions on inhibitory functioning in borderline personality disorder. Psychol. Med. 36, 1163–1172. doi: 10.1017/s0033291706007756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donkers, F. C. L., and van Boxtel, G. J. M. (2004). The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain Cogn. 56, 165–176. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, X., Zhang, Y., Zhou, Y., Cheng, L., Li, J., Wang, Y., et al. (2016). Resting-state coupling between core regions within the central-executive and salience networks contributes to working memory performance. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:27. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fertuck, E. A., Lenzenweger, M. F., Clarkin, J. F., Hoermann, S., and Stanley, B. (2006). Executive neurocognition, memory systems, and borderline personality disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 346–375. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, J. R., and Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology 45, 152–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geuens, M., and De Pelsmacker, P. (2002). Developing a short affect intensity scale. Psychol. Rep. 91, 657–670. doi: 10.2466/pr0.91.6.657-670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glahn, D. C., Ragland, J. D., Abramoff, A., Barrett, J., Laird, A. R., Bearden, C. E., et al. (2005). Beyond hypofrontality: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of working memory in schizophrenia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 60–69. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gvirts, H. Z., Harari, H., Braw, Y., Shefet, D., Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., and Levkovitz, Y. (2012). Executive functioning among patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD) and their relatives. J. Affect. Disord. 143, 261–264. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagenhoff, M., Franzen, N., Koppe, G., Baer, N., Scheibel, N., Sammer, G., et al. (2013). Executive functions in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 210, 224–231. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoertel, N., Peyre, H., Wall, M. M., Limosin, F., and Blanco, C. (2014). Examining sex differences in DSM-IV borderline personality disorder symptom expression using Item Response Theory (IRT). J. Psychiatr. Res. 59, 213–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houston, R. J., Bauer, L. O., and Hesselbrock, V. M. (2004). Effects of borderline personality disorder features and a family history of alcohol or drug dependence on P300 in adolescents. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 53, 57–70. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacob, G. A., Gutz, L., Bader, K., Lieb, K., Tüscher, O., and Stahl, C. (2010). Impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: impairment in self-report measures, but not behavioral inhibition. Psychopathology 43, 180–188. doi: 10.1159/000304174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judd, P. H. (2005). Neurocognitive impairment as a moderator in the development of borderline personality disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 17, 1173–1196. doi: 10.1017/s0954579405050558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karayanidis, F., Robaey, P., Bourassa, M., De Koning, D., Geoffroy, G., and Pelletier, G. (2000). ERP differences in visual attention processing between attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and control boys in the absence of performance differences. Psychophysiology 37, 319–333. doi: 10.1017/s004857720098051x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kayser, J., Tenke, C. E., Gates, N. A., Kroppmann, C. J., Gil, R. B., and Bruder, G. E. (2006). ERP/CSD indices of impaired verbal working memory subprocesses in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology 43, 237–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00398.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., Liu, Z., Glizer, D., Tannock, R., and Woltering, S. (2014). Adult ADHD and working memory: neural evidence of impaired encoding. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1596–1603. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.12.094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38, 557–577. doi: 10.1017/s0048577201990559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause-Utz, A., Keibel-Mauchnik, J., Ebner-Priemer, U., Bohus, M., and Schmahl, C. (2016). Classical conditioning in borderline personality disorder: an fMRI study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 266, 291–305. doi: 10.1007/s00406-015-0593-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazzaretti, M., Morandotti, N., Sala, M., Isola, M., Frangou, S., De Vidovich, G., et al. (2012). Impaired working memory and normal sustained attention in borderline personality disorder. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 24, 349–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2011.00630.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, I., Strawn, J. R., Dwivedi, A. K., Walters, M., Fleck, A., Schwieterman, D., et al. (2015). Childhood trauma in patients with self-reported stress-precipitated seizures. Epilepsy Behav. 51, 210–214. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linden, D. E. J. (2005). The p300: where in the brain is it produced and what does it tell us? Neuroscientist 11, 563–576. doi: 10.1177/1073858405280524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lis, E., Greenfield, B., Henry, M., Guilé, J. M., and Dougherty, G. (2007). Neuroimaging and genetics of borderline personality disorder: a review. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 32, 162–173.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Maffei, C., Fossati, A., Agostoni, I., Barraco, A., Bagnato, M., Deborah, D., et al. (1997). Interrater reliability and internal consistency of the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis II personality disorders (SCID-II), version 2.0. J. Pers. Disord. 11, 279–284. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1997.11.3.279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myatchin, I., Mennes, M., Wouters, H., Stiers, P., and Lagae, L. (2009). Working memory in children with epilepsy: an event-related potentials study. Epilepsy Res. 86, 183–190. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2009.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Natamba, B. K., Achan, J., Arbach, A., Oyok, T. O., Ghosh, S., Mehta, S., et al. (2014). Reliability and validity of the center for epidemiologic studies-depression scale in screening for depression among HIV-infected and -uninfected pregnant women attending antenatal services in northern Uganda: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry 14:303. doi: 10.1186/s12888-014-0303-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuhaus, A. H., Trempler, N. R., Hahn, E., Luborzewski, A., Karl, C., Hahn, C., et al. (2010a). Evidence of specificity of a visual P3 amplitude modulation deficit in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 124, 119–126. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuhaus, A. H., Urbanek, C., Opgen-Rhein, C., Hahn, E., Ta, T. M. T., Koehler, S., et al. (2010b). Event-related potentials associated with attention network test. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 76, 72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, H. B. T., Kao, K.-L. C., Chan, Y. C., Chew, E., Chuang, K. H., and Chen, S. H. A. (2016). Modality specificity in the cerebro-cerebellar neurocircuitry during working memory. Behav. Brain Res. 305, 164–173. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.02.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nittono, H., Shibuya, Y., and Hori, T. (2007). Anterior N2 predicts subsequent viewing time and interest rating for novel drawings. Psychophysiology 44, 687–696. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00539.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Leary, K. M. (2000). Borderline personality disorder. Neuropsychological testing results. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 23, 41–60, vi. doi: 10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70142-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberauer, K., Süß, H.-M., Wilhelm, O., and Wittman, W. W. (2003). The multiple faces of working memory: storage, processing, supervision, and coordination. Intelligence 31, 167–193. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00115-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., and Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, S. H., and Azzam, P. N. (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: selected studies of the event-related potential. Int. J. Med. Sci. 2, 147–154. doi: 10.7150/ijms.2.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., and Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portella, C., Machado, S., Arias-Carrión, O., Sack, A. T., Silva, J. G., Orsini, M., et al. (2012). Relationship between early and late stages of information processing: an event-related potential study. Neurol. Int. 4:e16. doi: 10.4081/ni.2012.e16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Vizueta, N., Levy, K. N., Evans, D. E., Thomas, K. M., et al. (2002). Attentional mechanisms of borderline personality disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 99, 16366–16370. doi: 10.1073/pnas.252644699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, R., Lanfredi, M., Pievani, M., Boccardi, M., Rasser, P. E., Thompson, P. M., et al. (2015). Abnormalities in cortical gray matter density in borderline personality disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 30, 221–227. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruchsow, M., Groen, G., Kiefer, M., Buchheim, A., Walter, H., Martius, P., et al. (2008). Response inhibition in borderline personality disorder: event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task. J. Neural Transm. 115, 127–133. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0819-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., and Davis-Stober, C. P. (2004). Development of the time course for processing conflict: an event-related potentials study with 4 year olds and adults. BMC Neurosci. 5:39. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-5-39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speranza, M., Revah-Levy, A., Cortese, S., Falissard, B., Pham-Scottez, A., and Corcos, M. (2011). ADHD in adolescents with borderline personality disorder. BMC Psychiatry 11:158. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprock, J., Rader, T. J., Kendall, J. P., and Yoder, C. Y. (2000). Neuropsychological functioning in patients with borderline personality disorder. J. Clin. Psychol. 56, 1587–1600. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(200012)56:12<1587::AID-9>3.0.CO;2-G

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, A., Burkhardt, M., Hautzinger, M., Schwarz, J., and Unckel, C. (2004). Borderline personality disorder: impaired visual perception and working memory. Psychiatry Res. 125, 257–267. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2003.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroux, D., Shushakova, A., Geburek-Hofer, A. J., Ohrmann, P., Rist, F., and Pedersen, A. (2016). Deficient interference control during working memory updating in adults with ADHD: an event-related potential study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 452–463. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svaldi, J., Philipsen, A., and Matthies, S. (2012). Risky decision-making in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 197, 112–118. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tek, C., Gold, J., Blaxton, T., Wilk, C., McMahon, R. P., and Buchanan, R. W. (2002). Visual perceptual and working memory impairments in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 146–153. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.2.146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, S. J., Gonsalvez, C. J., and Johnstone, S. J. (2013). Neural time course of threat-related attentional bias and interference in panic and obsessive-compulsive disorders. Biol. Psychol. 94, 116–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Zutphen, L., Siep, N., Jacob, G. A., Goebel, R., and Arntz, A. (2015). Emotional sensitivity, emotion regulation and impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: a critical review of fMRI studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 51, 64–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, J., Qiu, Y., He, Y., Cui, L., Auerbach, R. P., McWhinnie, C. M., et al. (2016). ‘Weakest link’ as a cognitive vulnerability within the hopelessness theory of depression in chinese university students. Stress Health 32, 20–27. doi: 10.1002/smi.2571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, S., Yang, H., and Zhu, X. (2007). An examination of the psychometric properties of the chinese version of the barratt impulsiveness scale, 11th version in a sample of chinese adolescents. Percept. Mot. Skills 104, 1169–1182. doi: 10.2466/pms.104.3.1169-1182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, X., Zhang, Y., Li, L., Zhou, Y., Li, H., and Yang, S. (2005). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of childhood trauma questionnaire. Chin. J. Clin. Rehabil. 9, 105–107. doi: 10.3321/j.issn:1673-8225.2005.20.052

CrossRef Full Text

Zhong, M., Yi, J., and Ling, Y. (2010). The reliability and validity of the short affect intensity scale-Chin. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 18, 556–558.

Keywords: borderline personality disorder, working memory, event-related potential, N-back task, workload

Citation: Liu Y, Zhong M, Xi C, Jin X, Zhu X, Yao S and Yi J (2017) Event-Related Potentials Altered in Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder during Working Memory Tasks. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:67. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00067

Received: 10 October 2016; Accepted: 03 April 2017;
Published: 18 April 2017.

Edited by:

Nicholas Morton, Tickhill Road Hospital, UK

Reviewed by:

Alexander Easton, Durham University, UK
Johannes Schiebener, University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany

Copyright © 2017 Liu, Zhong, Xi, Jin, Zhu, Yao and Yi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jinyao Yi, jinyaoyi2001@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Co-first authors.

Download